Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 16 April 2021
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic The Relationship Between Neural Circuitry and Biomechanical Action View all 27 articles

Effects of Perturbation Velocity, Direction, Background Muscle Activation, and Task Instruction on Long-Latency Responses Measured From Forearm Muscles

  • Human Robotics Laboratory, Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States

The central nervous system uses feedback processes that occur at multiple time scales to control interactions with the environment. The long-latency response (LLR) is the fastest process that directly involves cortical areas, with a motoneuron response measurable 50 ms following an imposed limb displacement. Several behavioral factors concerning perturbation mechanics and the active role of muscles prior or during the perturbation can modulate the long-latency response amplitude (LLRa) in the upper limbs, but the interactions among many of these factors had not been systematically studied before. We conducted a behavioral study on thirteen healthy individuals to determine the effect and interaction of four behavioral factors – background muscle torque, perturbation direction, perturbation velocity, and task instruction – on the LLRa evoked from the flexor carpi radialis (FCR) and extensor carpi ulnaris (ECU) muscles after velocity-controlled wrist displacements. The effects of the four factors were quantified using both a 0D statistical analysis on the average perturbation-evoked EMG signal in the period corresponding to an LLR, and using a timeseries analysis of EMG signals. All factors significantly modulated LLRa, and their combination nonlinearly contributed to modulating the LLRa. Specifically, all the three-way interaction terms that could be computed without including the interaction between instruction and velocity significantly modulated the LLR. Analysis of the three-way interaction terms of the 0D model indicated that for the ECU muscle, the LLRa evoked when subjects are asked to maintain their muscle activation in response to the perturbations was greater than the one observed when subjects yielded to the perturbations (p < 0.001), but this effect was not measured for muscles undergoing shortening or in absence of background muscle activation. Moreover, higher perturbation velocity increased the LLRa evoked from the stretched muscle in presence of a background torque (p < 0.001), but no effects of velocity were measured in absence of background torque. Also, our analysis identified significant modulations of LLRa in muscles shortened by the perturbation, including an interaction between torque and velocity, and an effect of both torque and velocity. The time-series analysis indicated the significance of additional transient effects in the LLR region for muscles undergoing shortening.

Introduction

Countering unexpected and unpredictable loads is a ubiquitous occurrence of everyday life. Humans can precisely perform movements and interact with the environment even in the presence of these external perturbations. These mechanical perturbations require the nervous system to induce a compensatory action in order to ensure the task success. An important component of the compensatory actions produced by the central nervous system is the long-latency response (LLR). In upper limb muscles, the LLR is evident as the burst of muscle activity occurring 50–100 ms following a limb displacement. Accordingly, this event occurs between the fastest nervous system response, i.e., the short-latency reflex (SLR) occurring within 20–50 ms, and the delayed voluntary reaction which begins 100 ms after the imposed perturbation (Hammond, 1956; Lee and Tatton, 1975; Kurtzer, 2015).

After a seminal study by Hammond in 1956 (Hammond, 1956), several investigators have utilized a limb perturbation paradigm to investigate the physiological mechanisms subserving the muscle stretch responses to the externally applied loads (Allum, 1975; Crago et al., 1976; Evarts and Granit, 1976; Thomas et al., 1977; Kurtzer, 2015; Zonnino et al., 2019). In these paradigms, the muscle responses including the LLRs are recorded through surface electromyography (EMG) activity evoked in the muscle stretched by an imposed angular joint displacement induced by a mechanical perturbation of known and controllable characteristics (Rothwell et al., 1980; Tarkka and Larsen, 1987; Cody and Plant, 1989; Matthews, 1989, 1993; Noth et al., 1991; Kurtzer, 2015).

There is a body of evidence supporting the practical importance of muscle stretch responses – specifically the LLR component – in the neurological research. Previous studies showed that LLR can be considered as the primary outcome measure in various rehabilitation and robot-aided training protocols for several neurological diseases including stroke, Parkinson’s disease (PD), spinal cord injury, and cerebellar ataxia (Sinkjær and Hayashi, 1989; Hayashi et al., 2001; Trumbower et al., 2013; Mirbagheri et al., 2015; Banks et al., 2019; Deneri et al., 2020). One recent study in 2019 (Banks et al., 2019), distinguished LLR as a promising physiological marker of walking dysfunction in chronic stroke. Trumbower et al., also demonstrated that there is a bilateral impaired regulation of the LLR during tasks which require increased stability in both the paretic and non-paretic upper limbs of stroke survivors (Trumbower et al., 2013). Moreover, previous studies on Parkinson’s disease reported that LLR might be a useful objective physiological measure of muscle stiffness and rigidity in PD patients (Sinkjær and Hayashi, 1989; Hayashi et al., 2001).

Several behavioral factors are known to affect the amplitude of LLRs, including the neuromechanical state of the muscle prior perturbation (i.e., muscle length and activation) (Bedingham and Tatton, 1984; Calancie and Bawa, 1985), the direction of perturbation (i.e., whether the perturbation stretches or shortens the muscle) (Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2004, 2010), the kinematic features of the applied perturbation (i.e., perturbation velocity, duration, amplitude, velocity profile) (Lee and Tatton, 1982; Lewis et al., 2005; Schuurmans et al., 2009), and the instructions provided to participants as to how to respond to the applied perturbations (Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2006; Kurtzer et al., 2014).

Although investigators mostly focused on studying LLR features in stretched muscles, there is evidence of EMG activity evoked in the muscle shortened by the applied perturbation (Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2004). Specifically, an increase in the EMG activity of the extensor carpi radialis (ECR) muscle which was shortened due to the applied wrist extension perturbation was documented in a study conducted in 2004 (Lewis et al., 2004). Although the shortened muscle response was smaller in amplitude than the one evoked in the stretched (flexor) muscle, both had two separate components of SLR and LLR with a similar onset timing. Another study also observed a similarly timed, low-amplitude EMG response with an onset of about 50 ms, evoked in the ECR muscle in response to a rapid wrist extension (Miscio et al., 2001). The authors suggest that part of the measured effects may be due to cross-talk (a volume-conducted response from the stretched muscle). However, because significant perturbation-evoked responses were measured in muscles undergoing shortening even using intramuscular EMG (Lewis et al., 2010), it may be reasonable that LLR are evoked in muscles subject to both a shortening and a stretching perturbation.

Studies also examined the effects of background muscle activation prior to the imposed perturbation on the LLR amplitude. In general, an increase in background activation results in an increase in the magnitude of muscle activity in both the proximal and distal muscles of the upper limbs (Bedingham and Tatton, 1984; Miscio et al., 2001; Pruszynski et al., 2009). Affecting the background motoneuron pool excitability, pre-existing background muscle activation is thought to reflect an automatic adjustment mechanism, known as the automatic gain component of the LLR (Bedingham and Tatton, 1984; Matthews, 1986; Miscio et al., 2001; Pruszynski et al., 2009).

Several studies quantified the effects of the kinematic features of the applied perturbation on the LLR amplitude (Lee and Tatton, 1975, 1982; Lewis et al., 2005, 2006; Schuurmans et al., 2009). It is generally accepted that the LLR amplitude increases as a function of the velocity of the applied perturbations (Tatton and Bawa, 1979; Bedingham and Tatton, 1984). In the common ramp-and-hold perturbation paradigms, which are conducted at constant velocity, perturbation duration may also play a factor when the duration of the perturbation is within the range of neuromuscular delays expected for the LLR. However, the details of the interaction between perturbation velocity and duration in modulating LLR amplitude are not yet completely understood. A study by Lewis et al. showed that LLR amplitudes of the biceps brachii undergoing stretch are modulated by velocity in all conditions, but the slope of the relationship is also modulated by duration (Lewis et al., 2005). Yet, the range of velocities and durations that modulate the response in such a way is likely limited. In fact, we know that for FCR, a very high velocity and short duration (<40 ms) perturbation is not sufficient to evoke a long-latency response, whereas a perturbation of low velocity and long duration (>60 ms) generates well-developed LLRs (Lee and Tatton, 1982).

Task instruction also plays a key role in modulation of the LLR response. Accumulating evidence shows that the temporal overlap of two different responses including a task-dependent response and an automatic response results in the task-dependent change in LLR amplitude (Rothwell et al., 1980; Lewis et al., 2006; Pruszynski et al., 2011). The task-dependent response is larger when participants attempt to counter a perturbation than yield to the perturbation (Calancie and Bawa, 1985; Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2006; Kurtzer et al., 2014). Participants can be instructed to respond to the perturbation in different ways: they can be asked to relax immediately following the perturbation (Miscio et al., 2001) — a condition referred to as “yield”; to maintain the background torque and avoid a voluntary response to the perturbation (Calancie and Bawa, 1985) — a condition referred to as “Do Not Intervene”; or to explicitly compensate by activating their muscles in the opposite direction of the perturbation (Lewis et al., 2006), or to compensate cued by a visual feedback of the hand position (Kurtzer et al., 2014) — conditions referred to as “Resist.” In conditions where the subject was instructed to counter the stretch, LLR amplitude typically increased compared to a control condition, demonstrating that the LLR can be modulated to functionally adapt to the task at the upper limb (Crago et al., 1976; Colebatch et al., 1979; Rothwell et al., 1980). However, the ‘yield’ or the ‘DNI’ instructions were never directly compared, with the exception of one study (Calancie and Bawa, 1985), which only recruited two individuals. Because the state of the muscle in the “yield” and the “DNI” conditions is fundamentally different, it would be useful to quantify the differential effects of these two conditions on the evoked LLR.

