Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 January 2021
Sec. Speech and Language

Disrupted Subcortical-Cortical Connections in a Phonological but Not Semantic Task in Chinese Children With Dyslexia

  • 1State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2Faculty of Education, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 3Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON, Canada

Reading disability has been considered as a disconnection syndrome. Recently, an increasing number of studies have emphasized the role of subcortical regions in reading. However, the majority of research on reading disability has focused on the connections amongst brain regions within the classic cortical reading network. Here, we used graph theoretical analysis to investigate whether subcortical regions serve as hubs (regions highly connected with other brain regions) during reading both in Chinese children with reading disability (N = 15, age ranging from 11.03 to 13.08 years) and in age-matched typically developing children (N = 16, age ranging from 11.17 to 12.75 years) using a visual rhyming judgment task and a visual meaning judgment task. We found that the bilateral thalami were the unique hubs for typically developing children across both tasks. Additionally, subcortical regions (right putamen, left pallidum) were also unique hubs for typically developing children but only in the rhyming task. Among these subcortical hub regions, the left pallidum showed reduced connectivity with inferior frontal regions in the rhyming judgment but not semantic task in reading disabled compared with typically developing children. These results suggest that subcortical-cortical disconnection, which may be particularly relevant to the phonological and phonology-related learning process, may be associated with Chinese reading disability.

Introduction

Recently, an increasing number of studies have reported that subcortical regions are involved in the language and reading neural network (Tomasi and Volkow, 2012; Hebb and Ojemann, 2013; Liu et al., 2018; Tang et al., 2018; also see in a review: Price, 2012). For example, a resting-state functional connectivity study including 970 healthy subjects found that Broca's and Wernicke's areas connect not only classic cortical regions engaged in language processing (i.e., pre-frontal, temporal, and parietal regions) but also to subcortical regions (i.e., bilateral caudate, left pallidum, left putamen, and left thalamus) (Tomasi and Volkow, 2012). Further, previous studies have shown that thalamus and basal ganglia are involved in multiple language-related processes such as phonological processing (Booth et al., 2007b; Hebb and Ojemann, 2013), speech production (Bohland and Guenther, 2006; Tang et al., 2018), orthographic processing (Braun et al., 2019), lexical semantic processing (Kotz et al., 2002; Ketteler et al., 2008), as well as non-linguistic processes, such as procedural learning (Schultz et al., 1997; Ullman, 2004; Shohamy et al., 2008; Schreiweis et al., 2014). For example, the left putamen was reported to be activated in a visual rhyming judgment task and was thought to be involved in the initiation of the phonological processing (Booth et al., 2007b). Bohland and Guenther (2006) found that bilateral putamen involved in overt speech production and bilateral anterior thalamus and caudate were related to sequence complexity during overt speech production. And the left pallidus and the left caudate were found to be more activated when recognizing words with higher frequency and more similar-orthographic neighbors in a silent reading task, suggesting these regions engage in the matching of orthographic language input onto the representations in long-term memory (Braun et al., 2019). Pugh et al. (2013) found that activation of the left thalamus was significantly correlated with reading scores in children, and they suggested that future studies investigating reading and reading disorders should include both cortical and subcortical regions. Taken together, these studies suggest that subcortical structures play an important role in language processing.

Developmental dyslexia is a neurological disorder, in which individuals have difficulty acquiring fluency in reading, despite having normal intelligence and motivation (Lyon et al., 2003; Shaywitz and Shaywitz, 2005). Many studies have also found that atypical brain function in individuals with dyslexia in alphabetic scripts includes not only cortical brain regions (see in meta-analysis: Maisog et al., 2008; Richlan et al., 2009; Paulesu et al., 2014; Martin et al., 2016) but also some subcortical regions, such as hypoactivation in the left thalamus (Gaab et al., 2007; Díaz et al., 2012) and hyperactiveation in the bilateral caudate (Hoeft et al., 2007; Richlan et al., 2011 only in the left caudate) and right thalamus (Hoeft et al., 2007). A recent review of neurobiological studies in children with language impairment and dyslexia suggested that dysfunction of corticostriatal systems, which may impact language learning processes due to impairment in extracting and decoding complex rules, may be an alternative cause for dyslexia aside from the dysfunction of the cortical system for phonological representation and manipulation (Krishnan et al., 2016).

The prevalence of dyslexia ranges from 5 to 8% in Chinese school-aged children (Stevenson et al., 1982; Zhang et al., 1996). Similar to research on alphabetic dyslexia, research on Chinese dyslexia has also reported dysfunction in subcortical regions, including the left putamen (Siok et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2019) and left thalamus (Siok et al., 2008), although most studies (Siok et al., 2004, 2008; Liu et al., 2012, 2013) have mainly reported brain abnormality in cortical brain regions. Specifically, Siok et al. (2008) found reduced activation in left putamen and thalamus in Chinese dyslexic children compared to age-matched controls when performing a visual character rhyming judgment task. Wang et al. (2019) found reduced gray matter volume and altered functional abnormality of the putamen in Chinese dyslexic children. All of these above studies have indicated that abnormal subcortical regions may be associated with or be a cause of reading impairment.

