Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 August 2020
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation

Gray Matter Changes in Adolescents Participating in a Meditation Training

  • 1Department of Radiology & Biomedical Imaging, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Biomedical Sciences, Florida State University College of Medicine, Tallahassee, FL, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The Langley Porter Psychiatric Institute, Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Weill Institute for Neurosciences, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Clinical Science/Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden

Meditation has shown to benefit a wide range of conditions and symptoms, but the neural mechanisms underlying the practice remain unclear. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have investigated the structural brain changes due to the practice by examining volume, density, or cortical thickness changes. However, these studies have focused on adults; meditation’s structural effects on the adolescent brain remain understudied. In this study, we investigated how meditation training affects the structure of the adolescent brain by scanning a group of 38 adolescents (16.48 ± 1.29 years) before and after participating in a 12-week meditation training. Subjects underwent Training for Awareness, Resilience, and Action (TARA), a program that mainly incorporates elements from mindfulness meditation and yoga-based practices. A subset of the adolescents also received an additional control scan 12 weeks before TARA. We conducted voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to assess gray matter volume changes pre- to post-training and during the control period. Subjects showed significant gray matter (GM) volume decreases in the left posterior insula and to a lesser extent in the left thalamus and left putamen after meditation training. There were no significant changes during the control period. Our results support previous findings that meditation affects regions associated with physical and emotional awareness. However, our results are different from previous morphometric studies in which meditation was associated with structural increases. We posit that this discrepancy may be due to the differences between the adolescent brain and the adult brain.

Introduction

Meditation is a mind-body practice that has become increasingly popular both recreationally and clinically (Goyal et al., 2014). It can be defined as a form of mental training that aims to improve one’s psychological capabilities and it encompasses multiple practices, most notably mindfulness meditation (Ospina et al., 2008; Tang et al., 2015). Overall, meditation’s extensive health benefits are well-documented. In addition to reducing stress (Grossman et al., 2004), mindfulness meditation has demonstrated efficacy in treating multiple conditions including substance abuse (Carim-Todd et al., 2013), pain management (Cherkin et al., 2016; Nascimento et al., 2018), hypertension (Park et al., 2014), anxiety (Zeidan et al., 2014), depression (Kasala et al., 2014), and offering preventative effects in a healthy population (Wolsko et al., 2004; Barnes et al., 2008). However, the specific neural mechanisms of the practices’ broad range of benefits remain unknown.

Mindfulness meditation has received major attention in neuroscience research. It can be described as “non-judgmental attention to present-moment experiences” (Ospina et al., 2008). A proposed mechanism for the practice is that it works through a combination of enhanced self-regulation, including attention control, emotion regulation, and self-awareness. In general, there are two distinct regulation strategies (Chiesa et al., 2013). The first is “top-down,” where active reinterpretation of stimuli modifies emotional impact (Gross, 1998). The second strategy is “bottom-up,” where emotional regulation is achieved through direct modulation of brain regions associated with emotion-generation (Chambers et al., 2009; Westbrook et al., 2013).

Much of this evidence comes from functional and structural neuroimaging studies, typically employing magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Structural studies often use voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to investigate changes in gray matter (GM; Fox et al., 2014). This well-established method entails a whole-brain structural analysis that examines local changes in gray matter volume (GMV). For a thorough review of morphometric studies of meditation, see Fox et al. (2014).

Morphometric studies of meditation have consistently found GM changes in regions related to the self-regulation mechanism mentioned above (attention control, emotion regulation, and self-awareness). These include the insula (involved in interoception, homeostatic awareness, and emotional awareness; Lazar et al., 2005; Hölzel et al., 2008; Luders et al., 2012), somatomotor cortices (tactile processing, conscious proprioception; Lazar et al., 2005; Grant et al., 2010; Luders et al., 2012; Kang et al., 2013), amygdala (fear, memory, decision-making, and detecting salient events; Pickut et al., 2013; Lu et al., 2014; Gotink et al., 2018), and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; emotional salience and memory; Hölzel et al., 2011; Kang et al., 2013).

There are important questions that remain unanswered. For example, studies typically recruit experienced/expert adult meditators (Fox et al., 2014). Few studies are focusing on novice or meditation-naïve participants, and there are nearly no studies examining meditation’s morphometric effects in adolescents. This is an important knowledge gap because the adolescent brain is quite distinct from the adult brain and meditation effects may differ significantly (Giedd et al., 2015).