Gathered together, several factors concerning the mechanics of the applied perturbations and the active role of muscles prior or during the perturbation can modulate the amplitude of long-latency responses in the upper limbs. Hence, it is of a paramount importance to study how the interaction of these factors would affect the muscle stretch responses in a single study. However, the majority of previous studies has systematically studied only one or two of the factors modulating LLRa, with the interaction between perturbation velocity and task instruction studied in Lewis et al. (2006), the interaction between perturbation velocity and background torque studied in Bedingham and Tatton (1984), and the interaction between task instruction and background torque studied in Calancie and Bawa (1985). One previous study (Miscio et al., 2001) has studied the effects of three of the factors highlighted above (i.e., task instructions, perturbation direction, and torque), though not with a full factorial design capable of quantifying the interactions among all factors. As such, to the best of our knowledge, no previous study conducted a full factorial design capable of quantifying the effects of and the interactions among all combinations of four factors known to modulate LLR amplitude, i.e., task instructions, perturbation duration, background torque, and perturbation velocity.

The goal of this study is to determine the effects of and the interactions among several experimental factors modulating the LLR amplitude during ramp-and-hold perturbation. Specifically, the goal of this study is to establish the effect and interaction of background muscle torque, perturbation direction, perturbation velocity, and task instruction on the LLR amplitude evoked from the flexor carpi radialis (FCR) and extensor carpi ulnaris (ECU) muscles following the application of controlled angular displacements of the wrist in both the extension and flexion directions.

Materials and Methods

Thirteen healthy individuals were recruited to participate in this study (protocol approved by the University of Delaware Institutional Review Board, protocol no. 1097082-6). Subjects – age (mean ± s.d.: 24 ± 3 years) were naïve to the purpose of the study and free from known neurological or orthopedic disorders affecting arm function. Subjects were exposed to an experiment that aimed to quantify the amplitude of long-latency responses via the recording of EMG activity from a wrist flexor and extensor pair, the flexor carpi radialis (FCR) and extensor carpi ulnaris (ECU). These responses were evoked by flexion or extension perturbations applied by a robot to a subject’s wrist in various conditions.

Materials

The equipment used for this experiment is shown in Figure 1, and includes several components, described below in detail.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup. (1) Monitor displaying the GUI that cued subjects to the desired level of wrist flexion or extension torque and provides task instructions. (2) EMG amplifier. (3) Control box with the power supply, motor driver, and data acquisition board. (4) MR-StretchWrist robot. (5) Force sensor preamplifier box and analog/digital I/O device for force sensor data. (6) Laptop running EMG collection software, Simulink, and real-time QUARC software.

Perturbation Robot

A custom-developed robot, the MR-StretchWrist, was used to apply perturbations to subject’s wrists. The MR-StretchWrist is a 1-degree of freedom robot that can provide wrist flexion and extension between −45 to 45 degrees (Zonnino et al., 2019). The robot employs an ultrasonic piezoelectric motor (EN6060, Shinsei Motor Inc., Japan) that can provide 500 mNm of torque and can move at velocities of up to 900 degrees/second.

To provide torque for sufficient muscle stretch within the desired time of 50 ms, a capstan transmission with a 3:1 gear ratio was included in the design. The capstan drive contains two pulleys with different diameters connected via a microfiber braided line (SpiderWire Stealth SPW-0039, 0.4 mm diameter braided fishing line). The cable is wrapped around each pulley multiple times to ensure zero slippage. The capstan transmission is an ideal candidate for this application because it has no backlash, low friction, and high bandwidth.

Measurement of the wrist flexion/extension angle was obtained using an incremental encoder (resolution: 0.09 deg) placed on the motor shaft, with a resulting resolution in measuring the wrist flexion/extension angle of 0.03 deg.

EMG Amplifier and Electrodes

Electromyography data was recorded with an OTBioelettronica EMG-USB2+ amplifier (OTBioelettronica s.r.l., Torino, Italy), using OTBiolab Software (OTBioelettronica s.r.l., Torino, Italy). Disposable Silver/Silver chloride surface electrodes with conductive gel (HEX Dual Electrodes, Noraxon USA, Scottsdale, AZ, United States) were placed on the skin of the subject. A moistened conductive band was wrapped around the wrist, ensuring contact with the radial and ulnar stylar processes, to serve as a reference electrode. Bipolar cables were attached to the disposable electrodes and connected to the amplifier.

Force Measurement

An ATI Mini 27 Ti force sensor integrated in the MR-StretchWrist is used to measure the wrist flexion/extension torque (Full-scale Load (FSL): 2 Nm, Resolution: 0.5 mNm, Measurement uncertainty: 1.5% of FSL). Transducer signals are processed by a preamplifier box (ATI Industrial Automation, Apex, NC, United States), and digitized by an analog/digital I/O device (PCIe-6321, National Instruments, Austin, TX, United States) connected to a laptop.

Control Software

Software for robot position control, perturbation timing, and for the graphical user interface was developed in Matlab and Simulink, and executed in real-time (sample rate: 1 kHz) using the QUARC real-time control software (Quanser, Markham, ON, Canada). Encoder data were acquired using the Q2 USB data acquisition board (DAQ).

Methods

EMG Electrode Positioning

To determine the location of the electrodes to measure activity of the FCR and ECU, manual palpations were performed on the right arm of the subjects. Repeated wrist flexion during palpation while the wrist was in its neutral position aided in locating the FCR. Similarly, repeated wrist extension in this same orientation aided in locating the ECU. Points parallel to the muscle’s fibers were drawn 3 cm apart. The skin was then prepped with 70% isopropyl alcohol wipes and the application a thin layer of conductive skin prep gel (Nuprep, Weaver and Co., Aurora, CO, United States).

Experimental Procedures

In this study, subjects were seated with their forearms resting in a stationary support connected to a normal desk, with the forearm extending anteriorly in front of the body. Their hand was strapped inside of a mold such that their wrist was in a semiprone/neutral condition, and any static wrist flexion/extension torque (range: 0 – 2 Nm) would be supported by the mold with little deflections. A GUI was shown on a computer screen indicating the amount of wrist flexion/extension torque to apply. After the subject reached the appropriate torque target, and maintained it within a 50 mNm range for 0.4 to 0.8 s, the robot perturbed the wrist in the direction that would stretch the agonist muscle with respect to the cued torque (i.e., if the background torque was flexion, the perturbation was wrist extension). Perturbations were applied for a duration of 200 ms in all conditions, so to avoid undesired oscillations of the EMG signal due to impact dynamics arising from the abrupt end of a perturbation. After each wrist perturbation, the robot halted for 1 second before returning to the neutral position for the following perturbation. Numerous conditions were studied in this protocol, defined by factorial combinations of four factors: (1) perturbation velocity, (2) perturbation direction, (3) background torque, (4) task instructions.

Perturbation velocity assumed three levels: 50, 125, and 200 deg/s. Perturbation direction assumed two levels: wrist flexion or wrist extension. Background torque was set to either 0 or 200 mNm. Task instruction assumed two levels: “yield” (Y) and “do not intervene” (DNI). In the Y condition, subjects were told to not provide any resistance after the perturbation and yield (i.e., relax) to the movement. In the DNI condition, subjects were told to continue applying the same amount of torque that they were applying prior to perturbation. These two instructions are fundamentally the same in the absence of a background torque.

Ten trials per condition (combination of each level of velocity, direction, background torque, instruction) were collected, resulting in each experiment consisting of a total of 240 perturbations. The order in which conditions were applied was randomly generated by the Simulink and MATLAB files. Furthermore, the time between the end of the prior perturbation and the start of the next perturbation cue was randomized between 3 and 7 s.

EMG Processing

Pre-processing of the raw EMG data was conducted using Matlab code (Matlab 2017a, MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States). After on-board analog amplification (gain = 2000, amplifier settings: low-pass cut-off frequency: 0.3 Hz, high-pass cut-off frequency: 4.4 kHz), data were sampled at 10240 Hz. A band-pass filter, a 4th order Butterworth filter (fLP = 250 Hz and fHP = 20 Hz), was used to remove low frequency noise related to movement artifacts and high frequency noise related to intrinsic measurement noise. Matlab’s filtfilt function was used to obtain a filtered waveform without any phase shift of the signal. The filtered waveform was then rectified. Digital outputs produced by the DAQ board (Q2-USB, Quanser, Markham, ON, Canada) were sent to the EMG amplifier to identify instants of perturbation onset, used for segmentation of the EMG signal into 200 ms long timeseries, one for each perturbation, each starting at the time of perturbation onset. EMG tracks were shifted in time such that t = 0 ms corresponded to the instant of arrival of the pulse.

