Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 09 April 2020
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Sensory Neuroscience Editor's Pick 2021 View all 10 articles

Explicit and Implicit Own's Body and Space Perception in Painful Musculoskeletal Disorders and Rheumatic Diseases: A Systematic Scoping Review

\nAntonello Viceconti
Antonello Viceconti1*Eleonora Maria CameroneEleonora Maria Camerone1Deborah LuzziDeborah Luzzi1Debora PentassugliaDebora Pentassuglia1Matteo Pardini,Matteo Pardini2,3Diego RistoriDiego Ristori1Giacomo RossettiniGiacomo Rossettini1Alberto Gallace,Alberto Gallace4,5Matthew R. LongoMatthew R. Longo6Marco Testa
Marco Testa1*
  • 1Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health, University of Genova, Savona, Italy
  • 2Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health, University of Genova, Genoa, Italy
  • 3Policlinico S. Martino IRCCS, Genova, Italy
  • 4Neuromi, Università di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
  • 5Mind and Behavior Technological Center- Mibtec, Università di Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
  • 6Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom

Background: Pain and body perception are essentially two subjective mutually influencing experiences. However, in the field of musculoskeletal disorders and rheumatic diseases we lack of a comprehensive knowledge about the relationship between body perception dysfunctions and pain or disability. We systematically mapped the literature published about the topics of: (a) somatoperception; (b) body ownership; and (c) perception of space, analysing the relationship with pain and disability. The results were organized around the two main topics of the assessment and treatment of perceptual dysfunctions.

Methods: This scoping review followed the six-stage methodology suggested by Arksey and O'Malley. Ten electronic databases and grey literature were systematically searched. The PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews was used for reporting results. Two reviewers with different background, independently performed study screening and selection, and one author performed data extraction, that was checked by a second reviewer.

Results: Thirty-seven studies fulfilled the eligibility criteria. The majority of studies (68%) concerned the assessment methodology, and the remaining 32% investigated the effects of therapeutic interventions. Research designs, methodologies adopted, and settings varied considerably across studies. Evidence of distorted body experience were found mainly for explicit somatoperception, especially in studies adopting self-administered questionnaire and subjective measures, highlighting in some cases the presence of sub-groups with different perceptual features. Almost half of the intervention studies (42%) provided therapeutic approaches combining more than one perceptual task, or sensory-motor tasks together with perceptual strategies, thus it was difficult to estimate the relative effectiveness of each single therapeutic component.

Conclusions: To our knowledge, this is the first attempt to systematically map and summarize this research area in the field of musculoskeletal disorders and rheumatic diseases. Although methodological limitations limit the validity of the evidence obtained, some strategies of assessment tested and therapeutic strategies proposed represent useful starting points for future research. This review highlights preliminary evidence, strengths, and limitations of the literature published about the research questions, identifying key points that remain opened to be addressed, and make suggestions for future research studies. Body representation, as well as pain perception and treatment, can be better understood if an enlarged perspective including body and space perception is considered.

Introduction

The body is a unique multisensory object (Longo et al., 2008a) integrating a large variety of inputs both from the outside and from within the body (Gallace and Spence, 2008), thus offering the opportunity for a better interaction with the complex surrounding world (Medina and Coslett, 2016a). We can experience our own body through the basic somatic sensations of touch, warmth, cold, proprioception, nociception and itch coming from peripheral receptors to central specific cortical areas (somatosensation). However, our body interaction with the surrounding world is also made by more rich and complex experiences, as the estimation of body size and shape, or the perception of body parts localization in external space (somatoperception) (Taylor-Clarke et al., 2004; Longo and Haggard, 2010) for which there are no specialized sensory receptors. The achievement of this more sophisticated perceptual experience requires moving beyond pure somatosensation to a higher-order level of neural machinery in which a combination of somatic information converges in associative areas (Murata and Ishida, 2007; Murata et al., 2016) to produce a multimodal representation of the body as a whole (the so called body matrix) (Moseley et al., 2012). This “on line” organization of somatic information is checked for congruence against internal body models (somatorepresentations) (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005; de Vignemont, 2007; Carruthers, 2008; Tsakiris and Fotopoulou, 2008; Berlucchi and Aglioti, 2010; Longo, 2015; Medina and Coslett, 2016a): if the “on line” representation does not match (Azañón and Haggard, 2009) the “off line” body memory (Riva, 2018) we experience a body incoherence, from which misperceptions and bodily illusions may arise.

In addition, as a part of our body interaction with the surrounding world, how we experience our own's body relates also to our sense of self, understood as the perceptual feeling that a body part belong to us (ownership), and is under our own control (agency) (Tsakiris et al., 2006; Longo et al., 2008a). Internal mental representation of the body includes the shape and contours of own body, the perceived location of body parts, and the boundaries between them and external objects. Body ownership and body agency can be tested experimentally through the Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) paradigm (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998), in which tactile stimuli are applied synchronously over a prosthetic hand placed in front of the participant, and on his actual hand hidden from view. This produces an illusory sense of incorporation of the rubber hand as it was the participant's own hand (Botvinick, 2004). Overall, how we experience our body and space around us results from the integration of at least three different sub-functions: (a) the perception we have of our own body (somatoperceptionSoP); (b) the perception of the space around us in which we are immersed (space perception—SpP); and (c) the integration of the two body experiences in order to produce a coherent sense of self (body ownershipBO). Up to now we have only a partial knowledge of the operational mechanisms guiding SoP because a large number of studies conducted in the fields of experimental psychology and neurophysiology have mainly studied the basic mechanisms of somatosensations while we know much less about the higher-order mechanisms involved in SoP (Longo et al., 2010). Moreover, the research lines have increased the interest on BO and SpP only in the last one or two decades (Ramakonar et al., 2011; Trojan et al., 2014).

Musculoskeletal Disorders and Rheumatic Diseases (MDRDs) are a group of diseases commonly affecting bones, muscles and joints (van der Heijde et al., 2018) that often cause chronic pain with a severe impact on the quality of life of patients (March et al., 2014; Blyth et al., 2019), loss of work productivity (Daneshmandi et al., 2017), and significant economic costs for the community (Bevan, 2015; Vos et al., 2017; Briggs et al., 2018). Notably, pain and body perception are essentially two subjective mutually influencing perceptual experiences (Haggard et al., 2013; Trojan et al., 2014): the fast and accurate perception of pain is essential to protect the body, and the perception of body integrity is needed to avoid pain (Wand et al., 2016). Thus, the study of errors in processing the explicit (conscious) and implicit (unconscious) body experience, as in the case of illusion phenomena (Medina and Coslett, 2016b), may represent a useful opportunity to understand how the brain constructs functional representations of the body in patients with MDRDs, and on pain perception itself (Pamment and Aspell, 2017; Fang et al., 2019) in these clinical conditions. However, existing studies on SoP, SpP, and BO were largely conducted on healthy subjects (Longo et al., 2008a; Fuentes et al., 2013; Longo, 2017), and clinical research has mostly investigated neurological conditions (Haggard and Wolpert, 2005; Pia et al., 2013, 2016), eating disorders (Keizer et al., 2011; Scarpina et al., 2014; Spitoni et al., 2015; Gadsby, 2017), and neuropathic pain syndromes such as Complex Regional Pain Syndrome-CRPS (Galer and Jensen, 1999; Förderreuther et al., 2004; Lewis et al., 2007; Reinersmann et al., 2013). A large body of literature on the field of MDRDs has instead investigated primary somatosensations (Tsay et al., 2015), mainly tactile acuity (Catley et al., 2013, 2014; Harvie et al., 2018) and proprioceptive precision (Stanton et al., 2016; Tong et al., 2017; Lin et al., 2019), referring generically to disturbances at the level of perception or mental representations. However, both two-point discrimination and joint repositioning error (two of the most frequently investigated tasks) cannot be considered as having a higher-order somatoperceptual involvement (Longo and Haggard, 2010; Hillier et al., 2015; Spitoni et al., 2015). The area of MDRDs thus lacks a comprehensive knowledge about the more complex implicit and explicit body and space perception.

