Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 16 October 2020
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding the Link Between the Developing Brain and Behavior in Adolescents View all 9 articles

Myelin Water Imaging Demonstrates Lower Brain Myelination in Children and Adolescents With Poor Reading Ability

  • 1Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
  • 2Department of Physics and Astronomy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 3Department of Education and Counseling Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 4Philips Health Care, Hamburg, Germany
  • 5Department of Radiology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 6Department of Radiology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 7Department of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 8International Collaboration on Repair Discoveries (ICORD), University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) provides a means to non-invasively investigate the neurological links with dyslexia, a learning disability that affects one’s ability to read. Most previous brain MRI studies of dyslexia and reading skill have used structural or diffusion imaging to reveal regional brain abnormalities. However, volumetric and diffusion MRI lack specificity in their interpretation at the microstructural level. Myelin is a critical neural component for brain function and plasticity, and as such, deficits in myelin may impact reading ability. MRI can estimate myelin using myelin water fraction (MWF) imaging, which is based on evaluation of the proportion of short T2 myelin-associated water from multi-exponential T2 relaxation analysis, but has not yet been applied to the study of reading or dyslexia. In this study, MWF MRI, intelligence, and reading assessments were acquired in 20 participants aged 10–18 years with a wide range of reading ability to investigate the relationship between reading ability and myelination. Group comparisons showed markedly lower MWF by 16–69% in poor readers relative to good readers in the left and right thalamus, as well as the left posterior limb of the internal capsule, left/right anterior limb of the internal capsule, left/right centrum semiovale, and splenium of the corpus callosum. MWF over the entire group also correlated positively with three different reading scores in the bilateral thalamus as well as white matter, including the splenium of the corpus callosum, left posterior limb of the internal capsule, left anterior limb of the internal capsule, and left centrum semiovale. MWF imaging from T2 relaxation suggests that myelination, particularly in the bilateral thalamus, splenium, and left hemisphere white matter, plays a role in reading abilities. Myelin water imaging thus provides a potentially valuable in vivo imaging tool for the study of dyslexia and its remediation.

Introduction

Dyslexia is a complex learning disability that affects one’s ability to read regardless of intelligence (Siegel, 1989a; Fletcher, 2009). Although the exact cause of dyslexia is still unknown, current hypotheses based primarily on quantitative neuroimaging methods point towards a neurological basis with an emphasis on a left cerebral hemisphere network (Peterson and Pennington, 2012), and in particular the left occipito-temporal cortex (Wandell and Le, 2017; Kronbichler and Kronbichler, 2018). Similarly located anatomical differences have even been seen in pre-readers at risk for dyslexia (Vandermosten et al., 2016). The brain regions implicated include both gray matter (Eckert et al., 2016) and white matter tracts (Wandell and Yeatman, 2013), the latter of which have largely been investigated thus far using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Since the earliest DTI papers demonstrating correlations of reading ability with DTI parameters (Klingberg et al., 2000; Nagy et al., 2004; Beaulieu et al., 2005; Deutsch et al., 2005; Niogi and McCandliss, 2006), a vast literature over the last 15 years has highlighted key white matter connections presumed to underlie the potentially inefficient communication or “disconnection” between brain regions in dyslexia (see Ben-Shachar et al., 2007; Vandermosten et al., 2012b; for comprehensive reviews and a meta-analysis of earlier DTI papers). These DTI reading findings are not limited to English with similar results across multiple languages (Qiu et al., 2008; Steinbrink et al., 2008; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2014; Cui et al., 2016; Takeuchi et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2016; de Moura et al., 2016; Vanderauwera et al., 2017; Su et al., 2018; Žarić et al., 2018; Moulton et al., 2019). While many of these studies reported DTI changes in the left temporo-parietal white matter, other bilateral regions and the corpus callosum have also shown associations with reading, implicating a broader network.

Regional white matter DTI has been correlated to language ability and pre-reading skills in younger “pre-readers” (Saygin et al., 2013; Vandermosten et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2017; Dodson et al., 2018; Vanderauwera et al., 2018; Walton et al., 2018; Ozernov-Palchik et al., 2019; Hutton et al., 2020), and even in infants with a family history of developmental dyslexia (Langer et al., 2017). Longitudinal DTI has shown that both baseline values and changes of diffusion parameters between scans predict future reading ability in healthy controls (Hoeft et al., 2011; Yeatman et al., 2012; Myers et al., 2014; Gullick and Booth, 2015; Takeuchi et al., 2016; Vanderauwera et al., 2017; Borchers et al., 2019; Bruckert et al., 2019; Lebel et al., 2019) and in neurodevelopmental disorders such as fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (Treit et al., 2013). Further compelling evidence of a link between regional white matter plasticity and function comes from DTI parameter changes with remediation in grade-school poor readers (Keller and Just, 2009; Huber et al., 2018), training in spelling-impaired children (Gebauer et al., 2012), and acquisition of literacy in those learning to read in adulthood (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2014). Most DTI studies of reading focus on the white matter and do not analyze the deep gray matter structures, but DTI has shown correlations in the left and right thalamus with reading ability in adolescents and young adults (Lebel et al., 2013).

Although DTI has implicated certain white matter regions and connections for adequate reading, the interpretation of what is underpinning these findings at the microstructural level is not specific. Myelin is a major component of white matter and, given its function of optimizing impulse transmission, may be important in performing complex, “multi-cortex” tasks efficiently, as well as promoting plasticity during learning (Zatorre et al., 2012). Water diffusion parameters are not specific to myelin (Beaulieu and Allen, 1994) and can be affected by gross anatomical changes as well as myelin content, axon diameter, axon packing density, extra-axonal space, and dispersion of fiber orientations within an imaging voxel (for review see Beaulieu, 2002). More specific microstructural interpretation may come from the diffusion coefficients relative to the length of the axons including radial diffusivity (RD), where increases are thought to reflect demyelination (Song et al., 2002, 2003). Reading intervention studies have shown reductions of RD over time, interpreted as increases in myelin (Keller and Just, 2009; Gebauer et al., 2012); however, RD from the DTI model is inadequate in regions of crossing fibers (Wheeler-Kingshott and Cercignani, 2009) and may only be specific after a known timed injury to a tract (Concha et al., 2006). Although more comprehensive models of diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data have been applied in several reading studies to reveal better tracking of white matter tracts at crossing regions and correlations of new diffusion model parameters (e.g., HMOA, hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy; AWF, axonal water fraction; ICVF, intracellular volume fraction) with reading ability (Vanderauwera et al., 2015; Zhao et al., 2016; Huber et al., 2019), these advanced diffusion MRI models are not specific to myelin.