The amplitude of the segmented EMG signal measured during a perturbation was normalized by the average magnitude of the rectified EMG signal measured from that same muscle during agonist background contractions preceding all perturbations. This procedure allowed us to conduct group analysis of normalized EMG data. With this procedure, an EMG signal of unitary magnitude indicated that the rectified EMG signal measured during each perturbation had the same amplitude of the average EMG signal generated by that muscle for a 200 mNm isometric torque. Units of the normalized EMG timeseries are referred to as normalized units (nu).

These procedures resulted in 10 segmented timeseries extracted from each subject per combination of conditions (velocity, direction, torque, instruction). Long-latency response amplitude (LLRa), defined as the average signal of the EMG tracks in the [50 100] ms interval, was calculated for each perturbation for both muscles, and indexed as a function of subject, repetition, and combination of experimental conditions. The subject-specific average of LLRa for each combination of perturbation conditions was used as the outcome measure for the 0-D statistical analysis.

Repeated measurements from each subject were averaged to yield the timeseries with average rectified EMG response for each subject EMGsub,ν,d,t,inst used for the 1-D statistical analysis. Group averages EMG¯ν,d,t,i and corresponding standard deviations s¯ν,d,t,i were then calculated for display purposes.

Statistical Analysis

Two four-way full factorial linear mixed model ANOVAs were conducted using the subject-specific average LLRa measured for FCR and ECU, respectively, as outcome measure. The four factors included in the ANOVA were perturbation velocity (0, 125, 200 deg/s), background torque (0, 200 mNm), instruction (yield, do not intervene), and perturbation direction (stretch vs. shorten), defined based on the effect that the perturbation would have on the length of each muscle. As an example, a flexion perturbation would correspond to the “stretch” level for ECU, and the “shorten” level for FCR. Statistical analysis was conducted using JMP, and all variables were coded as nominal variables, with the default conditions of 50 deg/s for velocity, 0 mNm for background torque, yield for instruction, non-stretch for perturbation direction. JMP Pro Version 14 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, United States) was used for this analysis.

The linear mixed model included 16 terms for fixed effects (4 main effects, 6 two-way interactions, 4 three-way interactions, one four-way interaction term, and an intercept), plus an additional set of offset variables for random subject-specific effects. The Satterthwaite method was used to determine the number of degrees of freedom in the model. All terms are reported if their estimated effect is significant at the type I error rate α = 0.05. In those cases, Tukey HSD post hoc tests were also used to determine pairs of levels with significant differences.

A 1-D ANOVA was also conducted on the timeseries of rectified EMG signal (EMGsub,ν,d,t,i) measured during the post-perturbation interval comprised between 0 and 200 ms using the spm1d software (Pataky et al., 2015, 2016). 1-D statistical analysis models are useful to analyze the effects of the experimental conditions on the perturbation-induced muscle response without prior hypotheses on the specific time interval where an effect is expected (Pataky et al., 2015, 2016). While with the 0D analysis we restricted our focus on the time interval ensuing the perturbation comprised between 50 and 100 ms (thus obtaining a scalar, or 0D, outcome measure), with the 1D model we sought to determine whether there is an effect on the timeseries of measured EMG amplitudes associated with all the experimental conditions, and their combinations, at any time point in the post-perturbation interval comprised between 0 and 200 ms. As such, the 1-D ANOVA is useful to establish effects of all factors and their interaction at multiple time-points, but controlling for the multiple comparisons resulting from this type of analysis.

Because the current version of the spm1D Matlab software only allows to build full factorial models with a maximum of three main effects, we broke each four-way model into two three-way models and performed our analysis using the spm1d function anova3rm. Each model included the factors speed, background torque, and instruction, with one model including LLRs measured during stretch, and the other model including LLRs measured during shortening. This setup allowed us to study the effects and interaction of all factors studies in the 0D analysis, with the exception of all terms involving perturbation direction. Given the software limitations, we chose to exclude perturbation direction from this analysis as it was an effect that had been largely neglected by most other studies.

The 1-D analysis was implemented as a mixed model ANOVA for measurements collected at all time points, which controls for the associated multiple comparisons using random field theory (Pataky et al., 2016). As such, the output of the 1D ANOVA procedure is a time-series of F scores for all main effects and their interaction, combined with the identification of time intervals where those effects are significant at α = 0.05, corrected for the multiple comparisons performed at multiple time points. When an effect or an interaction was significant during the LLR time-window (i.e., from 50 to 100 ms after perturbation onset), we conducted post hoc tests to determine pairs of levels with significant modulation in EMG signal.

Results

Of the 13 participants recruited for this study, data sets of 2 subjects for ECU and 3 subjects for FCR were excluded from analyses due to poor EMG recordings. Therefore, statistical analysis was performed on data collected on n = 11 individuals for the ECU muscle, and n = 10 individuals for the FCR muscle. Timeseries extracted in the different experimental conditions for the ECU muscle are shown in Figures 2, 3. Similar representations are provided for the FCR muscle in Supplementary Figures 1, 2 of the Supplementary Materials.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Time series of EMG signal (left half-panels) and LLRa (boxplot in the right half-panels) from all subjects resulting with perturbations in the flexion direction (stretching the ECU muscle). Color indicates the speed of the perturbation (red = 50 deg/s, orange = 125 deg/s, green = 200 deg/s), with the line indicating the mean and the shaded area indicating the ± 1 s.d. region. Panels in different rows include measurements at two levels of task instruction – Yield (top), and DNI (bottom); columns include measurements at two levels of background torque – 200 mNm (right), and 0 mNm (left). In the boxplots, darker shades represent 1 standard deviation from the mean, and lighter shades indicate the 95% confidence interval.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Time series of EMG signal and LLRa from all subjects resulting with perturbations in the extension direction (shortening the ECU muscle).

0-D Analysis

The linear mixed model computed an adjusted R2 of 0.627 for FCR, and an adjusted R2 of 0.788 for ECU. The model reported a significant effect of all four main factors. However, since all factors are involved in several two- and three-way interactions, only the interactions will be analyzed and discussed below. Results are presented below as least square means ± standard error (LLRa) or difference in least square means ± standard error (ΔLLRa) in units of the outcome measure, i.e., normalized units (nu). A report including the significant fixed effects from the linear mixed model ANOVA is provided below in Tables 1, 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Significant main effects and interactions for ECU LLRa.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Main effects and interactions for FCR LLRa.

A significant three-way interaction between perturbation direction, instruction, and torque was measured for the ECU muscle (Figure 4). A significant effect of instruction or perturbation direction on LLRa was observed only in presence of a background torque for both muscles. In presence of background torque, the LLRa associated with stretch perturbations was greater than the LLRa associated with shortening perturbation both when task instructions were DNI (LLRa — Stretch DNI: 3.80 ± 0.17 nu, Shorten DNI: 0.79 ± 0.17 nu, p < 0.001), and when task instructions were to yield (LLRa — Stretch Y: 2.04 ± 0.17 nu, Shorten Y: 0.74 ± 0.17 nu, p < 0.001). In presence of background torque and muscle stretch, LLRa was greater in the DNI condition compared to yield (ΔLLRa — DNI vs. Y: 1.76 ± 0.16 nu, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Least square means (mean displayed with a circle and whisker extending to ± one standard error of the mean) for the significant three-way interaction between perturbation direction, instruction, and torque, conducted for the ECU muscle. Plots are split by torque level – 0 mNm (top), 200 mNm (bottom). Line colors indicate instruction, and different levels of perturbation direction are displayed along the x axis. Letters on the plots are representative of post hoc Tukey HSD tests; pairs of elements that do not share a letter are significantly different.

A significant three-way interaction between perturbation direction, torque, and velocity was measured for the ECU muscle (Figure 5). A significant effect of perturbation velocity or direction was observed only in presence of a background torque. In presence of background torque, the LLRa associated with stretch perturbations was greater than the LLRa associated with shortening perturbations at all velocities (200 deg/s: Stretch: 3.91 ± 0.19 nu, Shorten: 0.97 ± 0.19 nu, p < 0.001; 125 deg/s: Stretch: 2.97 ± 0.19 nu, Shorten: 0.80 ± 0.19 nu, p < 0.001; 50 deg/s: Stretch: 1.88 ± 0.19 nu, Shorten: 0.53 ± 0.19 nu, p < 0.001). In presence of background torque and for perturbations that stretched the muscle, LLRa increased with velocity (ΔLLRa 200-125 deg/s: 0.94 ± 0.20 nu, p < 0.001; ΔLLRa 125-50 deg/s: 1.09 ± 0.20 nu, p < 0.001). Instead, no velocity-dependent effect was measured for perturbations shortening the muscle.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Least square means for the significant three-way interaction between perturbation direction, velocity, and torque, conducted for the ECU. Plots are split by torque level – 0 mNm (top), 200 mNm (bottom) – line color indicates velocity, and different levels of perturbation direction are displayed along the x axis.