Adolescence is a crucial maturational period that begins at the onset of puberty (Dahl et al., 2018). In childhood, brain development is characterized by cortical gray matter growth. Cortical gray matter volume increases rapidly in the first few years of life and peaks in early childhood (Giedd et al., 1999; Shaw et al., 2008). In adolescence, however, this trend reverses. GMV decreases throughout adolescence towards young adulthood, with most regions following an inverted U-shaped trajectory (Shaw et al., 2006, 2008; Giedd et al., 2008). There are regional differences in the timing of this trend, with different structures maturing at different times and different rates (Blakemore, 2012; Wierenga et al., 2014; Narvacan et al., 2017). These unique developmental trajectories suggest that the findings from adult meditation studies may not translate to the adolescent population. Currently, only Friedel et al. (2015) have published structural findings in adolescents related to trait mindfulness. The authors found that trait mindfulness (measured at 19 years) was associated with less cortical thinning in the left anterior insula (AI) between mid- and late adolescence. While furthering our understanding of trait mindfulness in adolescents, the study did not employ any form of meditation training.

In the present study, we examined adolescents without previous meditation experience who underwent Training for Awareness, Resilience, and Action (TARA), a 12-week training program that incorporates elements from mindfulness meditation and yoga-based movements (Henje Blom et al., 2014, 2017). We compared participants’ gray matter volumes immediately before and after the training, as well as before and after a control period without any training.

Materials and Methods

Sample and Training

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the University of California, San Francisco, and all participants in the study provided written informed assent and their parent(s) or legal guardian(s) provided written informed consent following the Declaration of Helsinki. A community sample of 38 adolescent volunteers (16.48 ± 1.29 years, range 13.92–18.99 years, 24F) participated in this longitudinal neuroimaging study. Adolescent participants were recruited using IRB-approved flyers posted in the neighborhood. Exclusion criteria were any mental health conditions preventing effective group participation, such as active psychosis, severe anorexia nervosa, acute, and severe posttraumatic stress disorder, severe self-harm, suicidal ideation, or attempts in the past 3 months, or severe substance use disorder. Additionally, individuals with a diagnosis of intellectual disability or autism spectrum disorder were excluded. Adolescents undergoing current mindfulness training (e.g., mindfulness-based stress reduction, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, or dialectical behavioral therapy) and/or an ongoing meditation and/or yoga practice of >20 min twice a week or more for the past 2 months were also excluded. Lastly, individuals with contraindications for MRI scans such as pregnancy, metallic implants, braces, or cardiac pacemakers were excluded. Given the relevance of TARA to conditions such as major depressive disorder (MDD), anxiety disorder, and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Henje Blom et al., 2014), we did not exclude participants with these disorders. Instead, we performed analyses with and without these participants. See Table 1 for specific demographic details.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information of the participating sample.

As part of the study, all participants underwent the TARA program (Henje Blom et al., 2014, 2017). TARA is a training program that incorporates elements from mindfulness meditation and yoga-based practices, which specifically promotes both bottom-up and top-down strategies to target depression and anxiety symptoms (Henje Blom et al., 2017). Subjects attended 90-min weekly sessions for 12 consecutive weeks. Two TARA-trained facilitators led the group in exercises such as guided breathing practices, yoga-based movement synchronized with breaths, and interoceptive/sensory awareness meditation practices such as body-scans. Sessions also included brief psychoeducational presentations and group discussions. Participants were instructed to continue practicing outside of the weekly session.

MRI Data Acquisition

All 38 subjects underwent two MRI scans: one immediately before the TARA training (“pre”) and one immediately after the training ended (“post”). A subset of 21 subjects had received a third, control MRI scan. This additional scan was acquired 12 weeks before the training commenced. This subset was formed based on the time of study enrollment concerning the MRI and TARA schedule and not based on any other systematic difference between participants.

Each MRI scan was performed using a 3T General Electric MR750 MRI (Waukesha, WI) scanner. The scan included a standard T1-weighted (T1w) IR-SPGR sequence, with TR / TI / TE = 10.2 s / 450 ms / 4.2 s, flip angle = 15°, and 1 mm isotropic resolution. The ASSET acceleration factor was set to 2, with a total scan time of 3 min and 50 s.

VBM Processing, Analysis, and Statistics

The optimized voxel-based morphometry was performed using FSL v5.0.8 tools (Good et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2004). The processing and analysis were not conducted blindly to timepoint. Non-brain tissue was removed from T1w images using bet (Smith, 2002). Each subject’s image was visually inspected for quality. Poorly extracted volumes were manually skull-stripped using a combination of bet and the 3dUnifize and 3dSkullStrip tools from the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI version 18.1.18) suite (Cox, 1996). Images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (Zhang et al., 2001). The GM images were registered to the MNI152 brain at a resolution of 2 × 2 × 2 mm using affine (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002) and non-linear (Andersson et al., 2007a,b) transforms. Thereafter, a study-specific template was created as part of the optimized VBM protocol implemented in FSL to which the GM images were non-linearly reregistered. The registered GM images were modulated by the Jacobian of the warp field to compensate for contraction/enlargement due to the non-linear transformation (Good et al., 2001). This obviated the need to correct for total intracranial volume (Scorzin et al., 2008) and permitted inference on local GM volume differences. Finally, smoothing with a Gaussian kernel [σ = 2 mm ≈ 4.7 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) was performed].