A few alternative imaging strategies beyond diffusion MRI have been applied to reading and language studies to potentially enhance specificity to microstructure. Myelin content in brain regions has been inferred or estimated primarily via water (hydrogen) relaxation properties in the tissue. Myelin water fraction (MWF), derived from an MRI technique called mcDESPOT (multicomponent driven equilibrium single pulse observation of T1 and T2), and its hemispheric asymmetry has been positively correlated with receptive and expressive language ability in typically developing infants and children under 6 years of age (O’Muircheartaigh et al., 2013, 2014). The left arcuate fasciculus showed higher T1-weighted image intensity (by ~1%) in preliterate children at risk for dyslexia (Kraft et al., 2016); however, this is not a quantitative measure of relaxation. The absolute longitudinal relaxation time (T1 in ms) was lower in “reading” tracts relative to “math” tracts, which the authors interpreted as greater myelination in the former (Grotheer et al., 2019), although T1 is also non-specific and there was no assessment of correlations of T1 with reading ability in their typical adult cohort.

A promising myelin-specific imaging option is multi-exponential T2 relaxation based on the premise that a multi-echo T2 decay curve from the brain can be fit to different components (see reviews by MacKay and Laule, 2016; Does, 2018). The shortest T2 component arises from water trapped between myelin bilayers, and the ratio of the short T2 component signal to the total signal yields the MWF per voxel. This short T2 component appears to be specific to myelin, as validated by histology in excised nerve samples (Fenrich, 1992; Does and Snyder, 1996; Beaulieu et al., 1998; Webb et al., 2003) as well as brain and spinal cord tissue from multiple sclerosis patients (Laule et al., 2006, 2008, 2016). While the T2-relaxation based MWF method has been used to investigate normal development and many diseases (MacKay and Laule, 2016), to our knowledge, it has not been used to study reading in children. We aimed to examine the relationship between reading ability and regional brain myelin content determined by multi-exponential T2 in children and adolescents with a wide range of reading abilities.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 20 participants (four females, 16 males; a range of 10–18 years with 15/20 between 10–13 years; median = 12.2 years) with diverse reading ability were recruited from several school districts in the Greater Vancouver Regional District, British Columbia. The participants were carefully selected from either a prior non-MRI longitudinal reading study where they were assessed yearly from kindergarten to grade 4 (Lipka et al., 2006) or from students who attended a specialized school for learning disabilities. Individuals identified with poor reading scores over multiple years in the longitudinal study or those identified as having a reading disability from the school were invited, as well as good readers from the longitudinal study. Informed written consent, as approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Board, was obtained for all participants.

Cognitive and Reading Assessments

The assessments were performed by two trained and experienced individuals (coauthor PL and a research assistant) overseen by a reading specialist (coauthor LS). The Woodcock-Johnson-III (WJ-III) test of cognitive abilities was used for a brief assessment of intelligence that includes three components: verbal comprehension (a test of vocabulary and analogies), concept formation (a test of fluid reasoning), and visual matching (a test of processing speed). The results of the three components were combined to give a Brief Intelligence Ability (BIA) score that provided a measurement of intelligence in approximately 10–15 min. Reading ability was measured using three reading assessments: (1) the Reading Subset of the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT3 Reading)—a test of recognition and naming of letters as well as pronunciation of words with increasing complexity and unfamiliarity; (2) WJ-III Letter-Word Identification (Word ID)—a test of the ability to name letters and words from a list; and (3) WJ-III Word Attack—a test of phonetic skills by pronouncing nonsense words quickly. All raw scores were converted to age-normalized standard scores and population percentiles relative to the test’s published norms.

Concerning the classification of participants as either good or poor readers, the IQ-reading discrepancy definition was not used here because of the overwhelming evidence of its lack of validity (see e.g., Siegel, 1989b; Fletcher et al., 1992; Tanaka et al., 2011; Siegel and Hurford, 2019). Specifically, children were classified as poor readers (dyslexia) if at least two of their reading assessments were below the 25th percentile, while good readers had all reading scores above the 35th percentile. If participants did not meet this poor reader criterion but had at least one score below the 35th percentile, they were classified as intermediate readers. Intermediate readers were included in the correlation analysis, but not the group comparisons. The 25th percentile cut-off has been used previously to define children with a reading disability in group comparisons (Siegel, 1989a; Stanovich and Siegel, 1994).

MRI Acquisition

Brain MRI was acquired on a 3T Philips Achieva scanner. T2 relaxation was measured with a 32-echo 3D GRASE (Prasloski et al., 2012b) with TR = 1,500 ms, TE = 10 ms, echo spacing 10 ms, two gradient echoes per spin echo, 8 transverse slices, voxel size = 1 × 1.8 × 5 mm3, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 240 × 180 × 40 mm3, scan time = 11:39 min. The inferior/superior coverage was limited by the MRI scanner technology at the time of data collection [circa 2006—(Yip et al., 2010)] and considerations for keeping the scan time minimized for our children/adolescent cohort. The 3D slab was centered on the corpus callosum to provide sufficient and consistent coverage over all participants for many-core white and deep gray matter regions of interest (Figure 1). To assist in the identification of structures for region-of-interest (ROI) analysis, a high resolution 3D T1-weighted Turbo Field Echo was also acquired with TR = 10 ms, TE = 6 ms, TI = 848 ms, 120 axial slices, voxel size = 1.1 × 1.1 × 1.1 mm3, flip angle = 80°, TFE factor = 154, sense factor 1.7, field of view = 212 × 181 × 132 mm3, scan time = 5:02 min.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The 4 cm coverage over eight slices of the multi-echo T2 scan was centered on the body of the corpus callosum.

MRI Analysis

The raw 3D GRASE multi-echo images were inspected to confirm the lack of motion artifacts. T2 distributions were calculated for every voxel in the T2 relaxation decay data set using a regularized non-negative least squares (NNLS) algorithm (Whittall and MacKay, 1989) accounting for stimulated echo artifacts due to radiofrequency (B1+), as previously described (Prasloski et al., 2012a). MWF was defined by the area under the T2 distribution below 40 ms divided by the total area to yield voxel-wise MWF maps.

Multi-echo T2 images were registered to the T1 images using FSL (Smith et al., 2004), the latter used for manual ROI placement by one individual (coauthor EY) of 10 white matter and six gray matter structures including the genu and splenium of the corpus callosum, as well as bilateral (left and right kept separate) posterior limb of the internal capsule, anterior limb of the internal capsule, minor forceps, centrum semiovale, thalamus, putamen, and caudate nucleus. These ROIs were selected as they are readily identifiable in the acquired slab and cover commissural, projection, and association white matter regions, as well as core deep gray matter regions, some of which have been implicated in prior imaging studies of dyslexia/reading. For example, the centrum semiovale has been shown in numerous previous DTI studies to have a link to reading test performance (e.g., see Figure 2 in Ben-Shachar et al., 2007).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Myelin water fraction (MWF) maps of two slices in three poor readers and three good readers of similar ages. The MWF maps highlight the white matter, as expected, but shows noticeably higher MWF in the good readers. Note that the background has been cropped and the residual yellow skull is an artifact of the processing.