Five two-way interaction terms were significant for ECU, while three terms were significant for FCR. Three terms were common to both muscles and are described next.

The interaction between instruction and torque resulted from a greater increase in LLRa measured in the DNI conditions compared to the yield conditions measured in the presence of a background torque for both muscles (Figure 6). In the presence of background torque, LLRa measured during the DNI condition was significantly greater than LLRa measured in the Y condition (FCR LLRa — DNI: 3.67 ± 0.53 nu, Y: 2.39 ± 0.53 nu, p = 0.001; ECU LLRa — DNI: 2.30 ± 0.15 nu, Y: 1.40 ± 0.15 nu, p < 0.001). There was no difference in LLRa measured in absence of background torque between the two instructions (FCR LLRa — DNI: 1.12 ± 0.53 nu, Y: 1.22 ± 0.53 nu, p = 0.990; ECU LLRa — DNI: 0.52 ± 0.15 nu, Y: 0.52 ± 0.15 nu, p = 1). Significant differences between torque levels were measured in both the FCR and ECU for both instructions (FCR ΔLLRa — DNI: 2.55 ± 0.34 nu, p < 0.001, Y: 1.16 ± 0.34 nu, p = 0.004; ECU ΔLLRa — DNI: 1.77 ± 0.11 nu, p < 0.001, Y: 0.87 ± 0.11 nu, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Least square means for all significant two-way interactions for both the FCR (A) and the ECU (B). Letters on the plots are representative post hoc Tukey HSD tests; pairs of elements that do not share a letter are significantly different. Post hoc tests are, and the corresponding letter notation are separate for each panel.

The interaction between perturbation direction and velocity resulted from a greater increase in LLRa measured when muscles are stretched by a perturbation compared to when they are shortened measured at higher velocities (Figure 6). LLRa measured during stretch were significantly greater at higher velocities (FCR LLRa — 200 deg/s: 5.31 ± 0.56 nu, 125 deg/s: 3.37 ± 0.56 nu, 50 deg/s 1.79 ± 0.56 nu, 200-125 deg/s p < 0.001, 125-50 deg/s p = 0.003; ECU LLRa — 200 deg/s: 2.40 ± 0.16 nu, 125 deg/s 1.85 ± 0.16 nu, 50 deg/s 1.13 ± 0.16 nu, 200-125 deg/s p = 0.002, 125-50 deg/s p < 0.001), while there was no significant effect of velocity during perturbations where the muscle was shortened. The increase in LLRa between shorten and stretch conditions was greater at higher velocities (FCR ΔLLRa — 50 deg/s: 1.34 ± 0.42 nu, 125 deg/s 2.66 ± 0.42 nu, 200 deg/s 4.35 ± 0.42 nu, 200-125 deg/s p = 0.005, 200-50 deg/s p < 0.001, 125-50 deg/s p = 0.026; ECU ΔLLRa — 50 deg/s: 0.71 ± 0.14 nu, 125 deg/s 1.27 ± 0.14 nu; 200 deg/s 1.66 ± 0.14 nu, 200-125 deg/s p = 0.047, 200-50 deg/s p < 0.001, 125-50 deg/s p = 0.006).

The significant interaction between perturbation direction and torque resulted from the greater increase in LLRa associated with stretch perturbations compared to shortening measured in presence of background torque (Figure 6). LLRa associated with stretch perturbations were greater than those associated with shortening in presence of background torque (FCR ΔLLRa — 0 mNm: 1.62 ± 0.34 nu, 200 mNm: 3.95 ± 0.34 nu, p < 0.001; ΔLLRa ECU — 0 mNm: 0.27 ± 0.11 nu, 200 mNm: 2.15 ± 0.11 nu, p < 0.001). LLRa measured in presence of background torque were greater than in absence of background torque for both muscles when they were stretched (FCR ΔLLRa stretch: 3.02 ± 0.34 nu, p < 0.001; ECU ΔLLRa stretch: 2.26 ± 0.11 nu, p < 0.001), but only for ECU when shortened (FCR ΔLLRa shorten: 0.69 ± 0.34 nu, p = 0.180; ECU ΔLLRa shorten: 0.38 ± 0.11 nu, p = 0.006).

Two two-way interaction terms were significant only for the ECU muscle. One term, the interaction between torque and velocity, resulted from a greater increase in LLRa associated with higher velocity perturbations measured in the presence of a background torque (Figure 6). The change in LLRa associated with the two levels of background torque increased at greater velocities (ΔLLRa — 200 deg/s: 1.75 ± 0.14 nu, 125 deg/s: 1.34 ± 0.14 nu, 50 deg/s: 0.87 ± 0.14 nu, 200-125 deg/s p = 0.040, 200-50 deg/s p < 0.001, 125-50 deg/s p = 0.0181). LLRa measured in presence of background torque was significantly different for each velocity level and increased with greater velocity (ΔLLRa — 200-50 deg/s: 1.24 ± 0.14 nu, p < 0.001, 200-125 deg/s: 0.56 ± 0.14 nu p = 0.001, 125-50 deg/s: 0.68 ± 0.14 nu p < 0.001).

The second two-way interaction term significant only for the ECU muscle was the interaction between perturbation direction and instruction. This term resulted from a greater increase in LLRa measured in the DNI condition compared to yield condition measured when muscles are stretched (Figure 6). When muscles were stretched, LLRa associated with the DNI conditions were larger than the yield condition (LLRa — DNI: 2.23 ± 0.15 nu, Y: 1.35 ± 0.15 nu, p = 0.001), while no significant difference between instruction conditions was measured when muscles were shortened. A significant increase in LLRa was measured in both instruction conditions when muscles were stretched compared to shortened, but this increase was greater in the DNI condition compared to the yield condition (ΔLLRa — DNI: 1.64 ± 0.11 nu; Y: 0.79 ± 0.11 nu, p < 0.001).

The model returned a significant effect of each of the four factors, i.e., perturbation direction, instruction, background torque, and velocity. For the effect of perturbation direction, stretched muscles resulted in a significantly larger LLR amplitude than those of shortened muscles (FCR LLRa — stretch: 3.49 ± 0.50 nu, shorten: 0.71 ± 0.50 nu, p < 0.001; ECU LLRa — stretch: 1.79 ± 0.13 nu, shorten: 0.58 ± 0.13 nu, p < 0.001). With respect to the significant effect of instruction, the DNI condition resulted in larger LLR amplitudes compared to the yield condition for both the FCR and ECU (FCR LLRa — DNI: 2.39 ± 0.50 nu, yield: 1.81 ± 0.50 nu, p = 0.016; ECU LLRa — DNI: 1.41 ± 0.13 nu, yield: 0.96 ± 0.13 nu, p < 0.001). The presence of background torque at 200 mNm resulted in a significantly larger LLR amplitude compared to the absence of background torque for both muscles (FCR LLRa — 200 mNm: 3.03 ± 0.50 nu, 0 mNm: 1.17 ± 0.50 nu, p < 0.001; LLRa ECU — 200 mNm: 1.84 ± 0.13 nu, 0 mNm: 0.52 ± 0.13 nu, p < 0.001). The significant effect of velocity resulted from higher velocities associated with larger LLRas for both muscles (FCR LLRa — 50 deg/s: 1.12 ± 0.52 nu, 125 deg/s: 2.04 ± 0.52 nu, 200 deg/s: 3.14 ± 0.52 nu; ECU LLRa — 50 deg/s: 0.77 ± 0.14 nu, 125 deg/s: 1.21 ± 0.14 nu, 200 deg/s: 1.56 ± 0.14 nu). Tukey HSD post hoc analysis indicated that all velocity levels are significantly different from one another for both muscles. (FCR — 200-125 deg/s: p < 0.001, 125-50 deg/s: p = 0.006; ECU — 200-125 deg/s: p = 0.002, 125-50 deg/s: p < 0.001).

1D Analysis

The results of the 1D ANOVA are represented in terms of a timeseries of F scores, shown in Figures 7, 8 for the ECU and FCR, respectively. For effects and interactions that have significant upcrossings within the LLR region, post hoc 1D t-tests were used to further break down the effects.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Results of the 1D 3-way ANOVA for the shortened (A) and stretched (B) states of the ECU. Curves are a time-series of F scores for the effect of a factor or of an interaction of factors on the outcome measure. The dashed red line indicates the critical F-value for that main effect or interaction. Regions in green indicate significant upcrossings. The shaded gray region is representative of the LLR (50 to 100 ms).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Results of the 1D 3-way ANOVA for the shortened (A) and stretched (B) states of the FCR.