Previous brain connectivity studies have further suggested that alphabetic dyslexia is a disconnection syndrome (Horwitz et al., 1998; Stanberry et al., 2006; van der Mark et al., 2011; Vogel et al., 2012; Boets et al., 2013; Koyama et al., 2013; Schurz et al., 2015; Morken et al., 2017), and this is also true for Chinese dyslexia (Zhou et al., 2015; Cao et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2019). Particularly relevant to the current study, Zhou et al. (2015) found altered resting functional connectivity between left middle frontal and visual-orthographic processing regions (i.e., left visual word form area and left intraparietal sulcus) in Chinese children with dyslexia compared to age-matched controls (Zhou et al., 2015). In another study, lower connectivity was found between the left superior temporal gyrus and left fusiform gyrus in Chinese children with dyslexia than both age-matched controls and reading-matched controls in an auditory rhyming task, suggesting a weak connection between phonology and orthography (Cao et al., 2017). In addition, Cao et al. (2018) found that Chinese children with dyslexia showed weaker functional connectivity between the left inferior frontal gyrus (a phonological processing region) and the left middle occipital gyrus (an orthographical processing region) compared to age- and reading-matched controls in a visual orthography judgment task. However, these functional connectivity studies of Chinese dyslexia focused on the connectivity within the classic cortical reading network. To our knowledge, only one study (Wang et al., 2019) has reported altered functional connectivity between the left putamen and cortical regions relative to age- and reading-matched children in an auditory rhyming task, suggesting that an abnormal subcortical-cortical connectivity pattern may be a contributing factor to Chinese dyslexia.

Although a small number of studies (Siok et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2019) have reported abnormal brain structure, brain activation and functional connectivity in subcortical regions in Chinese dyslexia during language or reading tasks in Chinese children with dyslexia, the role of subcortical regions in reading and reading impairment is less understood compared with that of the cortical brain regions. There is a need to systematically evaluate the role of subcortical regions in typical as well as atypical reading development. Toward this aim, the current study used a graph theoretical approach to examine whether and which subcortical regions are hubs (that is, a key region connecting many other brain regions) in the brain network for reading in typically developing (TD) Chinese children and reading disabled (RD) Chinese children. Additionally, we sought to address whether hub distribution differed between groups, particularly within subcortical regions. Our third aim was to examine how these subcortical hubs interact with the classic cortical brain regions involved in reading, and whether group differences exist in the subcortical-cortical connections. Specifically, we adopted a visual rhyming judgment and a visual meaning judgment task to examine the connectivity of subcortical regions in the brain networks of phonological and semantic processing during Chinese character reading. We predicted that subcortical regions serve as hub in the reading process similar to cortical regions of typical readers, particularly in the phonological processing task as previous studies have implicated subcortical regions in phonology-related (Booth et al., 2007b; Hebb and Ojemann, 2013) and speech articulation (Bohland and Guenther, 2006; Kita et al., 2013; Tang et al., 2018) processes. Additionally, subcortical regions, particularly the striatum, play a role in procedural learning (Schultz et al., 1997; Ullman, 2004; Shohamy et al., 2008; Schreiweis et al., 2014), and learning phonology may involve more procedural processes than learning the meaning of a character. Finally, because previous work has shown altered putamen-cortical connectivity in Chinese children with dyslexia (Wang et al., 2019), we predicted that functional connectivity between subcortical regions and the cortical reading network would be altered in the RD group during the visual rhyming judgment task.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixteen typically developing (TD) children (5 females; age: mean = 11.79 years, SD = 0.39) and 15 children with reading disability (RD) (5 females; age: mean = 12.06 years, SD = 0.49) participated in this study. Amongst the participants, some (11 TD children and 11 RD children) came from a previously published study (Liu et al., 2012) by the same research group, and the other subjects (5 TD children, 4 RD children) were new participants. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Beijing Normal University. Informed consent was obtained from all children and their parents. The children were included according to the following criteria: (1) native Chinese speakers; (2) right-handed; (3) normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing; (4) no psychiatric or neurological disease; (5) not taking medication that influenced the central nervous system; and (6) not having attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Non-verbal intelligence and reading ability were measured in all children who met these criteria. Children's non-verbal intelligence was measured using the Chinese version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) (Wechsler, 1974) and scored by local norms (Lin and Zhang, 1986). Reading ability was measured by the Character Recognition Measure and Assessment Scale for Primary School Children (CRM) (Wang and Tao, 1993) and the character reading fluency (CRF) test (Liu et al., 2013). The CRM test measures how many characters a child can recognize among 3,500 of the most commonly used Chinese characters. CRM is a widely used test to screen dyslexic Chinese children (Liu et al., 2012, 2013; Qi et al., 2016). Here, we used the CRM test score as a criterion for screening children with reading disability. The CRF is a single-character reading fluency test. It comprises 135 high-frequency Chinese characters chosen from primary school textbooks. The characters are sorted into columns and ordered from easy to difficult. Children are asked to read aloud the characters sequentially, column by column, as fast and correctly as possible. The number of characters read correctly and the time participants spent are recorded. The speed (correct number/time) is used as the test score. Here, CRF was used as an index for reading fluency.

All children had a normal non-verbal IQ (above 90). Children whose character recognition scores tested by CRM were 1.5 years below the average level of the same age were included in the RD group. Children whose scores were not 1.5 years below their age level were included in the TD group. Demographic information and task performance of all children (16 in the TD group and 15 in the RD group) are shown in Table 1. The two groups were matched in age (t(29) = −1.717, p = 0.097), sex (Mann-Whitney U = 117.5, p = 0.900), and PIQ (t(29) = 1.748, p = 0.091). The RD group scored significantly lower on the CRM (t(29) = 9.759, p < 0.001) and CRF (t(29) = 6.803, p < 0.001) than the TD group. Data from one child in the RD group in the rhyming judgment task were excluded due to large head motion artifacts (larger than 3 mm). As a result, the following data analysis included 14 RD children in the rhyming judgment task. The group differences in demographics and reading performance remained similar after the one RD child was excluded (more details are shown in Supplementary Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and task performance of all the typically developing (TD) and reading disabled (RD) children.