Evidence supporting the interaction between pain and the three mentioned domains of body experience (SoP, SpP, BO) have been found in experimentally-induced pain (Moseley et al., 2008; Gallace et al., 2011; Mancini et al., 2011; Fang et al., 2019) (e.g., distorting the visual appearance of the body). Moreover, a correlation between body and space perception dysfunctions with pain intensity and its duration (Förderreuther et al., 2004; Peltz et al., 2011; Reinersmann et al., 2012), were found in CRPS, thus it would be clinically relevant to clarify if this interaction exists also in MDRDs.

In order to have a comprehensive and structured knowledge of how body experience has been investigated in MDRDs, we systematically reviewed the literature published about the implicit and explicit mechanisms of: (a) somatoperception (and indirectly on somatorepresentations); (b) body ownership; and (c) space perception.

The primary goal of this study was to map and examine the quantity and the nature of the scientific literature concerning the implicit and explicit own's body and space perception, organizing the findings around three main topics:

a) the adopted strategies of assessment for perceptual dysfunctions;

b) the impact of perceptual disorders in MDRDs compared to other disorders (e.g., CRPS) and in sub-groups of MDRDs;

c) the interventions proposed to approach perceptual disorders associated to MDRDs.

Materials and Methods

The scoping methodology has been adopted because represents the most appropriate method to overview the literature about an emergent research area that is still fragmented, complex, wide, poorly understood or not deeply investigated before (Colquhoun et al., 2014, 2017). The review followed the PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) (Tricco et al., 2018). A detailed PRISMA-ScR is provided Additional File 1. Every deviation from the published protocol (Viceconti et al., 2018) or added procedure were declared. Neuroscientists (ML, AG, MP) and physiotherapists (AV, DL, DP, DR, GR, MT) constituted an inter-professional and interdisciplinary research team with both clinical and scientific background to better approach, from a rehabilitative perspective, a research area that has been historically treated by neuropsychological disciplines (Head and Holmes, 1911; Haggard and Wolpert, 2005; Medina and Coslett, 2016b). In agreement with the concept of a “literature map,” the results are graphically presented in Figures 38.

Eligibility Criteria

Inclusion and exclusion criteria are reported in Table 2. An iterative process, rather than a fixed and pre-established searching schema, is one of the features characterizing scoping reviews: eligibility criteria were updated in progress by an iterative process based on feedback provided by authors, in order to better refine the searching process according to the research questions (see the step 1 of the Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Summary of the adopted guidance framework for scoping reviews.

We adopted a priori operational definitions of the key terms used (see Table 1 for terminological aspects) in order to avoid terminological misunderstanding. Themes like those dealing with somatosensation, somatosensory representation, body scheme, body structural description, body concept, and body affect were not considered as the main focus of this review. In particular, sensory representations and somatosensations were often used in literature as a surrogate for perceptual representations and somatoperception, mainly to describe their associated dysfunctions. Readers are invited to see Flor et al. (1997), Flor (2003), and Hotz-Boendermaker et al. (2016) for maladaptive reorganizations of somatosensory representations, and Tsay et al. (2015) for a comprehensive review on somatosensations. Moreover, we have deliberately avoided the use of the umbrella term “body image” given the controversies and interpretational difficulties with this term (de Vignemont, 2010; Pitron et al., 2018; Gadsby, 2019): it has often been used as a “passepartout” term, lumping together phenomena and psychological capacities quite different from each other, often referred to beliefs and affective attitudes related to the body (Mohr et al., 2010). We have to consider that words used in literature to describe body perception, mental representations and the relative assigned meanings are sometimes ambiguous or contradictory (Gallagher, 1986) and often depend on the observer's professional background., Thus, to better organize the results emerged from the review we have referred to the theoretical model proposed by Longo et al. (2010), Longo (2016) and adapted it for the purposes of this study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Terminological definitions.

Searching Strategy and Information Sources

In line with the published protocol, we followed a three-step search strategy as recommended by The Joanna Briggs Institute (2015). A preliminary search strategy was developed, pilot-tested and peer-reviewed by two authors with different background (a physiotherapist expert in research methodology—DR, and a neuropsychologist—AG), by using the PRESS 2015 Evidence-Based Checklist (McGowan et al., 2016). Modifications to the search string were made after reviewers' suggestions. Searching history and the peer-reviews of the search strategy are available under request. In addition to the procedure described in the protocol, the “Similar Articles” function of PubMed was used and the snowball technique adopted when additional articles were found (Greenhalgh and Peacock, 2005).

Electronic Databases

Electronic search was conducted by one author (AV) between May 2018 until September 2018 on 10 electronic databases and grey literature (a full description is provided in the Additional File 2). Very broad search terms were employed for a more sensitive rather than specific search of the literature aimed at meeting the primary goal of the scoping review to systematically map the literature. A secondary review was made by scanning the Gray Matters Checklist.1

Study Selection

Two reviewers with different background independently evaluated records for eligibility of title/abstract (DL and EC) and full texts screening (AV and MP). Any disagreement was resolved by discussion between reviewers or, in case of persistent disagreement, a third reviewer (MT) was introduced to reach a consensus. The reviewing process is detailed on the PRISMA flow chart (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flow diagram detailing the selection of sources of evidence.

Calibration Phase

Both for the screening of titles/abstracts and for the selection of full-texts the raters performed a series of pilot tests as a calibration exercise to improve the reliability of judgments and agreement between evaluators (Tricco et al., 2018). Each round of pilot testing was accompanied by explanatory documents in which eligibility criteria were updated and clarified, and specification about potentially conflicting terminology were provided in order to avoid interpretation errors. For all pre-formal screening test, inter-rater percentage agreement had to be >90% before starting the formal screening (Colquhoun et al., 2017) (further information are provided in the published protocol). Feedback from evaluators were used to refine the inclusion/exclusion criteria (see the difference between Table 2 in the present study and its counterpart in the published protocol).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Eligibility criteria for inclusion and exclusion studies.

Data Analysis

Findings emerged from the retrieved studies were organized around the research questions. Due to the heterogeneity of studies, in terms of research designs, methodological issues and clinical conditions investigated, we adopted a qualitative-descriptive synthesis, as suggested by the PRISMA-ScR (Tricco et al., 2018), and following the approach recommended by the Cochrane Group (Higgins and Green, 2008) and the (Centre for Reviews Dissemination, 2009). In case of incomplete or missing data, the authors of the included papers were contacted for further information. Data were extracted by a single reviewer (AV) using a standardized Excel spreadsheet designed for this study and adapted after the pilot trial charting exercise. A second author (DP) performed the crosschecking of data extracted. Information extracted from each study are detailed in Supplementary Files of the published study protocol. With respect to the original data-extraction form, the item “Future research direction” was deleted because it was not considered relevant The difference between groups means was used as an unstandardized measure for the size of the effect in intervention studies: in case of missed aggregated data, pooled mean and pooled standard deviation were calculated, as well as the 95% Interval Confidence (95% CI). The assessment of studies for clinical relevance was based on the Minimal Clinically Important Difference (MCID) thresholds established in literature for the outcomes used in included studies (see notes of the Supplementary Table S2).

Critical Appraisal of Individual Sources of Evidence

The primary goal of scoping studies is to systematically map and synthesize results coming from an emerging research area (Canadian Institutes of Health Research, 2010; Colquhoun et al., 2014), rather than provide the best available evidence. Considering also the methodological heterogeneity expected from the studies published on this topic, a qualitative appraisal for risk of bias was not conducted, in accordance to published guidelines on the conduct of scoping reviews (Peters et al., 2015).

Clinical Relevance of Studies Included in Qualitative Analysis

Clinical relevance was assessed by one author (AV) using the recommendations of the Cochrane Collaboration Back Review Group (Furlan et al., 2009) (a description of the items is provided in Supplementary Table S2). For evaluation studies, the item “Are the likely treatment benefits worth the potential harms?” was replaced with a “The involved procedure/s and/or setting are accessible in terms of cost and advanced technical knowledge required?” and the item “Are the interventions and treatment settings described well enough so that you can provide the same for your patients?” was replaced with “Are the settings and/or the methodology adopted, described well enough so that you can provide the same for your patients?”. Finally, referring to case-controls studies, the item “Is the size of the effect clinically important?” was substituted with “Are there clinically significant differences in the population investigated respect to the control group/s?” In studies without control groups it was used the item “Is there a correlation of the dysfunction detected with at least one out the two clinical variables of pain and disability?” Figures 7, 8 summarize the evaluation for clinical relevance, respectively for assessment and for intervention studies.

