Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 August 2021
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic When the Body Feels Like Mine: Constructing and Deconstructing the Sense of Body Ownership Through the Lifespan View all 12 articles

Could Brain–Computer Interface Be a New Therapeutic Approach for Body Integrity Dysphoria?

  • 1Occupational Therapy, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Christian Medical College and Hospital, Vellore, India
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience and School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland

Patients suffering from body integrity dysphoria (BID) desire to become disabled, arising from a mismatch between the desired body and the physical body. We focus here on the most common variant, characterized by the desire for amputation of a healthy limb. In most reported cases, amputation of the rejected limb entirely alleviates the distress of the condition and engenders substantial improvement in quality of life. Since BID can lead to life-long suffering, it is essential to identify an effective form of treatment that causes the least amount of alteration to the person’s anatomical structure and functionality. Treatment methods involving medications, psychotherapy, and vestibular stimulation have proven largely ineffective. In this hypothesis article, we briefly discuss the characteristics, etiology, and current treatment options available for BID before highlighting the need for new, theory driven approaches. Drawing on recent findings relating to functional and structural brain correlates of BID, we introduce the idea of brain–computer interface (BCI)/neurofeedback approaches to target altered patterns of brain activity, promote re-ownership of the limb, and/or attenuate stress and negativity associated with the altered body representation.

Introduction

For most people, the thought of becoming physically disabled is troubling and something to be avoided at all costs. For patients suffering from body integrity dysphoria (BID) however, the desire to become disabled is characteristic, arising from a mismatch between the desired body and the physical body in terms of functionality or shape. In its most common variant, i.e., the desire for amputation, on which we focus here, the defining features include an intense feeling of inappropriateness concerning their current body configuration. BID patients desire to become physically disabled in order to feel “complete” and become the person they envisage themselves to be (First, 2005; White, 2014). Ironically, achieving this completeness often involves desiring to amputate an otherwise healthy and fully functioning limb, raising many ethical and medical considerations. In most reported cases, amputation of the rejected limb entirely alleviates the distress of the condition and engenders substantial improvement in quality of life (Noll and Kasten, 2014). Individuals with BID are non-psychotic and do not otherwise present major psychiatric or neurological disorders. Sufferers often describe themselves as being trans-abled, drawing a parallel with transgendered individuals.

Since BID can lead to life-long suffering, it is essential to identify an effective form of treatment that causes the least amount of alteration to the person’s anatomical structure and functionality. Current forms of treatment such as amputation are extreme and lead to a permanent disability and presumably dependence on carers for activities of daily living. Other treatment methods such as medications, psychotherapy, and vestibular stimulation have proven largely ineffective (Ryan, 2008; Blom et al., 2012). In this article, we briefly discuss the characteristics, etiology, and current treatment options available for BID before highlighting the need for new, theory driven approaches. Drawing on recent findings relating to functional and structural brain correlates of BID, we introduce the idea of brain–computer interface (BCI)/neurofeedback approaches to target altered patterns of brain activity, promote re-ownership of the limb, and/or attenuate stress and negativity associated with the altered body representation.

Characteristics and Etiology of BID

In order to provide an in-depth evaluation of BID and its manifestations, it may be helpful to first differentiate from other similar conditions such as body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) and somatoparaphrenia, among others (Table 1). BID shares similarities with BDD, because individuals with BID are also found to be preoccupied with certain aspects of their body (Grocholewski et al., 2018). However, there are some key differences. The dysphoria experienced by individuals with BDD is derived from their concerns over how a part of their body physically appears to others (First and Fisher, 2012). Specifically, individuals with BDD believe that some aspect of their physical appearance is defective and as such is a source of intense embarrassment, shame, and low self-esteem.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Taxonomy of disorders related to body integrity dysphoria.

Somatoparaphrenia (SPP) is characterized by a denial of ownership combined with confabulatory signs (Gerstmann, 1942). It affects predominantly the left side of the body (McGeoch et al., 2011). Two main clinical features separate it from BID. First, while BID likely develops in childhood and may be related to amputation-related experiences (Barrow and Oyebode, 2019), SPP is an acquired condition in response to brain damage, typically of the right hemisphere, that typically also causes contralateral left limb paralysis. Moreover, while BID individuals recognize the unwanted limb as part of their current but undesired body, SPP is a disownership disorder, primarily characterized by a positive component, reflecting a “distorted mental representation of reality” (Bottini et al., 2009, p. 589), that is the delusional misattribution of the paralyzed limb to someone else (Gerstmann, 1942; Romano et al., 2014; Saetta et al., 2020). Another related clinical condition is asomatognosia, which is caused by a similar (more often a right lateral) cerebral lesion as SPP. In this condition, however, the limb is felt as non-existing rather than disowned (Critchley, 1954; Saetta et al., 2021).