Design and fMRI Paradigm

This study is a re-analysis of part of the data from a previously published study (Liu et al., 2012). The previously published study used traditional brain activation analysis to reveal the neural abnormalities of Chinese children with reading disability. However, the current study used a graph theoretical approach to examine the functional connectivity of subcortical regions. A rhyming judgment task and a meaning judgment task were used to investigate the two key processes of reading: orthography-to-phonology (O-P) mapping and orthography-to-semantics (O-S) mapping. Two Chinese characters were displayed on the center of the screen sequentially. Each character was displayed for 800 ms with a 200 ms interval between the two characters. Then, a red fixation cross was presented to remind the participants to respond. The duration of the red cross was jittered among 2,200, 2,600, and 3,000 ms with equal probability. Participants were asked to judge whether the two characters were rhyming or not in the rhyming judgment task and whether they were semantically related or not in the meaning judgment task. When two characters rhymed in the rhyming judgment task or were semantically related in the meaning judgment task, participants were asked to press a button with their right index finger; otherwise, they were asked to press another button with their right middle finger. In both tasks, half of the pairs presented in trials were rhyming or related, and half were not. Characters in both tasks were chosen from textbooks for primary school and were matched for frequency, acquisition age, and number of strokes. More detailed information about the materials could been found in the previous paper (Liu et al., 2012).

In addition to the character condition above, there were two control conditions for each task. These two conditions were designed to exclude brain activation due to basic visual processing, decision-making processing and motion execution processing. The two conditions were perceptual control and null control. In the perceptual control condition, participants were asked to determine whether two sequentially presented Tibetan symbols were identical or not. Half of the pairs presented in the perceptual condition were identical. In the null control condition, two black crosses were sequentially presented, and participants were asked to press a button with their right index finger when the second cross turned blue. The procedures and timing for these two conditions were the same as those for the character condition.

We used an event-related design in our experiment. There were four runs in total (two runs for each task). Each run was 6 min 44 s in length. Forty-eight pairs of character stimuli, 12 pairs of perceptual stimuli and 24 pairs of null stimuli were presented in each run. Instructions were promptly provided before each run to brief the participants about the task. In addition, there was a 12 s period at the beginning to acquire an equilibrium magnetization of the scanner and a 22 s period at the end to allow deconvolution of the entire hemodynamic response function (HRF) of the last trial.

Image Acquisition

Imaging data were acquired using a 3T Siemens scanner (MAGNETOM Trio, a Tim System) at Beijing Normal University. The susceptibility weighted single-shot echo planar imaging (EPI) method was used to obtain blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) images during the task. The following scan parameters were used: TR = 2,000 ms; TE = 20 ms; flip angle = 80°; number of slices = 32; slice thickness = 3 mm, gap = 0.48 mm; FOV = 220 * 206 mm; matrix = 128 * 120 * 32; and voxel size = 1.72 * 1.72 * 3.48 mm. In addition, we also acquired a structural T1 weighted 3D image (MPRAGE) with the following parameters: TR = 2,300 ms; TE = 3.36 ms; number of slices = 160; slice thickness = 1 mm; FOV = 256 * 256 mm; matrix = 256 * 256 * 160; and voxel size = 1 * 1 * 1 mm.

Before formal imaging acquisition, the children were first put in a simulated scanner to get used to the scanning environment. They then completed a short practice session for each of the two tasks to help them become familiar with the experiment instructions and procedures.

Data Pre-processing

The imaging data was preprocessed using Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF)1. First, we deleted the first 10 volumes of each participant's functional imaging data to ensure the steadiness of the signal. We then performed slice-timing to correct the scanning time delay. Realignment was then used to correct head motion during scanning. After excluding the participants whose head motion was larger than 3 mm, we coregistered each participant's T1 structural image to their mean functional images. Next, we segmented and normalized the coregistered T1 images to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. The functional images were normalized to a 3 * 3 * 3 mm3 voxel size. The resampled functional images were smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM (full width at half maximum) Gaussian filter. Detrending with a high-pass filter (0.01–0.08 Hz) was also applied to the images. Finally, we regressed out the six head-motion parameters, cerebrospinal fluid signal, and white mater signal.

Graph Theory and Network Construction

Graph theory is an effective method to investigate interregional connectivity networks within the whole brain. Graph theory treats the structural or functional brain network as a graph that is composed of nodes (brain regions) and edges (structural or functional connections) (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009, 2012). Previous studies using this method have reported many novel findings in terms of the alterations in the structural (Hosseini et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2015; Qi et al., 2016) and functional (Finn et al., 2014; Edwards et al., 2018; Yang and Tan, 2020) brain networks of children with dyslexia.

Network construction and network property calculations were processed using Graph Theoretical Network Analysis Toolbox (GRETNA)2. We used the same network construction method as a previously published paper (Liu et al., 2018). Briefly, according to the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) tool (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002), we first divided the whole brain (excluding the cerebellum) of each participant into 90 regions. For each region, the mean time series of all voxels within this region was used as the regional time series. Then, we extracted 2 TR time series after the onset of the first stimulus in each trial individually for 90 regions of each participant for both tasks separately. We chose the current duration because the duration from the onset of the first stimulus to the end of the trial was about 4.4 s in average in each trial. Next, we concatenated the trials for the three conditions separately in each task (character condition, perceptual condition and null condition) (Ekman et al., 2012). As a result, we obtained time series data for 90 regions for each participant in each of the three conditions of the two tasks separately.

To construct an interregional correlation network, we then calculated Pearson's correlation coefficient between each pair of regional time series for each participant, for each condition, for each task. As a result, each participant had six 90*90 connectivity matrices (interregional correlation network). Binary matrices were then generated from the 90*90 connectivity matrices of each participant using different sparsity threshold values. If the absolute value of the correlation coefficient was above the threshold, then its value in the binary matrix was set to 1; otherwise, its value was set to 0. We applied a series of sparsity thresholds (from 0.05 to 0.4, with a step size of 0.01) to ensure that the networks were fully connected and still maintained small-world architecture (Achard et al., 2006; Liang et al., 2016). The networks were not fully connected at lower sparsity values (<0.05) and were less likely to maintain small-world architecture at higher sparsity values (>0.4).