MWF Group Comparison and Correlation Analysis

Age, intelligence scores, and reading scores were compared between good and poor readers with a two-tailed Student’s test. A one-tailed Student’s t-test was performed for the group comparison to determine if MWF was lower in poor readers relative to good readers. Pearson’s correlation coefficients assessed linear correlations between age, intelligence (WJIII BIA Brief IQ), and the three age-standardized reading scores (WRAT-3 Reading, WJ-III Word ID, WJ-III Word Attack) with MWF within each of the 16 ROIs over the entire group. Note that age was not a covariate for the MWF assessments since MWF did not correlate with age (see “Results” section). A Benjamini Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR) correction of 0.05 was applied to account for multiple comparisons (uncorrected p-values surviving FDR correction are reported).

Results

Demographics

Of the 20 participants, seven were classified as poor readers (one female, six males), 11 as good readers (three females, eight males), and two as intermediate readers (two males). There were marked group differences in all three age-normalized reading tests with the good readers yielding much higher scores (mean 107–112) than the poor readers (mean 84–89; Table 1). The handedness was right in 18 participants and left in two participants (both good readers). There were no significant differences in age (mean ~12.8 years) or intelligence scores between good and poor readers, and no significant correlations between age and any age-standardized cognitive or reading assessment.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject demographics with age-standardized scores showing similar age and intelligence, but significantly different reading scores between the good and poor reading groups [mean (SD)].

Group Comparisons of Myelin Water Fraction Between Good and Poor Readers

The MWF maps showed the expected greater MWF in white matter than cortical or deep gray matter, as well as regional variation across the white matter (Figures 2, 3). Notably, the MWF map intensity was visually lower in the poor readers relative to the good readers (see Figure 2 for three examples of each group). MWF was significantly lower in the poor reader group relative to the good reader group in the left and right thalamus by 69% (p = 0.02) and 56% (p = 0.005), respectively, and in six white matter regions including left (−37%, p = 0.01) and right (−31%, p = 0.03) anterior limb of internal capsule, left (−22%, p = 0.02) and right (−16%, p = 0.03) centrum semiovale, left posterior limb of internal capsule (−21%, p = 0.02), and splenium of corpus callosum (−26%, p = 0.03; Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean (± standard error) MWF for good (dark bars, n = 11) and poor (light bars, n = 7) readers for various bilateral (L, left; R, right) white and gray matter structures: genu of corpus callosum (GE), splenium of corpus callosum (SP), posterior limb of the internal capsule (PIC), anterior limb of the internal capsule (AIC), minor forceps (MF), thalamus (TH), putamen (PU) and head of caudate nucleus (CN). MWF was significantly lower (p < 0.05) in 8 of 16 regions and showed trend level reduction (p = 0.05–0.08) in another five regions for poor readers. As expected, the MWF was higher in the white matter regions (0.06–0.14 for good readers, 0.04–0.11 for poor readers) relative to deep gray matter (0.015–0.03 for good readers, 0.005–0.02 for poor readers).

When all 10 white matter ROIs were averaged together (left and right combined) per individual, the poor reader group had 20% less MWF compared to the good reader group (MWF mean ± SE: poor 0.079 ± 0.004 vs. good 0.098 ± 0.004, p = 0.011; Figure 4). Similarly, the mean of the six deep gray matter ROIs also showed that MWF was lower by 46% in the poor reader group compared to the good reader group (MWF poor 0.012 ± 0.002 vs. good 0.022 ± 0.002, p = 0.011; Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Boxplots of MWF for good (n = 11) and poor (n = 7) readers over all 10 white matter regions combined and all six deep gray matter regions combined showing marked MWF differences (horizontal line, median; x, mean; box lower edge, first quartile; box upper edge, third quartile; error bars, minimum and maximum data values).

Correlation Analysis of Myelin Water Fraction With Reading

There were 10 significant (FDR 0.05) positive correlations (R = 0.59–0.73) of MWF with the age-standardized reading scores (WRAT3 Reading; WJIII Word ID, Word Attack) over 6 ROIs including the right thalamus with correlations to all three reading scores, left thalamus and splenium of the corpus callosum with two correlations each, and then three left white matter regions with one correlation each (the left posterior limb of the internal capsule, left anterior limb of the internal capsule, left centrum semiovale)—see Table 2 and example plots for three structures in Figure 5. All 6 of these ROIs showed MWF correlations to the WRAT3 Reading score whereas WJ III Word ID was limited to three regions (bilateral thalamus and splenium) and WJ III Word Attack to only the right thalamus. There were no significant correlations between MWF and age (mean R = −0.01, mean p = 0.67; data not shown) or WJIII BIA Brief IQ (mean R = 0.22, mean p = 0.63; data not shown). Note that adding age as a covariate to the linear regression yielded comparable R values of 0.62–0.75 for these same 10 correlations (data not shown). Notably, MWF was positively correlated at p ≤ 0.05 (not FDR corrected) with age-standardized reading scores for 23/48 correlations performed covering 11/16 ROIs (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Linear Pearson correlations (R/uncorrected p) between three reading assessment scores and myelin water fraction from 16 bilateral (L—left and R—right) white and deep gray matter structures in the overall cohort of 20 participants.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Examples are shown from three regions (right thalamus, splenium of the corpus callosum, and left posterior limb of internal capsule) that show significant positive correlations [*false discovery rate (FDR) 0.05, uncorrected p shown] between MWF and the three reading assessment scores (WRAT III Reading, WJ III Letter Word ID, WJ III Word Attack) in the full group of 20 individuals. The reader groups are shown with different colored symbols.

Discussion

Multicomponent T2 relaxation measurements have demonstrated that on average MWF is 20% lower in white matter and 46% lower in deep gray matter ROIs for children who are poor readers compared to good readers. Of all regions, the group comparison highlighted the left and right thalamus with the most marked MWF reductions of 69% and 56% in poor readers. These proportional differences in MWF, which have not been measured before in reading studies, are much greater than those reported for DTI parameters or volumes. MWF is a completely different and more specific tissue microstructure measurement, and such notable differences in proportional changes between imaging measurements are not incongruent. These preliminary results are encouraging in a small, highly selected, and well-characterized sample, and provide the impetus for confirmation in a larger independent population using faster MWF methods with greater brain coverage. The positive correlation of MWF with three different age-standardized scores of reading ability over the entire cohort of 20 readers also implicated the bilateral thalamus, as well as white matter including the splenium of the corpus callosum and left hemisphere regions of the anterior and posterior limb of the internal capsule and more superior in the centrum semiovale, which are consistent with the locations identified in many previous DTI reports of reading ability (reviewed in Vandermosten et al., 2012b). MWF was not correlated with intelligence supporting the notion that dyslexia is a condition that affects reading ability independently of intelligence (Siegel, 1989a; Fletcher, 2009). MWF was also not correlated with age over our 10–18-year-old group, but this finding should be interpreted with caution as 75% of the children were in a narrow range of 10–13 years and the only individual older than 15 years was a poor reader. Other myelin-sensitive MR methods have reported changes across a similar age span. For example, the volume fraction of myelin derived by mcDESPOT has been shown to increase in the white matter of 50 typically developing children between 6–15 years (Geeraert et al., 2019), although they used a different myelin content imaging method than our study, with a larger sample that included younger participants, but did not include poor readers.