Results

Study Selection

A summary of the main findings is presented in Tables 3, 4, organized with the acronym PCC-Population-Concept-Context (more detailed data can be found in Additional Materials).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Synopsis of included assessment studies.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Synopsis of included intervention studies.

The first calibration test was conducted on 239 titles and abstracts, and the second on 12 full-texts: the inter-rater agreement was, respectively, of 93 and 100%, and was reached for both procedures at the third round, at the end of which the reviewers express no need of further training. The search strategies initially produced 7,818 records from all the databases, and 13 from authors' personal databases (Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012, 2016; Wand et al., 2013a; Hirakawa et al., 2014; Louw et al., 2017; Stanton et al., 2017, 2018; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Magni et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019). After removal of duplicates and exclusion of clearly irrelevant records on the basis of the title and abstract, 123 full-texts were screened. Five full-texts were not found and 96 were excluded with reasons (see the Supplementary Table S7). Ten additional studies (Grod and Diakow, 2002; Barker et al., 2008; Wand et al., 2010, 2011; Preston and Newport, 2011; Nishigami et al., 2012; Diers et al., 2013; Ryan et al., 2014; Treleaven and Takasaki, 2015; Beales et al., 2016) were identified and considered eligible by searching the reference lists of included papers and reviews considered of interest for the aim of this work. Thirty-seven studies, analysing an overall sample of 1291 patients (1,094 in evaluation studies, and 197 for interventions ones), were included in the qualitative analysis and the end of the selection process (see the Figure 2 for a flow-chart of the entire process). Agreement between raters in formal screening was 95% for titles/abstracts screening, and 91% in full-texts inclusion: all disagreements were resolved upon discussion and clarification of eligibility criteria, and the intervention of the third independent assessor (MT) was not needed. A graphical distribution of included studies grouped by clinical conditions examined is shown in Figure 3. Twenty-five studies (Grod and Diakow, 2002; Moseley, 2008; Wand et al., 2010, 2013b, 2014, 2016; Docherty et al., 2012; Lauche et al., 2012a; Valenzuela-Moguillansky, 2013; Hirakawa et al., 2014; Gilpin et al., 2015; Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015, 2017, 2018; Treleaven and Takasaki, 2015; Beales et al., 2016; Janssens et al., 2017; Moreira et al., 2017; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Magni et al., 2018; Martínez et al., 2018) studies concerned the assessment of SoP dysfunctions, while twelve interventional studies (Barker et al., 2008; Preston and Newport, 2011; Wand et al., 2011, 2013a; Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012; Diers et al., 2013; Vetrano et al., 2013; Ryan et al., 2014; Louw et al., 2017; Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019) investigated the effects of perception-based intervention to reduce pain or to correct perceptual distortions. One study, (Lauche et al., 2012a) was included both in assessment and in intervention studies: it is a qualitative study investigating the explicit SoP in chronic neck pain (CNP) patients at baseline, and also at follow-up because it was embedded in a RCT study investigating the effect of cupping therapy (Lauche et al., 2012b). Figure 4 displays the domains investigated by assessment and intervention studies.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Summary of the distribution of included studies for clinical conditions examined.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Summary of the distribution of domain investigated for study typology.

Research Designs of Included Studies

The distribution of research designs adopted for assessment studies is graphically represented in Figure 5. A conspicuous number of these studies is composed by those validating the Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ) (Wand et al., 2014) and the Fremantle Knee Awareness Questionnaire (FreKAQ) (Nishigami et al., 2017), including validation studies into other languages (Janssens et al., 2017; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2018), and cross-sectional or case-control investigations across different clinical samples (Wand et al., 2013a, 2016; Beales et al., 2016). Figure 6 shows the research designs adopted in the intervention studies included, of which a large part were pre-clinical experimental studies (Diers et al., 2013; Nishigami et al., 2019).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Summary of the distribution of evaluation studies for research design adopted.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Summary of the distribution of intervention studies for research design adopted.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Summary of the evaluation for clinical relevance of included assessment studies.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Summary of the evaluation for clinical relevance of included treatment studies.

Assessment Studies

Five studies were conducted in an experimental setting (Grod and Diakow, 2002; Docherty et al., 2012; Gilpin et al., 2015; Treleaven and Takasaki, 2015; Martínez et al., 2018), while the remaining 19 studies were clinical investigations (Moseley, 2008; Wand et al., 2010, 2013a,b, 2014, 2016; Lauche et al., 2012a; Valenzuela-Moguillansky, 2013; Hirakawa et al., 2014; Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015, 2017, 2018; Beales et al., 2016; Janssens et al., 2017; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Magni et al., 2018).

The main domain studied was explicit SoP (58%; 22/38), followed by implicit SoP (32%; 12/38), and SpP (8%; 3/38), while the BO made up only the 2% (1/38) of the sample.

Implicit Somatoperception

Only 4/24 (17%) of the included assessment studies investigated implicit SoP. Adamczyk et al. (2018a,b) preliminarily validated a methodology for the objective evaluation of implicit body size perception, the two-point estimation (2-PET) task. Among all the included assessment studies, it is the only one adopting an objective methodology to assess metric features of SoP. Authors found an underestimation of the distance between two tactile stimuli delivered with a caliper on the back (46 and 56%, respectively on the verbal and manual version of test) in both the painful and pain-free low-back side. Duration and pain intensity predicted the presence of perceptual dysfunctions and accounted for the 42% of the total variance of 2-PET scores in the regression analysis. The same 2-PET task were used by the same authors in a two-case report study (Adamczyk et al., 2018a) in which one patient showed an overestimation of the painful side, compared to non-painful locations (range: 45–206%), while a second patient showed the opposite pattern with an underestimation ranging between 12 and 22%. In this double case-study, authors used also another test, the point-to-point test (PTP): the distance error between the site touched by the examiner and that touched by the patient was greater on the painful side than on the pain-free location, for a magnitude of 24–84%. With this second patient, the authors used also a qualitative version of the PTP test: subject were asked to point with a pen to the site stimulated by the examiner. In case of error, the examiner drew the error trajectories directly on the patient's back by moving the pen from the incorrect location indicated by the patient to the correct one: on the painful side all the trajectories were outside the referred symptomatic area and considerably spaced between them, indicating large errors in pointing the site of tactile stimuli.

Only one study (Wand et al., 2013b) adopted the localization task of tactile stimuli to assess the superficial schema. Subjects, after being stimulated by the experimenter with tactile and painful stimuli, were asked to mark on a body chart with 12 pre-defined areas of the trunk and thighs, the perceived localization of the applied stimuli. Authors found that 67% of chronic lower back pain (CLBP) patients made at least one localization error compared to only 25% of healthy controls, but no correlations were found between mislocalization errors and either pain and disability. Of the possible maximum 28 mislocalizations, five were reported in the worst cases by patients and three by controls. The study involved tactile and pinprick stimuli, but the authors reported combined results for both type of stimulations, without differentiating between types of task (personal communication with authors).

The Letter recognition task (or graphesthesia) involves the recognition of letters drawn on the skin. This task was tested only in the study of Wand et al. (2010): CLBP patients showed 10% more errors respect to healthy controls (p < 0.05), however this score was not correlated with clinical data.

Explicit Somatoperception

Body image drawing task

The majority of selected assessment studies evaluated the explicit SoP: five studies adopted the Body Image Drawing (BID) task (Moseley, 2008; Lauche et al., 2012a; Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017), eight studies used the FreBAQ (Wand et al., 2014, 2016; Wand et al., 2013a; Beales et al., 2016; Janssens et al., 2017; Nishigami et al., 2017, 2018; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018) or the FreKAQ (Nishigami et al., 2017), two studies used the Neglect-Like Symptoms Questionnaire (NLSQ) (Hirakawa et al., 2014; Magni et al., 2018), one study investigate the visual size estimation in an experimental setting (Gilpin et al., 2015), one study investigated the rubber hand illusion (RHI) (Martínez et al., 2018), and one was a qualitative study on subjectively referred body perception (Valenzuela-Moguillansky, 2013).