Network Properties Calculation

The network properties of the functional brain binary networks we obtained above were characterized using graph theoretical measures. As the major aim of the current study was to explore the role of the subcortical regions in typical as well as atypical reading development, we concentrated on the nodal properties of the subcortical nodes. The nodal degree of a particular node is the number of direct connections with other nodes in the whole network. A high nodal degree indicates that the node is highly connected in the network. The nodes whose normalized degrees are 1 SD above the mean degree of all nodes in the network are defined as hubs.

Statistical Analysis

Hub Distribution Analysis

We identified the hubs for each group in each task and classified them as group-shared or group-unique hubs. Because we applied a series sparsity threshold to obtain the nodal degree, the degree changed when the threshold changed. Therefore, we calculated the area under the curve (AUC) to obtain the index of nodal topology, which was independent of the threshold. Then, we transformed the degree into a Z score using the mean and standard deviation of all nodes. Nodes whose degrees were 1 SD above the mean were identified as hubs. Using the method above, we obtained a list of hubs for each group per task. We excluded the hubs that were also identified as hubs in the two control conditions.

Inter-Regional Correlation Analysis

We first examined how the subcortical hubs interacted with the classic cortical reading regions in both tasks separately for each group by performing inter-regional correlation analysis. We then used two sample t-tests to explore whether these significant subcortical-cortical correlations for the TD or the RD group would significantly differ. Specifically, based on previous meta-analyses of Chinese reading (Bolger et al., 2005; Tan et al., 2005; Wu et al., 2012), we chose 16 cortical regions as the classic reading network, including regions in the left frontal lobe (i.e., middle frontal gyrus, the opercular part of inferior frontal gyrus and the triangular part of inferior frontal gyrus), the left parietal lobe (i.e., inferior parietal lobe and superior parietal gyrus), the left temporal lobe (i.e., inferior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus), and in the bilateral occipital lobe (i.e., inferior occipital gyrus, middle occipital gyrus, and lingual gyrus) and bilateral fusiform gyri. The interregional correlation coefficients were first transformed into normalized z-scores using the Fisher z-transformation before evaluating them via t-tests. We added age, the PIQ and the ACC of the task as covariates in the t-tests. A significance level of p < 0.05 corrected using a false discovery rate (FDR) was adopted in group comparisons. In order to validate our results, we further conducted a permutation test on the interregional correlations showing significant group difference in the t-test.

Brain-Behavior Correlation Analysis

In order to reveal the relationship between the altered subcortical-cortical connections and the deficits in reading process, we further calculated the Pearson's correlation coefficient between the task performance and the subcortical-cortical connections showing significant group difference for each group separately in each task. Before calculating the correlation coefficient, we firstly transformed the inter-regional correlation coefficients and the score of task performance into normalized z-scores using the Fisher z-transformation.

Results

Task Performance of the Two Groups

The RD group responded significantly faster(t(28) = −2.464, p = 0.020) but less accurate (t(28) = 3.819, p < 0.001) in the rhyming judgment task than in the TD group (Supplementary Table 1). As for the meaning judgment task, the accuracy of the RD group was significantly lower (t(29) = 2.660, p = 0.013) than the TD group, but there was no group difference the in the response time (Table 1).

Hub Distribution of the Two Groups

In the rhyming judgment task, 16 hubs were identified for each group, of which eight (33% overlapping rate) were shared (Table 2; Figure 1). The unique hubs in the TD group were mainly subcortical regions, including the bilateral thalami (THA) and the right putamen (PUT). In the RD group, the unique hubs were mainly regions in the frontal lobe, including the right medial superior frontal gyrus (SFGmed), left dorsolateral superior frontal gyrus (SFGdor), right opercular inferior frontal gyrus (IFGoperc), and left gyrus rectus (REC).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hub distribution for the two groups in the rhyming and meaning judgment task.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hub distribution of the two groups in the rhyming judgment task (left) and in the meaning judgment task (right). Both groups had some subcortical regions as hubs. In addition, the two groups had more shared hubs in the meaning than the rhyming judgment task. TD, typically developing group; RD, reading disabled group; RH, rhyming judgment task; ME, meaning judgment task; L, left; R, right. See Abbreviations for full names of the abbreviations.

In the meaning judgment task (Table 2; Figure 1), there were 15 hubs for each group, 13 of which (76% overlapping rate) were shared. The unique regions in the TD group were again subcortical regions (bilateral THA), while the unique regions in the RD group were cortical regions (right middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and left SFGmed). The two groups shared more hubs in the meaning judgment task than in the rhyming judgment task (76 vs. 33%, respectively).

Focusing on subcortical regions (Supplementary Table 2), within the TD group the bilateral THA were identified as unique hubs in both tasks, and the right PUT and left pallidum (PAL) in the rhyming judgment task. There were no TD-unique subcortical hubs specific to the meaning judgment task, and no RD-unique subcortical hubs in either task. When examining subcortical hubs that were common between groups, several regions were identified. Specifically, both groups shared the bilateral insula (INS) and left PUT as hubs across tasks, while the bilateral PAL and right PUT were shared within the meaning judgment task. There were no shared subcortical hubs specific to the rhyming judgment task.