The resulting data were subjected to voxel-wise permutation-based nonparametric methods (Nichols and Holmes, 2002) that corrected for multiple comparisons across space and incorporated threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE; Smith and Nichols, 2009). The tests used a repeated-measures paired t-test design that controlled for gender and included 5,000 permutations. This yielded two TFCE statistical maps, one for each contrast direction (pre > post or pre < post). Each map was thresholded at p = 0.025 (TFCE-corrected for family-wise errors) to account for the two contrast directions. The obtained regions were identified anatomically using the MNI Structural Atlas (Grabner et al., 2006).

To calculate the effect size, we extracted pre- and post-training GMV values for each participant in significant regions. GMV values were obtained from individuals’ modulated and smoothed GM images with fslmeants, using the significant cluster as a binary mask. Cohen’s d was calculated using a pooled standard deviation in R (v3.6.2; R Core Team, 2019) using the effectsize package.

To assess the potential relationship of age to GMV change, we conducted a bivariate Pearson correlation analysis between participants’ ages at the beginning of the training and their GMV changes across the training, also in R.

Control and Unmedicated Analysis

A second control, VBM analysis was conducted with the subset of participants who received the control scan (n = 21, 16.70 ± 1.07 years, 14F). This analysis examined structural changes between the control and pre-training timepoints, and it provided a comparative window of subjects’ typical development that incorporated background factors such as biological development and environmental stressors such as school.

A third, VBM analysis was conducted to compare medicated and unmedicated subjects (n = 32, 16.48 ± 1.33 years, 22F). This analysis examined unmedicated subjects’ changes between pre- and post-training. Both the control and unmedicated analyses used the same processing and statistical tests as the primary VBM analysis.

Results

Data Acquisition

MRI scans were well-tolerated by all participants. Scans were read by a board-certified neuroradiologist. No one had any abnormality and was excluded from the analysis.

Primary VBM Results

The primary VBM analysis identified one cluster that showed significant GMV reduction between pre- and post-training. The cluster extended medially into the left thalamus and left putamen with its peak in the left posterior insula (p = 0.019, d = 0.47). No regions showed significant GMV increase. See Figure 1 and Table 2 for results.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Three-plane view of the region showing significant gray matter (GM) volume decrease after meditation training. The cluster showed the most significant change at the left posterior insula [PI; p ≤ 0.025, threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE)-corrected for family-wise errors]. The cluster also overlapped with the left thalamus and left putamen. The cluster is overlaid over the MNI 2 mm brain template, and coordinates are in MNI standard space. The right-side color bar corresponds to the cluster’s significance (1-p).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Primary VBM results of posterior insula gray matter volume decrease pre- to post-training.

There was no significant association between participants’ age and GMV changes during the training, r(36) = 0.0197, p = 0.907.

Control and Medication Effect Analysis

There were no regions of significant GMV increase or decrease in the control VBM analysis. The VBM analysis comparing medicated with unmedicated subjects showed a trending decrease of GMV at the left posterior insula peak from the primary analysis (p = 0.07). See Table 3 for details.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the control and unmedicated VBM analyses.

Discussion

Overview

Our primary VBM analysis identified a left posterior insula cluster showing significant GMV decrease after meditation training. Insular changes have been reported in previous meditation studies and this region is key for physical and emotional awareness. The cluster also overlapped into the left thalamus and left putamen. The following sections will discuss the regions involved and the potential mechanism of change.

Insula

Structural differences in the insula (insular cortex) are one of the most well-replicated findings amongst morphometric studies of meditation (Lazar et al., 2005; Hölzel et al., 2008; Luders et al., 2012; Murakami et al., 2012; Friedel et al., 2015). The insula is crucial for interoception, that is, one’s sense of one’s internal physiological state (Craig, 2002). It is thought that the insula accomplishes this by receiving homeostatic stimuli and sensations and refining them into higher-level awareness (Craig, 2009). It has three cytoarchitecturally distinct components that work together to carry out this function: the granula posterior insula (PI), the dysgranular middle insula (MidI), and the agranular anterior insula (AI; Gu et al., 2013).

