Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 July 2020
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation

Abnormal Contingent Negative Variation Drifts During Facial Expression Judgment in Schizophrenia Patients

\r\nQian Wang*Qian Wang1*Shenglin SheShenglin She2Lu LuoLu Luo3Haijing LiHaijing Li2Yuping NingYuping Ning2Jianjuan RenJianjuan Ren2Zhangying WuZhangying Wu2Rongcheng HuangRongcheng Huang2Yingjun ZhengYingjun Zheng2
  • 1Department of Clinical Psychology, Sanbo Brain Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
  • 3School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China

Schizophrenia patients often show impaired facial expression recognition, which leads to difficulties in adaptation to daily life. However, it remains unclear whether the deficit is at the perceptual or higher cognitive level of facial emotion processing. Recent studies have shown that earlier face-evoked event-related potential (ERP) components such as N170 and P100 can effectively distinguish schizophrenia patients from healthy controls; however, findings for later waveforms are ambiguous. To clarify this point, in this study we compared electroencephalographic signals in schizophrenia patients and control subjects during a facial expression judgment task. We found that group effects of the occipital N170 and frontal lobe contingent negative variation (CNV) were both significant. The effect sizes (ESs) of N170 and CNV amplitudes were generally medium or small, whereas that of CNV slope for an upright face was large (>0.8). Moreover, N170 amplitude and CNV slope but not CNV amplitude was correlated with Personal and Social Performance (PSP) Scale score. These results suggest that the slope of CNV drift during facial expression processing has a potential clinical value for schizophrenia.

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder characterized by social cognition deficits at the behavioral and neural levels (Onitsuka et al., 2013; Savla et al., 2013). One of the most prominent aspects of social cognition is the recognition of different facial expressions (Kohler et al., 2009; Savla et al., 2013). The neural mechanisms underlying this process have been investigated by neuroimaging and electroencephalography (EEG; Kohler et al., 2009; McCleery et al., 2015; Earls et al., 2016). Impaired social cognition in schizophrenia patients includes a failure to perceive social cues and the inability to perceive or share emotional experiences (reviewed by Green et al., 2015). Most EEG studies to date have focused on early-stage (P100 and N170) deficits in schizophrenia (Herrmann et al., 2004; Onitsuka et al., 2006; Tsunoda et al., 2012; Maher et al., 2016; Zheng et al., 2016). However, neuroimaging data suggest that in addition to the visual system, higher-order brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex are abnormally activated in schizophrenia patients (Li et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2012; Delvecchio et al., 2013), suggesting that there is a late event-related potential (ERP) component that distinguishes these individuals from healthy subjects.

To investigate this possibility, previous face processing ERP studies in schizophrenia focused on the group effects of N170 and N250 (Streit et al., 2001; Turetsky et al., 2007; Wynn et al., 2008, 2013; Lee et al., 2010; Jung et al., 2012). The occipitotemporal N170 waveform is observed in response to face stimuli, which showed lower amplitude and longer latency in schizophrenia patients (Zheng et al., 2016). The frontal-central N250, which could be modulated by the facial expressions, familiarity, and repetitions, also showed lower amplitude in schizophrenia patients (Streit et al., 2001; Wynn et al., 2008, 2013). A recent meta-analysis study systematically compared the effect sizes (ESs) of the N170 and N250 between schizophrenia patients and healthy controls and showed no significant difference (McCleery et al., 2015).

Contingent negative variation (CNV) is a slow negative potential that is presumed to reflect prefrontal functioning in top-down modulation (Walter et al., 1964). CNV deficits (decreased amplitude) preceding motor response has been reported in schizophrenia patients (Klein et al., 2000) and is thought to be associated with the frontal lobe hypometabolism observed in regional cerebral blood flow studies during the execution of tasks sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction (Paulman et al., 1990; Frith et al., 1991; Andreasen et al., 1992). A recent study revealed that the emotional face evoked CNV amplitudes were lower in schizophrenia patients (Zhang et al., 2016). However, whether facial expression-related CNV could act as a better ERP index in distinguishing schizophrenia patients from healthy controls.