Group Differences in the Subcortical-Cortical Inter-Regional Correlations

Figure 2 and Table 3 demonstrates significant inter-regional correlations between subcortical and classic cortical reading regions in the rhyming and meaning judgment tasks separately for the TD and RD group. Specifically, in the rhyming judgment task, both TD and RD showed significant inter-regional correlations between bilateral INS and left STG and between left INS and left IFGoperc. Additionally, the TD group showed significant inter-regional correlations between multiple subcortical regions (i.e., bilateral INS, left PAL) and left IFG regions. The RD group additionally showed significant inter-regional correlations between the right PUT and left STG (Figure 2A). Similar to the rhyming judgment task, in the meaning judgment task both TD and RD groups showed significant inter-regional correlations between left INS and left IFGoperc and between bilateral INS and left STG. The RD group additionally showed significant inter-regional correlations between the left PUT and left STG, and between the left PUT and two left IFG regions (Figure 2B). Taken together, the subcortical regions mainly connected to phonological processing regions (i.e., the left STG and IFG) in both the TD and RD groups for both tasks. Among those significant subcortical-cortical inter-regional correlations, the connectivity strength between the left PAL and the two left IFG regions was significantly larger in the TD than the RD group in the rhyming judgment task (as shown in Figure 2C and Table 4). No significantly larger inter-regional correlations were found in the RD than the TD group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significantly larger subcortical-cortical correlations in the TD than RD group were found in the rhyming judgment task. (A) Significant subcortical-cortical correlations of the two groups in the rhyming judgment task. (B) Significant subcortical-cortical correlations of the two groups in the meaning judgment task. Blue lines represent significant correlations in the TD group, pink lines represent the significant correlations in the RD group, purple lines represent the significant correlations sharing by both groups. (C) Significantly larger inter-regional correlations between PAL and left IFG regions (the orange lines) in TD than RD group in the rhyming judgment task. No significantly larger subcortical-cortical correlations were found in RD than TD. The significant alpha level was set at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected). RH, rhyming judgment task; ME, meaning judgment task; TD, typically developing group; RD, reading disabled group; L, left; R, right. See Abbreviations for full names of the abbreviations.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Significant inter-regional correlations between subcortical regions and classic cortical reading regions in the rhyming and meaning judgment tasks separately for the TD and RD group.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Significantly larger inter-regional correlations between the left PAL and the left IFG regions in the TD than RD group in the rhyming judgment task.

Brain-Behavior Correlation

Brain-behavior correlation between the inter-regional connections with significant group difference (i.e., the correlation between the left PAL and the two inferior frontal regions) and task performance of the rhyming judgment task for each group was calculated. We found significant positive brain-behavior correlations between the two inter-regional correlations and the respond time in the rhyming task in the RD group (the left PAL- the left IFGoperc: r = 0.626, p = 0.022; the left PAL-the left IFGtriang: r = 0.653, p = 0.015). As for the TD group, the same brain-behavior correlation showed an opposite tendency but didn't reach significant (i.e., the left PAL- the left IFGoperc: r = −0.092, p = 0.754; the left PAL-the left IFGtriang: r = −0.245, p = 0.398). And we found no significant correlations between the accuracy of the rhyming task and inter-regional correlations in either of the two groups.

Discussion

In this study, we used graph theoretical analysis to examine the connectivity of subcortical regions within the reading network in typically developing (TD) and reading disabled (RD) Chinese children during two reading tasks. Two major findings emerged. First, reading disabled children engaged fewer subcortical hubs, and showed decreased subcortical-cortical connectivity compared to typically developing children. Second, alterations of the RD group in hub distribution and brain connectivity were mainly found in the rhyming judgment task compared to the meaning judgment task.

Decreased Connectivity of Subcortical Regions in Chinese Children With Reading Disability

Perhaps the most interesting finding of the current study was that the RD group showed lower connectivity of subcortical regions than the TD group, particularly in the rhyming task, as demonstrated in hub distribution and inter-regional correlation analyses. When examining hub distribution, we found that multiple subcortical regions served as hubs in both groups across both tasks. However, subcortical regions were less involved within the RD group compared with the TD group. Specifically, the TD group had multiple unique hubs located in subcortical regions in both tasks (such as the bilateral thalamus, right putamen and left pallidum in the rhyming judgment task and the bilateral thalamus in the meaning judgment task). A hub is an essential node that is highly connected with other nodes within a network. Therefore, fewer subcortical regions serving as hubs in the RD group suggests less involvement of those subcortical regions during reading. When examining group differences in inter-regional correlations of the subcortical hubs, the left PAL in the RD group had weaker connections with the two left inferior frontal regions (i.e., the IFGtriang and IFGoperc) than those in the TD group during the rhyming judgment task. The left PAL has been suggested to be involved in motor process during speech articulation (Wise et al., 1999; Manes et al., 2014, 2018). The IFG, particularly the triangular and opercular regions, is also involved in speech articulation and phonological processing (Tan et al., 2005; Booth et al., 2007a; Cone et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2012). Thus, the weak connection between the left PAL and the inferior frontal regions may indicate a deficit in speech articulation, a process that lays the foundation for proficient reading (Shaywitz and Shaywitz, 2005; Rueckl et al., 2015). In addition, the correlations between the response time of the rhyming judgment task and the strength of these two connections showed opposite tendency between the two groups (i.e., positive in the RD group and negative in the TD group), indicating that these subcortical-cortical disconnections were related to the deficit in the phonological processing in the RD group. We assume that subcortical regions may play a role in “scaffolding”, allowing children to switch from sensory-motor based reading to abstract linguistic-based reading. This argument is consistent with previous findings that typical reading development is characterized by a shift from reliance on connections of subcortical regions to reliance on connections of cortical regions (Liu et al., 2018). Alternatively, the PAL is also closely connected with the striatum-frontal loop involved in procedural learning; the weaker connection between left PAL and the inferior frontal regions may instead indicate a deficit in procedural learning, and previous fMRI studies have implicated the cortico-striatal system in language learning (Bohland and Guenther, 2006; Booth et al., 2007b; Kita et al., 2013; Pugh et al., 2013). Taken together, these findings suggest that subcortical regions play an important role in typical development of Chinese reading, and that disconnection of the PAL with cortical phonological processing regions may be associated with a phonological processing deficit in Chinese dyslexic children.