The three-way interaction between instruction, torque, and velocity was not significant within the LLR for both muscles and both directions, however, there is a narrow significant upcrossing during the SLR for ECU Stretch (25.8 to 28.3 ms, peak F2,20 = 11.495).

The two-way interaction between instruction and torque is significant within the LLR region for both directions of the ECU muscle, but not for the FCR muscle. Within the LLR region, there are significant upcrossings for ECU during both muscle shortening and stretching (ECU Stretch: 52.6 to 70.0 ms, local peak in LLR region F1,10 = 26.152; ECU Shorten: 62.2 to 66.8 ms, peak F1,10 = 37.434). There are also significant upcrossings within the voluntary region for the stretch of the ECU and FCR (ECU Stretch: 148.6 to 200 ms, peak F1,10 = 26.152; FCR Stretch: 183.0 to 196.0 ms, 199.3 to 200 ms, peak F1,10 = 27.377).

The two-way interaction between instruction and torque measured for the ECU muscle is broken down in the 1D post hoc t-tests to analyze the measured effect at all time points, as shown in Figure 9. Analysis of post hoc tests highlights how the DNI has positive or negative effect on the amplitude of processed EMG recordings at different time points, and the effect differs as a function of stretch and background torque condition. When the ECU was stretched, the normalized EMG signal in DNI was greater than yield within the LLR and voluntary regions in the presence of background torque, (ECU Stretch: 49.1 to 200 ms, peak T = 13.703, Figure 9B, center). No significant change in EMG signal was measured in absence of background torque as a function of task instructions (Figure 9B, left). As a result, the change in EMG signal measured between the DNI and Y condition was greater in presence of background torque than in absence only after the delay similar to that considered for forearm LLRs (ECU Stretch: 38.7 ms to 200 ms, peak ΔT = 14.450, peak in the LLR region ΔT = 9.397, Figure 9B, right). When the ECU was shortened, EMG recordings measured in presence of background torque in the DNI condition were greater than those measured in the yield condition in the initial part of LLR, but then EMG recordings measured during DNI were smaller than yield at a later time in the LLR time period (ECU Shorten: 57.7 to 77.0 ms, local peak T = 4.470, 94.6 ms to 129.5 ms, local peak T = –3.925, Figure 9A, center). Instead, no significant change in EMG signal was measured in absence of background torque as a function of task instructions (Figure 9A, left). The change in T scores between torque levels for the shortened condition also indicated significant upcrossings in the LLR and voluntary regions (ECU Shorten: 54.2 to 73.5 ms, local peak ΔT = 4.961, 92.5 to 132.9 ms, local peak T = −5.461, Figure 9A, right).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Results of the 1D t-tests for the interaction between task instruction and torque for the shortened (A) and stretched (B) states of the ECU. Plots indicate the statistical score of 1D paired t-tests between the DNI and yield instructions and split in columns by torque level. Plots in the right column report the difference between the T-scores in the second and first column’s tests, used to demonstrate interaction between these factors. The dashed red line indicates the Bonferroni-adjusted critical T values and regions in green indicate significant upcrossings.

The two-way interaction between torque and velocity is significant in the LLR region for the ECU when stretched and for the FCR when it is both shortened and stretched. For both the ECU and FCR there is a significant upcrossing within the LLR region (ECU Stretch: 52.6 to 74.7 ms, peak F2,20 = 19.872; FCR Shorten: 47.9 to 65.8 ms, peak F2,20 = 22.421; FCR Stretch: 58.0 to 68.6 ms, peak F2,20 = 19.244).

The two-way interaction between torque and velocity for muscle and perturbation direction is broken down in 1D post hoc tests for ECU Stretch, FCR Shorten, and FCR Stretch (Figures 1012). For the stretch of the ECU (Figure 10), significant upcrossings were present in post hoc tests comparing EMG signals measured at different velocity levels and at multiple torque levels, primarily during the LLR time period. Analysis of the timeseries of t-scores demonstrate that EMG signal increases with velocity and with background torque, and that the region where such an increase is measured largely overlaps with the expected latency of a LLR. The largest upcrossing was measured for the 200-50 deg/s comparison in presence of a background torque (40.8 to 98.1 ms, peak T = 10.202, Figure 10, center row, center column). Upcrossings within the LLR region were also measured for the 200-50 deg/s t-test in the absence of background torque, the 200-125 deg/s t-test in the presence of background torque, and the 125-50 degree/second t-test in both torque conditions. Significant upcrossings were measured within the LLR region for the 200-50 deg/s comparison and the 200-125 degrees/second comparison (Δ200-50 deg/s: 56.4 to 73.8 ms, peak ΔT = 5.915, Figure 10 center row, right column, Δ200-125 deg/s: 56.0 to 62.7 ms, peak ΔT = 4.304, Figure 10, top row, right column). The significance of the between-torque condition difference of the t-scores resulting from comparing pairs of velocity levels indicates that background torque differentially modulates the velocity dependence of EMG signals.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Results of the 1D t-tests of the interaction between torque and velocity for the stretched state of the ECU. Plots in different rows indicate the statistical score of 1d paired t-tests between the velocity levels (200-125, 200-50, 125-50 deg/s) and columns indicate different torque levels. Plots in the right column report the difference between the T-scores in the second and first column’s tests, used to demonstrate interaction between these factors. The dashed red line indicates the Bonferroni-adjusted critical T-values and regions in green indicate significant upcrossings.

FIGURE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 11. Results of the 1D t-tests of the torque and velocity interaction for the stretched state of the FCR.

FIGURE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 12. Results of the 1D t-tests of the torque and velocity interaction for the shortened state of the FCR.

Qualitatively similar results were measured for the stretch of FCR (Figure 11). Significant upcrossings were measured for t-test comparisons mostly during the LLR time period. The largest t-scores were generated for the 200-50 deg/s t-test in the presence of background torque (23.7 to 80.9 ms, peak T = 12.856, Figure 11 center row, center column). Significant upcrossings were also present for the 200-125 deg/s t-test in presence of background torque, 200-50 degree/second t-test in the absence of background torque, the 200-125 deg/s t-test in both torque conditions, and the 125-50 deg/s t-test in both torque conditions. One significant upcrossing was measured for the between-torque condition difference of t-scores resulting from comparing pairs of velocity conditions (Δ200-50 deg/s: 57.9 to 64.0 ms, peak ΔT = 4.604, Figure 11 center row, right column).

For FCR responses measured during muscle shortening (Figure 12), significant upcrossings were present in t-test comparisons between velocity levels in the presence of background torque. Like in the previous conditions, the largest t-scores were generated for the 200-50 degree/second t-test in the presence of background torque (46.0 to 93.3 ms, peak T = 8.601, Figure 12 center row, center column). There are also smaller, significant upcrossing for this t-test in the voluntary region (149.1 to 165.1 ms, 187.9 to 200 ms). Significant upcrossings were also present for the 200-125 deg/s t-test in presence of background torque, 200-50 degree/second t-test in the absence of background torque, and the 125-50 degree/second t-test in both torque conditions. Significant upcrossings within the LLR region were calculated for difference between t-tests of different background torque levels (Δ200-50 deg/s: 48.7 to 63.2 ms, peak ΔT = 6.587, Figure 12 center row, right column, Δ200-125 deg/s: 50.6 to 59.2 ms, peak ΔT = 5.176, Figure 12, top row, right column). Such differential effect of background torque at multiple velocities on EMG signal collected from a muscle under shortening was only observed for the ECU muscle in the 0D analysis (Figure 5, significant difference between conditions A – background torque: 0 mNm and velocity: 50 deg/s — and B – background torque: 200 mNm and velocity: 200 deg/s).

The two-way interaction between instruction and velocity is only significant for the shortened state of the FCR during a narrow time window outside of the LLR region (189.8 to 193.1 ms, peak F2,20 = 10.959).

With respect to the main effects, both the FCR and ECU had significant upcrossings within the LLR region. The main effect of instruction was significant within the LLR region for the stretch of the FCR and ECU only for a very short time period (FCR stretch: 69.8 to 73.2 ms, local peak F1,10 = 26.080; ECU stretch: 81.1 to 82.8 ms, local peak F1,10 = 22.546) and has upcrossings within the voluntary region for both the stretch of the FCR and ECU (FCR Stretch: 184.6 to 200 ms, peak F1,10 = 30.620; ECU Stretch: 151.1 to 200 ms, peak F1,10 = 32.771). There were otherwise no significant upcrossings for the effect of instruction for the shortened state of the muscles.