Schizophrenic patients also showed difficulty in processing face holistically (Shin et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2010; Bauser et al., 2012; Megreya, 2016). That defects could be measured by the face inversion effect (FIE), which is observed as a reduction in face discrimination performance for inverted faces compared to upright faces (Yin, 1969, 1970; Bauser et al., 2012). Previous studies suggested that the FIE of N170 is significantly reduced in people with schizophrenia (Tsunoda et al., 2012; Zheng et al., 2016). However, whether the FIE effect of late ERP components such as CNV exits defects in schizophrenic patients is still open to be explored.

Clinical symptoms of schizophrenia include positive symptoms such as hallucination and delusions and negative symptoms such as apathy and avolition; these are evaluated with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). There is increasing awareness that quality of life is important for individuals with mental illness. The Personal and Social Performance (PSP) scale—which measures social functioning in socially useful activities, personal and social relations, self-care, and disturbing and aggressive behavior—was developed based on the Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (Goldman et al., 1992) and is highly reliable for evaluating patients with schizophrenia. However, few studies have investigated the relationship between electrophysiological indices of facial expression processing and PSP scores.

This was addressed in the current study by EEG recordings using a 3 (facial expression: neutral/happy/sad) × 2 (face orientation: upright/inverted) design to simultaneously compare the group effects of N170 and CNV during an expression judgment task in schizophrenia patients and healthy control subjects. ESs and their correlations with PSP scale scores were calculated to determine whether ERP components are useful for schizophrenia evaluation.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 20 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (10 females, mean age: 34.0 ± 7.2 years old) and 20 age-matched healthy control subjects (10 females, mean age: 32.4 ± 8.4 years old) were recruited for this study. All of the participants had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of medical or neurological disorders. Psychiatric symptoms were evaluated by a trained psychiatrist or psychologist according to the PANSS (Kay et al., 1987). The PSP scale was used to assess participants’ social functioning. Basic information on the study population is shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical and demographic characteristics of schizophrenia patients and healthy control subjects.

There was no history of any major psychiatric disorders or physical illnesses or use of medication known to affect the central nervous system among healthy controls. The exclusion criteria for both groups were obvious abnormalities by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), neurological illness, traumatic brain injury, and substance use or addiction. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Guangzhou Brain Hospital. All participants provided written, informed consent, and were remunerated for their participation.

Stimuli and Procedure

In total, 54 different schematic faces with a neutral, sad, or happy expression were used as visual stimuli, with 18 individual schematic faces included for each stimulus type (Figure 1; She et al., 2017). Thus, each stimulus type included 18 models that were varied by altering the distance between the shapes of the facial features. The stimuli were presented at the fixation point with a visual angle of 7.27° × 6.06°.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Examples of schematic face stimuli consisting of three facial expressions (happy, neutral, and sad) in the upright and inverted orientations.

During the EEG recording, participants were seated in a dimly lit room in a comfortable chair and were instructed to classify the facial expressions that were presented on the screen as neutral, happy, or sad, and to respond by pressing one of three different keys on the keyboard. The labels on the response buttons were balanced across participants. Each face was presented for 300 ms, with an inter-trial interval ranging randomly from 600 to 800 ms after the response. All 324 faces (three facial expressions, 108 faces for each) were randomly presented in three blocks. The participants first completed a practice sequence of 18 stimuli (six for each expression) that were not included in the analysis. Response accuracy and reaction time (RT) were recorded.

Electrophysiological Recordings

Continuous EEG recording was performed with a set of 16 Ag/AgCl electrodes placed according to the 10/20 system. The EEG recording sites were F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, PO7, PO8, O1, Oz, and O2. Electrooculography (EOG) was performed via electrodes placed on the bilateral external canthi and left infraorbital and supraorbital areas to monitor eye movements and blinking. Both EEG and EOG were sampled at 1,000 Hz with a 0.1–100 Hz band-pass using a Neuroscan NuAmps digital amplifier system (Neuroscan Labs, El Paso, TX, USA). The tip of the nose was used as the reference during recording, and approximate zero references was determined offline according to the reference electrode standardization technique1 (Yao, 2001). Electrode impedance was maintained below 5 kΩ.