The posterior insula provides the primary interoceptive representation of one’s physiological condition. Bodily stimuli, such as temperature, pain, itch, and respiration, are brought to the posterior insula via lamina I spinothalamic neurons (Craig, 2002; Brooks, 2006; Craig, 2009; Strigo and Craig, 2016). These are sent forward to the AI and during this posterior-to-anterior transition through the MidI, the viscero-somatic information is refined. Evidence suggests that the MidI integrates interoceptive stimuli with other neural inputs (Craig, 2011). This helps create a salient representation of an individual’s homeostatic features. For example, the MidI was associated with both taste perception (Dalenberg et al., 2015) and the coding of a food’s pleasantness (van Rijn et al., 2018). The AI assigns emotional relevance to the incoming homeostatic stimuli, thus underlying emotional awareness (Craig, 2009; Fox et al., 2018). Functionally, the AI is a key node of the Salience Network (SN), the intrinsic functional network that identifies biologically and cognitively relevant events to guide behavior (Seeley et al., 2007; Menon and Uddin, 2010; Menon, 2011).

Our results were in the posterior insula, which differs from previous insular findings which are typically located in the anterior insula (Fox et al., 2014). This distinction is important because the PI and MidI have been associated with objective ratings of stimuli, whereas the AI has been associated with their subjective ratings (Craig, 2002, 2011; Singer et al., 2004). For example, objective changes in temperature stimuli were associated with the PI, but subjective evaluation of the same stimuli was associated with the AI (Craig et al., 2000; Kong et al., 2006; Craig, 2009). The posterior findings may reflect the TARA training’s emphasis on interoceptive physical awareness practices, such as breathing exercises and “body-scan” meditations.

Prior studies with significant insula findings utilized meditation paradigms which explicitly focused on body awareness, posture, breathing, and other physical sensations (Lazar et al., 2005; Hölzel et al., 2008; Kang et al., 2013; Fox et al., 2014; Tang et al., 2015). Thus, the anterior-posterior position of insula findings may correspond to the type of meditation practice applied.

Thalamus

The significant cluster also overlapped with the left thalamus. This is important because of the thalamus’ function and its relationship with the insula. The thalamus is a large collection of nuclei that relays information to the cortex, and it is particularly crucial for the transfer of sensorimotor information (Vertes et al., 2015). The GMV changes in our VBM analysis could reflect this specific function, as participants repeatedly practiced exercises involving sensory awareness and physical movement. Additionally, the thalamus projects lamina I spinothalamic nuclei into the posterior insula (Craig, 2002, 2009). Significant GM changes in a cluster containing both the thalamus and the posterior insula further support an awareness-based mechanism of meditation’s effects.

Previous studies have implicated the thalamus in meditation practice, but these used other imaging modalities such as single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT; Newberg et al., 2001) and functional MRI (fMRI; Lutz et al., 2008). Luders et al. were the only group to have found GM changes in the thalamus associated with meditation. They observed larger right thalamic GMV in long-term meditators (Luders et al., 2009), in contrast to our VBM results of GMV decreases. This discrepancy could be due to differences in the participants’ ages (adults vs. adolescents) and meditation experience (experienced meditators vs. novices).

Putamen

The left PI cluster also overlapped into the left putamen. The putamen is a subcortical structure and is part of the dorsal striatum. It is involved in the refinement and control of motor movement (Balleine et al., 2007). Additionally, it is associated with the reinforcement of learning (Viñas-Guasch and Wu, 2017), reward-related behavior (Packard and Knowlton, 2002), and emotions (Brooks, 2006; Lanciego et al., 2012). Amongst its many connections, the putamen is part of multiple corticostriatal loops and has projections into the thalamus.

Our findings in this region can potentially be explained by the training’s focus on physical movements. In addition to the awareness practices previously mentioned, all sessions included yoga-based movement exercises. Here, subjects initially learned a few movements. These previously-learned movements were repeated each session while new sequences and poses were added. One of the key components of the motor circuit is a corticostriatal projection from the putamen (Lanciego et al., 2012). GMV decreases in the putamen could be due to repeated engagement of motor circuitry to learn these coordinated motor movements.

Multiple fMRI studies have reported changes in putamen with meditation (Baerentsen et al., 2010; Ding et al., 2015; Kirk and Montague, 2015; Hernández et al., 2018). The only morphometric structural findings are reported by Pagnoni and Cekic (2007). They also found GMV changes in the left putamen, although their finding was that controls, not practitioners, showed GMV decline. The authors suggest that meditation provided a neuroprotective effect that halted the decline in their adult practitioners. As our sample consisted of adolescents, meditation’s neuroprotective effect could be manifesting itself in some other manner, such as enhancing the brain’s development.