These findings are generally consistent with previous studies showing subcortical abnormality in dyslexia [see Krishnan et al. (2016), for a review]. For example, a previous study found that people with dyslexia exhibited hyperactivation in the left caudate and thalamus during a rhyme judgment task using visual words (Hoeft et al., 2007). Other work has found that the left medial geniculate body of the thalamus was hypoactivated in adults with dyslexia compared to controls while performing an auditory processing task (Díaz et al., 2012). Siok et al. (2008) found that Chinese children with dyslexia showed hypoactivation in the left putamen and thalamus during a rhyming judgment task. Another study of functional connectivity found that Chinese children with dyslexia had weaker connections between the left putamen and the right inferior occipital gyrus, a visual orthographic processing region, during an auditory rhyming task (Wang et al., 2019), suggesting a deficit in linking orthography and phonology. In contrast, here we found weaker connections between another subcortical region (left PAL) with a traditional phonological processing region (IFG) in the Chinese children with reading disability in the visual rhyming judgment task, which may indicate a deficit in motor-based phonological processing neural system. The inconsistent findings between the study of Wang et al. (2019) and our study might be due to the two reasons. First, Wang et al. (2019) used an auditory rhyming judgment task, while we used a visual rhyming judgment task. The cognitive process involved in the rhyming judgment in the two modalities might be different. Second, the two studies used different data analysis approaches. Wang et al. (2019) firstly found reduced gray matter volume in the left putamen of the dyslexic group compared to the controls, so they further examined the role of this region in phonological processing by calculating functional connectivity from this region to other brain regions. But we compared the inter-regional functional connectivity between multiple subcortical regions and reading-related cortical regions. Despite of the inconsistent findings, both studies indicated essential roles of the subcortical regions in reading and the disconnection between subcortical regions and cortical regions might possibly be the neural basis of deficit in phonological processing in Chinese children with reading disability.

RD Group Showed Larger Deficits in the Phonological Processing Than in the Semantic Processing

In terms of general hub distribution (as shown in Figure 1), the two groups shared more hubs in the meaning judgment task than in the rhyming judgment task. Specifically, the two groups shared only 33% of the hubs in the rhyming judgment task but 76% of the hubs in the meaning judgment task, suggesting that the two groups were more similar in the semantic processing than in the phonological processing to visual Chinese characters. In terms of subcortical hubs, significant group differences in inter-regional correlations were mainly found in the rhyming judgment task compared to the meaning judgment task (as shown in Figure 2; Table 3). These results suggest that phonological processing may be more deficient than semantic processing in the RD group from the perspective of connectivity. Further, although Chinese has a deep orthography, these results suggest that Chinese children with reading disability may have a deficit in accessing phonology from orthography. This finding is consistent with previous studies suggesting a phonological deficit in alphabetic dyslexia (e.g., Richlan et al., 2011) as well as in Chinese dyslexia (Siok et al., 2008; Cao et al., 2017). However, this finding is inconsistent with a previous study that reported brain activation abnormalities in Chinese children with dyslexia that were similar for both phonological and semantic processing (Liu et al., 2012). This inconsistency suggests that connectivity analysis may reveal subtler group differences than conventional brain activation analysis, leading to novel findings.

The finding of larger deficits in phonological vs. semantic processing in Chinese children with dyslexia might also be related to the properties of Chinese characters. Most Chinese characters are made of radicals, which can provide phonological or semantic clues. Semantic clues, which are often consistent with the meaning of the characters, can facilitate learning with respect to orthography to semantic mapping (Cao et al., 2010). However, phonological clues are often ambiguous, which makes learning the mapping between orthography and phonology much more difficult than learning the mapping between orthography and semantics (Shu, 2003). The accuracy data of the two tasks also showed that mapping from orthography to phonology was more difficult than mapping from orthography to semantic. That is, the nature of Chinese characters (i.e., consistent mapping between orthography to semantic vs. inconsistent mapping from orthography to phonology) may lead to a larger deficit in orthography to phonology mapping in Chinese children with dyslexia.

Limitations

There are several limitations needed to be noted about our study. First, due to lacking a reading-level matched group, it is hard to address whether the alterations in the connectivity of subcortical regions in the RD group observed in the current study reflect poor reading experience or dyslexia itself. Further, the TD and RD groups were not perfectly matched as there were marginal differences in age and PIQ between the two groups. Although we had involved these two variables as control variables when performing the group difference analysis to reduce their potential influence, it's impossible to eliminate all the interference to the results. Second, the sample size of the current study is relatively small, which might limit the generalization of our findings. Third, the signal extraction method used in this study might induce some kind of collinearity problem, although we used jitter design trying to reduce its influence. Taken together, future studies are needed with a larger sample size, particularly with a reading-level matched group. In addition, more sophisticated time-series extraction method shall be used to reduce collinearity problem in future large-scale brain network studies.

Conclusion

Subcortical, like cortical regions, can play a “hub” role in reading process of typical as well as atypical children readers. Reading impairment in Chinese is associated with less engagement of subcortical hubs and the disconnection between the subcortical hub (i.e., PAL) and the cortical region involved in speech articulation (i.e., IFG), suggesting a deficit in linking the subcortical sensory-motor system with cortical linguistic system. These alterations in hub distribution as well as in functional connectivity are more obvious in phonological than semantic processing to Chinese characters, suggesting the major deficit of Chinese children with reading disability lies in accessing and manipulating the phonology rather than meaning of the visual characters.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions generated for the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board at Beijing Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

LZ analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. JH collected and pre-processed the data. XL collected the data and helped analyse the data. EN and CL revised the manuscript. LL designed the experiment and wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31970977 and 31571155), the 973 Program (2014CB846103), the Interdisciplinary Research Funds of Beijing Normal University, and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2015KJJCB28).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2020.611008/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

ACG, anterior cingulate and paracingulate gyri; ORBinf, inferior frontal gyrus, orbital part; CAU, caudate nucleus; ORBsupmed, superior frontal gyrus, medial orbital; DCG, median cingulate and paracingulate gyri; PAL, pallidum; FFG, fusiform gyrus; PCG, posterior cingulate gyrus; IFGoperc, inferior frontal gyrus, opercular part; PUT, putamen; IFGtriang, inferior frontal gyrus, triangular part; REC, gyrus rectus; INS, insula; ROL, rolandic operculum; IOG, inferior occipital gyrus; SFGdor, superior frontal gyrus, dorsolateral; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; SFGmed, superior frontal gyrus, medial; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area; LING, lingual gyrus; SPG, superior parietal gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; THA, thalamus.