Data Analysis

Electrophysiological data were pre-processed with the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) in the MATLAB environment. Long-term EEG signals were first filtered with a band-pass filter (0.5–40 Hz) and then segmented into epochs from −200 to 1,400 ms around the onset. To avoid the influence of hand reaction on electromyography, the time window of interest was then limited to −100 to 500 ms and the baseline correction was performed in the time window of −100 to 0 ms. Epochs containing potentials greater than ±1 mV were rejected as artifacts; the remaining epochs were then averaged to obtain an ERP for each electrode node and then low-pass filtered at 15 Hz.

In the individual level, mean amplitudes for N170 (160–180 ms) and CNV (200–400 ms) components were calculated. To calculate the slope of CNV, the ERP waveforms from 200–400 ms were first linear fit using the polyfit function in MATLAB environment. For each condition, the slope of the best fit function was taken as the CNV slope.

Statistical analyses including analysis of variance (ANOVA), the t-test, and Pearson’s correlation analysis were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). ESs were calculated with parietal η2 and Cohen’s d. P-values were corrected with the Bonferroni adjustment to avoid multiple comparisons. The null hypothesis rejection level was set at 0.05.

Results

Behavioral Performance

Behavioral performance was assessed for response accuracy and RT by 2 (group: patient/control) × 3 (facial expression: neutral/happy/sad) × 2 (face orientation: upright/inverted) three-way mixed-design ANOVA.

In the response accuracy analysis, only the main effect of group (F (1,38) = 10.188, p = 0.003, partial η2 = 0.211) was significant. None of the other main and interaction effects were significant (all p > 0.05). A post hoc Bonferroni test showed no significance under facial expression conditions in the control group (Figure 2C). However, in the patient group, a happy or sad expression was detected less accurately than a neutral face (all corrected p < 0.05). The results of an independent samples t-test between groups (Figure 2A) showed that control subjects had higher response accuracy than patients (all p < 0.05, all Cohen’s d > 0.8), except under the inverted happy condition (t(1,38) = 1.856, p = 0.071, Cohen’s d = 0.587).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Performance in facial expression discrimination. (A,B) Response accuracy was higher (A) whereas reaction time (RT) was shorter (B) in control subjects than in schizophrenia patients. (C,D) Evaluation of differences among the six conditions. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

In the RT analysis, the main effects of group (F(1,38) = 29.131, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.434) and facial expression (F(2,76) = 11.541, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.233) were significant. No other main or interaction effects were significant (all p > 0.05). A post hoc Bonferroni test showed that in both groups, RT was longer for detecting a happy or sad expression compared with a neutral expression (all corrected p < 0.01; Figure 2D). On the other hand, both groups had a shorter RT to detect a happy expression in an upright vs. an inverted face (both p < 0.01). The results of an independent-sample t-test between groups showed that control subjects had shorter RTs than patients (all p < 0.001, all Cohen’s d > 1.2; Figure 2B). These results indicate that compared with healthy control subjects, schizophrenia patients react more slowly and are more error-prone during the facial expression judgment task, suggesting an impairment in emotion processing.

Main Effects of CNV and N170

The distributions of CNV (200–400 ms; Figure 3A) and N170 (160–180 ms; Figure 4A) were centered in the central-frontal and right temporo-occipital areas, respectively. Thus, electrodes Fz and PO8 were selected for three-way mixed-measure ANOVA for CNV and N170, respectively.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparison of contingent negative variation (CNV) topographic distribution in control and patient groups. (A,B) Topographic distribution of CNV mean amplitude (200–400 ms; A) and waveform of facial expression-evoked CNV (Fz electrode; B) under six conditions in control subjects (left panels) and schizophrenia patients (right panels). (C,D) Average CNV amplitude (C) and slope (D) under six conditions in the two groups, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparison of N170 topographic distribution in schizophrenia patients and control subjects. (A,B) Topographic distribution of N170 mean amplitude (160–180 ms; A) and waveform of facial expression-evoked N170 (PO8 electrode; B) under six conditions in control subjects (left panels) and schizophrenia patients (right panels). (C) Average N170 amplitude under six conditions in the two groups, *p < 0.05.