Anatomy of Reading Circuitry

The areas seen here have been previously highlighted by neuroimaging studies of brain structure and reading. It has been suggested that learning to read is mediated by white matter connections through the splenium of the corpus callosum (Carreiras et al., 2009). Left lateralization of white matter tracts is commonly observed showing links to genetic factors and age-related changes in the perisylvian language networks (Budisavljevic et al., 2015) and links to phonological processing with the arcuate fasciculus in children (Lebel and Beaulieu, 2009). Additional evidence for a direct role of white matter in reading comes from a neurosurgical case report of subcortical stimulation of a specific anterior/posterior tract connecting the parietal and frontal lobes in the left hemisphere (Motomura et al., 2014). Another case report highlighted the inability to read or identify letters after a biopsy of a left thalamic mass that also induced damage to the splenium of the corpus callosum (Tamhankar et al., 2004); our results suggest that both these structures play a role in reading. Left lateralized white matter connections, and the splenium of the corpus callosum through its role in vision, are core parts of the reading circuitry (Wandell and Le, 2017). Although the left hemisphere has been a primary focus of many studies, brain regions in the right hemisphere have also been implicated in DTI (for example Hoeft et al., 2011; Gebauer et al., 2012; Vandermosten et al., 2012b; Lebel et al., 2013) and fMRI (Maisog et al., 2008) studies, suggesting a more distributed widespread network for reading skill, which is also in-line with our observations showing reduced myelin in several right-sided brain regions.

The Role of Myelin in Reading Circuitry and Plasticity

The microstructural underpinnings which contribute to abnormalities in this circuitry are not confirmed, but it is conceivable that myelination is an important factor. As the primary role of myelin is to increase conduction speed along axons, poor myelination of key brain regions involved in reading may contribute to dyslexia. This theory is consistent with several studies that have shown that individuals with dyslexia manifest rate processing problems (Cohen-Mimran and Sapir, 2007; Murphy and Schochat, 2009; Wright and Conlon, 2009). Myelination is also important for white matter plasticity which is critical for brain circuit formation and function (Zatorre et al., 2012; Chorghay et al., 2018). Longitudinal DTI studies of remediation in dyslexia have shown post-intervention reductions of RD in the left anterior centrum semiovale (Keller and Just, 2009) as well as the left/right corona radiata and posterior limb of the internal capsule (Gebauer et al., 2012), which may be consistent with increased myelination (Song et al., 2002, 2003). Other DTI studies reporting lower RD with better reading scores have also implicated that myelination is driving the white matter fractional anisotropy differences in the reading disabled cohorts in white matter regions such as left arcuate fasciculus (Vandermosten et al., 2012a; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2014; Christodoulou et al., 2017; Hutton et al., 2020), anterior and superior corona radiata (Frye et al., 2011), and left anterior limb of the internal capsule (Qiu et al., 2008). Notably, these regions are all consistent with our findings. The typical trajectory of RD reduction with age over 6–16 years observed in controls was not present in children with dyslexia in the inferior frontal occipital and posterior limb of the internal capsule (Rollins et al., 2009).

MWF and Cognitive Measures

This study contributes to a fairly sparse literature on how MWF relates to cognitive measures. A small cross-sectional study in preadolescent males aged 9–12 years found a strong correlation between MWF and verbal IQ (Whitaker et al., 2008). Experience-dependent changes in myelin due to motor training have been shown with multi-exponential T2 relaxation derived MWF, suggesting that interventions can change MWF (Lakhani et al., 2016). mcDESPOT is a methodologically different approach to estimating myelin content which makes use of T1-weighted and steady-state gradient-echo acquisitions to estimate T1 and T2 times of the tissue components in the brain (Deoni et al., 2008). mcDESPOT has yielded measures related to myelin content that correlate with performance in language tasks, in particular, hemispheric asymmetry positively correlating with receptive and expressive language ability in typically developing infants and children under 6 years of age (O’Muircheartaigh et al., 2013, 2014). mcDESPOT derived myelination measures in the splenium of the corpus callosum also correlated positively with Early Learning Composite score (sum of fine motor, visual reception and expressive and receptive language) in children less than 5 years old, with more widespread bilateral white matter involvement if only examining 1–2-year-old toddlers (Deoni et al., 2016), further supporting a link between enhanced myelination in white matter and cognitive function. However, it should be noted that the spatial patterns of T2-based and mcDESPOT-based myelin measures are quite different in the human brain (Zhang et al., 2015a,b).

Deep Gray Matter and Reading Ability

Our results suggest that myelination of brain circuitry involved in reading ability is not limited to white matter. The bilateral thalamus MWF in poor readers was much lower (69%% left and 56% right) than good readers (MWF poor ~0.01 vs. good 0.02–0.025). It is important to keep in mind that the absolute MWF in deep gray matter is much smaller than in white matter. The right thalamus MWF yielded strong positive correlations independently with all three reading scores (R = 0.59, 0.67, 0.73), while the left thalamus MWF correlated with two reading scores (R = 0.61, 0.71). These observations are in line with the possible functional consequences of myelin abnormalities within the thalamus, which is critical for cognitive functioning and language (Llano, 2013; Sherman, 2016), and acts as a “relay station” for connections between various brain regions. The different thalamic subparts are separated by myelinated lamellae, and less myelin could lead to deficits in information integration and relay. Post-mortem studies of dyslexia have shown cytoarchitecture abnormalities of neurons within the thalamus including the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), the primary processing center for visual information located inside the thalamus (Wandell and Le, 2017), and the left medial geniculate nucleus (MGN), a portion of the auditory thalamus hypothesized to influence attention (Galaburda et al., 1994).

Other MRI studies have also reported several thalamic abnormalities related to dyslexia and reading ability. Individuals with dyslexia have smaller and differently shaped LGN (Giraldo-Chica et al., 2015). Fractional anisotropy in the left and right thalamus has correlated with reading ability (Lebel et al., 2013) and diffusion tensor tractography has identified differences in thalamic connections in individuals with dyslexia (Fan et al., 2014; Müller-Axt et al., 2017; Žarić et al., 2018; Tschentscher et al., 2019). These studies align with our observations of lower MWF in the anterior limb of the internal capsule which contains axons connecting the thalamus and frontal lobe. Functional MRI activity of the thalamus (including right side) also differs in people with dyslexia (Maisog et al., 2008; Díaz et al., 2012; Paz-Alonso et al., 2018) and may be altered following reading intervention (Barquero et al., 2014). These functional changes could be partly a result of changes in myelination, but fMRI was not acquired in our study for comparison to MWF.

