Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 16 November 2020
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroscience of Eating: From Physiology to Pathology View all 16 articles

Caudate Functional Connectivity Associated With Weight Change in Adolescents

\r\nYuko Nakamura*Yuko Nakamura1*Sachiyo OzawaSachiyo Ozawa1Shinsuke Koike,,,Shinsuke Koike1,2,3,4
  • 1UTokyo Center for Integrative Science of Human Behavior, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2International Research Center for Neurointelligence, Tokyo, Japan
  • 3University of Tokyo Institute for Diversity and Adaptation of Human Mind, Tokyo, Japan
  • 4Center for Evolutionary Cognitive Sciences, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan

Background: Childhood obesity has become a global epidemic and the etiology of maladaptive ingestive behavior in children warrants further research. Mounting evidence suggests that the caudate is associated with body weight gain and obesity in adults. In adolescents, however, how caudate-related neural networks are associated with body weight gain is unclear because their central nervous systems are still developing.

Objectives: The current longitudinal resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) study was conducted to investigate the hypothesis that caudate-related neural networks have a role in weight gain in adolescents.

Methods: The study included 20 healthy adolescents with a mean age of 17.5 ± 2.0 years and a mean body mass index of 20.6 ± 2.1 who underwent baseline rs-fMRI then follow-up rs-fMRI approximately 1 year later. Body mass index (BMI) was measured at both timepoints. Seed-based functional connectivity analysis was utilized to analyze caudate-related functional connectivity (FC) using the caudate as a seed. Associations between caudate-related FC and BMI at baseline were assessed, as were associations between change in BMI and caudate-related FC between baseline and follow-up.

Results: At baseline, greater caudate-lateral prefrontal cortex FC was correlated with lower BMI (family wise error-corrected p < 0.05). Compared to the baseline, increased FC in the caudate-lateral prefrontal cortex at follow up were negatively associated with increased BMI (p < 0.05).

Conclusion: Given that the lateral prefrontal cortex and caudate are associated with inhibitory control, the caudate-lateral prefrontal cortex FC may have a preventive effect on weight gain in adolescents. The results of the current study suggest that developing inhibitory control would lead to the prevention of childhood obesity.

Introduction

Childhood obesity has become a global epidemic in the last three decades (Abarca-Gómez et al., 2017), and it may cause deficits in structural and functional brain development (Laurent et al., 2019; Ronan et al., 2020) as well as various non-communicable diseases such as diabetes and cardiovascular conditions (Sahoo et al., 2015). A better understanding of the neuropathology of childhood obesity is thus needed to improve strategies for its prevention and treatment.

Weight change is reportedly associated with structural and functional alterations in reward regions of the brain such as the midbrain, pallidum, orbitofrontal cortex (Stice and Yokum, 2016), and especially the caudate. In one study, reduced caudate responses to palatable liquid consumption were correlated with increased body mass index (BMI), and this caudate response was associated with weight gain at a 1-year follow-up timepoint (Stice et al., 2008a). In another study, reduced caudate responses to sugary liquid consumption were correlated with increased BMI at a 6-month follow-up timepoint in women (Stice et al., 2010). In adolescents, altered caudate responses to food cues were reportedly associated with body fat gain at a 3-year follow-up timepoint (Stice et al., 2015) and BMI gain at an 1-year follow-up (Yokum et al., 2014). In a weight loss trial, greater reduction in brain responses to high-calorie food cues at a 1-month follow-up timepoint predicted greater weight loss at a 6-month follow-up timepoint (Hermann et al., 2019). In addition, altered caudate-related functional connectivity is reportedly predictive of increased BMI 6-months later (Gao et al., 2018) and 1 year later (Contreras-Rodríguez et al., 2017). Collectively, these observations suggest that aberrant functions or connectivity in the caudate are likely to be linked to disrupted weight maintenance.

The caudate is deep inside the brain near the thalamus, and is connected to parts of the brain associated with nearly all of its facets, including cognition, emotion, perception, and action-related structures and networks (Grahn et al., 2008; Robinson et al., 2012). The caudate responses to various food stimuli (van der Laan et al., 2011; Huerta et al., 2014; Arsalidou et al., 2020; Chen and Zeffiro, 2020) and caudate-related networks are changed by internal states (e.g., hunger or fullness) or food consumption in adults (Siep et al., 2009; Contreras-Rodriguez et al., 2019; Chen and Zeffiro, 2020) and adolescents (Page et al., 2013; Jastreboff et al., 2016). The caudate is connected to basal ganglia (e.g., substantia nigra and globus pallidus) as well as the prefrontal cortex, and is involved in reward processing, learning, goal-directed behavior, and decision-making (Balleine et al., 2007). In concert with the prefrontal cortex, it is also involved in inhibitory control (Guo et al., 2018; Cai et al., 2019) and cognitive appetite control (Tuulari et al., 2015). Given that the caudate and related neural networks are associated with perception of food stimuli, reward processing, and cognitive appetite control, it is likely to have an important role in ingestive behavior and weight change.