Of the five study using the BID task, two were conducted on CLBP patients (Moseley, 2008; Nishigami et al., 2015) and three on Chronic Neck Pain (CNP) patients (Lauche et al., 2012a; Mibu et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017). All studies reported distortions in BID in a variable percentage of patients: Moseley (2008) found that five out of six patients with CLBP reported difficulties in drawing their trunk along all the entire extension. Four out of six showed the tendency to draw their vertebrae displaced from the midline toward the painful side. Moreover, two patients reported a feeling of shrunken trunk. In a larger sample of patients (n = 42), matched with 17 healthy controls, Nishigami et al. (2015) found distorted BID in about 50% of patients: half of them showed an enlarged image of their back, while the other half drew a shrunken BID. The other 50% of patients had a normal BID. However, the authors found no significant differences in pain intensity, duration, or disability between the three groups. Mibu et al. (2015) found a distorted neck drawing in 50% of patients with CNP (significantly more than the 5% of healthy controls), however there were no differences either for pain duration or intensity within CNP sub-groups, or with respect to healthy controls. Moreira et al. (2017) in their preliminary case-control study found a less symmetric and uniform outline of neck and shoulders in CNP patients than in controls. Two patients drew their neck and shoulders enlarged, while another two were unable to delineate one side of the neck, as well as two subjects in control group. Participants with a clearly distorted neck image or unable to draw body parts, tended to report higher pain intensity and duration. Finally, in the qualitative study of Lauche et al. (2012a) the authors used both interviews and BID. In four out six CNP patients, the qualitative analysis of the drawings showed noticeable discrepancies compared to a normal body silhouette, with missing lines and overestimated size of the neck and shoulders.

Visual size estimation procedure

Among the assessment studies analysing the subjective visual appearance of body parts, only that of Gilpin et al. (2015) was conducted in an experimental setting. Patients with hand osteoarthritis were asked to judge what photograph corresponded to their actual hand. Photographs were experimentally manipulated in percentage of the real length dimension: patients significantly underestimated the size of their hand, selecting photos showing hands 8% smaller compared to healthy subjects (99.8% of the real hand dimension in patients vs. to 107.8% in healthy controls). Although the MIRAGE system used in this last study induced a visual illusion correcting the distortion evaluated at the baseline, we considered this study only as an evaluation study because the authors did not provide pain measure, nor disability questionnaire as outcome measure (see Table 2 for exclusion criteria).

Self-administered questionnaire

Fremantle back and knee awareness questionnaire Seven studies were conducted adopting the FreBAQ validated by Wand et al. (2014, 2016) on CLBP patients (Janssens et al., 2017; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2018), in post-partum (Beales et al., 2016) or pregnancy-related pelvic pain (Wand et al., 2017), and knee osteoarthritis (Nishigami et al., 2017). Three studies are psychometric validations of the FreBAQ in other languages (Janssens et al., 2017; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2018). Fifty of 51 patients with CLBP (98%) reported some level of misperception, and only one patient recorded zero points (corresponding to no misperceptions). The mean score ranged between 8.8 (Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018) and 11.7 (Nishigami et al., 2018) in patients, and 0.5 to 3.3 points in healthy controls (Wand et al., 2014; Janssens et al., 2017; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018) on a 0–36 scale where higher scoring indicating larger number of misperceptions. Ehrenbrusthoff et al. (2018) also reported a significant (p < 0.01) mean difference between patients and controls in German population adjusted for age, gender and body mass index (5.4 points; 95% CI = 3.0–7.8). However, this was lower than that found by Wand et al. (2014) of 11 points (p < 0.001). In all included studies investigating CLBP there was a significant correlation between FreBAQ score with, pain (intensity, duration or interference) (Wand et al., 2014, 2016; Janssens et al., 2017; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2018) and disability (Wand et al., 2014, 2016; Ehrenbrusthoff et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2018). Janssens et al. (2017) found also a difference between patients at different level of disability: the sub-group with higher disability (Oswestry Disability Index ≥20%) scored significantly higher (p = 0.005) than lower-disability group (13 ± 8 vs. 8 ± 6 points).

Beales et al. (2016) administered the FreBAQ to women with lumbo-pelvic pain (LPP) raised minimum 3 months post-partum, and found significantly (p = 0.02) more disturbances in explicit SoP in the moderate disability sub-groups of patients (median score: 8/36 points) than in pain free controls (median score: 2/36 points). They also found more perceptual dysfunctions in moderate-disability sub-groups respect to low-disability patients (median score: 6.5/36 points) and pain free controls (median score: 2/36 points), however there was no statistical significance (respectively, p = 0.282 and p = 0.095; personal communication). Wand et al. (2017) instead collected data on pregnancy-related LPP (within the 3rd trimester of pregnancy and not over the 38th week): women with pain referred significantly (p = 0.005) more perceptual dysfunctions than those pain-free (median score: 3.5/36 vs. 1/36), and authors found a significant correlation (p = 0.027) of FreBAQ score with pain intensity (r = 0.378), despite it was not correlated with self-reported disability (p = 0.143).

Finally, Nishigami et al. (2017) adopted the FreBAQ for knee osteoarthritis patients, validating the FreKAQ. They found significantly (p = 0.001) more perceptual dysfunctions in patients than in controls (median difference: 9 points), and a significant correlation (p < 0.002) between pain in motion (rho = 0.37) and disability (rho = −0.41), but not with pain duration (rho = −0.06, p = 0.76).

Neglect-like symptoms questionnaire Two studies using the NLSQ developed by Galer and Jensen (1999) and Frettlöh et al. (2006) in CRPS. The NLSQ measures the cognitive and motor neglect, with higher scoring indicating more neglect referred symptoms. Hirakawa et al. (2014) found that 36% of patients with knee osteoarthritis scored more than 100 on a 0–500 range three weeks after arthroplasty, decreasing at 19% at six weeks (p-value not reported). The mean NLSQ score decreased from 77.7 to 42.2 points (p-value not reported); however, the standard deviation was high due to a large inter-subject variation. The motor neglect sub-scale (MNss) was associated in multiple regression analysis with pain both at 3 (β = 0.50; p < 0.01) and 6 weeks (β = 0.53; p < 0.01), where β represents points on MNss per unit of pain intensity, and with the improvement of range of motion at 6 weeks (β = −0.28; p < 0.01), with β describing changes in MNss score per range of motion degrees. Magni et al. (2018) reported a presence of neglect-like symptoms in hand osteoarthritis more often than in control healthy subjects (p < 0.001)(prevalence rate not reported); however, the magnitude of the difference was very low (median difference = 0.5 points; personal communication).

Body Ownership

The study of Martínez et al. (2018) is the only one to analyse body ownership through the RHI paradigm. They found that fibromyalgic patients were more susceptible to experience the illusion compared to healthy controls, scoring significantly (p < 0.05) higher both in proprioceptive drift sub-scale and in 4 out 5 items of the agency scale.

Perception of Surrounding Space

No studies assessed the personal and peri-personal space in MDRDs. Three studies investigated perception of extra-personal space using the Computerized Rod And Frame test (CRAF) in (Docherty et al., 2012; Treleaven and Takasaki, 2015) acute/recurrent and CNP (Grod and Diakow, 2002). During the CRAF subjects were asked to set a rod to the true vertical or horizontal: it provided a measure of the absolute error for the subjective perception of visual verticality/horizontality (SVV—SVO), and assessed the dependence on visual input for spatial orientation. Treleaven and Takasaki (2015) found a significant difference (p < 0.05) between patients with idiopathic CNP and both whiplash affected patients for SVV error (mean difference: 0.37°), and healthy controls (mean difference: 0.5°). This difference was referred to the Variable Error (VE), indicating the variability of the performance. Also the Root Mean Square Error (RMSE), representing the overall accuracy in achieving the true vertical, resulted significantly different (p = 0.01) between idiopathic neck pain patients and the other two groups (mean difference: 0.51° respect to healthy controls, and 0.58° respect to whiplash patients. By contrast, the absolute error and the direction of error were not able to detect between-groups differences (p-values respectively of 0.99 and 0.6). Unexpectedly, difference between patients with Whiplash Associated Disorders (WAD) and healthy controls was not significant in all error measurements evaluated, despite a higher level of disability in this sub-group respect to that with idiopathic neck pain. Docherty et al. (2012) (Docherty et al., 2012) assessed the (SVH error) in addition to the SVV error. Although both parameters were significantly different (p < 0.05) between CNP patients and healthy controls, they nevertheless fell into the range considered normal (<0.5°). A significant greater error (p < 0.001) in patients than in healthy controls was also found when using a variant with the frame tilted clockwise or counter-clockwise by 18°, but even in this case the median difference was small (<2°). Notably, 16% of patients with CNP scored higher than normal error in both the SVV and SVO, and they reported significantly (p < 0.016) higher disability at the Neck Disability Index respect to other patients with errors falling within the reference range of normality. The same small difference between groups (mean difference = 1.99°; p < 0.001) in SVV was found by (Grod and Diakow, 2002): in this case the experimental group was constituted by acute or recurrent neck pain, instead of CNP.