Footnotes

References

Achard, S., Salvador, R., Whitcher, B., Suckling, J., and Bullmore, E. (2006). A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J. Neurosci. 26, 63–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-05.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boets, B., Op De Beeck, H. P., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D., et al. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science 342, 1251–1254. doi: 10.1126/science.1244333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohland, J. W., and Guenther, F. H. (2006). An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. Neuroimage 32, 821–841. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A., and Schneider, W. (2005). Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 92–104. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Cho, S., Burman, D. D., and Bitan, T. (2007a). Neural correlates of mapping from phonology to orthography in children performing an auditory spelling task. Dev. Sci. 10, 441–451. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00598.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Wood, L., Lu, D., Houk, J. C., and Bitan, T. (2007b). The role of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in language processing. Brain Res. 1133, 136–144. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, M., Kronbichler, M., Richlan, F., Hawelka, S., Hutzler, F., and Jacobs, A. M. (2019). A model-guided dissociation between subcortical and cortical contributions to word recognition. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41011-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336–349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, F., Lee, R., Shu, H., Yang, Y., Xu, G., Li, K., et al. (2010). Cultural constraints on brain development: evidence from a developmental study of visual word processing in Mandarin Chinese. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1223–1233. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, F., Spray, G. J., Deng, Y., Liu, Y., and Yan, X. (2018). Brain mechanisms underlying visuo-orthographic deficits in children with developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:490. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, F., Yan, X., Wang, Z., Liu, Y., Wang, J., Spray, G. J., et al. (2017). Neural signatures of phonological deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Neuroimage 146, 301–311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cone, N. E., Burman, D. D., Bitan, T., Bolger, D. J., and Booth, J. R. (2008). Developmental changes in brain regions involved in phonological and orthographic processing during spoken language processing. Neuroimage 41, 623–635. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz, B., Hintz, F., Kiebel, S. J., and Kriegstein, K. V. (2012). Dysfunction of the auditory thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 13841–13846. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119828109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, E. S., Burke, K., Booth, J. R., and McNorgan, C. (2018). Dyslexia on a continuum: a complex network approach. PLoS ONE 13:e0208923. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, M., Derrfuss, J., Tittgemeyer, M., and Fiebach, C. J. (2012). Predicting errors from reconfiguration patterns in human brain networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 16714–16719. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207523109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finn, E. S., Shen, X., Holahan, J. M., Scheinost, D., Lacadie, C., Papademetris, X., et al. (2014). Disruption of functional networks in dyslexia: a whole-brain, data-driven analysis of connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 397–404. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., and Temple, E. (2007). Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: an fMRI study. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 295–310.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hebb, A. O., and Ojemann, G. A. (2013). The thalamus and language revisited. Brain Lang. 126, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeft, F., Meyler, A., Hernandez, A., Juel, C., Taylor-Hill, H., Martindale, J. L., et al. (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 4234–4239. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609399104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., and Donohue, B. C. (1998). Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 8939–8944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.15.8939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosseini, S. M. H., Black, J. M., Soriano, T., Bugescu, N., Martinez, R., Raman, M. M., et al. (2013). Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks in children with familial risk for reading difficulties. Neuroimage 71, 260–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ketteler, D., Kastrau, F., Vohn, R., and Huber, W. (2008). The subcortical role of language processing. High level linguistic features such as ambiguity-resolution and the human brain; an fMRI study. Neuroimage 39, 2002–2009. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kita, Y., Yamamoto, H., Oba, K., Terasawa, Y., Moriguchi, Y., Uchiyama, H., et al. (2013). Altered brain activity for phonological manipulation in dyslexic Japanese children. Brain 136, 3696–3708. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotz, S. A., Cappa, S. F., Von Cramon, D. Y., and Friederici, A. D. (2002). Modulation of the lexical-semantic network by auditory semantic priming: an event-related functional MRI study. Neuroimage 17, 1761–1772. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koyama, M. S., Di Martino, A., Kelly, C., Jutagir, D. R., Sunshine, J., Schwartz, S. J., et al. (2013). Cortical signatures of dyslexia and remediation: an intrinsic functional connectivity approach. PLoS ONE 8:e0055454. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, S., Watkins, K. E., and Bishop, D. V. M. (2016). Neurobiological basis of language learning difficulties. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 701–714. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, X., Zou, Q., He, Y., and Yang, Y. (2016). Topologically reorganized connectivity architecture of default-mode, executive-control, and salience networks across working memory task loads. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1501–1511. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. D., and Zhang, H. C. (1986). The Chinese Version of WSIC-R. Beijing: Beijing Teachers College Press.