Other conditions that have been discussed in relation to BID due to common features are alien hand syndrome and anarchic hand syndrome. For patients with alien hand syndrome, the motor control of the affected limb is disinhibited, and the limb executes smooth but non-volitional reflexive movements of grasping and compulsive manipulation of tools in the environment, i.e., utilization behavior (Doody and Jankovic, 1992; Feinberg et al., 1992). Typical damaged brain areas include fronto-mesial and anterior cingulate areas as well as the anterior corpus callosum (Feinberg et al., 1992). Anarchic hand syndrome (AHS) is a related disorder, which ensues from a relatively circumscribed lesion of the anterior corpus callosum (Feinberg et al., 1992, but see Pacella et al., 2021 for a more recent account). In this syndrome, the individuals’ hands move outside of the volitional control as well (Della Sala, 2009; Jenkinson et al., 2015); however, intermanual conflicts are more typical than utilization behavior.

Gender incongruity/dysphoria (GD) is a condition in which there is a conflict between aspects of the physical body (sex) and the desired body, often leading to sex-reassignment through surgery and lifelong hormonal treatment. Before the release of the DSM-V, GD was referred to as “Gender Identity Disorder.” The GD diagnosis separates this condition from sexual dysfunctions while framing it in terms of incongruence between phenomenological and assigned gender, instead of “cross-gender” identification disorder. Analogously, while BID has been previously defined as “Body Integrity Identity disorder” (First, 2005), the BID nomenclature focuses on the incongruence between the functionality and/or morphology of the current and assigned body. Conducting a direct comparison between attitudes of those with BID and GD (Ostgathe et al., 2014) revealed that both share similar onset (early childhood or adolescence), discontent in the individual with their identity, perceived reduction of desire post-surgical intervention, or through mimicking of desired identity such as cross-dressing in Gender Dysphoria and pretending of disability by using wheelchairs or crutches in BID. Comorbidity of BID with GID has been described in 19% of the cases (First, 2005; Lawrence, 2010). The groups differed, however, in the intensity of their aversion toward the unwanted body parts, with Gender Dysphoric individuals expressing an intense rejection or hatred, whereas BID individuals exhibited an indifference toward the limb. Remarkably, in both BID and GID individuals, the examination of reality is preserved in that the discomfort sensations are “illusions, not delusions” (Garcia-Falgueras, 2014; p. 161), and do not typically persist after surgical intervention (Garcia-Falgueras, 2014).

Before being recently included in the release of the 11th Revision of the International Classification of Diseases (World Health Organization, 2018) as a “disorder of bodily distress or bodily experience,” BID has also been interchangeably referred to as xenomelia, apotemnophilia, and body-integrity identity disorder (BIID). While the lowest common denominator between these nomenclatures is the desire for amputation, each of them underlies a specific etiological hypothesis (Sedda and Bottini, 2014).

Apotemnophilia (from ancient Greek “love for amputation”) proposes psychological or psychiatric features related to sexual disturbances to be the centerpiece of the disorder, whereas Xenomelia (from ancient Greek “foreign limb”) put the disorder in the context of a neurological syndrome originating from alterations of the right parietal lobe (McGeoch et al., 2011). A lack of consensus regarding the nomenclature concerned with these conditions and their etiological basis is a topic of ongoing scientific debate.

Major characteristics observed in the amputation variant of BID include (1) dysphoric feelings about a normal body morphology and/or functionality originating from a mismatch between the mental body image, i.e., the conscious representation of body size and shape, and the perceived body (Saetta et al., 2020), (2) lack of ownership over a limb, (3) excessive attention devoted to the limb, and (4) intact insight regarding the condition and its unusual nature (Giummarra et al., 2011). Initial studies on BID have shown that in the case of a desire for amputation, most patients with BID desired amputation of either a left-sided limb or both limbs (First, 2005).

Ostgathe et al. (2014) found that justifications given by those with BID desiring an amputation included triggering childhood events, restoring a congruence between sentiment and body, feeling attracted by people with physical disabilities, a fascination for physically impaired people, as well as the perception of body impairment as a proper way of living. Typically observed in BID are also paraphilic interests, such as preferences for an amputated sexual partner and sexual arousal by the idea of being an amputee (Blom et al., 2017).

Current Approaches to Treatment for BID

Individuals with BID find symptomatic relief by spending substantial amounts of time engaged in so-called pretending behaviors in which they simulate the desired disability (e.g., walking with one’s leg tied back, sitting in a wheelchair, using crutches, First, 2005). It is believed that the pretending behavior allows for the alignment of the perceived and desired body, reducing distress associated with the mismatch (Saetta et al., 2020).