CNV waveforms in the frontal lobe differed under the six experimental conditions in each group (Figure 3B). Furthermore, the CNV waveform had a lower amplitude and steeper slope in patients than in controls.

In the CNV amplitude analysis, the main effects of group (F(1,38) = 4.222, p = 0.047, partial η2 = 0.100), facial expression (F(2,76) = 3.496, p = 0.035, partial η2 = 0.084), and inversion (F(1,38) = 16.896, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.308) were all significant. No interaction effect was found (all p > 0.05). The results of the post hoc Bonferroni test showed that in the control group, the CNV evoked by an upright face was larger than those evoked by an inverted face (all corrected p < 0.001), whereas the same inversion effect was also found in the patient group (all corrected p < 0.05). Facial expression was non-significant in both groups.

In the CNV slope analysis, the main effects of group (F(1,38) = 9.805, p = 0.003, partial η2 = 0.205) and inversion (F(1,38) = 6.213, p = 0.017, partial η2 = 0.141) were significant, but the main effect of facial expression was not (F(2,76) = 0.049, p = 0.953, partial η2 = 0.001). No interaction effect was found (all p > 0.05). The results of the post hoc Bonferroni test showed that in the control group, the slope of CNV evoked by an upright face was smaller than that evoked by inverted face for all three facial expressions (all corrected p < 0.001), whereas in the patient group, the inversion effect was only observed under the neutral condition (corrected p = 0.014) and not under the happy or sad condition (both corrected p > 0.1). No significance was found for facial expression in either group.

In the N170 amplitude analysis, only the main effect of group (F(1,38) = 4.705, p = 0.036, partial η2 = 0.110) was significant; other main and interaction effects were non-significant (all p > 0.05). The results of the post hoc Bonferroni test found no significant effects (both corrected p > 0.05).

In summary, although the main effects of each group were significant for the three ERP indices, the ES of CNV slope was larger than those of CNV and N170 amplitude.

ESs of CNV Amplitude and Slope and N170 Amplitude

Independent t-tests were performed for the three ERP indices to estimate the between-group difference under each condition. For CNV amplitude, inter-group difference was only significant under upright neutral (t(38) = 2.522, p = 0.016, Cohen’s d = 0.789), upright happy (t(38) = 2.191, p = 0.035, Cohen’s d = 0.693), and inverted neutral (t(38) = 2.047, p = 0.048, Cohen’s d = 0.647) conditions with medium ESs (Figure 3C). For CNV slope, the difference was significant under upright neutral (t(38) = 3.781, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.196), upright happy (t(38) = 2.857, p = 0.007, Cohen’s d = 0.904), and upright sad (t(38) = 3.020, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.955) conditions with large ESs and under inverted neutral (t(38) = 2.397, p = 0.022, Cohen’s d = 0.758), inverted happy (t(38) = 2.406, p = 0.021, Cohen’s d = 0.761), and inverted sad (t(38) = 2.184, p = 0.035, Cohen’s d = 0.691) conditions with medium ESs (Figure 3D). For N170 amplitude, the inter-group difference was only significant under upright happy (t(38) = 2.238, p = 0.031, Cohen’s d = 0.797), inverted neutral (t(38) = 2.12, p = 0.041, Cohen’s d = 0.731), and inverted happy (t(38) = 2.306, p = 0.027, Cohen’s d = 0.764) conditions with medium ESs (Figure 4). A comparison of ESs of the three ERP indices under each condition revealed that only the CNV slope under the upright face condition had a large ES (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Comparison of effect size (ES; Cohen’s d) for CNV slope, CNV amplitude, and N170 amplitude between schizophrenia patients and control subjects. Cohen’s d > 0.2, small effect; d > 0.5, medium effect; d > 0.8, large effect.