Qualitative Studies

From the interviews administered by Lauche et al. (2012a) emerged a distorted subjective perception of neck proportion (as if it was swollen) in CNP patients persisting even when patients were aware that this perception did not match actual appearance. The perception of enlarged body parts was found also by Valenzuela-Moguillansky (2013) in fibromyalgic patients through the administration of 'elicitation interviews', a methodology stemming from the phenomenological approach. They also reported other modification of the explicit SoP as changes in perceived heaviness, thickness and ownership: in stages of elevated level of pain, some patients described a paradoxical experience as if the painful body parts did not belong to them. Finally, they referred also the inability to localize painful body parts and an associated narrowing of the near space, as if it was shrunk while the body became larger.

Intervention Studies

Four out twelve intervention studies were conducted in an experimental setting (Preston and Newport, 2011; Diers et al., 2013; Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019), while the remaining eight were conducted in clinical settings (Barker et al., 2008; Wand et al., 2011, 2013a; Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013; Ryan et al., 2014; Louw et al., 2017). Supplementary Table 6 in Additional materials reports the methodology applied in each study, the clinical characteristics of patients, the outcome measures, the follow-up periods, and results. Only three studies monitored the treatment effect at follow-up periods (Wand et al., 2011; Morone et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013). The study of Gilpin et al. (2015), despite adopting an intervention tool (the MIRAGE system), was excluded from the intervention studies because it lacked an end-point that measured pain and/or disability, thus it was assessed only under the evaluation studies for baseline data reported the perceived distortion of osteoarthritis patients' hand. One study (Diers et al., 2013), adopted the term 'upper back pain' with no details about the definition and boundaries of the functional diagnosis: first author declared to have enrolled patients with CNP (personal communication).

Tactile Localization Training

Three studies (Barker et al., 2008; Wand et al., 2013a; Louw et al., 2017) adopted the concept of the somatic localization of touch (Longo et al., 2010), as a trainable perceptual ability. Wand et al. (2013a), in their cross-over randomized trial, administered a single session of acupuncture on the low back of two groups of 25 CLBP patients, asking in one group to localize where the needles have been inserted by depicting the point of needle insertion on a body chart. Pain was significantly (p = 0.008) less post-treatment (11-Numeric Rating Scale—NRS: −0.9; 95% CI = −0.3 to −1.5), regardless of the order of treatment administration (p = 0.182), but the magnitude of change was not clinically relevant. Pain reduction was higher (p = 0.011) for the group where the acupuncture was associated with the sensory discrimination training, however the effect size was not clinically relevant (11-Numeric Rating Scale: −0.8; 95% CI = −1.4 to −0.3). Louw et al. (2017) described a series of sixteen CLBP patients on which they administered a single, 5-min session of tactile localization, measuring pain intensity (11-NRS) and functionality (active lumbar flexion in centimetres). Patients were touched with the back of a pen on nine zones of the lower back in a random order; they were asked to localize the stimuli on a corresponding 9-block grid. Immediately after the treatment, pain decreased by 1.9 points (range: 0–6), and lumbar flexion increased of 4.8 centimetres (range: −1 to 21), in both cases over the clinical significance. Barker et al. (2008) compared the effect of a device (the FairMed) that is based on the principle of the localisation task, with a conventional TENS. The FairMed contains 16 vibrating points, controlled at distance and randomly activated. The subject has to localise where the vibrating point is acting and the device signals the correct responses through a visual and auditory feedback. Pain and disability improved significantly (p = 0.05), but without significant difference between the two devices.

Combined Therapy

Three studies (Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013) adopted the “SuPeR” (Surface for Perceptive Rehabilitation tool) and other two used a gradual perceptual re-training program (Wand et al., 2011; Ryan et al., 2014); all studies enrolled CLBP patients. The SuPeR treatment provided the adoption of postures and the execution of active exercises while lying supine on a table with a series of deformable latex cones of different hardness having the goal of stimulating the trunk skin surface: patients were asked to count and localize tactile stimuli, or to discriminate the hardness of cones. Morone et al. (2012) found an effectiveness in pain and disability levels reduction both for SuPeR treatment and Back School program respect to control group (medical and pharmacological assistance only) post-treatment and after 24 weeks (p < 0.001), but differences between two groups, despite statistically significant (p < 0.001), were never clinically relevant. The same trend was found also by Paolucci et al. (2012). Vetrano et al. (2013) studied a variant of the SuPeR treatment against the standard procedure described by Morone et al. (2012). The efficacy of the two proposed version of the SuPeR treatment were substantially equal (p > 0.05), and both procedures improves pain and disability respect to baseline values (p < 0.001), with variable clinical relevance (11-VAS range: −2 to −5.5, Oswestry Disability Index range: −14 to −21). Although an improvement in pain level and disability was globally reported for SuPeR approach, the effect size was variable and not always clinically significant at follow-up periods. Wand et al. (2011) described three cases of patients with CLBP treated with a mixed treatment comprising education, graded perceptual training (localization and graphesthesia tasks) and graded motor retraining for a minimum of 10 weeks: pain decreased at the end of treatment and after 1 month (2.9 and 3.9 on 11-NRS), as well as disability (5.2–9.6 on the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire-RMDQ), with statistical significance (p < 0.001). Finally, Ryan et al. (2014) adopted in a pilot-randomized trial controlled with a placebo group, the graded perceptual re-training protocol described by Wand et al. (2011), in adjunct to usual physiotherapy cares. Pain and disability improved after treatment in experimental group (−8 on 101-VAS and −1.6 on RMDQ), but without statistical significance (p>0.05), respect to placebo group (−33.2 on 101-VAS and −4 on RMDQ; p < 0.0).

Experimental Setting

Among studies conducted in experimental settings, Diers et al. (2013) tested the visual manipulation of the neck in CNP patients and healthy subjects Authors provided visual feedback, of the neck (neutral, enlarged or downscaled visual appearance) and of a neutral body part (hand dorsum), during pressure and electrical pain stimulation of the trapezius muscle. They found that all visual conditions of the neck (p < 0.001) but not of the neutral hand (p>0.05), reduced the perceived intensity of applied acute painful stimuli, both in patients and in controls, but changes were all under the clinical significance. Preston and Newport (2011) adopted the MIRAGE-multisensory illusion system, in patients with hand osteoarthritis, while Stanton et al. (2018) applied it in knee osteoarthritis patients. The MIRAGE system involved the visuo-tactile manipulation of a body part, inducing their stretching or a shrinking visual appearance, in addition to a tactile stimulation applied by the examiner that may be directed in a congruent or incongruent modality (tactile stimulation in the same direction of the visual illusion (e.g., in stretching direction, or in the opposite way). Both studies reported an analgesic effect, with a reduction in pain varying between 45 and 50% respect to the baseline in the study of Preston and Newport (2011), and 25% in the study of Stanton et al. (2018). In this last study it was also found that repetition of the illusion, better than prolonging the exposure, produced additional pain relief (40% respect to the baseline). Finally, Nishigami et al. (2019) adapted and preliminary tested the MIRAGE system in two CLBP patients without the adjunct of the tactile stimulation to the visual manipulation. In this pilot-study, the authors proposed a visual manipulation of the trunk modifying its muscular appearance, based on the common maladaptive beliefs of CLBP subjects about robustness and perceived vulnerability of their back. This kind of “cognitive” illusion seems to reduce pain only in the subject A, having higher level of body perception dysfunction (FreBAQ score: 29/36), catastrophization (Pain Catastrophizing Scale score—PCS: 50/52) and maladaptive beliefs (Back Beliefs Questionnaire score—BBQ: 67/45), respect to the subject B with lower scoring on these outcomes (FreBAQ: 0/36; BBQ: 39/45; PCS: 8/52).