While this provides some transient relief, the preoccupation with becoming disabled has harmful consequences. Time spent engaging in pretending behaviors interferes with the individual’s productivity, leisure activities, and social functioning. In the case of desired amputation, pretending behaviors such as daily binding up of the leg can also cause injury over extended periods of time. At the most extreme end of the spectrum, attempts to actually achieve the desired disability can have catastrophic effects on the health of the individual, potentially putting their life at risk.

The only truly satisfying solution for BID sufferers seems to be the amputation of the rejected limb (Noll and Kasten, 2014). In most countries, this course of action will not be considered by physicians (Bayne and Levy, 2005) and is strictly illegal, knowing that it renders the individual with a new and unnecessary lifelong physical disability. Thus, alternative approaches targeting the comorbid anxiety and depression symptoms to reduce distress have been the key focus of treatment to date (e.g., Ryan, 2008; Blom et al., 2012). Psychotherapy has been shown to reduce psychological distress, but concurrently increase the individual’s desire for amputation, most likely as a result of intense focus and discussion on the topic of their BID with the therapist (Kröger et al., 2014).

Reports of the effectiveness of pharmacological interventions are very limited, and sample sizes are small, often limited to single case reports. One such case report suggested a reduction of distress in one individual with BID from a course of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), and limited effects of psychotherapy (Braam et al., 2006). Another report with 25 BID patients found that 12 had been prescribed medications including antidepressants, neuroleptic drugs, and tranquilizers but with no positive effects on symptoms (Kröger et al., 2014).

Another potential treatment that received prior attention was artificial vestibular stimulation, initially proposed by Ramachandran et al. (2007). As disorders of the bodily self have been linked to the vestibular system for over a century (Bonnier, 2009), artificial vestibular stimulation (caloric or galvanic) has been shown to induce temporary remission of the denial of ownership over a limb in SPP (Lopez, 2013) and other bodily self disorders (Grabherr et al., 2015). Given the above-mentioned parallels between the BID and SPP clinical pictures, vestibular stimulation has been tentatively used to restore limb ownership in BID and thus to reduce the desire for amputation. Despite initial excitement and early adoption of the technique clinically, subsequent research studies have demonstrated that it provides no substantial benefit to individuals with BID (Lenggenhager et al., 2014).

Upon review of the available literature, it is evident that aside from amputation, existing treatment options are largely ineffective, and fresh approaches are required to alleviate distress for those with BID. Furthermore, persisting with the current ineffective treatment programs comes at a financial cost to the patients and healthcare providers. Amputation, although effective, carries the largest financial burden at an estimated $24,010 for the procedure itself with inpatient stay, and $878,927 in associated costs over the lifetime of the amputated individual (United States data according to health insurance claims, Lo et al., 2020). With advances in neuroimaging, the field of BID is set to undergo a paradigm shift, availing of new knowledge on brain structural and functional correlates of the disorder that may provide insights toward potential avenues for treatment exploration. In the next sections, we review recent developments for understanding the neurological mechanisms and consider how these may inform the inception of new theory driven approaches to treatment.

Structural and Functional Connectivity Abnormalities in BID

It has been well established since the mid 20th century that brain lesions in the right parietal lobe lead to altered perception and spatial awareness of body parts (Brain, 1941; Critchley, 1954). This area of the cortex is divided into the superior and inferior parietal lobules (the SPL and the IPL, respectively), with the latter being further subdivided into the supramarginal and angular gyri. Within the last decade, research specifically relating to structural and functional brain abnormalities in BID has emerged. In a recent review of all available neuroimaging evidence since 2012, Fornaro et al. (2020) reported that areas consistently found to be altered in xenomelia include right superior and inferior parietal lobule (rSPL and rIPL), premotor cortex, and right insula. A general pattern across studies emerged that cortical abnormalities tended to be right lateralized, whereas subcortical were left lateralized. Hänggi et al. (2016) specifically focused upon subcortical correlates of BID and found distinctive differences in the shape of several structures involved in somatotopic representation of the leg. Patients had thinner bilateral dorsomedial putamina, left ventromedial caudate nucleus, and left medial pallidum. This was accompanied by thicker bilateral lateral pallida and left frontolateral thalamus.

The aforementioned structural evidence is corroborated by data concerning functional connectivity between brain regions at rest in patients with BID compared to controls. In a relatively large and homogenous sample of BID patients who all desired amputation of the left leg, Saetta et al. (2020) found reduced functional connectivity between the primary sensorimotor area (S1) of the disowned leg and the rest of the brain, and the rSPL with the rest of the brain. Further, gray matter atrophy was evident in the rSPL of BID patients, and the extent of this atrophy correlated with desire for amputation and degree of engagement in pretending behaviors. These findings led Saetta et al. (2020) to suggest that the distressing feeling of non-acceptance of one’s limb in BID might ensue from a discrepancy between preserved projections of somatosensory inputs from the limb to the respective primary cortical areas, and an impaired representation of the body at the highest level of integration, the so-called body image. The authors proposed that only a smooth interplay between sensory and higher level bodily processing may provide the phenomenal experience of a limb belonging to the body and the self. The previously identified neural network for (healthy) body image and ownership has been suggested to span a broad network involving primary sensorimotor, premotor, parietal, insular, and extrastriatal areas (see, e.g., Tsakiris, 2010; Grivaz et al., 2017; Park and Blanke, 2019; Seghezzi et al., 2019; Salvato et al., 2020; for reviews), which were, at least to some extent, also found to be altered in BID (Fornaro et al., 2020).