It has been repeatedly reported that impulsivity is associated with binge eating that could be a cause of excessive weight gain (Meule, 2013) in young adults (Claes et al., 2005; Oliva et al., 2019, 2020) and adolescents (Stice et al., 2013). In addition, increased impulsivity is associated with altered caudate function or connectivity (Babbs et al., 2013; Davis et al., 2013; Oliva et al., 2019) and obstruct function or structure in the prefrontal cortex, which is a key region for inhibitory or self-control behavior (Lowe et al., 2019). Given that adolescents exhibit greater impulsivity than adults because their inhibitory control mechanisms are still developing (Romer, 2010), and they are evidently more prone to over consumption of rewarding high-calorie food (van Meer et al., 2016). It is thus important to elucidate the associations between caudate-related neural networks and body weight change in adolescents, but these associations have not been well characterized. In the current longitudinal study resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) and seed-based functional connectivity (FC) analysis were used to examine associations between caudate-related FC and BMI change in adolescents. We hypothesized that increased BMI or BMI change would be associated with reduced caudate-related FC in the inhibitory region and increased caudate-related FC in reward processing or motivation regions, which were associated with food reward processing and weight gain (e.g., striatum, midbrain, amygdala, insula, pre-/postcentral gyrus, and orbitofrontal cortex) (García-García et al., 2014; Stice and Yokum, 2016).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study included 20 adolescents (seven male, 13 female) with a mean age of 17.5 years [standard deviation (SD) 2.0, range 14–19 years] and a mean BMI of 20.6 (SD 2.1, range 17.2–25.7) at baseline who were recruited from the metropolitan area of Tokyo. All participants and parents/guardians of the high school or middle school participants provided written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences at the University of Tokyo (approval number 513-2). All participants were healthy and had no history of psychological or neurological disorders or head injury. Detailed demographic data are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics.

Experimental Timepoints

All participants underwent rs-fMRI at baseline and approximately 1 year or more thereafter (mean 12.95 ± 3.02 months, range 11–22 months). Within 1 month before or after each rs-fMRI session, each participant visited the laboratory and their weight and height were measured by trained experimenters (YN or SO) using a multi-frequency body composition meter (MC-780A, TANITA CORPORATION, Tokyo, Japan) to calculate BMI (kg/m2). We also collected self-reported weight and height on the scanning day. We calculate BMI using self-reported weight and height. There was no significant difference between self-reported BMI and measured BMI (p = 0.86, t = −0.18 at baseline; p = 0.79, t = −0.27 at follow-up). Therefore, we assumed that the participants’ BMI did not significantly change within a month from the rs-fMRI scanning day.

At this visit, participants also completed the Japanese version Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (BIS-11) (Patton et al., 1995; Someya et al., 2001; Kobashi and Ida, 2013), which was a questionnaire designed to assess the personality/behavioral construct of impulsiveness (Someya et al., 2001). This self-reported questionnaire has been adapted to assess impulsivity in adolescents (Pechorro et al., 2016; Huang et al., 2017). The BIS-11 contains a total of 30 items scored on a Likert scale (ranging from never = 1 point to very frequently = 6 points) yielding impulsivity measures on three scales: attentional (inability to focus or concentrate), motor (tendency to act without thinking), and non-planning impulsivity (lack of future planning and forethought). The total score ranges from 30 to 180, with a higher score indicating greater impulsivity.

Rs-fMRI Procedure

Approximately three to 4 h after a meal, the level of appetite generally returns to almost the same level as that of the pre-meal appetite (Leidy et al., 2010; Gibbons et al., 2013; DeBenedictis et al., 2020). Since caudate-related functional connectivity could be changed by internal states (e.g., hunger or fullness) (Siep et al., 2009; Page et al., 2013; Jastreboff et al., 2016; Contreras-Rodriguez et al., 2019; Chen and Zeffiro, 2020), the participants were instructed to abstain from food or drink except for water for at least 3 h before their visit to the laboratory to avoid the effect of the meal on caudate FC. In an effort to standardize participants’ internal states, they were instructed to consume pre-fixed snacks (280 kcal) approximately 30 min before rs-fMRI scanning (Figure 1). The participant was then escorted to the fMRI room to undergo rs-fMRI scanning. Approximately 10 min before rs-fMRI scanning, each participant rated their internal states (e.g., hunger and fullness) using an 8-point Likert scale, where 1 equated to “not at all” and 8 equated to “more than ever.” Before rs-fMRI scanning, each participant underwent an fMRI experiment to measure brain responses to food cues. After the food cue fMRI experiment, we asked all participants if they felt any fatigue or anxiety. All participants responded that they were comfortable to continue scanning. The participant underwent rs-fMRI scanning (10 min 10 s, 244 volumes).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging session. All participants were instructed to abstain from any food or drinks apart from water for at least 3 h before their visit to the laboratory. Approximately 30 min before the resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) scanning, each participant was instructed to consume pre-prepared snacks (280 kcal) to standardize their internal states of hunger and fullness. Approximately 10 min before the rs-fMRI scanning, participants rated their levels of hunger and fullness.