Discussion

In the present study, we aimed to investigate the effects of and the interactions among four behavioral factors in modulating the LLR amplitude (LLRa) evoked from FCR and ECU muscles during ramp-and-hold perturbations applied to the wrist joint. The four behavioral factors studied in this work were perturbation direction (stretch vs. shorten), background muscle activation (0 vs. 200 mNm of joint torque requiring agonist muscle activity), perturbation velocity (50, 125, and 200 deg/s), and task instructions (yield vs DNI). In line with previous studies (Lee and Tatton, 1982; Bedingham and Tatton, 1984; Calancie and Bawa, 1985; Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2004, 2005, 2006, 2010; Schuurmans et al., 2009; Kurtzer et al., 2014), we observed that all factors modulate the LLRa in the FCR and ECU muscles. However, given the use of full factorial design involving all combinations of the four factors, we were able to study higher-order interactions among the four behavioral factors. Moreover, the use of a 1D analysis of stretch-evoked EMG data allowed us to establish the effects of the behavioral factors on EMG amplitude at multiple timepoints in the post-perturbation period, without restricting the analysis to a-priori selected time windows where a modulation would be expected.

In line with previous studies (Tatton and Bawa, 1979; Bedingham and Tatton, 1984), our findings demonstrated that perturbation velocity significantly modulates LLRa, as larger LLRa resulted from perturbations with 200 deg/s velocity compared to the 125 and 50 deg/s velocity conditions, while the 50 deg/s perturbation velocity evoked the lowest LLRa. Note that given the chosen task design, where perturbations are applied for a constant duration (200 ms), perturbation velocity and displacement are associated, so the measured effect could be due to the fact that the joint is subject to a greater displacement in the higher velocity conditions. From studies that systematically spanned a set of perturbation velocity and durations (Lee and Tatton, 1982; Lewis et al., 2005; Schuurmans et al., 2009), we know that total joint displacement is the primary contributor to LLRs, with perturbations at high velocity but brief duration not generating significant LLRa. In our experience, the execution of LLRa studies on perturbation durations that require stopping the limb within the [50 100] ms interval proved to be challenging, as in preliminary data we observed the occurrence of motion artifacts associated with the oscillations induced by the manipulator stopping. For this reason, we decided to focus on perturbations of longer duration (200 ms), which allowed to completely avoid those artifacts.

Results also revealed a significant increase in LLRa when a 200 mNm background torque was applied prior to the perturbations in comparison to the perturbations with no background torque. Pre-existing background muscle activation is thought to reflect an automatic adjustment mechanism, known as the automatic gain component of LLR (Bedingham and Tatton, 1984; Matthews, 1986; Miscio et al., 2001; Pruszynski et al., 2009).

Task instruction also significantly modulated the LLR in this study. The majority of previous studies examined the ‘yield’ or ‘DNI’ instruction with a ‘resist’ or ‘compensate’ instruction (Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2006; Kurtzer et al., 2014). However, no prior study tested whether the ‘yield’ and ‘DNI’ task instruction differentially modulate the LLRa, with the exception of one study by Calancie and Bawa (1985) who recruited only two healthy individuals. Our findings showed that larger LLRs were evoked when participants attempt keep constant the muscles’ activation state (i.e., DNI), compared to when they are asked to yield to the perturbations, which is justifiable in accordance to the evidence that the temporal overlap of two different responses including a task-dependent response and an automatic response results in the task-dependent change in LLR amplitude (Rothwell et al., 1980; Lewis et al., 2006; Pruszynski et al., 2011).

Also, in line with a few previous studies quantifying LLR from both stretched and shortened muscles (Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2004), the above-mentioned modulations had more prominent effects on the LLRa evoked from the muscle stretched in response to the perturbations vs. the shortened muscle, however several significant effects of the behavioral factors were also measured in muscles undergoing shortening (see below for details).

With our full factorial design, we were able to observe more granular modulations of the LLR response induced by the four behavioral factors reported above. Specifically, significant results were observed for all the three-way interaction terms that can be computed without including the interaction between instruction and velocity, as detailed below.

Perturbation Direction, Task Instruction and Background Torque

As illustrated in Figure 4, perturbation direction did not affect the LLRa evoked from the perturbations with no background torque (i.e., the LLR measured during perturbations shortening the muscle were of similar amplitude as the one measured during perturbations stretching the muscle). This finding is in contrast with a previous study by Miscio et al. (2001), which demonstrated that there would be a lower LLRa evoked from the shortened muscle in comparison to the LLRa evoked from the stretched muscle even in absence of background muscle activity. In addition, task instruction did not modulate the LLRa evoked in absence of background torque. This result is expected and serves as a validation of the pursued experimental design, as in this case the behavior elicited in the Y and DNI conditions is expected to be identical.

Results from the perturbations with 200 mNm background torque showed a significant difference between LLRa evoked from stretched muscle in the Y and DNI task instructions. Larger LLRa was detected from perturbations when participants were instructed to maintain their level of activation (DNI) after the perturbation, compared to when they were instructed to yield to the displacement. In contrast, task instructions did not modulate the LLRa evoked from the shortened muscle neither in presence of a 200mNm background torque. Also, the LLRa for shortened muscle was significantly lower than the LLRa for stretched muscle in both instruction conditions. This interaction is also consistently reported also in the 1D analysis (Figure 9).

In agreement with this result, Miscio et al. (2001) also reported a smaller LLRa for shortened muscle in comparison to the stretched muscles when there was a 10% maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) background torque and subjects were instructed to not intervene to the perturbations. However, their task instructions did not include a ‘yield’ condition for comparison. The EMG activity recorded from the shortened muscle in the previous study was interpreted as a volume conducted response based on their direct nerve stimulation results. Besides, their regression analysis showed a positive correlation between the area of FCR stretch response and the low-level activity recorded from the shortened antagonist muscle (ECR) (r2 = 0.59). Instead, our data supports a differential modulation of the response for the stretched and shortened muscles between different levels of task instructions, suggesting that the source of the observations may not be due to cross-talk, but to true physiological decoupling of the muscle responses.

Perturbation Direction, Background Torque, Perturbation Velocity

Results of the 0D analysis (Figure 5) support a significant interaction between the effects of torque and velocity on the LLRa evoked from perturbations stretching the ECU muscle, but not from perturbations shortening the ECU muscle. In support of this observation, the 1D analysis (Figure 10) revealed a significant interaction between torque and velocity on the LLRa for ECU muscle only under stretching, but not under shortening. For the FCR muscle, the interaction was significant in response to the both stretching and shortening perturbations, though the differential effects of background activity are smaller, and last for a smaller duration for this muscle (Figures 11, 12). The smaller size and duration of the effects of torque and velocity, combined with the greater between-subject variability of FCR data might be a possible reason why the 0D analysis did not detect a significant effect of this interaction term for the FCR muscle.

Previous studies also demonstrated a significant effect of the interaction between perturbation torque and velocity on the LLRa evoked from the stretched muscle (Bedingham and Tatton, 1984). However, based on our literature review, no prior study evaluated this interaction for the muscle subject to a perturbation that shortened the muscle. Berardelli et al. (Berardelli and Hallett, 1984), reported that background torque and velocity would increase the amplitude of the EMG activity evoked from the shortened tibialis anterior muscle with a latency within 100–150 ms, but no interaction analysis was conducted in that study.

For both muscles, perturbation direction did not modulate the LLRa elicited by any velocity condition in absence of background torque. However, Miscio et al. (2001), observed lower LLRa for the shortened muscle compared with the stretched muscle which were perturbed with 500 deg/s velocity with no background torque. It might be possible that higher perturbation velocities are needed to differentiate the long latency stretch response of the agonist and antagonist muscles when there is no background activity. Moreover, to the best of our knowledge, our study is the first study comparing the effects of different perturbation velocities on the LLRa between the shortened and stretched upper limbs’ muscles, and the first to observe an interaction between background torque and velocity in the LLRa of a muscle undergoing shortening, an effect that was visible for the FCR muscle.

Interaction Between Task Instruction and Velocity

No analysis has revealed any interaction between the task instruction and perturbation velocity. The majority of previous work studied the interaction between perturbation velocity with perturbation duration and amplitude (Lee and Tatton, 1982; Lewis et al., 2005, 2006; Schuurmans et al., 2009). Among them, Lewis et al. (Lewis et al., 2006), was the only study that examined the interaction between task instruction and perturbation velocity. In contrast to our results, they reported a significant interaction, which resulted in a greater facilitation in the LLRa evoked from the Biceps brachii muscle, for the ‘Flex’ instructed perturbations with a 90 degree/s velocity. In their ‘Flex’ instruction, subjects were instructed to flex their elbows as soon as possible in response to the perturbation, a condition which wasn’t included in our experiment. Hence, it is possible that countering perturbations applied to the proximal upper limb joints (such as the elbow) would elicit a different modulation in the LLR from the one observed here for forearm muscles.