The presence of underlying neuroanatomical structural abnormalities is evidence that BID has both psychological and neurophysiological components, and that recovery may entail modifying hard wired brain circuitry. However, this does not necessarily imply permanency of the condition as both gray and white matter are highly neuroplastic and structural changes over time can be documented following treatment programs for other neuropsychological disorders including depression (Liu et al., 2015; Bouckaert et al., 2016) and alcohol addiction (Pfefferbaum et al., 2014). By altering functional connectivity using targeted therapeutic interventions to promote desired neural patterns, changes in brain structure often follow over time. For example, using brain–computer interfaces, functional and structural neuroplastic changes can be induced in regions specifically involved in the trained task (Ghaziri et al., 2013; Qian et al., 2018; Marins et al., 2019; Nierhaus et al., 2021; Sampaio-Baptista et al., 2020).

A New Generation of Research Into BID Treatment Using Neurofeedback/Brain–Computer Interface

Brain–computer interfaces and neurofeedback have been used in attempts to tune pathological brain rhythms or patterns of functional connectivity toward more neurotypical patterns (Ros et al., 2014). This is achieved by training individuals to use on-screen (or robotic) feedback of real-time brain activity to self-regulate some aspect of their own neural functioning. While BCI can come in a vast array of different arrangements using different neuroimaging modalities (see Simon et al., 2021 for a review), a commonly used setup involves recording electrical activity from the scalp using electroencephalography (EEG), and presenting feedback on screen in the form of a computer game. With this type of BCI, different aspects of brain activity can be targeted for modification depending on the method used to extract and present feedback. Positive outcomes using this method for symptom reduction have been demonstrated for ADHD (Qian et al., 2018), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD, van der Kolk et al., 2016; Banerjee and Argáez, 2017), and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD, Banerjee and Argáez, 2017; Mennella et al., 2017). In the case of BID, given that distinct functional connectivity patterns are evident involving S1, rSPL, and the rest of the brain (Saetta et al., 2020), there may be value in testing whether targeting this pattern using BCI would result in reduction of distress associated with the desire for amputation. For example, training could provide on-screen feedback in a computer game where rewards are achieved contingent on the strength of connectivity between the aforementioned nodes. Over multiple sessions playing the BCI game, strengthening of connectivity could be hypothesized as participants develop or refine mental strategies, and compared to a control condition where pseudofeedback is delivered. Experimental work would be essential to determine effective mental imagery strategies to recommend for participants (or alternatively whether to allow strategy-free implicit learning), and how many sessions of this type of feedback may be required in order to achieve symptomatic relief.

As Saetta et al. (2020) found that gray matter atrophy in BID correlated with the extent of pretending behaviors (such as binding up the leg and simulating amputation), and desire for amputation, it is also necessary to consider whether brain-related changes are a root cause of the disorder or an effect of the resultant coping behaviors. This too has implications for designing interventions based on structural or functional brain connectivity, as the intervention may be off target if focus is upon an epiphenomenal mechanism arising from the pretending behavior. Even if this is the case, however, BCI approaches to train functional connectivity patterns may still hold value. The simulation of the amputated state may in itself alter brain connectivity patterns for body representations, which may in turn enhance desire for amputation. BCI could be a means to disrupt this loop and find an alternative (less dangerous) method for symptomatic relief that does not perpetuate the altered activity in the brain’s body representation regions.

An alternative but related approach would be to use BCI to implicitly promote re-ownership of the limb and engender more positive associations with it. In a study using a foot-specific variant of the rubber hand illusion, Lenggenhager et al. (2015) demonstrated that individuals with BID behaved similarly to healthy controls in that they could take ownership for a fake rubber foot. When stroked synchronously with their opposite foot, they reported sensations as if it were part of their own body. This implies that in the unwanted limb, not only are the basic senses functioning normally but also that the integration of visual, tactile, and proprioceptive information and visual capture is broadly intact. Thus, the authors suggested that behavioral training could be useful for those with BID based on the fact that they can feel ownership for an artificial limb and deny it for their own limb. Further, recent data from a small case study with two BID individuals revealed that augmented reality (AR) technology could successfully reduce symptoms by engendering the illusion that the unwanted limb is missing (Turbyne et al., 2021).