Gray Matter Volume Decrease

The most surprising result of our VBM analysis was the direction of GMV change. Subjects showed significant decreases in GMV after the meditation training. This contrasts with most structural MRI studies of meditation, in which GM measures increase with practice (Fox et al., 2014). However, findings of morphometric decrease (or those in which changes in controls were larger than in meditators), are not without precedent (Kang et al., 2013; Gotink et al., 2018).

Structural MRI studies of meditation have focused on adult samples. The discrepancy between our results and previous studies’ results could be partly explained by the younger age range of our sample (16.48 ± 1.29 years). It is important to take into consideration the differences between the adolescent brain and the adult brain. Several studies have shown that GM morphometric measures (volume, thickness, etc.) follow an inverted-U shaped trajectory, peaking in early adolescence and then declining towards young adulthood (Giedd et al., 1999; Shaw et al., 2006, 2008; Narvacan et al., 2017). Developmental evidence also indicates that the relationship between GM structure and functional abilities undergoes significant changes throughout the lifespan. A study by Schnack et al. (2015) found that in childhood, thinner cortices were associated with higher intelligence. However, this relationship reversed in young adulthood (~21 years) so strongly that in adulthood (42 years), thicker cortices were associated with higher intelligence (Schnack et al., 2015). Thus, the decreased GMV findings in our study could reflect a regionally-specific increase in maturation during the 12 weeks of TARA-training. While possibly the observed GMV decreases were driven solely by maturation, the lack of GMV decreases during the control period render this unlikely.

In mid- to late adolescence, the brain undergoes global stabilization and refinement that ultimately enhance functioning and efficiency (Dahl et al., 2018). One mechanism is through activity-dependent stabilization and synaptic pruning (Changeux and Danchin, 1976). Functionally, this process is thought to improve neuronal stabilization by solidifying experience-dependent learning, reducing spine density, and improving connection efficiency (Anderson et al., 1995; Dahl et al., 2018). This process is a key component of adolescent maturation. Animal studies have observed higher rates of dendritic spine formation and elimination within the adolescent brain compared to adults (Drzewiecki et al., 2016). Additionally, pruning has been hypothesized to account for the GMV decreases observed in late adolescence (Sowell et al., 2001; Gogtay et al., 2004; Blakemore, 2012).

We speculate that our decreased GMV findings could be explained through this developmental maturation context. Meditation practices could have bolstered the maturation of neuronal connections in physical awareness regions (posterior insula) and regions necessary to carry out and learn such techniques (thalamus and putamen). This refinement may have reduced transient spine formation within the regions, ultimately leading to GMV volume decrease. Though speculative, this mechanism can explain the differences between our GMV findings and those from the meditation studies on adults.

Limitations

This study’s findings should be interpreted in light of its limitations. First, the study was not randomized. However, the study design included a within-subject control: a subset of the adolescent subjects received an additional scan before the pre-training timepoint, allowing us to compare findings to a period where subjects had no training. VBM analysis yielded no significant change during the control period, supporting the conclusion that the primary findings were due to training-related practices. This controlled within-subject design can be potentially more powerful than a between-subject design, as within-subject variability is much smaller than between-subject variability (Poldrack, 2000). Second, the study sample was heterogeneous: nine subjects had a psychiatric diagnosis (six with ADD/ADHD, two with MDD, and one with GAD). Six subjects were receiving medication and due to this small number and the different types of medications, we could not thoroughly examine the effects of medication. We performed an additional VBM analysis excluding participants taking medication to compare overall differences and the findings in the left posterior insula were preserved at a trending level, supporting the primary findings. Third, TARA training is an amalgamation of different practices (i.e., meditation, breathing exercises, and yoga-based practices). Currently, the individual contribution of each component practice is unknown. Future studies are needed to disentangle whether and how much each component contributes to the overall structural effect. Fourth, VBM is a method based on T1-weighted MR images and as such, any tissue property that affects T1 relaxation times (e.g., cell density, cell size, myelination) could affect a voxel’s intensity (Zatorre et al., 2012). For example, cerebral blood flow changes (via caffeine ingestion or sensorimotor task), can cause apparent tissue changes in VBM analysis (Ge et al., 2017). It is important to interpret VBM findings with this limitation in mind. Finally, we did not assess behavioral changes, such as measures of anxiety and depression psychopathology. Additional research is needed to study whether these morphometric changes are reflected in behavioral changes.