Limitations and Future Directions

A clear limitation of our study is the small sample size of 20 participants, although they were a carefully selected sample from a previous longitudinal reading study from kindergarten to Grade 4 and were identified with a reading disability from a specialized school for learning disabilities as supervised by a long time expert in dyslexia (coauthor LS; Siegel, 1989a; Stanovich and Siegel, 1994; Lipka et al., 2006). Further, if the large MWF changes are confirmed in future studies, very large sample sizes may not be required if reading disabled participants are selected and characterized appropriately. The influence of sex and handedness was not possible to evaluate as there were only two left-handers (both good readers) and only four females where one was classified as a poor reader vs. six males. The MWF-reading linear correlations could be driven by marked group differences of MWF between good and poor readers, although such an MWF-reading relationship appears to exist within each group (e.g., right thalamus MWF vs. WRAT III in good readers; Figure 5), granted such an assessment is beyond the scope of our small sample size. Our pilot study provides compelling evidence for larger replication studies of myelin in reading and dyslexia using faster MWF imaging techniques yielding more extensive data collection over the whole brain (Alonso-Ortiz et al., 2015; Lee et al., 2020). That said, our average white matter MWF was 0.10 in the good readers, which fits with a very recent report of 0.10–0.11 in adults using the same type of MRI pulse sequence (Ocklenburg et al., 2019). Our study focused on manual ROI measurements in nine specific regions that were identifiable in all participants within the 4 cm slab and covered various types of white matter as well as the deep gray matter relevant to the reading literature. With whole-brain MWF coverage and the acquisition of coregistered diffusion imaging, future work could include tract specific analysis methods with tractography to extract MWF along tracts of interest in reading, e.g., the arcuate fasciculus (Baumeister et al., 2020). Like many quantitative MRI techniques, the assignment of the short T2 component to “myelin water fraction” is not proof that there are myelin differences in the dyslexic brain, but there is much histological evidence supporting this hypothesis, as outlined in the Introduction. However, the presence of iron can shorten the water T2 in tissue and could interfere with the correspondence of the short T2 component to myelin specifically (Birkl et al., 2019). Although iron is typically far more concentrated in deep gray matter structures, it is also present in the oligodendrocytes, whose cell processes wrap the myelin sheaths of white matter. Hence an alternative interpretation of our results could, in part, be reduced iron (fewer oligodendrocytes) in poor readers, which could be evaluated using quantitative susceptibility mapping MRI.

In conclusion, multi-exponential T2 relaxation can help elucidate the microstructural brain differences associated with the acquisition and performance of cognitive abilities such as reading. This study suggests that a defect in myelination could underlie inefficient communication between brain regions in individuals who are poor readers. Myelin water imaging is complementary to other quantitative MRI techniques and is a potentially valuable in vivo imaging tool for the study of dyslexia and its remediation.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Clinical Research Ethics Board, University of British Columbia. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to writing the manuscript, interpreting the data, and approved the submitted version. Also, CB: designed the study and secured the funding. EY: analyzed the data. PL: acquired the data. BM: designed the study and acquired the data. CAL: designed the study. LS: designed the study, acquired the data, and secured the funding. AM: designed the study and acquired the data. CL: analyzed the data.

Funding

This work was funded by a grant from the Canadian Language and Literacy Research Networks of Centres of Excellence to CB and LS and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council Discovery grant to AM. CL was the recipient of the Women Against MS (WAMS) endMS Research and Training Network Transitional Career Development Award from the Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada. CB and CAL acknowledge salary support from the Canada Research Chairs program.

Conflict of Interest

BM was employed by the company Philips Healthcare.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the volunteers and the MRI technologists at the UBC MRI Research Centre for their assistance in data collection. We also wish to thank Dr. Irene Vavasour for assistance with analysis training. We thank Philips Healthcare for their research support.

References

Alonso-Ortiz, E., Levesque, I. R., and Pike, G. B. (2015). MRI-based myelin water imaging: a technical review. Magn. Reson. Med. 73, 70–81. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barquero, L. A., Davis, N., and Cutting, L. E. (2014). Neuroimaging of reading intervention: a systematic review and activation likelihood estimate meta-analysis. PLoS One 9:e83668. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, T. R., Kolind, S. H., MacKay, A. L., and McKeown, M. J. (2020). Inherent spatial structure in myelin water fraction maps. Magn. Reson. Imaging 67, 33–42. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2019.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, C. (2002). The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—a technical review. NMR Biomed. 15, 435–455. doi: 10.1002/nbm.782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, C., and Allen, P. S. (1994). Determinants of anisotropic water diffusion in nerves. Magn. Reson. Med. 31, 394–400. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910310408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, C., Fenrich, F. R., and Allen, P. S. (1998). Multicomponent water proton transverse relaxation and T2-discriminated water diffusion in myelinated and nonmyelinated nerve. Magn. Reson. Imaging 16, 1201–1210. doi: 10.1016/s0730-725x(98)00151-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, C., Plewes, C., Paulson, L. A., Roy, D., Snook, L., Concha, L., et al. (2005). Imaging brain connectivity in children with diverse reading ability. NeuroImage 25, 1266–1271. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Shachar, M., Dougherty, R. F., and Wandell, B. A. (2007). White matter pathways in reading. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 258–270. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birkl, C., Birkl-Toeglhofer, A. M., Endmayr, V., Hoftberger, R., Kasprian, G., Krebs, C., et al. (2019). The influence of brain iron on myelin water imaging. NeuroImage 199, 545–552. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borchers, L. R., Bruckert, L., Dodson, C. K., Travis, K. E., Marchman, V. A., Ben-Shachar, M., et al. (2019). Microstructural properties of white matter pathways in relation to subsequent reading abilities in children: a longitudinal analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 891–905. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1813-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruckert, L., Borchers, L. R., Dodson, C. K., Marchman, V. A., Travis, K. E., Ben-Shachar, M., et al. (2019). White matter plasticity in reading-related pathways differs in children born preterm and at term: a longitudinal analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 139–905. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budisavljevic, S., Dell’Acqua, F., Rijsdijk, F. V., Kane, F., Picchioni, M., McGuire, P., et al. (2015). Age-related differences and heritability of the perisylvian language networks. J. Neurosci. 35, 12625–12634. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1255-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreiras, M., Seghier, M. L., Baquero, S., Estevez, A., Lozano, A., Devlin, J. T., et al. (2009). An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature 461, 983–986. doi: 10.1038/nature08461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chorghay, Z., Káradóttir, R. T., and Ruthazer, E. S. (2018). White matter plasticity keeps the brain in tune: axons conduct while glia wrap. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:428. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christodoulou, J. A., Murtagh, J., Cyr, A., Perrachione, T. K., Chang, P., Halverson, K., et al. (2017). Relation of white-matter microstructure to reading ability and disability in beginning readers. Neuropsychology 31, 508–515. doi: 10.1037/neu0000388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Mimran, R., and Sapir, S. (2007). Auditory temporal processing deficits in children with reading disabilities. Dyslexia 13, 175–192. doi: 10.1002/dys.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concha, L., Gross, D. W., Wheatley, B. M., and Beaulieu, C. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging of time-dependent axonal and myelin degradation after corpus callosotomy in epilepsy patients. NeuroImage 32, 1090–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, Z., Xia, Z., Su, M., Shu, H., and Gong, G. (2016). Disrupted white matter connectivity underlying developmental dyslexia: a machine learning approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1443–1458. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Moura, L. M., Cogo-Moreira, H., de Avila, C. R., Pan, P. M., Gadelha, A., Moriyama, T., et al. (2016). Children with poor reading skills at the word level show reduced fractional anisotropy in white matter tracts of both hemispheres. Brain Connect. 6, 519–523. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deoni, S. C. L., Rutt, B. K., Arun, T., Pierpaoli, C., and Jones, D. K. (2008). Gleaning multicomponent T1 and T2 information from steady-state imaging data. Magn. Reson. Med. 60, 1372–1387. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deoni, S. C., O’Muircheartaigh, J., Elison, J. T., Walker, L., Doernberg, E., Waskiewicz, N., et al. (2016). White matter maturation profiles through early childhood predict general cognitive ability. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1189–1203. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0947-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, G. K., Dougherty, R. F., Bammer, R., Siok, W. T., Gabrieli, J. D., and Wandell, B. (2005). Children’s reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex 41, 354–363. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70272-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz, B., Hintz, F., Kiebel, S. J., and von Kriegstein, K. (2012). Dysfunction of the auditory thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, 13841–13846. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119828109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodson, C. K., Travis, K. E., Borchers, L. R., Marchman, V. A., Ben-Shachar, M., and Feldman, H. M. (2018). White matter properties associated with pre-reading skills in 6-year-old children born preterm and at term. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 60, 695–702. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Does, M. D. (2018). Inferring brain tissue composition and microstructure via MR relaxometry. NeuroImage 182, 136–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Does, M. D., and Snyder, R. E. (1996). Multiexponential T2 relaxation in degenerating peripheral nerve. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 207–213. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckert, M. A., Berninger, V. W., Vaden, K. I. Jr., Gebregziabher, M., and Tsu, L. (2016). Gray matter features of reading disability: a combined meta-analytic and direct analysis approach (1,2,3,4). eNeuro 3:ENEURO.0103-15.2015. doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0103-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Q., Davis, N., Anderson, A. W., and Cutting, L. E. (2014). Thalamo-cortical connectivity: what can diffusion tractography tell us about reading difficulties in children? Brain Connect. 4, 428–439. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenrich, F. (1992). A Simulation and Experimental Study of Water Proton Relaxation in a White Matter Model. University of Alberta. Master of Science Thesis.