Some suggest that the desire to amputate arises from a failure to adequately represent the affected limb in the relevant area of cortex. For example, McGeoch et al. (2015) recorded somatosensory evoked fields using magnetoencephalography (MEG) during tactile stimulation of the feet of BID participants. They observed a significant reduction in activity over rSPL in the patients compared to controls. A BCI approach targeting this aspect of BID may involve neurofeedback of motor evoked potentials (MEPs) in response to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). This TMS-NF approach has been shown to increase (or decrease) corticospinal excitability (responsiveness) of the pathways connecting brain to muscle, depending on whether rewarding feedback was provided for large or small MEPs (Ruddy et al., 2018; Liang et al., 2020). In a BID tailored variant of this, it may be possible to incorporate feedback of MEPs from TMS over motor cortical representations of both limbs into the computer game, but bias the feedback over time to gradually reinforce engagement of the unwanted limb. This could be achieved by providing larger or more frequent rewards for large MEPs from the targeted limb. As self-regulation of MEP amplitude is achieved via engagement of motor imagery strategies, over time this may promote re-integration of the limb into the mental schema of body representation/body image. Reducing the discrepancy between actual and desired body representation may subsequently engender more positive affect toward the unwanted limb, albeit implicitly.

Conclusion

As BID is a condition with both psychological and neurophysiological attributes, it is yet unclear whether interventions targeting one of these aspects in isolation can serve any benefit. Further, clarification of the relative mechanistic contribution of body ownership vs. disrupted integration of the limb into the body image will be necessary in order to accurately tailor interventions tackling these facets of the disorder. Upon reviewing the available evidence, what is known to date is that pharmacological and psychological therapies are largely ineffective, and that there are no known neurophysiological interventions that are sufficient to alleviate the suffering associated with the desire for amputation. Amputation itself is an extreme solution raising many ethical issues, and one that will not even be considered by most healthcare providers. Pretending behaviors adopted by BID patients as coping strategies raise health concerns and often have dangerous consequences. Thus, new theory driven approaches that leverage recent advances in neuroimaging and technology are greatly needed in order to improve quality of life for BID patients. Those presented here are intended to stimulate debate among those interested in working toward novel solutions, and may ultimately pave the way for a new dialog on approaches to tackle challenging neuropsychological phenomena.

Author Contributions

SC wrote the first draft and edited the final draft. KR and SC conceptualized the idea for the manuscript. KR, SC, CS, BL, and GS worked on editing subsequent drafts of the manuscript and providing feedback. GS and BL significantly extended the sections of the manuscript. All authors reviewed the final version and were involved in discussions throughout.

Funding

KR and CS were supported by a grant from the Health Research Board of Ireland (HRB-EIA-2019-003). GS and BL received funding from the Swiss National Science Foundation (PP00P1_170511).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Arzy, S., Overney, L. S., Landis, T., and Blanke, O. (2006). Neural mechanisms of embodiment: asomatognosia due to premotor cortex damage. Arch. Neurol. 63, 1022–1025. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.7.1022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atmaca, M., Bingol, I., Aydin, A., Yildirim, H., Okur, I., Yildirim, M. A., et al. (2010). Brain morphology of patients with body dysmorphic disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 123, 258–263. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, S., and Argáez, C. (2017). Neurofeedback and Biofeedback for Mood and Anxiety Disorders: A Review of Clinical Effectiveness and Guidelines. Canadian Agency for Drugs and Technologies in Health. Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK531603/ (accessed July 7, 2021)

Google Scholar

Barrow, E., and Oyebode, F. (2019). Body integrity identity disorder: clinical features and ethical dimensions. BJPsych Adv. 25, 187–195. doi: 10.1192/bja.2018.55