ERP Indices Correlated With Schizophrenia Symptoms and PSP Scores

Pearson correlations between ERP indices and positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and PSP scores were calculated. The N170 amplitude was positively correlated with positive symptoms (Table 2), whereas N170 amplitude and CNV slope but not CNV amplitude was negatively correlated with PSP score (all p < 0.05). These results indicate that both N170 amplitude and CNV slope predict social performance in schizophrenia patients and distinguishes them from healthy controls.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson correlation coefficients between event-related potential (ERP) indices and clinical evaluation scores.

CNV Indices Correlated With Behavioral Indices

Pearson correlations between CNV indices (slope and amplitude) and behavioral indices (RT and accuracy) were calculated. As shown in Figure 6A, no significance was found between CNV slope and accuracy, which might be still limited by the accuracy variation. No significant correlation was found between CNV slopes and RTs (Figure 6B). No significance was found between CNV amplitude and behavioral indices (Table 3). Together, these results failed to reveal any meaningful relationship between CNV and behavioral performance.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Pearson correlation measurements between CNV slopes and (A) response accuracy and (B) RT under six conditions. The yellow triangle represents the outlier point.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients between contingent negative variation (CNV) indices and behavioral indices.

Discussion

The dysfunction in affective face processing in schizophrenia patients has been widely explored in previous studies using behavioral and neurophysiological approaches (Kohler et al., 2009; Li et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2012; Delvecchio et al., 2013; Savla et al., 2013; McCleery et al., 2015); however, only a few have simultaneously compared the perceptual and cognitive ERP components evoked by task-relevant facial expressions between schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects (reviewed by McCleery et al., 2015). In the current study, we measured early and late ERP components during a facial expression detection task and found the following: (1) schizophrenia patients exhibited abnormalities in early (N170) and late (CNV) facial expression processing; (2) N170 amplitude and CNV slope were correlated with social performance; and (3) only the CNV slope under the upright face condition had a large ES (>0.8).

Face perception is one of the most important social cues in daily life and is impaired in individuals with schizophrenia (Green et al., 2015). Following previous findings (Kohler et al., 2009; Savla et al., 2013), in the current study schizophrenia patients performed poorly in the identification of facial expressions, reacting more slowly and making more errors in the facial expression judgment task.

The neural correlates of schizophrenia-related deficits have been extensively studied by functional fMRI and ERP analysis (Li et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2012; Delvecchio et al., 2013; McCleery et al., 2015) fMRI studies have revealed that facial expression processing is correlated not only with activation of association cortices but also with altered activation of high-order limbic-cortical circuits, frontal cortex, and putamen (Haxby et al., 2002; Vuilleumier and Pourtois, 2007; Sabatinelli et al., 2011). On the other hand, ERP studies with high temporal resolution have revealed consistent and significant impairment in both early (P100 and N170) and late (N250 and CNV) visual ERP amplitudes in schizophrenia (McCleery et al., 2015; Earls et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2016). In the current study, significant group effects were observed not only for N170 amplitude but also for CNV amplitude and slope, suggesting aberrant hierarchical processing in individuals with schizophrenia. However, it is unclear whether there is greater impairment in the earlier or later stage of affective face processing. Meta-analyses have demonstrated that ES estimates reveal a smaller degree of impairment at the neural level (P100, ES = 0.41; N170, ES = 0.64; N250, ES = 0.49; McCleery et al., 2015; Earls et al., 2016) than that observed in emotion-based tasks (ES = 0.89; Savla et al., 2013). In the current study, we compared parietal η2 values of the group main effect in the ANOVA to estimate whether these can distinguish schizophrenia patients from healthy controls and determined that parietal η2 values of N170 and CNV amplitudes (both approximately 0.1) were lower than those of accuracy (parietal η2 = 0.211) and RT (parietal η2 = 0.434), whereas only the ES of the CNV slope was comparable to the behavioral indices (parietal η2 = 0.205). A recent ERP study of facial expression in schizophrenia showed that the parietal η2 of the group effect of CNV amplitude was markedly smaller than those of P100 and N170 amplitudes (Zhang et al., 2016); however, these investigators did not examine the slope the CNV evoked by facial affect.