Qualitative Studies

The study of Lauche et al. (2012a) is a qualitative investigation embedded in a RCT on the effect of the cupping therapy in CNP patients, compared to similar patients on a waiting list to receive treatment (Lauche et al., 2012b). From the interviews emerged a subjectively referred reduction in neck size (smaller), as consequences of pain relief. The Body Image Drawing (BID) appeared changed in both groups, but drawings were more complete and lines more symmetric, in the cupping therapy group.

Methodological Considerations

Only 5 out of the 38 included studies (Barker et al., 2008; Wand et al., 2013a, 2016; Adamczyk et al., 2018b; Nishigami et al., 2018) provided a-prori calculations of the sample size, thus their results cannot be generalized to larger population. A rich variety of research designs was adopted: 9 out 38 studies were case studies, case series, pilot trials or preliminary investigations (aggregated they represent 24% of all included studies) (Moseley, 2008; Preston and Newport, 2011; Wand et al., 2011; Louw et al., 2017; Moreira et al., 2017; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b; Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019) This strongly limit the comparison of findings between studies. These nine studies had also small sample sizes ranging between 2 and 20 subjects in nine studies (24% of cases), limiting the generalizability of results. Inclusion and exclusion criteria have been poorly documented in 29/38 studies (76%) (Figures 7, 8): the selection of the target population may have been not adequately performed, potentially biasing the validity of results. Sixteen percent of all included studies were conducted in an experimental setting (Grod and Diakow, 2002; Wand et al., 2010; Docherty et al., 2012; Gilpin et al., 2015; Treleaven and Takasaki, 2015; Martínez et al., 2018), limiting the applicability of methodologies proposed in clinical practice.

Assessment Studies

Groups of control subjects were not tested in six studies (Wand et al., 2013a, 2016; Hirakawa et al., 2014; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b; Nishigami et al., 2018), and two case-control investigations had no healthy subjects comparisons (Moseley, 2008; Beales et al., 2016): this issue represents a major limitation of these works, potentially limiting the validity of their findings. Although we can presume that explicit SoP of body parts should be normal in unaffected people (Longo and Haggard, 2012), a degree of distortion for implicit SoP has been found even in healthy people (Fuentes et al., 2013; Longo, 2017). For this reason the absence, or small size of healthy control groups weighs more on the validity of the studies investigated the implicit SoP (Wand et al., 2010, 2013b; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b) respect to those investigated the explicit SoP.

The FreBAQ and FreKAQ questionnaires were adapted from the NLSQ of Galer and Jensen (1999) and Frettlöh et al. (2006) validated in CRPS patients. Studies adopting these outcomes represented the majority of all the assessment studies (40%). It should be noted that questionnaires measuring explicit SoP are not validated against standard measures of reference. Of course, the criterion-validity of scales measuring this construct remain unknown and the contribution of the implicit body model and of the explicit cognitive representation are difficult to disentangle. Nevertheless, it must be considered that FreBAQ and FreKAQ involve two items asking for the explicit size and shape perception of body parts (items 6–9): they could be validated adopting recently proposed objective measure for the explicit SoP itself and for the body model (Longo and Haggard, 2010; Fuentes et al., 2013; Spitoni et al., 2015; Adamczyk et al., 2018b), accounting for the implicit perception of metric sizes of body parts. Moreover, item 5 of FreBAQ/FreKAQ asked about the explicit perceived location of body parts in space: it may be validated adopting the objective methodologies proposed by Longo and Haggard (2010) for the implicit position sense. Finally, other items investigated the body ownership (item 1) and agency (items 3 and 4), two constructs that have been extensively studied through the RHI paradigm and relative psychometric measures (Longo et al., 2008a).

Notably, we found only one study investigated the responsiveness of assessment tools respect to changes in clinical status (Lauche et al., 2012a). However, the qualitative nature of this investigation provide us only indicative data.

Treatment Studies

As highlighted in Figure 8, some issues threatened the clinical relevance of included studies. Four studies recruited not adequate control groups: for e.g., the study of Barker et al. (2008) compared the FairMed device with the TENS in CLBP patients. However, Cochrane Reviews (Khadilkar et al., 2005, 2008) found limited evidence for the use of TENS in CLBP treatment, and the international guidelines recommend to use a mixed-type of intervention in patients with this kind of disorder, composed by physiotherapy, exercises and psychological treatments (National Guideline Centre, 2016). Thus, the sole use of the TENS cannot be considered as the gold standard treatment for LBP, and FairMed device should be tested against a placebo-control group or with another more effective treatment. The same issue involved also the study of Morone et al. (2012), Paolucci et al. (2012), and Vetrano et al. (2013) in which the experimental SuPeR treatment approach for CLBP was compared to a group of patients who performed back school exercises, and to another group who performed a variant of the standard SuPeR treatment. The qualitative study of Lauche et al. (2012a) was embedded in a RCT (Lauche et al., 2012b) with a waiting-list control group and therefore it lacks of a comparison with other usual cares or placebo interventions.

Only two studies (Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019) evaluated at the baseline the presence of perceptual dysfunction thus, a large part of the treatments provided perceptual tasks aimed to reduce pain and/or disability without taking in consideration the potential relationship between SoP and pain perception. This methodological issue represents a major limitation of all intervention studies. Failure to detect possible sub-groups, as those found in some assessment studies for explicit SoP (Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017), may have limited the effectiveness of therapeutic procedures because authors may not have taken into account that different kind of misperception could produce variable results to the same treatment. Currently, therefore, it is not possible to draw any conclusions regarding their potential clinical value.

The adverse events were not reported across all included studies (Figure 8): although the majority of studies adopted non-invasive procedures we lack evidence about the occurrence of side effects, especially for two studies using invasive procedures (acupuncture and cupping therapy) (Lauche et al., 2012a; Wand et al., 2013a). Moreover, authors of the studies where bodily illusions were administered through mediated-reality systems (Preston and Newport, 2011; Diers et al., 2013; Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019), despite the minimal invasiveness of the procedures, did not report information for the tolerability of the equipment and of the illusions itself. In eight studies, the therapeutic procedures required dedicated technological (Barker et al., 2008; Preston and Newport, 2011; Diers et al., 2013; Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019) or homebuilt equipment (Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013): even if the materials assembly procedure is well described (Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013), costs were not reported, potentially limiting the clinical applicability of these therapeutic tools. Sixty-three percent of patients in the study of Barker et al. (2008) reported faults of the FairMed device during the experiments, a concern that may have limited the efficacy of the treatment tested.

Except for Morone et al. (2012) and Vetrano et al. (2013), almost all studies provided follow-up periods no longer than 4 weeks, thus limiting the possibility to assess long terms effect for treatments proposed. From a conceptual and terminological point of view, in some studies authors reported using sensory-based interventions (Barker et al., 2008; Morone et al., 2012; Ryan et al., 2014) where, instead, perceptual-based therapeutic strategies were tested. This distinction is not trivial because primary sensory processing is different respect to higher-order mechanisms underlying SoP (Longo and Haggard, 2010; Hillier et al., 2015; Mancini et al., 2015; Spitoni et al., 2015). This terminological misuse may hide an important conceptual issue: some studies may have been conceived and designed with the goal to ameliorate primary somatosensations (tactile acuity, proprioception, etc.), rather than SoP, and the results found may have been consequently biased by these conceptual and practical mismatches.

Finally, calculation of effect sizes' confidence intervals were not possible in four studies (Preston and Newport, 2011; Morone et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013; Louw et al., 2017) due to lack of relevant information: this reporting bias may have compromised the accuracy of results.