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayne, T., and Levy, N. (2005). Amputees by choice: body integrity identity disorder and the ethics of amputation. J. Appl. Philos. 22, 75–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5930.2005.00293.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., and Metzinger, T. (2009). Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blom, R. M., Hennekam, R. C., and Denys, D. (2012). Body integrity identity disorder. PLoS One 7:e34702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blom, R. M., van der Wal, S. J., Vulink, N. C., and Denys, D. (2017). Role of sexuality in body integrity identity disorder (BIID): a cross-sectional internet-based survey study. J. Sex. Med. 14, 1028–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2017.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blom, R. M., van Wingen, G. A., van der Wal, S. J., Luigjes, J., van Dijk, M. T., Scholte, H. S., et al. (2016). The desire for amputation or paralyzation: evidence for structural brain anomalies in body integrity identity disorder (BIID). PLoS One 11:e0165789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnier, P. (2009). Asomatognosia P. Bonnier. L’aschématie. Revue Neurol 1905;13:605-9. Epilepsy Behav. 16, 401–403. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottini, G., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., and Paulesu, E. (2009). Productive symptoms in right brain damage. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 22, 589–593. doi: 10.1097/wco.0b013e328332c71d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouckaert, F., Dols, A., Emsell, L., De Winter, F.-L., Vansteelandt, K., Claes, L., et al. (2016). Relationship between hippocampal volume, serum BDNF, and depression severity following electroconvulsive therapy in late-life depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 2741–2748. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braam, A. W., Visser, S., Cath, D. C., and Hoogendijk, W. J. G. (2006). Investigation of the syndrome of apotemnophilia and course of a cognitive-behavioural therapy. Psychopathology 39, 32–37. doi: 10.1159/000089661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brain, W. R. (1941). Visual disorientation with special reference to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain 64, 244–272. doi: 10.1093/brain/64.4.244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, M. (1954). The parietal lobes. Neurology 4, 402–402. doi: 10.1212/WNL.4.5.402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Della Sala, S. (2009). Dr. Strangelove syndrome. Cortex 45, 1278–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doody, R. S., and Jankovic, J. (1992). The alien hand and related signs. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 806–810. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.9.806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, T. E., Schindler, R. J., Flanagan, N. G., and Haber, L. D. (1992). Two alien hand syndromes. Neurology 42, 19–24. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.1.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. (2005). Desire for amputation of a limb: paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity disorder. Psychol. Med. 35, 919–928. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., and Fisher, C. E. (2012). Body integrity identity disorder: the persistent desire to acquire a physical disability. Psychopathology 45, 3–14. doi: 10.1159/000330503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornaro, S., Patrikelis, P., and Lucci, G. (2020). When having a limb means feeling overcomplete. Xenomelia, the chronic sense of disownership and the right parietal lobe hypothesis. Laterality 1–20. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2020.1866000 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gandola, M., Invernizzi, P., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Sterzi, R., Sberna, M., et al. (2012). An anatomical account of somatoparaphrenia. Cortex 48, 1165–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Falgueras, A. (2014). gender dysphoria and body integrity identity disorder: similarities and differences. Psychology 05, 160–165. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.52025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerstmann, J. (1942). Problem of imperception of disease and of impaired body territories with organic lesions: relation to body scheme and its disorders. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 48, 890–913. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1942.02290120042003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghaziri, J., Tucholka, A., Larue, V., Blanchette-Sylvestre, M., Reyburn, G., Gilbert, G., et al. (2013). Neurofeedback training induces changes in white and gray matter. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 44, 265–272. doi: 10.1177/1550059413476031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giummarra, M. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Nicholls, M. E. R., Hilti, L. M., and Brugger, P. (2011). Body integrity identity disorder: deranged body processing, right fronto-parietal dysfunction, and phenomenological experience of body incongruity. Neuropsychol. Rev. 21, 320–333. doi: 10.1007/s11065-011-9184-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabherr, L., Macauda, G., and Lenggenhager, B. (2015). The moving history of vestibular stimulation as a therapeutic intervention. Multisens. Res. 28, 653–687. doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grivaz, P., Blanke, O., and Serino, A. (2017). Common and distinct brain regions processing multisensory bodily signals for peripersonal space and body ownership. Neuroimage 147, 602–618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grocholewski, A., Bücker, C., Kasten, E., Osen, B., and Heinrichs, N. (2018). Mental images in body integrity identity disorder (BIID) and in body dysmorphic disorder (BDD): an exploratory study. Behav. Change 35, 174–184. doi: 10.1017/bec.2018.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hänggi, J., Bellwald, D., and Brugger, P. (2016). Shape alterations of basal ganglia and thalamus in xenomelia. Neuroimage Clin. 11, 760–769. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilti, L. M., Hänggi, J., Vitacco, D. A., Kraemer, B., Palla, A., Luechinger, R., et al. (2013). The desire for healthy limb amputation: structural brain correlates and clinical features of xenomelia. Brain 136, 318–329. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, P. M., Edelstyn, N. M. J., Preston, C., and Ellis, S. J. (2015). Anarchic hand with abnormal agency following right inferior parietal lobe damage: a case report. Neurocase 21, 471–478. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2014.925936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kröger, K., Schnell, T., and Kasten, E. (2014). Effects of psychotherapy on patients suffering from Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID). Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 6, 110–115. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20140305.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, A. A. (2010). Sexual orientation versus age of onset as bases for typologies (subtypes) for gender identity disorder in adolescents and adults. Arch. Sex. Behav. 39, 514–545. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9594-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenggenhager, B., Hilti, L., and Brugger, P. (2015). Disturbed body integrity and the “rubber foot illusion.”. Neuropsychology 29, 205–211. doi: 10.1037/neu0000143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenggenhager, B., Hilti, L., Palla, A., Macauda, G., and Brugger, P. (2014). Vestibular stimulation does not diminish the desire for amputation. Cortex 54, 210–212. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, W., Xu, Y., Schmidt, J., Zhang, L., and Ruddy, K. L. (2020). Upregulating excitability of corticospinal pathways in stroke patients using TMS neurofeedback. A pilot study. Neuroimage Clin. 28:102465. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C.-S., Ku, H.-L., Chao, H.-T., Tu, P.-C., Li, C.-T., Cheng, C.-M., et al. (2014). Neural network of body representation differs between transsexuals and cissexuals. PLoS One 9:e85914. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Du, L., Li, Y., Liu, H., Zhao, W., Liu, D., et al. (2015). Antidepressant effects of electroconvulsive therapy correlate with subgenual anterior cingulate activity and connectivity in depression. Medicine 94:e2033. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000002033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, J., Chan, L., and Flynn, S. (2020). A systematic review of the incidence, prevalence, costs, and activity and work limitations of amputation, osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, back pain, multiple sclerosis, spinal cord injury, stroke, and traumatic brain injury in the united states: a 2019 Update. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 102, 115–131.