Cohen’s d under each stimulation condition was further compared across N170 and CNV amplitudes and CNV slope. We found that Cohen’s d values of N170 and CNV amplitudes ranged from 0.389–0.798 (small to medium ESs), whereas that of CNV slope under the upright face condition were all larger than 0.8 (large ESs; Figure 5). On the other hand, the d value of the CNV slope under the inverted face condition ranged from 0.691–0.761. These results explain why the amplitude of the late ERP component has an ES similar to those of early components such as N170 (McCleery et al., 2015), and suggest that slow drifts in CNV are a characteristic of schizophrenia.

In this study, we examined N170 originating in the temporo-occipital lobe based on the CNV waveform detected by frontal lobe electrodes. Previous research has shown that schizophrenia patients exhibit biochemical, molecular, and physiological changes in the frontal lobe (Johnston-Wilson et al., 2000; Jaffe et al., 2016). Neuroimaging studies have revealed that in addition to abnormal or lack of activation in the frontal cortex during an emotion judgment task (Takahashi et al., 2004; Ferrarelli et al., 2008; Minzenberg et al., 2009; Ursu et al., 2011), a disconnect between the frontal and other cortices is observed in schizophrenia (Ellison-Wright and Bullmore, 2009; Bjorkquist et al., 2016).

Given that slow CNV drifts reflect low-frequency EEG components related to top-down processing (Chennu et al., 2013), the change in CNV slope suggests a deficit in the high-order cognitive processing in schizophrenia patients. As shown in Figure 5, the ESs of CNV slopes were generally larger under upright face conditions, rather than inverted face conditions. On the other hand, the behavioral performance was also correlated with CNV slopes were generally larger under upright face conditions, but not under inverted face conditions. Therefore, the deficits of CNV slopes might be the neural correlates which interpret the abnormal social communications in schizophrenia patients.

Limitations of this study were the small sample sizes and the absence of conditions without facial expression judgment, which made it difficult to distinguish whether the CNV slope is involved in emotion analysis, movement initiation, or executive control. Moreover, it remains unclear whether the observed change in CNV slope is specific to schizophrenia or is a general feature of mental disorders. This question will be addressed in future studies.

In conclusion, although meta-analyses of EEG studies of facial expression processing in schizophrenia have shown consistent results for early-stage components, the utility of late components for discriminating schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects is unknown. Our findings reveal that the slope of CNV originating in the frontal lobe has clinical value for individuals with schizophrenia, indicating that late ERP components warrant closer examination.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of Guangzhou Brain Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

QW, SS, LL, HL, YN, JR, ZW, RH, and YZ designed and wrote the protocol for the study. QW, SS, and HL performed the experiments. QW and LL analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 31700994).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.neuro.uestc.edu.cn/rest