Discussion

To “live” our own body constitutes a fascinating and complex experience because the body represents a unique multisensory object. The body experience is not direct, as well as bodily illusions and pain perception, two perceptual experiences that illustrate the complexity of mental organizations. Rather, it is filtered by a numbers of factors such as somatosensory inputs, perceptual information and body memory (Riva, 2018). Therefore, to study perceptual disorders in painful conditions represents an opportunity to explore the mechanisms underlying how our brain generates the experience of one's body. The aim of this review was to provide a comprehensive map about the literature published on perceptual disorders in painful MDRDs, with the main goal to identify gaps in current knowledge and to obtain useful information for the future research agenda. Our findings should be interpreted considering the large methodological variety of included studies.

The amount of literature found (37 articles) attests that, since the first investigation of Moseley (2008), overall these topics have been received some attention during the last decade. Specific sub-groups of MDRSs have been investigated more extensively, as in the case of spinal pain. CLBP, CNP and Pelvic Pain, taken together, represent about 80% of all the included studies. At the same time, it is noticeable how some others clinical conditions such as the rheumatic diseases remained with little or sparse interest. For example, rheumatoid arthritis was not investigated, and only two studies (5%) investigated fibromyalgia (Valenzuela-Moguillansky, 2013; Martínez et al., 2018). Another poorly explored area is pain in upper and lower limbs (15% of included studies), among which osteoarthritis was the only studied condition (Preston and Newport, 2011; Hirakawa et al., 2014; Gilpin et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2017; Magni et al., 2018; Stanton et al., 2018).

Summary of Evidence and Clinical Interpretation

Assessment Studies

We found a wide and heterogeneous literature published in the field of MDRDs about SoP, SpP and BO. It predominantly concerned about the assessment (66% of included studies, 25/38), respect to the intervention strategies (34%, 13/38). Some preliminary evidence of distorted body experience in MDRDs emerged, mainly in the area of the explicit SoP for spinal pain assessed through the BID task (Moseley, 2008; Lauche et al., 2012a; Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017), questionnaires or visual estimation tasks (Gilpin et al., 2015). These preliminary findings are in line with evidence found in CRPS (Galer and Jensen, 1999; Förderreuther et al., 2004; Frettlöh et al., 2006), although with apparent less magnitude and frequency. In fact, NLS were reported in 54.4–90.2% of CRPS patients respect to 19–36%. found in MDRDs (Hirakawa et al., 2014). Notably, the results obtained through the BID are difficult to interpret and compare to each other due to the qualitative nature of this task: the assessment of the altered explicit SoP is left to the clinician's subjective judgment, potentially leaving a large margin of error in interpreting the results of the test. Moreover, both the BID task and questionnaires like FreBAQ/FreKAQ and NLSQ, involved a self-description and depiction of own's body parts in which are involved both perceptive and cognitive/affective contributions that are not easily separable. For this reason, these tasks should be considered as a complex and multidimensional way to assess explicit body experience. On the other hand, although some promising assessment methodologies have been proposed, a substantial gap in knowledge exists in the area of the implicit mechanisms guiding perceptual abilities, like the estimation of body parts' size and its location in space. The absence of studies that investigate the sub-domain of the implicit SoP may be interpreted as a lack of appropriate tools in MDRDs able to investigate this construct, or as a sparse knowledge about the distinction between implicit and explicit mechanisms underlying SoP. This may be not surprising if we consider that: a) this area of investigation is peculiar of the neuropsychology rather than rehabilitation sciences dealing with MDRDs; and (a) the comprehension about neural and operational mechanisms of body experience has grown only in the last few years (Longo et al., 2008b, 2015; Longo, 2015; Gallace and Bellan, 2018).

We found only three studies investigating the implicit SoP in MDRDs (Wand et al., 2013b; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b), of which one is a preliminary validation study and another is a case-study. Therefore, it appears that implicit SoP in MDRDs has received little attention, as well as in CRPS, where only sparse studies have been investigated this sub-domain of body perception (Lewis et al., 2010b; Reiswich et al., 2012). Despite this, some methodologies proposed in preliminary studies showed good psychometric values, as in the case of the 2-PET (Adamczyk et al., 2018b), and are easy to be implemented both in clinical practice (Wand et al., 2013b; Adamczyk et al., 2018a) and in future research studies.

Variable results were found for the association of SoP disorders with pain intensity, duration and disability. Higher and stronger associations were found in studies that examined the explicit SoP (mainly those adopting the FreBAQ and FreKAQ) compared to other methodologies, while conflicting results were found for studies assessing the implicit SoP. However, in this last case, the small number of studies (Wand et al., 2013b; Adamczyk et al., 2018a,b) and of subjects recruited may have influenced the findings. Overall, it is not possible to draw conclusions about causation due to the lack of cohort studies. In fact only one study had a longitudinal research design (Hirakawa et al., 2014): authors found a decreased level of perceptual dysfunctions 6 weeks post-knee arthroplasty but standard deviation values were high, indicating large variation among patients. Thus, it is unclear whether SoP dysfunctions are a consequence of persistent painful states, potential contributing factors or an epiphenomenon of pain. One proposed hypothesis has been reported in pregnancy-related pelvic pain in which body changes precede the onset of pain: in this case, anatomical variations in body sizes may have caused pain-related thoughts and fear of movements, generating maladaptive behaviours and altered body perceptions (Beales et al., 2016). In other conditions different than pregnancy-related ones, this combination of factors may explain misperceptions occurring only in body parts potentially affected by increment of size (e.g., axial joints, interphalangeal and metacarpal joints) where SoP distortions could occur subsequently to swelling phases (McCabe et al., 2004), but they may be not able to explain misperceptions affecting the spine.

Sub-groups detection

In some studies (Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017) seem to emerge sub-groups of patients with different features for the explicit SoP (normal, augmented and shrunken), although the association of each group with higher disability levels, or pain duration and intensity remains unclear. Similarly, for implicit SoP, Adamczyk et al. (2018a) presented a two-case report in which one patients showed an overestimation of the painful low-back side compared to non-painful locations in 2-PET (range: 45–206%) and an opposite trend in the second subject (underestimation ranged between 12 and 22%). However, it cannot be established if two or more different sub-groups emerged also for the implicit domain of SoP because this is the only study that found this apparent trend. The same authors, in another study with larger sample of CLBP patients (Adamczyk et al., 2018b), were not able to find the same sub-groups split found in the first double-case study. They also identified an overall underestimation of both sides of the spine in 2PET, challenging the relationship between pain location and body perception distortion.

Body ownership

As a part of our body experience, we have to consider that mental representations of own body include the concept of shape and contours perception of body parts (James, 1890) and the boundaries between them and the external space. The plasticity of this kind of body representation has been extensively studied through the RHI paradigm (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998). However, we found only one study investigating the response to the RHI in MDRDs (Martínez et al., 2018). These authors found that fibromyalgic patients were more prone to experience the illusion than controls. The capacity to localize and confine body sensations within the corporeal boundaries requires an intact somatorepresentation: a misperception in which a rubber hand 'taking the place' of own's real hand (body representation instability), could indicate a dysfunction in multisensory integration underlying SoP function, but it is not clear the relationship with clinical relevant variables, and thus the potential role played in pathophysiology of chronic pain. To the best of our knowledge, only one study was published assessing the RHI in CRPS patients (Reinersmann et al., 2013). Authors found preserved multisensory integration despite the presence of neglect-like symptoms, indicating a possible dissociation between the mechanisms involved in BO and explicit SoP. Noteworthy, it seems to appear a potential analogy between fibromyalgia and eating disorders: both conditions seem to have a more instable BO respect to healthy controls (Mussap and Salton, 2006; Eshkevari et al., 2012; Keizer et al., 2014) and are joined by augmented vigilance to internal body signals. In addition, they seemed to show dissatisfaction regarding some body parts, those more painful in fibromyalgic subjects and emotional-sensitive ones in anorectic and bulimic patients. Body dissatisfaction is thought to be caused by the discrepancy between an ideal body model and the current self-perception (Strauman et al., 1991; Vartanian, 2012). Despite the causation relationship is still unclear, it was found a correlation between negative body affective perception and pain severity in fibromyalgic patients (Akkaya et al., 2012). In our opinion, in order to avoid ineffective and limited approaches, as already found in eating disorders (Eshkevari et al., 2014), it should not be neglected the presence of such negative body-cognition appraisal also in fibromyalgic patients. The variable contribution to body experience of cognitive, affective and perceptual mechanisms should be considered in further studies, as already proposed for eating disorders treatment (Riva, 2011; Keizer et al., 2014; Serino et al., 2016a,b).