Google Scholar

Liu, K., Shi, L., Chen, F., Waye, M. M. Y., Lim, C. K. P., Cheng, P., et al. (2015). Altered topological organization of brain structural network in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Neurosci. Lett. 589, 169–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Tao, R., Wang, W., You, W., Peng, D., and Booth, J. R. (2013). Chinese dyslexics show neural differences in morphological processing. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Wang, W., You, W., Li, Y., Awati, N., Zhao, X., et al. (2012). Similar alterations in brain function for phonological and semantic processing to visual characters in Chinese dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 50, 2224–2232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Gao, Y., Di, Q., Hu, J., Lu, C., Nan, Y., et al. (2018). Differences between child and adult large-scale functional brain networks for reading tasks. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 662–679. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyon, R. G., Shaywitz, S. E., and Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 53, 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maisog, J. M., Einbinder, E. R., Flowers, D. L., Turkeltaub, P. E., and Eden, G. F. (2008). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1145, 237–259. doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manes, J. L., Parkinson, A. L., Larson, C. R., Greenlee, J. D., Eickhoff, S. B., Corcos, D. M., et al. (2014). Connectivity of the subthalamic nucleus and globus pallidus pars interna to regions within the speech network: a meta-analytic connectivity study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3499–3516. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manes, J. L., Tjaden, K., Parrish, T., Simuni, T., Roberts, A., Greenlee, J. D., et al. (2018). Altered resting-state functional connectivity of the putamen and internal globus pallidus is related to speech impairment in Parkinson's disease. Brain Behav. 8, 1–20. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., Kronbichler, M., and Richlan, F. (2016). Dyslexic brain activation abnormalities in deep and shallow orthographies: a meta-analysis of 28 functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2676–2699. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morken, F., Helland, T., Hugdahl, K., and Specht, K. (2017). Reading in dyslexia across literacy development: a longitudinal study of effective connectivity. Neuroimage 144, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulesu, E., Danelli, L., and Berlingeri, M. (2014). Reading the dyslexic brain: multiple dysfunctional routes revealed by a new meta-analysis of PET and fMRI activation studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:830. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage 62, 816–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., Preston, J. L., Mencl, W. E., Austin, A. C., Sibley, D., et al. (2013). The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain Lang. 125, 173–183. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qi, T., Gu, B., Ding, G., Gong, G., Lu, C., Peng, D., et al. (2016). More bilateral, more anterior: alterations of brain organization in the large-scale structural network in Chinese dyslexia. Neuroimage 124, 63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., and Wimmer, H. (2009). Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3299–3308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., and Wimmer, H. (2011). Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage 56, 1735–1742. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueckl, J. G., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Molfese, P. J., Kuo, W. J., Bick, A., Frost, S. J., et al. (2015). Universal brain signature of proficient reading: evidence from four contrasting languages. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 15510–15515. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509321112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreiweis, C., Bornschein, U., Burguière, E., Kerimoglu, C., Schreiter, S., Dannemann, M., et al. (2014). Humanized Foxp2 accelerates learning by enhancing transitions from declarative to procedural performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 14253–14258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414542111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W., Dayan, P., and Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Wimmer, H., Richlan, F., Ludersdorfer, P., Klackl, J., and Kronbichler, M. (2015). Resting-state and task-based functional brain connectivity in developmental dyslexia. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3502–3514. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaywitz, S. E., and Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (Specific Reading Disability). Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1301–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Kalanithi, J., and Gluck, M. A. (2008). Basal ganglia and dopamine contributions to probabilistic category learning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 219–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shu, H. (2003). Chinese writing system and learning to read. Int. J. Psychol. 38, 274–285. doi: 10.1080/00207590344000060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siok, W. T., Niu, Z., Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A., and Tan, L. H. (2008). A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 5561–5566. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801750105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., and Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature 431, 71–76. doi: 10.1038/nature02865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanberry, L. I., Richards, T. L., Berninger, V. W., Nandy, R. R., Aylward, E. H., Maravilla, K. R., et al. (2006). Low-frequency signal changes reflect differences in functional connectivity between good readers and dyslexics during continuous phoneme mapping. Magn. Reson. Imaging 24, 217–229. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2005.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., Lucker, G. W., Lee, S., Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., et al. (1982). Society for research in child development reading disabilities: the case of Chinese, Japanese, and English. Child Dev. 53, 1164–1181. doi: 10.2307/1129005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. H., Laird, A. R., Li, K., and Fox, P. T. (2005). Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 83–91. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, X., Chen, N., Zhang, S., Jones, J. A., Zhang, B., Li, J., et al. (2018). Predicting auditory feedback control of speech production from subregional shape of subcortical structures. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 459–471. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasi, D., and Volkow, N. D. (2012). Resting functional connectivity of language networks: characterization and reproducibility. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 841–854. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition 92, 231–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Mark, S., Klaver, P., Bucher, K., Maurer, U., Schulz, E., Brem, S., et al. (2011). The left occipitotemporal system in reading: disruption of focal fMRI connectivity to left inferior frontal and inferior parietal language areas in children with dyslexia. Neuroimage 54, 2426–2436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, A. C., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., and Schlaggar, B. L. (2012). The putative visual word form area is functionally connected to the dorsal attention network. Cereb. Cortex 22, 537–549. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. L., and Tao, B. P. (1993). Chinese Character Recognition Test Battery and Assessment Scale for Primary School Children. Shanghai: Shanghai Education Press.

Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Yan, X., Liu, Y., Spray, G. J., Deng, Y., and Cao, F. (2019). Structural and functional abnormality of the putamen in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130, 26–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wise, R. J. S., Greene, J., Büchel, C., and Scott, S. K. (1999). Brain regions involved in articulation. Lancet 353, 1057–1061. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)07491-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C. Y., Ho, M. H. R., and Chen, S. H. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. Neuroimage 63, 381–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., and Tan, L. H. (2020). Whole-brain functional networks for phonological and orthographic processing in Chinese good and poor readers. Front. Psychol. 10:2945. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, C., Zhang, J., Yin, R., Zhou, J., and Chang, S. (1996). Experimental research on the reading disability of Chinese students. Psychol. Sci. 19, 222–256.

Google Scholar

Zhou, W., Xia, Z., Bi, Y., and Shu, H. (2015). Altered connectivity of the dorsal and ventral visual regions in dyslexic children: a resting-state fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:495. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Chinese reading disability, subcortical regions, fMRI, functional connectivity, graph theory

Citation: Zhang L, Hu J, Liu X, Nichols ES, Lu C and Liu L (2021) Disrupted Subcortical-Cortical Connections in a Phonological but Not Semantic Task in Chinese Children With Dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:611008. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.611008

Received: 28 September 2020; Accepted: 21 December 2020;
Published: 18 January 2021.

Edited by:

Xiaolin Zhou, Peking University, China

Reviewed by:

Leilei Mei, South China Normal University, China
Jie Zhuang, Shanghai University of Sport, China

Copyright © 2021 Zhang, Hu, Liu, Nichols, Lu and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Li Liu, lilyliu@bnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.