Google Scholar

Lopez, C. (2013). A neuroscientific account of how vestibular disorders impair bodily self-consciousness. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:91. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marins, T., Rodrigues, E. C., Bortolini, T., Melo, B., Moll, J., and Tovar-Moll, F. (2019). Structural and functional connectivity changes in response to short-term neurofeedback training with motor imagery. Neuroimage 194, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGeoch, P. D., Brang, D., Huang, M., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2015). Primary somatosensory cortex hand representation dynamically modulated by motor output. Neurocase 21, 103–105. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2013.873060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGeoch, P. D., Brang, D., Song, T., Lee, R. R., Huang, M., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2011). Xenomelia: a new right parietal lobe syndrome. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 82, 1314–1319. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-300224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mennella, R., Patron, E., and Palomba, D. (2017). Frontal alpha asymmetry neurofeedback for the reduction of negative affect and anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 92, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moro, V., Pernigo, S., Tsakiris, M., Avesani, R., Edelstyn, N. M. J., Jenkinson, P. M., et al. (2016). Motor versus body awareness: voxel-based lesion analysis in anosognosia for hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia following right hemisphere stroke. Cortex 83, 62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mufaddel, A., Osman, O. T., Almugaddam, F., and Jafferany, M. (2013). A review of body dysmorphic disorder and its presentation in different clinical settings. Prim. Care Companion CNS Disord. 15:PCC.12r01464. doi: 10.4088/PCC.12r01464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, S. (2009). Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID)—Is the amputation of healthy limbs ethically justified? Am. J. Bioeth. 9, 36–43. doi: 10.1080/15265160802588194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nawata, H., Ogomori, K., Tanaka, M., Nishimura, R., Urashima, H., Yano, R., et al. (2010). Regional cerebral blood flow changes in female to male gender identity disorder. Seishin Shinkeigaku Zasshi 64, 157–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.02059.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nierhaus, T., Vidaurre, C., Sannelli, C., Mueller, K., and Villringer, A. (2021). Immediate brain plasticity after one hour of brain–computer interface (BCI). J. Physiol. 599, 2435–2451. doi: 10.1113/JP278118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noll, S., and Kasten, E. (2014). Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID): how satisfied are successful wannabes. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 3, 222–232. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20140306.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostgathe, A., Schnell, T., and Kasten, E. (2014). Body integrity identity disorder and gender dysphoria: a pilot study to investigate similarities and differences. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 314, 138–143. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20140306.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacella, V., Ricciardi, G. K., Bonadiman, S., Verzini, E., Faraoni, F., Scandola, M., et al. (2021). The role of white matter disconnection in the symptoms relating to the anarchic hand syndrome: a single case study. Brain Sci. 11:632. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11050632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, H.-D., and Blanke, O. (2019). Coupling inner and outer body for self-consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 377–388. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfefferbaum, A., Rosenbloom, M. J., Chu, W., Sassoon, S. A., Rohlfing, T., Pohl, K. M., et al. (2014). White matter microstructural recovery with abstinence and decline with relapse in alcohol dependence interacts with normal ageing: a controlled longitudinal DTI study. Lancet Psychiatry 1, 202–212.