Similarity of the Effects Between FCR and ECU

Some of the interaction terms were only significant for ECU, and not for FCR. Specifically, no three-way interaction term was significant for the FCR muscle (as opposed to two terms significant for FCR), and two of the five two-way interactions significant for the ECU muscle were not significant for FCR. Overall, the mismatch of results between the two muscles is primarily due to larger variability of the measurements obtained for the FCR muscle compared to the ECU muscle, while all effects were generally in the same direction for both muscles. In many cases, in fact, the magnitude of measured modulations in LLRa (expressed in normalized units, so roughly comparable between muscles) was actually larger for the FCR muscle than for the ECU muscle. However, the standard error of the mean estimated by the mixed model was several times larger for the FCR muscle compared to the ECU muscle.

As an example, the term corresponding to the three-way interaction between direction, instruction, and torque was close to the conventional type-I error rate for the FCR muscle (p = 0.055). Further inspection of the parameter estimates corresponding to that interaction term shows that the mean difference in LLRa measured between Y and DNI conditions was slightly larger in the FCR muscle than it was for the ECU muscle under stretch (FCR LLRa – DNI: 6.05 ± 0.58 nu, Y: 3.95 ± 0.58 nu; ECU LLRa – DNI: 3.80 ± 0.17 nu, Y: 2.04 ± 0.17 nu), while smaller effects were measured in the shorten condition (FCR LLRa – DNI: 1.28 ± 0.58 nu, Y: 0.83 ± 0.58 nu; ECU LLRa – DNI: 0.78 ± 0.17 nu, Y: 0.73 ± 0.17 nu) for both muscles. Yet, the four-fold larger s.e.m. measured for the FCR prevented this effect from becoming significant.

0D vs. 1D Analysis

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to apply a mixed-model 1D analysis to the timeseries of EMG signals measured in response to velocity-controlled perturbations. 1D analyses are used to quantify if and when a timeseries is significantly modulated by an experimental factor, without a-priori hypotheses on the duration of this modulation, or on the time window where such a modulation is expected. Typically, reflex studies use 0D variables such as the average of the rectified signal in a pre-defined time window (Kurtzer et al., 2008), or the cumulative sum of the rectified EMG signal within a region of supra-threshold response (Brinkworth and Türker, 2003) – the latter definition allowing to explicitly account for a combined measure of amplitude and duration of a reflex response.

A shortcoming of using 0D datapoints generated from a 1D data set is the possibility for false positives. It has been indicated that the false positive rate of the 0D data is greater than the desired false positive rate of α = 0.05 when using noisy outcome measures such as peak in an interval (Pataky et al., 2016), and that this may be the case also in presence of smoothing (Pataky et al., 2015, 2016). Less noisy outcome measures such as the mean EMG signal in a pre-defined time window are not likely to result in limited control of the false positive rate of inference tests. However, the main advantage of using a 1D analysis for the study of stretch reflexes is the fact that the 1D analysis gives a more granular insight on the shape of the waveform (e.g., one peak, two peaks) which can be masked by the operation of averaging necessary for the creation of a 0D dataset. Moreover, the 1D analysis allows to test hypothesis on whether an effect is only present in a specific time-region, while controlling for the type-I error rate of inferential statements.

As an example, our 0D analysis showed that, in presence of background activation, there is an effect of task instruction on the LLRa measured from the ECU when stretched, but no effect measured when the muscle is shortened (Figure 4). However, a closer inspection on this modulation is provided by the post hoc t-tests presented in Figure 9. It is true that the difference between rectified EMG signal measured in the DNI and Y condition for the ECU under stretch is greater in the 200 mNm condition, reflecting a larger signal measured in the DNI condition (Figure 9, center). However, the same contrast plot extracted from the ECU during shortening perturbations highlights the presence of a biphasic response, where the DNI condition results in an early increase in EMG signal in the LLR time window, followed by a decrease in EMG signal. The average of the positive and negative changes is likely the reason why the same effect could not be captured using the 0D analysis.

Also, insight from a 1D analysis provides information about the gradient in timing resulting from different behavioral factors. Specifically, the effect of instruction is usually visible later than the one of velocity and torque, which have upcrossings in SLR and early LLR (Figures 7, 8). Instruction has a narrow region of significant main effects in the late LLR period (70 to 73 ms for FCR stretch, 81 to 83 ms for ECU stretch), with significant upcrossings in the voluntary period. Velocity conditions are significant in the early to mid LLR period (FCR stretch ∼40 to 70 ms, ECU stretch ∼45 to 90 ms, FCR shorten ∼50 to 90 ms, ECU shorten ∼45 to 80 ms), all having significant peaks near 60 ms. The 1D analysis allowed us to deduct the presence of a 10 to 20 ms delay between the significant peaks due to instruction versus velocity. The time frame of these significant peaks are comparable with previous findings and the identification of two distinct peaks within the LLR period, named M2 and M3 in the literature. However, it is difficult to compare the exact timing in absolute terms since “latency” in the literature is often defined relative to the onset of EMG activity (Lee and Tatton, 1982; Calancie and Bawa, 1985; Miscio et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2006; Kurtzer et al., 2014).

Yet, the results of the 0D and 1D analyses do not perfectly overlap. This is in part also due to limitations in the current version of the software used for the analysis. The spm1D MATLAB package allows up to three input conditions, as seen through its anova3rm function. To analyze the four conditions studied, two analyses per muscle were conducted when in the shortened and stretched conditions. As such, none of the three-way interactions that were significant in the 0D analysis could be studied in the same form using the 1D analysis. Another limitation of the 1D analysis is related to its limited statistical power. Because of the large number of datapoints included in the analysis, and the fact that the EMG signal is somewhat noisy and composed of multiple resolution elements in a time window of 200 ms following a perturbation, 1D analyses afford a reduced statistical power compared to a 0D analysis that only looks at the average within a pre-defined time window (Pataky et al., 2015).

Study Limitations

The sample size available for this study is in line with the one used in previous behavioral experiments with LLR of forearm muscles (Miscio et al., 2001; Pruszynski et al., 2009; Lewis et al., 2010) in terms of number of subjects recruited. The number of datapoints collected from each subject is also in line with most previous studies, with 240 datapoints per subject collected. However, given the need to estimate a larger number of model terms, a larger sample size would have allowed more precise estimation of all model effects and reduced the variance of the outcomes between the different muscles and the different statistical analysis techniques pursued.

The estimate of the effects of background torque is potentially confounded by the fact that we only applied perturbations in one direction, and the direction was predictable only in the 200 mNm condition. Specifically, in the 200 mNm condition, we applied perturbations that would stretch the agonist muscle, and as such the direction was predictable by the subject. Instead, in the 0 mNm condition, perturbations were applied in a random direction, not predictable by the subject. As such, the coefficients estimated for the two levels of the factor background torque might be different also simply because in the 200 mNm condition, subjects were able to predict perturbation direction, while in the rest condition, they were unable to do so. It is possible that prior knowledge of perturbation direction modulates the LLRa. As such, it is possible that our model estimated the compound effect of two distinct factors: a factor due to muscle pre-load, and another factor due to knowledge of the perturbation direction. This ambiguity could be addressed in future studies by applying perturbations that either stretch or shorten agonist muscles after pre-load.

Conclusion

In summary, this study has quantified the effect of four behavioral factors – background torque, task instruction, perturbation velocity and direction – on the long-latency response (LLR) amplitude evoked from the FCR and ECU muscles during ramp-and-hold perturbations applied to the wrist joint in the flexion and extension direction. Our analysis demonstrated that all of those factors modulate LLRa, and that their combination nonlinearly contribute to modulating the LLRa. Specifically, all the three-way interaction terms that can be computed without including the interaction between instruction and velocity significantly modulated the LLR. The interaction analysis suggested that higher background torque augmented the LLRa evoked from the stretched muscle when subjects are asked to maintain their muscle activation in response to the perturbations. Besides, higher perturbation velocity increased the LLRa evoked from the stretched muscle in presence of a background torque. Also, our analysis identified significant modulations of LLRa also in muscles shortened by the perturbation, including an interaction between torque and velocity, and an effect of both torque and velocity. While a lot of the behavioral factors listed above nonlinearly contribute to modulating LLRa, we observed that the effects of task instruction and velocity do not combine more than linearly to modulate the LLRa.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article have been uploaded on a zenodo repository, https://zenodo.org/record/4641292#.YGEzVC2cZTZ.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The University of Delaware Institutional Review Board, protocol no. 1097082-6. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JW supported development of stimulus hardware and protocol, performed data collection and analysis, and contributed to manuscript writing. PA-F supported data analysis and manuscript writing. AZ developed stimulus hardware and protocol and supported data collection and analysis. RN supported manuscript writing. FS developed the study design, supervised data collection and analysis, manuscript writing. All authors reviewed and approved the manuscript in its current form.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the National Institutes of Health under award number R21NS111310. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Additional support was provided by the University of Delaware Research Foundation grant no. 16A01402, by ACCEL NIGMS IDeA grant no. U54-GM104941.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all our participants for their participation in this research.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.639773/full#supplementary-material

References

Allum, J. H. J. (1975). Responses to load disturbances in human shoulder muscles: the hypothesis that one component is a pulse test information signal. Exp. Brain Res. 22, 307–326.