Conclusion

Overall, we found significant gray matter volume decreases in adolescents participating in a 12-week training program involving meditation, breathing exercises, and yoga-based movements. The GMV changes were centered on the left posterior insula, a region crucial for interoceptive awareness. Whereas most structural neuroimaging studies have found increased GM measures with meditation practice, we found the reverse: participation in meditation training was associated with structural decreases. The discrepancy could be explained by the unique maturational stage of adolescence and its structural brain changes. These findings help further our understanding of meditation’s effects in the understudied adolescent brain.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the University of California, San Francisco. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

JY, EH, TY, DX, and OT designed the study. JY, EH, DX, and OT collected the data. LS read MRI scans for incidental findings. JY, CC, and OT processed and analyzed the data. JY wrote the manuscript. CC, EH, LS, TY, DX, and OT co-wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH) R21AT009173 and R61AT009864 to OT, TY, and EH; by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (CTSI), National Institutes of Health, through UCSF-CTSI UL1TR001872 to OT and TY; by the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP) SRG-1-141-18 to OT and TY; by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) R01HD072074 to DX and OT; by UCSF Research Evaluation and Allocation Committee (REAC) and J. Jacobson Fund to OT, EH, TY and DX; by the Fahs-Beck Fund for Research and Experimentation at The New York Community Trust to OT; by the Swedish Research Council 350-2012-303 to EH; by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) R01MH085734 and the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (formerly NARSAD) to TY. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or other funding agencies. The funding agencies did not play any role in study design, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the study participants and their parents who made this work possible.

References

Anderson, S. A., Classey, J. D., Condé, F., Lund, J. S., and Lewis, D. A. (1995). Synchronous development of pyramidal neuron dendritic spines and parvalbumin-immunoreactive chandelier neuron axon terminals in layer III of monkey prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 67, 7–22. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00051-j

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2007a). Non-linear optimisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA1. Oxford, UK: University of Oxford FMRIB Centre.

Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2007b). Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation FMRIB technical report TR07JA2. Oxford, UK: FMRIB Analysis Group of the University of Oxford, 2. doi: 10.14711/thesis-b1115909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baerentsen, K. B., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Sommerlund, B., Hartmann, T., Damsgaard-Madsen, J., Fosnaes, M., et al. (2010). An investigation of brain processes supporting meditation. Cogn. Process. 11, 57–84. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0342-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., Delgado, M. R., and Hikosaka, O. (2007). The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. J. Neurosci. 27, 8161–8165. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1554-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, P. M., Bloom, B., and Nahin, R. L. (2008). Complementary and alternative medicine use among adults and children: united states, 2007. Natl. Health. Stat. Report. 12, 1–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. NeuroImage 61, 397–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, D. J. (2006). Dopaminergic action beyond its effects on motor function: imaging studies. J. Neurol. 253, IV8–IV15. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-4003-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carim-Todd, L., Mitchell, S. H., and Oken, B. S. (2013). Mind-body practices: an alternative, drug-free treatment for smoking cessation? A systematic review of the literature. Drug Alcohol Depend. 132, 399–410. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, R., Gullone, E., and Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: an integrative review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 560–572. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Changeux, J. P., and Danchin, A. (1976). Selective stabilisation of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks. Nature 264, 705–712. doi: 10.1038/264705a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherkin, D. C., Sherman, K. J., Balderson, B. H., Cook, A. J., Anderson, M. L., Hawkes, R. J., et al. (2016). Effect of mindfulness-based stress reduction vs. cognitive behavioral therapy or usual care on back pain and functional limitations in adults with chronic low back pain: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA 315, 1240–1249. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.2323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiesa, A., Serretti, A., and Jakobsen, J. C. (2013). Mindfulness: top-down or bottom-up emotion regulation strategy? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 82–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D., Chen, K., Bandy, D., and Reiman, E. M. (2000). Thermosensory activation of insular cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 184–190. doi: 10.1038/72131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2011). Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1225, 72–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L., and Suleiman, A. B. (2018). Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature 554, 441–450. doi: 10.1038/nature25770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalenberg, J. R., Hoogeveen, H. R., Renken, R. J., Langers, D. R. M., and ter Horst, G. J. (2015). Functional specialization of the male insula during taste perception. NeuroImage 119, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, X., Tang, Y.-Y., Cao, C., Deng, Y., Wang, Y., Xin, X., et al. (2015). Short-term meditation modulates brain activity of insight evoked with solution cue. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 43–49. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drzewiecki, C. M., Willing, J., and Juraska, J. M. (2016). Synaptic number changes in the medial prefrontal cortex across adolescence in male and female rats: a role for pubertal onset. Synapse 70, 361–368. doi: 10.1002/syn.21909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K. C. R., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Mills, C., Dixon, M. L., Markovic, J., Thompson, E., et al. (2018). Affective neuroscience of self-generated thought. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1426, 25–51. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K. C. R., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., et al. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 48–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedel, S., Whittle, S. L., Vijayakumar, N., Simmons, J. G., Byrne, M. L., Schwartz, O. S., et al. (2015). Dispositional mindfulness is predicted by structural development of the insula during late adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, Q., Peng, W., Zhang, J., Weng, X., Zhang, Y., Liu, T., et al. (2017). Short-term apparent brain tissue changes are contributed by cerebral blood flow alterations. PLoS One 12:e0182182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Lenroot, R. K., Shaw, P., Lalonde, F., Celano, M., White, S., et al. (2008). Trajectories of anatomic brain development as a phenotype. Novartis Found. Symp. 289, 101–112. doi: 10.1002/9780470751251.ch9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Raznahan, A., Alexander-Bloch, A., Schmitt, E., Gogtay, N., and Rapoport, J. L. (2015). Child psychiatry branch of the national institute of mental health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 43–49. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 101, 8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage 14, 21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotink, R. A., Vernooij, M. W., Ikram, M. A., Niessen, W. J., Krestin, G. P., Hofman, A., et al. (2018). Meditation and yoga practice are associated with smaller right amygdala volume: the rotterdam study. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 1631–1639. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9826-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M. S., Gould, N. F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., et al. (2014). Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern. Med. 174, 357–368. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabner, G., Janke, A. L., Budge, M. M., Smith, D., Pruessner, J., and Collins, D. L. (2006). Symmetric atlasing and model based segmentation: an application to the hippocampus in older adults. Med. Image Comput. Comput. Assist. Interv. 9, 58–66. doi: 10.1007/11866763_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., Duerden, E. G., Duncan, G. H., and Rainville, P. (2010). Cortical thickness and pain sensitivity in zen meditators. Emotion 10, 43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0018334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., and Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, X., Hof, P. R., Friston, K. J., and Fan, J. (2013). Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 3371–3388. doi: 10.1002/cne.23368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., et al. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain grey matter density. Psychiatry Res. 191, 36–43. doi: 10.3410/f.9465956.10108054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hölzel, B. K., Ott, U., Gard, T., Hempel, H., Weygandt, M., Morgen, K., et al. (2008). Investigation of mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphometry. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 55–61. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henje Blom, E., Duncan, L. G., Ho, T. C., Connolly, C. G., LeWinn, K. Z., Chesney, M., et al. (2014). The development of an RDoC-based treatment program for adolescent depression: “Training for awareness, resilience and action” (TARA). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:630. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henje Blom, E., Tymofiyeva, O., Chesney, M. A., Ho, T. C., Moran, P., Connolly, C. G., et al. (2017). Feasibility and preliminary efficacy of a novel RDoC-based treatment program for adolescent depression: “Training for awareness resilience and action” (TARA)—a pilot study. Front. Psychiatry 7:208. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández, S. E., Barros-Loscertales, A., Xiao, Y., González-Mora, J. L., and Rubia, K. (2018). Gray matter and functional connectivity in anterior cingulate cortex are associated with the state of mental silence during sahaja yoga meditation. Neuroscience 371, 395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2001). A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 5, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, D.-H., Jo, H. J., Jung, W. H., Kim, S. H., Jung, Y.-H., Choi, C.-H., et al. (2013). The effect of meditation on brain structure: cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 27–33. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasala, E. R., Bodduluru, L. N., Maneti, Y., and Thipparaboina, R. (2014). Effect of meditation on neurophysiological changes in stress mediated depression. Complement. Ther. Clin. Pract. 20, 74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2013.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, U., and Montague, P. R. (2015). Mindfulness meditation modulates reward prediction errors in a passive conditioning task. Front. Psychol. 6:90. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, J., White, N. S., Kwong, K. K., Vangel, M. G., Rosman, I. S., Gracely, R. H., et al. (2006). Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 715–721.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lanciego, J. L., Luquin, N., and Obeso, J. A. (2012). Functional neuroanatomy of the basal ganglia. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2:a009621. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a009621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., et al. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport 16, 1893–1897. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, H., Song, Y., Xu, M., Wang, X., Li, X., and Liu, J. (2014). The brain structure correlates of individual differences in trait mindfulness: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroscience 272, 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luders, E., Kurth, F., Mayer, E. A., Toga, A. W., Narr, K. L., and Gaser, C. (2012). The unique brain anatomy of meditation practitioners: alterations in cortical gyrification. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:34. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luders, E., Toga, A. W., Lepore, N., and Gaser, C. (2009). The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of grey matter. NeuroImage 45, 672–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murakami, H., Nakao, T., Matsunaga, M., Kasuya, Y., Shinoda, J., Yamada, J., et al. (2012). The structure of mindful brain. PLoS One 7:e46377. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narvacan, K., Treit, S., Camicioli, R., Martin, W., and Beaulieu, C. (2017). Evolution of deep grey matter volume across the human lifespan. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 3771–3790. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nascimento, S. S., Oliveira, L. R., and DeSantana, J. M. (2018). Correlations between brain changes and pain management after cognitive and meditative therapies: a systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Complement. Ther. Med. 39, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2018.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., and d’Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Res. 106, 113–122. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(01)00074-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T. E., and Holmes, A. P. (2002). Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ospina, M. B., Bond, K., Karkhaneh, M., Buscemi, N., Dryden, D. M., Barnes, V., et al. (2008). Clinical trials of meditation practices in health care: characteristics and quality. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 14, 1199–1213. doi: 10.1089/acm.2008.0307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Packard, M. G., and Knowlton, B. J. (2002). Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 563–593. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagnoni, G., and Cekic, M. (2007). Age effects on grey matter volume and attentional performance in zen meditation. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 1623–1627. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J., Lyles, R. H., and Bauer-Wu, S. (2014). Mindfulness meditation lowers muscle sympathetic nerve activity and blood pressure in african-american males with chronic kidney disease. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 307, R93–R101. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00558.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickut, B. A., Van Hecke, W., Kerckhofs, E., Mariën, P., Vanneste, S., Cras, P., et al. (2013). Mindfulness based intervention in parkinson’s disease leads to structural brain changes on MRI: a randomized controlled longitudinal trial. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 115, 2419–2425. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A. (2000). Imaging brain plasticity: conceptual and methodological issues— a theoretical review. NeuroImage 12, 1–13. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team. (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/.