References

Andreasen, N. C., Rezai, K., Alliger, R., Swayze, V. W. II., Flaum, M., Kirchner, P., et al. (1992). Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 943–958. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820120031006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauser, D. S., Thoma, P., Aizenberg, V., Brüne, M., Juckel, G., and Daum, I. (2012). Face and body perception in schizophrenia: a configural processing deficit? Psychiatry Res. 195, 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorkquist, O. A., Olsen, E. K., Nelson, B. D., and Herbener, E. S. (2016). Altered amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during emotion perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 175, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chennu, S., Noreika, V., Gueorguiev, D., Blenkmann, A., Kochen, S., Ibanez, A., et al. (2013). Expectation and attention in hierarchical auditory prediction. J. Neurosci. 33, 11194–11205. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0114-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delvecchio, G., Sugranyes, G., and Frangou, S. (2013). Evidence of diagnostic specificity in the neural correlates of facial affect processing in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Psychol. Med. 43, 553–569. doi: 10.1017/s0033291712001432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Earls, H. A., Curran, T., and Mittal, V. (2016). A meta-analytic review of auditory event-related potential components as endophenotypes for schizophrenia: perspectives from first-degree relatives. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 519–527. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellison-Wright, I., and Bullmore, E. (2009). Meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 108, 3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrarelli, F., Massimini, M., Peterson, M. J., Riedner, B. A., Lazar, M., Murphy, M. J., et al. (2008). Reduced evoked gamma oscillations in the frontal cortex in schizophrenia patients: a TMS/EEG study. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 996–1005. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., and Frackowiak, R. S. (1991). Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET. Proc. Soc. Sci. 244, 241–246. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1991.0077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, H. H., Skodol, A. E., and Lave, T. R. (1992). Revising axis V for DSM-IV: a review of measures of social functioning. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 1148–1156. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.9.1148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. F., Horan, W. P., and Lee, J. (2015). Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 620–631. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 59–67. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01330-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Ellgring, H., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2004). Early-stage face processing dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 915–917. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.5.915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaffe, A. E., Gao, Y., Deep-Soboslay, A., Tao, R., Hyde, T. M., Weinberger, D. R., et al. (2016). Mapping DNA methylation across development, genotype and schizophrenia in the human frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 40–47. doi: 10.1038/nn.4181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston-Wilson, N. L., Sims, C. D., Hofmann, J. P., Anderson, L., Torrey, E. F., and Yolken, R. H. (2000). Disease-specific alterations in frontal cortex brain proteins in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. The Stanley Neuropathology Consortium. Mol. Psychiatry 5, 142–149. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, H. T., Kim, D. W., Kim, S., Im, C. H., and Lee, S. H. (2012). Reduced source activity of event-related potentials for affective facial pictures in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 136, 150–159. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., and Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. S., Shin, N. Y., Choi, J. S., Jung, M. H., Jang, J. H., and Kang, D. H. (2010). Processing of facial configuration in individuals at ultra-high risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 118, 81–87. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, C., Heinks, T., Andresen, B., Berg, P., and Moritz, S. (2000). Impaired modulation of the saccadic contingent variation preceding antisaccades in scihzophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 978–990. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00234-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohler, C. G., Walker, J. B., Martin, E. A., Healey, K. M., and Moberg, P. J. (2009). Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 1009–1019. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. H., Kim, E. Y., Kim, S., and Bae, S. M. (2010). Event-related potential patterns and gender effects underlying facial affect processing in schizophrenia patients. Neurosci. Res. 67, 172–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Chan, R. C., McAlonan, G. M., and Gong, Q. Y. (2010). Facial emotion processing in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 1029–1039. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maher, S., Mashhoon, Y., Ekstrom, T., Lukas, S., and Chen, Y. (2016). Deficient cortical face-sensitive N170 responses and basic visual processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 170, 87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCleery, A., Lee, J., Joshi, A., Wynn, J. K., Hellemann, G. S., and Green, M. F. (2015). Meta-analysis of face processing event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 116–126. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Megreya, A. M. (2016). Face perception in schizophrenia: a specific deficit. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 21, 60–72. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2015.1133407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minzenberg, M. J., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S., Carter, C. S., and Glahn, D. C. (2009). Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 811–822. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onitsuka, T., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Spencer, K. M., Frumin, M., Kuroki, N., Lucia, L. C., et al. (2006). Functional and structural deficits in brain regions subserving face perception in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 455–462. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onitsuka, T., Oribe, N., Nakamura, I., and Kanba, S. (2013). Review of neurophysiological findings in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 67, 461–470. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulman, R. G., Devous, M. D., Gregory, R. R., Herman, J. H., Jennings, L., Bonte, F. J., et al. (1990). Hypofrontality and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: dynamic single-photon tomography and neuropsychological assessment of schizophrenic brain function. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 377–399. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90549-h