Google Scholar

Qian, X., Loo, B. R. Y., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, S., Koh, H. L., Poh, X. W. W., et al. (2018). Brain-computer-interface-based intervention re-normalizes brain functional network topology in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 8:149. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0213-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramachandran, V. S., Brang, D., McGeoch, P. D., and Rosar, W. (2009). Sexual and food preference in apotemnophilia and anorexia: interactions between “beliefs” and “needs” regulated by two-way connections. between body image and limbic structures. Perception 38, 775–777. doi: 10.1068/p6350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramachandran, V. S., McGeoch, P. D., and Williams, L. (2007). Can vestibular caloric stimulation be used to treat Dejerine–Roussy Syndrome? Med. Hypotheses 69, 486–488. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano, D., Gandola, M., Bottini, G., and Maravita, A. (2014). Arousal responses to noxious stimuli in somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia: clues to body awareness. Brain 137(Pt 4), 1213–1223. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano, D., and Maravita, A. (2019). The dynamic nature of the sense of ownership after brain injury. Clues from asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. Neuropsychologia 132, 107119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ros, T. J., Baars, B., Lanius, R. A., and Vuilleumier, P. (2014). Tuning pathological brain oscillations with neurofeedback: a systems neuroscience framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:1008. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddy, K., Balsters, J., Mantini, D., Liu, Q., Kassraian-Fard, P., Enz, N., et al. (2018). Neural activity related to volitional regulation of cortical excitability. eLife 7:e40843. doi: 10.7554/eLife.40843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, C. J. (2008). Out on a limb: the ethical management of body integrity identity disorder. Neuroethics 13, 21–33. doi: 10.1007/s12152-008-9026-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saetta, G., Hänggi, J., Gandola, M., Zapparoli, L., Salvato, G., Berlingeri, M., et al. (2020). Neural correlates of body integrity dysphoria. Curr. Biol. 30, 2191–2195.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saetta, G., Zindel-Geisseler, O., Stauffacher, F., Serra, C., Vannuscorps, G., and Brugger, P. (2021). Asomatognosia: structured interview and assessment of visuomotor imagery. Front. Psychol. 11:544544. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.544544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvato, G., Richter, F., Sedeño, L., Bottini, G., and Paulesu, E. (2020). Building the bodily self-awareness: evidence for the convergence between interoceptive and exteroceptive information in a multilevel kernel density analysis study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 401–418. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampaio-Baptista, C., Neyedli, H. F., Sanders, Z.-B., Diosi, K., Havard, D., Huang, Y., et al. (2020). Neurofeedback fMRI in the motor system elicits bi-directional changes in activity and white-matter structure in the healthy adult human brain. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.08.06.234526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, M., Heinze, H.-J., and Galazky, I. (2010). Alien hand syndrome: neural correlates of movements without conscious will. PLoS One 5:e15010. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedda, A., and Bottini, G. (2014). Apotemnophilia, body integrity identity disorder or xenomelia? Psychiatric and neurologic etiologies face each other. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 10, 1255–1265. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S53385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghezzi, S., Giannini, G., and Zapparoli, L. (2019). Neurofunctional correlates of body-ownership and sense of agency: a meta - analytical account of self-consciousness. Cortex 121, 169–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, C., Bolton, D. A. E., Kennedy, N., Soekadar, S., and Ruddy, K. (2021). Challenges and opportunities for the future of Brain-Computer Interface in neurorehabilitation. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/mw6un

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body ownership. Neuropsychologica 4, 703–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turbyne, C., de Koning, P., Zantvoord, J., and Denys, D. (2021). Body integrity identity disorder using augmented reality: a symptom reduction study. BMJ Case Rep. 14:e238554. doi: 10.1136/bcr-2020-238554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kolk, B. A., Hodgdon, H., Gapen, M., Musicaro, R., Suvak, M. K., Hamlin, E., et al. (2016). A randomized controlled study of neurofeedback for chronic PTSD. PLoS One 11:e0166752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dijk, M. T., van Wingen, G. A., van Lammeren, A., Blom, R. M., de Kwaasteniet, B. P., Scholte, H. S., et al. (2013). Neural basis of limb ownership in individuals with body integrity identity disorder. PLoS One 8:e72212. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, A. (2014). Body integrity identity disorder beyond amputation: consent and liberty. HEC Forum 26, 225–236. doi: 10.1007/s10730-014-9246-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2018). International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics (11th Revision). Available online at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http%3a%2f%2fid.who.int%2ficd%2fentity%2f256572629 (accessed July 7, 2021)

Google Scholar

Keywords: apotemnophilia, body integrity dysphoria, body integrity identity disorder, body representation, brain–computer interface, neurofeedback, somatoparaphrenia, xenomelia

Citation: Chakraborty S, Saetta G, Simon C, Lenggenhager B and Ruddy K (2021) Could Brain–Computer Interface Be a New Therapeutic Approach for Body Integrity Dysphoria? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:699830. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.699830

Received: 24 April 2021; Accepted: 09 July 2021;
Published: 11 August 2021.

Edited by:

Laura Crucianelli, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Andreas Kalckert, University of Skövde, Sweden
Michele Scandola, University of Verona, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Chakraborty, Saetta, Simon, Lenggenhager and Ruddy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kathy Ruddy, ruddykl@tcd.ie

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.