Google Scholar

Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Dyslexia: the evolution of a scientific concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 501–508. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709090900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Rourke, B. P., Shaywitz, S. E., and Shaywitz, B. A. (1992). The validity of discrepancy-based definitions of reading disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 25, 555–561. doi: 10.1177/002221949202500903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frye, R. E., Liederman, J., Hasan, K. M., Lincoln, A., Malmberg, B., McLean, J., et al. (2011). Diffusion tensor quantification of the relations between microstructural and macrostructural indices of white matter and reading. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1220–1235. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galaburda, A. M., Menard, M. T., and Rosen, G. D. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 91, 8010–8013. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.17.8010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gebauer, D., Fink, A., Filippini, N., Johansen-Berg, H., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., et al. (2012). Differences in integrity of white matter and changes with training in spelling impaired children: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain Struct. Funct. 217, 747–760. doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0371-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geeraert, B. L., Lebel, R. M., and Lebel, C. (2019). A multiparametric analysis of white matter maturation during late childhood and adolescence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 4345–4356. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giraldo-Chica, M., Hegarty, J. P. II., and Schneider, K. A. (2015). Morphological differences in the lateral geniculate nucleus associated with dyslexia. Neuroimage Clin. 7, 830–836. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grotheer, M., Zhen, Z., Lerma-Usabiaga, G., and Grill-Spector, K. (2019). Separate lanes for adding and reading in the white matter highways of the human brain. Nat. Commun. 10:3675. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11424-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gullick, M. M., and Booth, J. R. (2015). The direct segment of the arcuate fasciculus is predictive of longitudinal reading change. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeft, F., McCandliss, B. D., Black, J. M., Gantman, A., Zakerani, N., Hulme, C., et al. (2011). Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 361–366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, E., Donnelly, P. M., Rokem, A., and Yeatman, J. D. (2018). Rapid and widespread white matter plasticity during an intensive reading intervention. Nat. Commun. 9:2260. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04627-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, E., Henriques, R. N., Owen, J. P., Rokem, A., and Yeatman, J. D. (2019). Applying microstructural models to understand the role of white matter in cognitive development. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 36:100624. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutton, J. S., Dudley, J., Horowitz-Kraus, T., DeWitt, T., and Holland, S. K. (2020). Associations between home literacy environment, brain white matter integrity and cognitive abilities in preschool-age children. Acta Paediatr. 109, 1376–1386. doi: 10.1111/apa.15124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, T. A., and Just, M. A. (2009). Altering cortical connectivity: remediation-induced changes in the white matter of poor readers. Neuron 64, 624–631. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klingberg, T., Hedehus, M., Temple, E., Salz, T., Gabrieli, J. D., Moseley, M. E., et al. (2000). Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron 25, 493–500. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80911-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraft, I., Schreiber, J., Cafiero, R., Metere, R., Schaadt, G., Brauer, J., et al. (2016). Predicting early signs of dyslexia at a preliterate age by combining behavioral assessment with structural MRI. NeuroImage 143, 378–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kronbichler, L., and Kronbichler, M. (2018). The importance of the left occipitotemporal cortex in developmental dyslexia. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 5, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s40474-018-0135-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakhani, B., Borich, M. R., Jackson, J. N., Wadden, K. P., Peters, S., Villamayor, A., et al. (2016). Motor skill acquisition promotes human brain myelin plasticity. Neural Plast. 2016:7526135. doi: 10.1155/2016/7526135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, N., Peysakhovich, B., Zuk, J., Drottar, M., Sliva, D. D., Smith, S., et al. (2017). White matter alterations in infants at risk for developmental dyslexia. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1027–1036. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laule, C., Kozlowski, P., Leung, E., Li, D. K., Mackay, A. L., and Moore, G. R. (2008). Myelin water imaging of multiple sclerosis at 7 T: correlations with histopathology. NeuroImage 40, 1575–1580. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laule, C., Leung, E., Lis, D. K., Traboulsee, A. L., Paty, D. W., MacKay, A. L., et al. (2006). Myelin water imaging in multiple sclerosis: quantitative correlations with histopathology. Mult. Scler. 12, 747–753. doi: 10.1177/1352458506070928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laule, C., Yung, A., Pavolva, V., Bohnet, B., Kozlowski, P., Hashimoto, S. A., et al. (2016). High-resolution myelin water imaging in post-mortem multiple sclerosis spinal cord: a case report. Mult. Scler. 22, 1485–1489. doi: 10.1177/1352458515624559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., and Beaulieu, C. (2009). Lateralization of the arcuate fasciculus from childhood to adulthood and its relation to cognitive abilities in children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3563–3573. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Benischek, A., Geeraert, B., Holahan, J., Shaywitz, S., Bakhshi, K., et al. (2019). Developmental trajectories of white matter structure in children with and without reading impairments. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 36:100633. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Shaywitz, B., Holahan, J., Shaywitz, S., Marchione, K., and Beaulieu, C. (2013). Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of reading ability in dysfluent and non-impaired readers. Brain Lang. 125, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Hyun, J. W., Lee, J., Choi, E. J., Shin, H. G., Min, K., et al. (2020). So you want to image myelin using MRI: an overview and practical guide for myelin water imaging. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging doi: 10.1002/jmri.27059 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipka, O., Lesaux, N. K., and Siegel, L. (2006). Retrospective analyses of the reading development of grade 4 students with reading disabilities: risk status and profiles over 5 years. J. Learn. Disabil. 39, 364–378. doi: 10.1177/00222194060390040901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llano, D. A. (2013). Functional imaging of the thalamus in language. Brain Lang. 126, 62–72. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, A. L., and Laule, C. (2016). Magnetic resonance of myelin water: an in vivo marker for myelin. Brain Plast. 2, 71–91. doi: 10.3233/bpl-160033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maisog, J. M., Einbinder, E. R., Flowers, D. L., Turkeltaub, P. E., and Eden, G. F. (2008). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1145, 237–259. doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motomura, K., Fujii, M., Maesawa, S., Kuramitsu, S., Natsume, A., and Wakabayashi, T. (2014). Association of dorsal inferior frontooccipital fasciculus fibers in the deep parietal lobe with both reading and writing processes: a brain mapping study. J. Neurosurg. 121, 142–148. doi: 10.3171/2014.2.jns131234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moulton, E., Bouhali, F., Monzalvo, K., Poupon, C., Zhang, H., Dehaene, S., et al. (2019). Connectivity between the visual word form area and the parietal lobe improves after the first year of reading instruction: a longitudinal MRI study in children. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1519–1536. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01855-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller-Axt, C., Anwander, A., and von Kriegstein, K. (2017). Altered structural connectivity of the left visual thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Curr. Biol. 27, 3692.e4–3698.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, C. F., and Schochat, E. (2009). How auditory temporal processing deficits relate to dyslexia. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 42, 647–654. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2009000700009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, C. A., Vandermosten, M., Farris, E. A., Hancock, R., Gimenez, P., Black, J. M., et al. (2014). White matter morphometric changes uniquely predict children’s reading acquisition. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1870–1883. doi: 10.1177/0956797614544511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagy, Z., Westerberg, H., and Klingberg, T. (2004). Maturation of white matter is associated with the development of cognitive functions during childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1227–1233. doi: 10.1162/0898929041920441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niogi, S. N., and McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Left lateralized white matter microstructure accounts for individual differences in reading ability and disability. Neuropsychologia 44, 2178–2188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ocklenburg, S., Anderson, C., Gerding, W. M., Fraenz, C., Schluter, C., Friedrich, P., et al. (2019). Myelin water fraction imaging reveals hemispheric asymmetries in human white matter that are associated with genetic variation in PLP1. Mol. Neurobiol 56, 3999–4012. doi: 10.1007/s12035-018-1351-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Muircheartaigh, J., Dean, D. C. III., Dirks, H., Waskiewicz, N., Lehman, K., Jerskey, B. A., et al. (2013). Interactions between white matter asymmetry and language during neurodevelopment. J. Neurosci. 33, 16170–16177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1463-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Muircheartaigh, J., Dean, D. C. III., Ginestet, C. E., Walker, L., Waskiewicz, N., Lehman, K., et al. (2014). White matter development and early cognition in babies and toddlers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4475–4487. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozernov-Palchik, O., Norton, E. S., Wang, Y., Beach, S. D., Zuk, J., Wolf, M., et al. (2019). The relationship between socioeconomic status and white matter microstructure in pre-reading children: a longitudinal investigation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 741–754. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paz-Alonso, P. M., Oliver, M., Lerma-Usabiaga, G., Caballero-Gaudes, C., Quinones, I., Suarez-Coalla, P., et al. (2018). Neural correlates of phonological, orthographic and semantic reading processing in dyslexia. Neuroimage Clin. 20, 433–447. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. L., and Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 379, 1997–2007. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prasloski, T., Mädler, B., Xiang, Q. S., MacKay, A., and Jones, C. (2012a). Applications of stimulated echo correction to multicomponent T2 analysis. Magn. Reson. Med. 67, 1803–1814. doi: 10.1002/mrm.23157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prasloski, T., Rauscher, A., MacKay, A. L., Hodgson, M., Vavasour, I. M., Laule, C., et al. (2012b). Rapid whole cerebrum myelin water imaging using a 3D GRASE sequence. NeuroImage 63, 533–539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, D., Tan, L.-H., Zhou, K., and Khong, P.-L. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging of normal white matter maturation from late childhood to young adulthood: voxel-wise evaluation of mean diffusivity, fractional anisotropy, radial and axial diffusivities and correlation with reading development. NeuroImage 41, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rollins, N. K., Vachha, B., Srinivasan, P., Chia, J., Pickering, J., Hughes, C. W., et al. (2009). Simple developmental dyslexia in children: alterations in diffusion-tensor metrics of white matter tracts at 3 T. Radiology 251, 882–891. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2513080884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, Z. M., Norton, E. S., Osher, D. E., Beach, S. D., Cyr, A. B., Ozernov-Palchik, O., et al. (2013). Tracking the roots of reading ability: white matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. J. Neurosci. 33, 13251–13258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M. (2016). Thalamus plays a central role in ongoing cortical functioning. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 533–541. doi: 10.1038/nn.4269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, L. S. (1989a). IQ is irrelelvant to the definition of learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 22, 469–478. doi: 10.1177/002221948902200803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, L. S. (1989b). Why we do not need intelligence test scores in the definition and analyses of learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 22, 514–518. doi: 10.1177/002221948902200814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, L. S., and Hurford, D. (2019). The case against discrepancy models in the evaluation of dyslexia. Perspect. Lang. Lit. 45, 23–28.