The participant was instructed to relax and lie still in the scanner while remaining calm and awake. During rs-fMRI scanning, the participant was instructed to fix their gaze on a white cross on the black screen, which was projected on a mirror attached to the head-coil from the monitor that was set behind the bore of the MRI machine.

Image Acquisition

Images were acquired on a 3 Tesla MRI scanner (Magnetom Prisma Fit, Siemens Medical Systems, Munich, Germany) using a 64-channel head/neck coil. T2-weighted images reflecting blood oxygen level-dependent signals were acquired using gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (repetition time 2500 ms, echo time 30 ms, 38 slices, flip angle 80°, field of view 212 mm × 212 mm, and resolution 3.3 mm × 3.3 mm × 4.0 mm) in ascending order (10 min and 10 s). Anatomical images were acquired using a T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE protocol (repetition time 1900 ms, echo time 2.53 ms, flip angle 9°, field of view 256 mm × 256 mm, resolution 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm).

Rs-fMRI Data Preprocessing

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data were preprocessed using FSL (version 6.0) (Jenkinson et al., 2012) and AFNI (version 20.0.05 “Galba”) (Cox, 1996) software. The first four volumes of each functional time series were excluded from the analysis to allow for magnetization equilibrium. Conventional preprocessing was conducted as follows: (1) Head-motion correction via realignment of the time-series to the middle volume; (2) fieldmap-based distortion-correction; (3) slice-timing correction; (4) non-brain tissue removal using the brain extraction tool; (5) spatial smoothing with a 5-mm full-width at half maximum Gaussian kernel; (6) high-pass temporal filtering (1/150 Hz cutoff); and (7) linear detrending. The exclusion criterion for head-motion was greater than one voxel size (3.3 mm × 3.3 mm × 4.0 mm). No participant met this exclusion criterion, thus data from all participants were included in further rs-fMRI analyses in which head-motion in rs-fMRI scanning was calculated. In baseline rs-fMRI, the mean head-motion values (x, y, z) were 0.16 ± 0.09, 0.38 ± 0.27, and 0.88 ± 0.63, and at follow-up, they were 0.17 ± 0.11, 0.34 ± 0.15, and 0.72 ± 0.48. Derivative or root mean square variance over voxels (DVARS) were also calculated, quantifying the mean change in image intensity between timepoints (Power et al., 2012). Time-series were then extracted from white matter and cerebrospinal fluid from preprocessed time-series data. Motion-regressors, DVARS, and white matter and cerebrospinal fluid time-series were included in a confounder matrix.

Caudate-Related Connectivity Maps

Based on a previous report (Contreras-Rodríguez et al., 2017), 3-mm spheres were placed centered on (x, y, z) = (13, 15, 9) and (x, y, z) = (−13, 15, 9) as right and left caudate seeds. Right and left seeds were then merged and regions outside the anatomical caudate area were cut out and binarized to create the caudate seed (Figure 2). A time-series was extracted from unsmoothed preprocessed rs-fMRI data using the caudate seed. To create individual caudate connectivity maps, the extracted time-series from the seed was included in a general linear model using FSL’s fMRI Expert Analysis Tool. This model also included a confounder matrix as a nuisance covariate.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Binarized caudate seed. Three-millimeter spheres were placed centered on (x, y, z) = (13, 15, 9) and (x, y, z) = (–13, 15, 9). Right and left seeds were merged and regions outside of the anatomical caudate area were cut out.

Statistical Analysis

The Effect of Gender on BMI

We tested for gender differences in BMI at baseline and follow-up, and in the change in BMI, using a two-sample t-test.

Association Between Caudate-Related Connectivity and BMI

First, paired t-tests were conducted on individual connectivity maps derived from baseline and follow-up data to compare caudate-related FC.

Then, to assess the correlation between BMI and individual connectivity maps at baseline and follow-up, a group-level voxel-wise linear regression analysis with BMI as a covariate of interest was performed on individual caudate-related connectivity maps. To control for variance in age, age was included as a covariate of no-interest in the regression analysis. The cluster-forming threshold was set at z > 3.1. Clusters were then formed, associated p values were calculated, and p values that were > 0.05 (family wise error (FWE) corrected) were disregarded.

From brain regions in which there was a significant correlation between caudate-related FC and BMI at baseline, FC values (z-values) were extracted from connectivity maps at baseline and follow-up and compared. Further, correlation coefficients between extracted FC values and BMI were compared at baseline and follow-up using the “cocor” package version 1.1-3 in R (Diedenhofen and Musch, 2015).