Google Scholar

Banks, C. L., Little, V. L., Walker, E. R., and Patten, C. (2019). Lower extremity long-latency reflexes differentiate walking function after stroke. Exp. Brain Res. 237, 2595–2605. doi: 10.1007/s00221-019-05614-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedingham, W., and Tatton, W. G. (1984). Dependence of EMG responses evoked by imposed wrist displacements on pre-existing activity in the stretched muscles. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 11, 272–280. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100045534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berardelli, A., and Hallett, M. (1984). Shortening reaction of human tibialis anterior. Neurology 34, 242–245. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.2.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brinkworth, R. S. A., and Türker, K. S. (2003). A method for quantifying reflex responses from intra-muscular and surface electromyogram. J. Neurosci. Methods 122, 179–193. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(02)00321-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calancie, B., and Bawa, P. (1985). Firing patterns of human flexor carpi radialis motor units during the stretch reflex. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 1179–1193. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.5.1179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cody, F. W., and Plant, T. (1989). Electromyographic reflexes evoked in human wrist flexors by tendon extension and by displacement of the wrist joint. J. Physiol. 411, 379–392.

Google Scholar

Colebatch, J. G., Gandevia, S. C., McCloskey, D. I., and Potter, E. K. (1979). Subject instruction and long latency reflex responses to muscle stretch. J. Physiol. 292, 527–534. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crago, P. E., Houk, J. C., and Hasan, Z. (1976). Regulatory actions of human stretch reflex. J. Neurophysiol. 39, 925–935. doi: 10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deneri, E., Tilek, N., Çoban, A., and Tataroğlu, C. (2020). Long latency reflexes in patients with postural instability and ataxia. Neurol. Sci. Neurophysiol. 37, 164–169. doi: 10.4103/nsn.nsn_44_20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evarts, E. V., and Granit, R. (1976). Relations of reflexes and intended movements. Prog. Brain Res. 44, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)60719-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, P. H. (1956). The influence of prior instruction to the subject on an apparently involuntary neuro-muscular response. J. Physiol. 132, 17–18.

Google Scholar

Hayashi, R., Hashimoto, T., Tada, T., and Ikeda, S. (2001). Relation between changes in long-latency stretch reflexes and muscle stiffness in Parkinson’s disease–comparison before and after unilateral pallidotomy. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 1814–1821.

Google Scholar

Kurtzer, I., Crevecoeur, F., and Scott, S. H. (2014). Fast feedback control involves two independent processes utilizing knowledge of limb dynamics. J. Neurophysiol. 11568, 1631–1645. doi: 10.1152/jn.00514.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtzer, I. L. (2015). Long-latency reflexes account for limb biomechanics through several supraspinal pathways. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:99. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtzer, I. L., Pruszynski, J. A., and Scott, S. H. (2008). Long-latency reflexes of the human arm reflect an internal model of limb dynamics. Curr. Biol. 18, 449–453. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R., and Tatton, W. (1982). Long latency reflexes to imposed displacements of the human wrist: dependence on duration of movement. Exp. Brain Res. 45, 207–216.

Google Scholar

Lee, R., and Tatton, W. G. (1975). Motor responses to sudden limb displacements in primates with specific CNS lesions and in human patients with motor system disorders. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 2, 285–293. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100020382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, G. N., MacKinnon, C. D., and Perreault, E. J. (2006). The effect of task instruction on the excitability of spinal and supraspinal reflex pathways projecting to the biceps muscle. Exp. Brain Res. 174, 413–425. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0475-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, G. N., MacKinnon, C. D., Trumbower, R., and Perreault, E. J. (2010). Co-contraction modifies the stretch reflex elicited in muscles shortened by a joint perturbation. Exp. Brain Res. 207, 39–48. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2426-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, G. N., Perreault, E. J., and MacKinnon, C. D. (2005). The influence of perturbation duration and velocity on the long-latency response to stretch in the biceps muscle. Exp. Brain Res. 163, 361–369. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2182-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, G. N., Polych, M. A., and Byblow, W. D. (2004). Proposed cortical and sub-cortical contributions to the long-latency stretch reflex in the forearm. Exp. Brain Res. 156, 72–79. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1767-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, P. B. (1986). Obseravtions on the automatic compensation of reflex gain on varying the pre-existing level of motor discharge in man. J. Physiol. 374, 73–90. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, P. B. (1989). Long-latency stretch reflexes of two intrinsic muscles of the human hand analysed by cooling the arm. J. Physiol. 419, 519–538. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, P. B. (1993). Interaction between short-and long-latency components of the human stretch reflex during sinusoidal stretching. J. Physiol. 462, 503–527. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirbagheri, M. M., Kindig, M. W., and Niu, X. (2015). Effects of robotic-locomotor training on stretch reflex function and muscular properties in individuals with spinal cord injury. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 997–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miscio, G., Pisano, F., Del Conte, C., Pianca, D., Colombo, R., and Schieppati, M. (2001). The shortening reaction of forearm muscles: the influence of central set. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 884–894. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00468-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noth, J., Schwarz, M., Podoll, K., and Motamedi, F. (1991). Evidence that low-threshold muscle afferents evoke long-latency stretch reflexes in human hand muscles. J. Neurophysiol. 65, 1089–1097. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.5.1089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pataky, T. C., Vanrenterghem, J., and Robinson, M. A. (2015). Zero- vs. one-dimensional, parametric vs. non-parametric, and confidence interval vs. hypothesis testing procedures in one-dimensional biomechanical trajectory analysis. J. Biomech. 48, 1277–1285. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2015.02.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pataky, T. C., Vanrenterghem, J., and Robinson, M. A. (2016). The probability of false positives in zero-dimensional analyses of one-dimensional kinematic, force and EMG trajectories. J. Biomech. 49, 1468–1476. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2016.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruszynski, J. A., Kurtzer, I., Lillicrap, T. P., and Scott, S. H. (2009). Temporal evolution of “automatic gain-scaling.” J. Neurophysiol. 102, 992–1003. doi: 10.1152/jn.00085.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruszynski, J. A., Kurtzer, I., and Scott, S. H. (2011). The long-latency reflex is composed of at least two functionally independent processes. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 449–459. doi: 10.1152/jn.01052.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothwell, J. C., Traub, M. M., and Marsden, C. D. (1980). Influence of voluntary intent on the human long-latency stretch reflex. Nature 286, 496–498. doi: 10.1038/286496a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuurmans, J., De Vlugt, E., Schouten, A. C., Meskers, C. G. M., De Groot, J. H., and Van Der Helm, F. C. T. (2009). The monosynaptic Ia afferent pathway can largely explain the stretch duration effect of the long latency M2 response. Exp. Brain Res. 193, 491–500. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1647-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinkjær, T., and Hayashi, R. (1989). Regulation of wrist stiffness by the stretch reflex. J. Biomech. 22, 1133–1140. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(89)90215-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarkka, I. M., and Larsen, T. A. (1987). Changes of electrically elicited reflexes in hand and forearm muscles in man. Am. J. Phys. Med. 66, 308–314. doi: 10.1097/00002060-198710000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatton, W. G., and Bawa, P. (1979). Input-output properties of motor unit responses in muscles stretched by imposed displacements of the monkey wrist. Exp. Brain Res. 37, 439–457.

Google Scholar

Thomas, J. S., Brown, J., and Lucier, G. E. (1977). Influence of task set on muscular responses to arm perturbations in normal subjects and Parkinson pateints. Exp. Neurol. 55, 618–628. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(77)90288-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trumbower, R. D., Finley, J. M., Shemmell, J. B., Honeycutt, C. F., and Perreault, E. J. (2013). Bilateral impairments in task-dependent modulation of the long-latency stretch reflex following stroke. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1373–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zonnino, A., Farrens, A. J., Ress, D., and Sergi, F. (2019). “StretchfMRI: a novel technique to quantify the contribution of the reticular formation to long-latency responses via fMRI,” in IEEE 16th International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics (ICORR), Toronto, ON, (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 1247–1253. doi: 10.1109/ICORR.2019.8779451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stretch reflex, long latency responses, motor neurophysiology, electromyography, robotics

Citation: Weinman J, Arfa-Fatollahkhani P, Zonnino A, Nikonowicz RC and Sergi F (2021) Effects of Perturbation Velocity, Direction, Background Muscle Activation, and Task Instruction on Long-Latency Responses Measured From Forearm Muscles. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:639773. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.639773

Received: 09 December 2020; Accepted: 15 March 2021;
Published: 16 April 2021.

Edited by:

Mario Bernardo-Filho, Rio de Janeiro State University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Renato Naville Watanabe, Federal University of ABC, Brazil
Mariagiovanna Cantone, Sant’Elia Hospital, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Weinman, Arfa-Fatollahkhani, Zonnino, Nikonowicz and Sergi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fabrizio Sergi, fabs@udel.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.