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23, S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S. K., Sun, S. W., Ju, W. K., Lin, S. J., Cross, A. H., and Neufeld, A. H. (2003). Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia. NeuroImage 20, 1714–1722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S. K., Sun, S. W., Ramsbottom, M. J., Chang, C., Russell, J., and Cross, A. H. (2002). Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. NeuroImage 17, 1429–1436. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E., and Siegel, L. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: a regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. J. Educ. Psych. 86, 24–53.

Google Scholar

Steinbrink, C., Vogt, K., Kastrup, A., Muller, H. P., Juengling, F. D., Kassubek, J., et al. (2008). The contribution of white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: insights from DTI and VBM at 3.0 T. Neuropsychologia 46, 3170–3178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, M., Zhao, J., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Zhou, W., Gong, G., Ramus, F., et al. (2018). Alterations in white matter pathways underlying phonological and morphological processing in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Asano, K., Asano, M., Sassa, Y., et al. (2016). Impact of reading habit on white matter structure: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. NeuroImage 133, 378–389. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamhankar, M. A., Coslett, H. B., Fisher, M. J., Sutton, L. N., and Liu, G. T. (2004). Alexia without agraphia following biopsy of a left thalamic tumor. Pediatr. Neurol. 30, 140–142. doi: 10.1016/s0887-8994(03)00417-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, H., Black, J. M., Hulme, C., Stanley, L. M., Kesler, S. R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2011). The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1442–1451. doi: 10.1177/0956797611419521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Cohen, L., Amemiya, E., Braga, L. W., and Dehaene, S. (2014). Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. Cereb. Cortex 24, 989–995. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treit, S., Lebel, C., Baugh, L., Rasmussen, C., Andrew, G., and Beaulieu, C. (2013). Longitudinal MRI reveals altered trajectory of brain development during childhood and adolescence in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Neurosci. 33, 10098–10109. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5004-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tschentscher, N., Ruisinger, A., Blank, H., Diaz, B., and von Kriegstein, K. (2019). Reduced structural connectivity between left auditory thalamus and the motion-sensitive planum temporale in developmental dyslexia. J. Neurosci. 39, 1720–1732. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1435-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderauwera, J., De Vos, A., Forkel, S. J., Catani, M., Wouters, J., Vandermosten, M., et al. (2018). Neural organization of ventral white matter tracts parallels the initial steps of reading development: a DTI tractography study. Brain Lang. 183, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderauwera, J., Vandermosten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Wouters, J., and Ghesquiere, P. (2015). Disentangling the relation between left temporoparietal white matter and reading: a spherical deconvolution tractography study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3273–3287. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderauwera, J., Wouters, J., Vandermosten, M., and Ghesquiere, P. (2017). Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Poelmans, H., Sunaert, S., Wouters, J., and Ghesquiere, P. (2012a). A tractography study in dyslexia: neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing. Brain 135, 935–948. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Wouters, J., and Ghesquiere, P. (2012b). A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1532–1552. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Hoeft, F., and Norton, E. S. (2016). Integrating MRI brain imaging studies of pre-reading children with current theories of developmental dyslexia: a review and quantitative meta-analysis. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Vanderauwera, J., Theys, C., De Vos, A., Vanvooren, S., Sunaert, S., et al. (2015). A DTI tractography study in pre-readers at risk for dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, M., Dewey, D., and Lebel, C. (2018). Brain white matter structure and language ability in preschool-aged children. Brain Lang. 176, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wandell, B. A., and Le, R. K. (2017). Diagnosing the neural circuitry of reading. Neuron 96, 298–311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wandell, B. A., and Yeatman, J. D. (2013). Biological development of reading circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 261–268. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Mauer, M. V., Raney, T., Peysakhovich, B., Becker, B. L. C., Sliva, D. D., et al. (2017). Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2469–2485. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, S., Munro, C. A., Midha, R., and Stanisz, G. J. (2003). Is multicomponent T2 a good measure of myelin content in peripheral nerve? Magn. Reson. Med. 49, 638–645. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., and Cercignani, M. (2009). About “axial” and “radial” diffusivities. Magn. Reson. Med. 61, 1255–1260. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitaker, K. J., Kolind, S. H., MacKay, A. L., and Clark, C. M. (2008). Quantifying development: investigating highly myelinated voxels in preadolescent corpus callosum. NeuroImage 43, 731–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittall, K. P., and MacKay, A. L. (1989). Quantitative interpretation of nmr relaxation data. J. Magn. Reson. 84, 134–152. doi: 10.1016/0022-2364(89)90011-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, C. M., and Conlon, E. G. (2009). Auditory and visual processing in children with dyslexia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 34, 330–355. doi: 10.1080/87565640902801882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeatman, J. D., Dougherty, R. F., Ben-Shachar, M., and Wandell, B. A. (2012). Development of white matter and reading skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, E3045–E3053. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206792109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yip, E., Low, P., Madler, B., Lebel, C., Beaulieu, C., Siegel, L., et al. (2010). “Myelin water imaging of children with diverse reading ability,” in International Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine Annual Conference, (Stockholm, Sweden), 2346.

Google Scholar

Žarić, G., Timmers, I., Gerretsen, P., Fraga Gonzalez, G., Tijms, J., van der Molen, M. W., et al. (2018). Atypical white matter connectivity in dyslexic readers of a fairly transparent orthography. Front. Psychol. 9:1147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Fields, R. D., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2012). Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 528–536. doi: 10.1038/nn.3045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Chen, C., Xue, G., Lu, Z. L., Mei, L., Xue, H., et al. (2014). Language-general and -specific white matter microstructural bases for reading. NeuroImage 98, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Kolind, S. H., Laule, C., and MacKay, A. (2015a). Comparison of myelin water fraction from multiecho T2 decay curve and steady-state methods. Magn. Reson. Med. 73, 223–232. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Kolind, S. H., Laule, C., and MacKay, A. (2015b). How does magnetization transfer influence mcDESPOT results? Magn. Reson. Med. 74, 1327–1335. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, J., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Altarelli, I., Dubois, J., and Ramus, F. (2016). Altered hemispheric lateralization of white matter pathways in developmental dyslexia: evidence from spherical deconvolution tractography. Cortex 76, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dyslexia, myelin water, MRI, reading ability, children, adolescent

Citation: Beaulieu C, Yip E, Low PB, Mädler B, Lebel CA, Siegel L, Mackay AL and Laule C (2020) Myelin Water Imaging Demonstrates Lower Brain Myelination in Children and Adolescents With Poor Reading Ability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:568395. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.568395

Received: 01 June 2020; Accepted: 31 August 2020;
Published: 16 October 2020.

Edited by:

Nandita Vijayakumar, Deakin University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Mustapha Bouhrara, National Institute on Aging (NIH), United States
Heidi M. Feldman, Stanford University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Beaulieu, Yip, Low, Mädler, Lebel, Siegel, Mackay and Laule. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christian Beaulieu, christian.beaulieu@ualberta.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.