Association Between Change in BMI and Change in Caudate-Related Connectivity

Changes in extracted FC values between baseline and follow-up (follow-up > baseline) were calculated, and the correlations between changes in extracted FC values and change in BMI (follow-up > baseline) were calculated using the Robust Correlation Toolbox in MATLAB R2020a (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) (Pernet et al., 2012). Robust correlation methods could provide better estimates of true associations, with accurate false-positive control and without loss of power by removing or down-weighting outliers and accounting for them in significance testing. To calculate skipped Pearson’s correlational coefficient, first the robust center of the data cloud was estimated using the minimum covariance determinant. Outliers were then identified using a projection technique. Lastly, Pearson’s correlation and associated t values were calculated using the remaining data. For correlational coefficients (r), 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated based on 1000 samples with the percentile bootstrap method implemented in the toolbox.

Results

Change in BMI and the Effect of Gender on BMI

The mean change in BMI was 0.25 ± 2.53 (range −4.33 to 6.53), and there was no significant difference in the change in BMI between male and female participants (p = 0.74, t = −0.34). The change in BMI was not significantly associated with the change in age (p = 0.09, r = 0.39).

There was no significant gender difference in BMI at baseline (p = 0.81, t = −0.24) or at follow-up (p = 0.36, t = −0.95).

Internal State at the Rs-fMRI Sessions

The mean hunger rating at baseline was 4.6 ± 1.2, and at the follow-up timepoint, it was 4.1 ± 1.3. The mean fullness rating at baseline was 4.2 ± 1.2, and at the follow-up timepoint, it was 4.8 ± 1.2. Ratings for hunger and fullness at baseline did not differ significantly from the corresponding ratings at follow-up (p = 0.25, t = 1.18 for hunger and p = 0.17, t = −1.43 for fullness). There were no significant differences between ratings for hunger and fullness at baseline (p = 0.38, t = 0.89) or follow-up (p = 0.15, t = −1.51), suggesting that participants were neither hungry nor full at either of their rs-fMRI sessions.

Association Between Caudate-Related Connectivity and BMI

There was no significant difference in caudate-related connectivity maps between baseline and follow-up.

Body mass index was inversely correlated with caudate-related FC in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), the opercular part [(x, y, z) = (−44, 10, 14), z = 4.07, PFWE–corrected = 0.0014, cluster size = 187 voxels], and the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) [(x, y, z) = (−22, 56, 16), z = 4.09, PFWE–corrected = 0.0048, cluster size = 154 voxels] (Figure 3). After controlling for age, the correlation between BMI and caudate-related FC in the IFG and SFG still remained significant [IFG: (x, y, z) = (−44, 10, 14), z = 3.94, PFWE–corrected = 0.0089, cluster size = 137 voxels, SFG: (x, y, z) = (−22, 56, 16), z = 4.09, PFWE–corrected = 0.0030, cluster size = 165 voxels].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Associations between caudate-related functional connectivity and body mass index at baseline. Scatter plots depict associations between body mass index and caudate-related functional connectivity in the inferior frontal gyrus (A) and superior frontal gyrus (B). The color bar depicts the z-value. IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus.

There was no significant positive nor negative correlation between caudate-related FC and BMI at follow-up.

The linear relationship between caudate-related connectivity maps and BMI did not differ between baseline and follow-up.

Extracted caudate-related FCs in the IFG or SFG at baseline did not differ from those at follow-up (p = 0.61, t = −0.94 for FC in the IFG and p = 0.34, t = −0.97 for FC in the SFG).

Correlations between extracted FCs in the IFG or SFG and BMI at baseline were greater than those at follow-up (p = 0.030, z = −2.17 for the IFG and p = 0.039, z = −2.07 for the SFG).

Time of intervals between baseline and follow-up rs-fMRI sessions were not significantly correlated with caudate-related FC in IFG (p = 0.18, r = 0.31) or SFG (p = 0.41, r = 0.20), but change in BMI (p = 0.047, r = 0.45).

Association Between Change in BMI and Change in Caudate-Related Connectivity

There were significant negative correlations between the change in BMI and change in FC values (z values) from the IFG (skipped Pearson’s r [15] = −0.65, p = 0.002, 95% CI = [−0.90 to −0.19], number of outliers (NO) = 3) and from the SFG (skipped Pearson’s r [16] = −0.54, p = 0.015, 95% CI = [−0.80 to −0.10], NO = 2) (Figure 4). These results indicated that increased connectivity was correlated with reduced BMI gain.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Associations between change in body mass index and change in caudate-related connectivity. Scatter plots depict associations between change in body mass index and caudate-related functional connectivity in the inferior frontal gyrus (A) and superior frontal gyrus (B). r values are skipped Pearson’s correlational coefficients. Red data-points indicate outliers. IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus.

Associations Between Impulsivity and Age, Caudate-Related Connectivity, and BMI

Associations between each subscale of impulsivity and age at baseline and follow-up were tested. Increased attentional and motor subscales were positively correlated with age at baseline (p = 0.003, r = 0.63 for attentional subscale; p = 0.033, r = 0.48 for motor subscale) and increased attentional subscale with age at follow-up (p = 0.018, r = 0.52).