Google Scholar

Schnack, H. G., Haren, V., E.m, N., Brouwer, R. M., Evans, A., Durston, S., et al. (2015). Changes in thickness and surface area of the human cortex and their relationship with intelligence. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1608–1617. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scorzin, J. E., Kaaden, S., Quesada, C. M., Müller, C.-A., Fimmers, R., Urbach, H., et al. (2008). Volume determination of amygdala and hippocampus at 1.5 and 3.0T MRI in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. 82, 29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2008.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., et al. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature 440, 676–679. doi: 10.1038/nature04513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Kabani, N. J., Lerch, J. P., Eckstrand, K., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., et al. (2008). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage 44, 83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23, S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Tessner, K. D., and Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping continued brain growth and grey matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. J. Neurosci. 21, 8819–8829. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.21-22-08819.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strigo, I. A., and Craig, A. D. (2016). Interoception, homeostatic emotions and sympathovagal balance. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160010. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y.-Y., Hölzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Rijn, I., de Graaf, C. and Smeets, P. A. M. (2018). It’s in the eye of the beholder: selective attention to drink properties during tasting influences brain activation in gustatory and reward regions. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 425–436. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9710-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vertes, R. P., Linley, S. B., and Hoover, W. B. (2015). Limbic circuitry of the midline thalamus. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 54, 89–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viñas-Guasch, N., and Wu, Y. J. (2017). The role of the putamen in language: a meta-analytic connectivity modeling study. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 3991–4004. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1450-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westbrook, C., Creswell, J. D., Tabibnia, G., Julson, E., Kober, H., and Tindle, H. A. (2013). Mindful attention reduces neural and self-reported cue-induced craving in smokers. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 73–84. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wierenga, L., Langen, M., Ambrosino, S., van Dijk, S., Oranje, B., and Durston, S. (2014). Typical development of basal ganglia, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellum from age 7 to 24. NeuroImage 96, 67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolsko, P. M., Eisenberg, D. M., Davis, R. B., and Phillips, R. S. (2004). Use of mind-body medical therapies. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 19, 43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.21019.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Fields, R. D., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2012). Plasticity in grey and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 528–536. doi: 10.1038/nn.3045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeidan, F., Martucci, K. T., Kraft, R. A., McHaffie, J. G., and Coghill, R. C. (2014). Neural correlates of mindfulness meditation-related anxiety relief. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 751–759. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2001). Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 20, 45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: meditation, adolescent brain, gray matter, voxel-based morphometry, MRI

Citation: Yuan JP, Connolly CG, Henje E, Sugrue LP, Yang TT, Xu D and Tymofiyeva O (2020) Gray Matter Changes in Adolescents Participating in a Meditation Training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:319. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00319

Received: 19 February 2020; Accepted: 17 July 2020;
Published: 14 August 2020.

Edited by:

Filippo Brighina, University of Palermo, Italy

Reviewed by:

Claudia Altamura, Campus Bio-Medico University, Italy
Rui Ferreira Afonso, Hospital Israelita Albert Einstein, Brazil

Copyright © 2020 Yuan, Connolly, Henje, Sugrue, Yang, Xu and Tymofiyeva. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Olga Tymofiyeva, Olga.Tymofiyeva@ucsf.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.