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabatinelli, D., Fortune, E. E., Li, Q., Siddiqui, A., Krafft, C., Oliver, W. T., et al. (2011). Emotional perception: meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage 54, 2524–2533. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savla, G. N., Vella, L., Armstrong, C. C., Penn, D. L., and Twamley, E. W. (2013). Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 979–992. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

She, S., Li, H., Ning, Y., Ren, J., Wu, Z., Huang, R., et al. (2017). Revealing the dysfunction of schematic facial-expression processing in schizophrenia: a comparative study of different references. Front. Neurosci. 11:314. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, Y.-W., Na, M. H., Ha, T. H., Kang, D.-H., Yoo, S.-Y., and Kwon, J. S. (2008). Dysfunction in configural face processing in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 538–543. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streit, M., Wölwer, W., Brinkmeyer, J., Ihl, R., and Gaebel, W. (2001). EEG-correlates of facial affect recognition and categorisation of blurred faces in schizophrenic patients and healthy volunteers. Schizophr. Res. 49, 145–155. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00041-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, H., Koeda, M., Oda, K., Matsuda, T., Matsushima, E., Matsuura, M., et al. (2004). An fMRI study of differential neural response to affective pictures in schizophrenia. NeuroImage 22, 1247–1254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. F., Kang, J., Brege, I. S., Tso, I. F., Hosanagar, A., and Johnson, T. D. (2012). Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of emotion perception and experience in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 136–145. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsunoda, T., Kanba, S., Ueno, T., Hirano, Y., Hirano, S., Maekawa, T., et al. (2012). Altered face inversion effect and association between face N170 reduction and social dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1762–1768. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turetsky, B. I., Kohler, C. G., Indersmitten, T., Bhati, M. T., Charbonnier, D., and Gur, R. C. (2007). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: when and why does it go awry? Schizophr. Res. 94, 253–263. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ursu, S., Kring, A. M., Gard, M. G., Minzenberg, M. J., Yoon, J. H., Ragland, J. D., et al. (2011). Prefrontal cortical deficits and impaired cognition-emotion interactions in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 276–285. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., and Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia 45, 174–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V. J., McCallum, W. C., and Winter, A. L. (1964). Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensori-motor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 203, 380–384. doi: 10.1038/203380a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wynn, J. K., Jahshan, C., Altshuler, L. L., Glahn, D. C., and Green, M. F. (2013). Event-related potential examination of facial affect processing in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 43, 109–117. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wynn, J. K., Lee, J., Horan, W. P., and Green, M. F. (2008). Using event related potentials to explore stages of facial affect recognition deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 679–687. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, D. (2001). A method to standardize a reference of scalp EEG recordings to a point at infinity. Physiol. Meas. 22, 693–711. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/22/4/305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. J. Exp. Psychol. 81, 141–145. doi: 10.1037/h0027474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, R. K. (1970). Face recognition by brain-injured patients: a dissociable ability? Neuropsychologia 8, 395–402. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(70)90036-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Zhao, Y., Liu, Y., and Tan, S. (2016). Perception of the duration of emotional faces in schizophrenic patients. Sci. Rep. 6:22280. doi: 10.1038/srep22280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Y., Li, H., Ning, Y., Ren, J., Wu, Z., Huang, R., et al. (2016). Sluggishness of early-stage face processing (N170) is correlated with negative and general psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:615. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, contingent negative variation (CNV), N170, facial expression, personal and social performance (PSP)

Citation: Wang Q, She S, Luo L, Li H, Ning Y, Ren J, Wu Z, Huang R and Zheng Y (2020) Abnormal Contingent Negative Variation Drifts During Facial Expression Judgment in Schizophrenia Patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:274. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00274

Received: 05 February 2020; Accepted: 18 June 2020;
Published: 14 July 2020.

Edited by:

Jinhui Wang, South China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Ye Zhang, Hangzhou Normal University, China
Joy Hirsch, Yale School of Medicine, United States

Copyright © 2020 Wang, She, Luo, Li, Ning, Ren, Wu, Huang and Zheng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qian Wang, aleinwangba@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.