There was no significant difference in the correlation between age and attentional subscale between baseline and follow-up (p = 0.42, z = 0.81).

Associations between each subscale of impulsivity and caudate-prefrontal cortex connectivity in the IFG and SFG at baseline and follow-up were tested. Motor subscale was negatively correlated with FC in the IFG (p = 0.047, r = −0.45) at baseline and in the SFG at follow-up (p = 0.032, r = −0.48). Change in motor subscale was negatively associated with FC in the SFG (p = 0.030, r = −0.49).

Associations between each subscale of impulsivity and BMI at baseline and follow-up were tested. A significant association was not observed (p > 0.07 for all).

After the Bonferroni adjustment was performed for a multiple-comparison correction, the positive correlation between attentional impulsivity and age at baseline remained significant (significant level: p < 0.0125).

There was no significant gender difference in any subscales (p > 0.12 for all).

Discussion

In the current study, greater caudate-IFG and caudate-SFG FCs were associated with lower BMI at baseline, and increased FC was negatively correlated with BMI gain at follow-up. Further, increased caudate-prefrontal cortex FC was associated with decreased impulsivity. To the best of our knowledge, the current longitudinal study is the first to show the association among the change in caudate-prefrontal cortex FC, BMI, and impulsivity in adolescents. The IFG, SFG, and caudate are involved in inhibitory control in adults (Guo et al., 2018; Cai et al., 2019) and children (Cai et al., 2019). Alterations in the IFG and SFG are involved in obesity, and gray matter reduction in the IFG has been reported in people with obesity (Herrmann et al., 2019). In adolescents, reduced cortical thickness in the IFG and SFG is reportedly associated with increased BMI and reduced cognitive function (Ronan et al., 2020). The IFG and SFG are involved in cognitive appetite control in adults (Tuulari et al., 2015) and attenuated caudate activation was observed during appetite control in middle aged people with obesity (Tuulari et al., 2015). Moreover, lower caudate responses to palatable liquid consumption in people with increased BMI are associated with elevated impulsivity (Babbs et al., 2013). Both caudate-IFG and caudate-SFG FC may be involved in cognitive appetite control and increases in these FCs may have preventive effects on excessive weight gain in adolescents.

In the present study, caudate-related FC at follow-up was not significantly associated with BMI, and the correlations between BMI and caudate-related FC in the prefrontal cortex were attenuated at follow-up. In young adolescents with healthy weight, increased impulsivity is associated with increased local FC in the caudate and decreased FC between the prefrontal cortex and caudate (Davis et al., 2013). Further, decreased caudate response to the inhibitory task was associate with increased impulsivity (Oliva et al., 2019). Given that age was positively associated with impulsivity at baseline in the current study, and the immature or obstruct PFC is involved with weight gain as well as elevated impulsivity (Lowe et al., 2019), attenuated FC between caudate-prefrontal cortex could be linked to increased impulsivity and lead to gained BMI at baseline. However, as the results showing that the associations between age and subscales of impulsivity at baseline differed from those at follow-up, the balance between sensation-seeking activation in the striatum and inhibitory control in the prefrontal cortex changes during adolescence (Romer, 2010), and this biological change may alter associations between caudate-related FC and BMI at follow-up compared to that at baseline. To examine this hypothesis, it is necessary to perform further follow-up observations.

Contrary to the study hypothesis, BMI was not significantly associated with caudate-related FC in reward regions, such as the striatum, midbrain, amygdala, insula, and orbitofrontal cortex (García-García et al., 2014; Stice and Yokum, 2016). Reward regions are related to weight gain and obesity (Stice and Yokum, 2016) and people with obesity reportedly have different caudate-related FC in reward-related regions (Lips et al., 2014). Therefore, associations between caudate-related FC in reward regions and BMI or BMI change may have been non-significant in the present study because almost all of the participants were of healthy weight.

The current study had several limitations. One is that it did not include obese adolescents. Compared with healthy weight adolescents, obese adolescents showed a greater caudate response to high calorie food images (Jastreboff et al., 2014), and a decreased caudate response following consumption of a milkshake (Stice et al., 2008b); therefore, caudate functional connectivity in adolescents with obesity is predicted to be different from that in healthy weight adolescents. While further investigation is needed to test this hypothesis, the current study provides insights into neural involvement in weight gain in adolescents. Second, not all participants’ BMI were measured on the day of the rs-fMRI session; however, all participants’ BMI were measured within 1 month before or after each rs-fMRI session. We assumed that their BMI did not change significantly within 1 month from the day of the rs-fMRI session because their self-reported BMI on the day of the rs-fMRI session were not significantly different from the measured BMI. However, since self-reported BMI is not reliable, small differences between the actual BMI on the day of the rs-fMRI session and that measured within a month from the day of the rs-fMRI session could potentially affect the association between BMI and caudate FC. Third, there was variation in the intervals between baseline and follow-up rs-fMRI sessions (mean 12.95 ± 3.02 months, range 11–22 months). Since there was no significant association between time of intervals and change in caudate FCs, this variation in the interval time appeared to have little effect on our analysis. However, as variance in the interval time could affect the correlation between the change in BMI and caudate FC, further studies should be conducted with fixed follow-up intervals. The fourth is that our sample size is not large. Although a preferable sample size for rs-fMRI studies has not been addressed in the literature to date, for a task-based fMRI experiment, Thirion et al. (2007) showed that a minimum sample size of 20 was necessary to ensure acceptable reliability. In addition, our sample is relatively homogeneous because all participants are Asian adolescents from similar residential areas, and we controlled the internal state for each participant. In addition, the effect size of correlation analysis between the connectivity values and BMI at baseline was calculated using GPower (Faul et al., 2007). All effect sizes were greater than 0.8. Therefore, the current results would be acceptable. However, it is preferable to perform further studies with greater sample size. Fifth, the results of the study do not demonstrate that there is a causal relationship between change in BMI and change in FC. Given that BMI gain is predicted by altered caudate function in adolescents, however (Stice et al., 2015), disrupted caudate-related FC may cause excessively increased BMI. Additional longitudinal studies are needed to investigate associations between development and the neuropathology of aberrant ingestive behavior in adolescents.

In conclusion, caudate-related FC in the inhibitory control regions has a preventive effect on excessive weight gain in adolescents. In the present longitudinal study, increased caudate-related FC in the prefrontal cortex – the inhibitory control region – was associated with lower BMI and impulsivity at baseline, and strengthened FC was inversely correlated with BMI gain and increment of impulsivity at follow-up in adolescents. These results suggest that developing inhibitory control may lead to successful prevention of childhood obesity.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences at the University of Tokyo (approval number 513-2). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

YN and SK: conceptualization and design, reviewing and editing the manuscript. YN and SO: data acquisition. YN: data analysis and writing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the 12th Hakuho Research Grant for Child Education from the Hakuho Foundation, Grants-in-Aid for Early-Career Scientists from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (grant ID 17K13931), and UTokyo Center for Integrative Science of Human Behavior (CiSHuB).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Naoko Aoki and Tomoko Kawashima for their help with conducting the study.

References

Abarca-Gómez, L., Abdeen, Z. A., Hamid, Z. A., Abu-Rmeileh, N. M., Acosta-Cazares, B., Acuin, C., et al. (2017). Worldwide trends in body-mass index, underweight, overweight, and obesity from 1975 to 2016: a pooled analysis of 2416 population-based measurement studies in 128.9 million children, adolescents, and adults. Lancet 390, 2627–2642.

Google Scholar

Arsalidou, M., Vijayarajah, S., and Sharaev, M. (2020). Basal ganglia lateralization in different types of reward. Brain Imaging Behav. [Internet]. doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00215-3 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Babbs, R. K., Sun, X., Felsted, J., Chouinard-Decorte, F., Veldhuizen, M. G., and Small, D. M. (2013). Decreased caudate response to milkshake is associated with higher body mass index and greater impulsivity. Physiol. Behav. 121, 103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., Delgado, M. R., and Hikosaka, O. (2007). The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 27, 8161–8165. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1554-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, W., Duberg, K., Padmanabhan, A., Rehert, R., Bradley, T., Carrion, V., et al. (2019). Hyperdirect insula-basal-ganglia pathway and adult-like maturity of global brain responses predict inhibitory control in children. Nat. Commun. 10:4798.

Google Scholar

Chen, E. Y., and Zeffiro, T. A. (2020). Hunger and BMI modulate neural responses to sweet stimuli: fMRI meta-analysis. Int. J. Obes. 44, 1636–1652. doi: 10.1038/s41366-020-0608-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., and Vertommen, H. (2005). Impulsivity-related traits in eating disorder patients. Personal Individ. Differ. 39, 739–749. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Rodriguez, O., Burrows, T., Pursey, K. M., Stanwell, P., Parkes, L., Soriano-Mas, C., et al. (2019). Food addiction linked to changes in ventral striatum functional connectivity between fasting and satiety. Appetite 133, 18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras-Rodríguez, O., Martín-Pérez, C., Vilar-López, R., and Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2017). Ventral and dorsal striatum networks in obesity: link to food craving and weight gain. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 789–796. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. Int. J. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, F. C., Knodt, A. R., Sporns, O., Lahey, B. B., Zald, D. H., Brigidi, B. D., et al. (2013). Impulsivity and the modular organization of resting-state neural networks. Cereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 23, 1444–1452. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeBenedictis, J. N., Nymo, S., Ollestad, K. H., Boyesen, G. A., Rehfeld, J. F., Holst, J. J., et al. (2020). Changes in the homeostatic appetite system after weight loss reflect a normalization toward a lower body weight. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 105, e2538–e2546.

Google Scholar

Diedenhofen, B., and Musch, J. (2015). cocor: a comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PLoS One 10:e0121945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. G. (2007). Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, X., Liang, Q., Wu, G., She, Y., Sui, N., and Chen, H. (2018). Decreased resting-state BOLD regional homogeneity and the intrinsic functional connectivity within dorsal striatum is associated with greater impulsivity in food-related decision-making and BMI change at 6-month follow up. Appetite 127, 69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-García, I., Horstmann, A., Jurado, M. A., Garolera, M., Chaudhry, S. J., Margulies, D. S., et al. (2014). Reward processing in obesity, substance addiction and non-substance addiction. Obes. Rev. Off. J. Int. Assoc. Study Obes. 15, 853–869. doi: 10.1111/obr.12221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbons, C., Caudwell, P., Finlayson, G., Webb, D.-L., Hellström, P. M., Näslund, E., et al. (2013). Comparison of postprandial profiles of ghrelin, active GLP-1, and total PYY to meals varying in fat and carbohydrate and their association with hunger and the phases of satiety. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 98, E847–E855.

Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., Parkinson, J. A., and Owen, A. M. (2008). The cognitive functions of the caudate nucleus. Prog. Neurobiol. 86, 141–155. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, Y., Schmitz, T. W., Mur, M., Ferreira, C. S., and Anderson, M. C. (2018). A supramodal role of the basal ganglia in memory and motor inhibition: meta-analytic evidence. Neuropsychologia 108, 117–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermann, P., Gál, V., Kóbor, I., Kirwan, C. B., Kovács, P., Kitka, T., et al. (2019). Efficacy of weight loss intervention can be predicted based on early alterations of fMRI food cue reactivity in the striatum. Neuroimage Clin. 23:101803. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Tesar, A.-K., Beier, J., Berg, M., and Warrings, B. (2019). Grey matter alterations in obesity: a meta-analysis of whole-brain studies. Obes. Rev. Off. J. Int. Assoc. Study Obes. 20, 464–471. doi: 10.1111/obr.12799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y.-H., Liu, H.-C., Tsai, F.-J., Sun, F.-J., Huang, K.-Y., Chiu, Y.-C., et al. (2017). Correlation of impulsivity with self-harm and suicidal attempt: a community study of adolescents in Taiwan. BMJ Open 7:e017949. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-017949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huerta, C. I., Sarkar, P. R., Duong, T. Q., Laird, A. R., and Fox, P. T. (2014). Neural bases of food perception: coordinate-based meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies in multiple modalities. Obes. Silver Spring Md. 22, 1439–1446. doi: 10.1002/oby.20659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastreboff, A. M., Lacadie, C., Seo, D., Kubat, J., Van Name, M. A., Giannini, C., et al. (2014). Leptin is associated with exaggerated brain reward and emotion responses to food images in adolescent obesity. Diabetes Care 37, 3061–3068. doi: 10.2337/dc14-0525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastreboff, A. M., Sinha, R., Arora, J., Giannini, C., Kubat, J., Malik, S., et al. (2016). Altered brain response to drinking glucose and fructose in obese adolescents. Diabetes 65, 1929–1939. doi: 10.2337/db15-1216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., and Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. Neuroimage 62, 782–790.

Google Scholar

Kobashi, M., and Ida, M. (2013). Making the revised version of barratt impulsiveness scale 11th in Japanese: a study on reliability and validity. J. Psychol. Rissho. Univ. 4, 53–61.

Google Scholar

Laurent, J. S., Watts, R., Adise, S., Allgaier, N., Chaarani, B., Garavan, H., et al. (2019). Associations among body mass index, cortical thickness, and executive function in children. JAMA Pediatr. 174, 170–177. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leidy, H. J., Apolzan, J. W., Mattes, R. D., and Campbell, W. W. (2010). Food form and portion size affect postprandial appetite sensations and hormonal responses in healthy, nonobese, older adults. Obes. Silver Spring Md. 18, 293–299. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lips, M. A., Wijngaarden, M. A., van der Grond, J., van Buchem, M. A., de Groot, G. H., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., et al. (2014). Resting-state functional connectivity of brain regions involved in cognitive control, motivation, and reward is enhanced in obese females. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 100, 524–531. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.080671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowe, C. J., Reichelt, A. C., and Hall, P. A. (2019). The prefrontal cortex and obesity: a health neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 349–361. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A. (2013). Impulsivity and overeating: a closer look at the subscales of the barratt impulsiveness scale. Front. Psychol. [Internet] 4:177. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00177/full

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliva, R., Morys, F., Horstmann, A., Castiello, U., and Begliomini, C. (2019). The impulsive brain: neural underpinnings of binge eating behavior in normal-weight adults. Appetite 136, 33–49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliva, R., Morys, F., Horstmann, A., Castiello, U., and Begliomini, C. (2020). Characterizing impulsivity and resting-state functional connectivity in normal-weight binge eaters. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 53, 478–488. doi: 10.1002/eat.23212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, K. A., Chan, O., Arora, J., Belfort-Deaguiar, R., Dzuira, J., Roehmholdt, B., et al. (2013). Effects of fructose vs glucose on regional cerebral blood flow in brain regions involved with appetite and reward pathways. JAMA 309, 63–70. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.116975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pechorro, P., Ayala-Nunes, L., Ray, J. V., Nunes, C., and Gonçalves, R. A. (2016). The barratt impulsiveness scale-11 among a school sample of portuguese male and female adolescents. J. Child. Fam. Stud. 25, 2753–2764. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0453-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pernet, C. R., Wilcox, R., and Rousselet, G. A. (2012). Robust correlation analyses: false positive and power validation using a new open source matlab toolbox. Front. Psychol. 3:606.

Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59, 2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, J. L., Laird, A. R., Glahn, D. C., Blangero, J., Sanghera, M. K., Pessoa, L., et al. (2012). The functional connectivity of the human caudate: an application of meta-analytic connectivity modeling with behavioral filtering. Neuroimage 60, 117–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: implications for prevention. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 263–276.

Google Scholar

Ronan, L., Alexander-Bloch, A., and Fletcher, P. C. (2020). Childhood obesity, cortical structure, and executive function in healthy children. Cereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 30, 2519–2528. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahoo, K., Sahoo, B., Choudhury, A. K., Sofi, N. Y., Kumar, R., and Bhadoria, A. S. (2015). Childhood obesity: causes and consequences. J. Fam. Med. Prim Care 4, 187–192.

Google Scholar

Siep, N., Roefs, A., Roebroeck, A., Havermans, R., Bonte, M. L., and Jansen, A. (2009). Hunger is the best spice: an fMRI study of the effects of attention, hunger and calorie content on food reward processing in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 198, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Someya, T., Sakado, K., Seki, T., Kojima, M., Reist, C., Tang, S. W., et al. (2001). The Japanese version of the barratt impulsiveness scale, 11th version (BIS-11): its reliability and validity. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 55, 111–114. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00796.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Burger, K. S., and Yokum, S. (2015). Reward region responsivity predicts future weight gain and moderating effects of the TaqIA allele. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 35, 10316–10324. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3607-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Marti, C. N., and Rohde, P. (2013). Prevalence, incidence, impairment, and course of the proposed DSM-5 eating disorder diagnoses in an 8-year prospective community study of young women. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 445–457. doi: 10.1037/a0030679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Spoor, S., Bohon, C., and Small, D. M. (2008a). Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science 322, 449–452. doi: 10.1126/science.1161550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Spoor, S., Bohon, C., Veldhuizen, M. G., and Small, D. M. (2008b). Relation of reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obesity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 924–935. doi: 10.1037/a0013600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., and Yokum, S. (2016). Neural vulnerability factors that increase risk for future weight gain. Psychol. Bull. 142, 447–471. doi: 10.1037/bul0000044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Yokum, S., Blum, K., and Bohon, C. (2010). Weight gain is associated with reduced striatal response to palatable food. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 30, 13105–13109. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2105-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thirion, B., Pinel, P., Mériaux, S., Roche, A., Dehaene, S., and Poline, J.-B. (2007). Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. Neuroimage 35, 105–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuulari, J. J., Karlsson, H. K., Hirvonen, J., Salminen, P., Nuutila, P., and Nummenmaa, L. (2015). Neural circuits for cognitive appetite control in healthy and obese individuals: an fMRI study. PLoS One 10:e0116640. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Laan, L. N., de Ridder, D. T. D., Viergever, M. A., and Smeets, P. A. M. (2011). The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage 55, 296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Meer, F., Charbonnier, L., and Smeets, P. A. M. (2016). Food decision-making: effects of weight status and age. Curr. Diab. Rep. 16:84.

Google Scholar

Yokum, S., Gearhardt, A. N., Harris, J. L., Brownell, K. D., and Stice, E. (2014). Individual differences in striatum activity to food commercials predict weight gain in adolescents. Obes. Silver Spring Md. 22, 2544–2551.

Google Scholar

Keywords: adolescents, caudate, lateral prefrontal cortex, resting-state functional connectivity, weight management

Citation: Nakamura Y, Ozawa S and Koike S (2020) Caudate Functional Connectivity Associated With Weight Change in Adolescents. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:587763. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.587763

Received: 27 July 2020; Accepted: 27 October 2020;
Published: 16 November 2020.

Edited by:

Kiyoshi Nakahara, Kochi University of Technology, Japan

Reviewed by:

Yi Zhang, Xidian University, China
Chiara Begliomini, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Nakamura, Ozawa and Koike. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuko Nakamura, bmFrYW11cmEueXVrbzA3MDdAbWFpbC51LXRva3lvLmFjLmpw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.