Space perception

Evidence emerged across included studies seemed to highlight the absence of SpP dysfunctions, at least for the extra-personal space measured through the CRAF test in a sample of patients with CNP and WAD: errors in SSV and SVO were under the limit of normality or very modest, and were not correlated with disability. The lack of studies conducted in disorders different than CNP and WAD makes it difficult to extent these findings to others MDRDs, or to compare these results with those found in CRPS (Sumitani et al., 2007a,b; Uematsu et al., 2009; Reinersmann et al., 2012; Christophe et al., 2016).

Treatment Studies

The majority of published intervention studies were preliminary pilot-tests, case studies and case series, or were conducted in experimental settings. For these reasons, evidence emerged about the intervention strategies proposed are very limited. Moreover, it is difficult to estimate the relative effectiveness of each single therapeutic component for studies adopting concomitant multiple approaches (Wand et al., 2011; Morone et al., 2012; Paolucci et al., 2012; Vetrano et al., 2013; Ryan et al., 2014). Overall, intervention studies suffered the absence of preliminary assessment for dysfunctions of SoP and BO at the baseline: this issue may have limited the effectiveness of the treatments because they were not appropriately focused on specific sub-groups of patients. In fact, as shown by assessment studies, some patients with MDRDs seem to present explicit or implicit SoP disorders respect to others (Mibu et al., 2015; Nishigami et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017; Adamczyk et al., 2018a).

Although they must be considered within the limits of their low evidence value, case studies and preliminary investigations showed promising results of dedicated interventions (Stanton et al., 2018; Nishigami et al., 2019) addressing specific kind of SoP disorders at the baseline, as found in CRPS (Lewis et al., 2019).

Despite the presence of major methodological limitations, some therapeutic strategies could be of potential clinical value, especially in light of the brief duration and frequency of administration (Wand et al., 2013a; Louw et al., 2017).

Clinical Implications

Currently, evidence is fragmented and insufficient to guide precise assessment and intervention actions in routinely clinical practice. Nevertheless, the majority of treatment methodologies and assessment tools described in this review represent simple, safe and inexpensive procedures, feasible for the use in daily clinical practice. Some of the therapeutic approaches proposed seem to improve movement and pain without performing any physical action (Louw et al., 2017; Nishigami et al., 2019). For this reason, they may be promising strategies to use in patients with elevated pain levels and movement restrictions, as in person having high level of fear-avoidance behaviours for movements and maladaptive beliefs, especially in early rehabilitation phases (Louw et al., 2017).

Patients affected by these kind of perception disturbances (as documented in CRPS) may be reluctant, if not directly questioned (Galer and Jensen, 1999; Lewis et al., 2010a), to talk with health care providers or within the family context (Galer and Jensen, 1999), due to the bizarre features that make them appear as having some form of psychological/psychiatric disturbance (Förderreuther et al., 2004), or fearful of not being believed (Lewis et al., 2007). The perception of body contours and ownership is usually taken for granted, but in circumstances in which derangements appear between what is perceived and what is real, both pain and stressful response may potentially increase as consequence to these conditions, together with fear-avoidant behaviour. For patients, not being able to rely on information coming from their bodies and experiencing such bodily illusions can be detrimental for quality of life, social interactions and, overall, for mental health (Lewis et al., 2007; Longo, 2015). For these reasons, despite the limited diagnostic capacity of the tools now available, we believe it is important that clinicians start to approach (Geri et al., 2019) and validate this kind of unpleasant experience. For instance, clinicians could tell patients that their clinical descriptions resemble the very common situation of receiving an injection from the dentist and thereby perceiving one's lips and cheeks as uncommonly swollen and distorted, despite one's awareness that they maintain their normal size. Moving from the preliminary findings of this review, clinicians should consider the role of distorted SoP, starting from directly asking patients about their body experience, or through the administration of easy and inexpensive qualitative and quantitative tools, as the BID and the 2-PET.

Recommendation for Future Research

Despite the range of methodological issues that limit the validity of the evidence we have discussed, some of the proposed assessment methodologies and therapeutic strategies, could represent useful starting points for further research.

Considering the complexity of the body experience phenomenon, future studies should consider the concomitant assessment of different domains of bodily experience (explicit and implicit SoP, BO, and SpP), in parallel to the clinical variables commonly used for clinical studies, as some authors have started to do with CRPS patients (Lewis et al., 2019). Intervention studies should determine the response of particular sub-groups of patients, for e.g., those with enlarged or diminished body perception (Mibu et al., 2015; Moreira et al., 2017; Adamczyk et al., 2018a), to dedicated perceptual training interventions (Lewis et al., 2019). Moreover, the preliminary detection of perceptual impairments and the potential identification of particular sub-groups in clinical studies may help to identify individuals who could potentially benefit from dedicated treatments, or sub-groups that may be resistant to usual cares.

Future studies may implement new advanced technologies for clinical purposes. For e.g., diagnostic studies may implement more accurate new digital tools (Turton et al., 2013), aimed at overcoming the excessive subjectivity of the clinicians in the assessment of BID, but preserving at the same time the subjectivity of patients in expressing their own's SoP. Virtual and augmented reality represent probably the new frontier for the study of body representation finalized at therapeutic clinical use in body perception dysfunctions.

It may be interesting to explore also the neural correlates of body experience disorders through the adoption of neuroimaging methods, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), during perceptual tasks execution, without limiting the investigation to the functionality of the primary sensory area, as primarily performed in MDRDs field since now.

The available studies on assessment and treatment described here need to be replicated in larger and higher-methodological quality studies with appropriate control groups, in order to confirm preliminary results emerged, and to determine whether perceptual disorders represent clinical consistent findings. At the same time, we encourage the production of diagnostic case studies/case series and the publication of preliminary validation studies aimed to describe new assessment and treatment methodology in MDRDs as already made in CRPS (Sumitani et al., 2007a; Uematsu et al., 2009; Christophe et al., 2016; Solcà et al., 2018). We think that these two preliminary steps may be useful starting points before large scale data collection, as in the case of Adamczyk et al. (2018a,b).

The implicit and explicit body experience represents certainly a complex construct to define and investigate. Despite the absence of recognized gold-standard procedures to validate perceptual dysfunctions, it's noteworthy that other psychophysical tests have been proposed (Longo and Haggard, 2010; Fuentes et al., 2013) and may be implemented in MDRDs.

Finally, both the FreBAQ/FreKAQ and the NLSQ items were not directly derived from patients' self-experience dedicated studies, as those represented by the qualitative research. Moreover, these questionnaires were borrowed and adapted from studies on CRPS patients, rather than from studies investigating directly MDRDs patients. For these reasons, qualitative interviews-based studies may represent a useful and appropriate methodological approach to obtain relevant themes to adopt for the implementation of questionnaire items directly based on patients' “first-person” perspective (Lewis et al., 2007; Valenzuela-Moguillansky, 2013). We must consider that SoP, SpP and BO, are essentially subjective phenomena. Therefore, we cannot achieve comprehensive and deeper knowledge on this personal experience without a direct involvement of patients with dysfunctional body experiences.

Strengths and Limitations

Our research team was multidisciplinary and multiprofessional in its composition aimed at limiting potential conflicting interpretations of terminology and concepts investigated (Anderson et al., 2008). Despite the comprehensive nature of this review and the amount of sources scanned, it is possible that the limitation of our search only to English language studies may have influenced the nature of the evidence found. In order to limit this potential publication bias, we have reported in additional materials (Supplementary Table 7) all potential eligible studies found in other languages, having at least the abstract in English. The presence of only one author for data extraction may have been a potential source of bias: to overcome this limitation we provide a secondary data check by a second reviewer. We limited our investigation to patients >16 years old, however potentially interesting findings and assessment strategies may be found in the research area of the idiopathic scoliosis (Picelli et al., 2016; Paolucci et al., 2017). The extensive heterogeneity of included studies prevents to draw robust conclusions for clinical practice. Nevertheless, the inclusion of quantitative, qualitative and mixed-method studies at this first literature mapping stage allowed to consider the different aspects involved in the complex phenomenon of the body experience. Finally, the theme of SoP is broad and this review cannot be considered exhaustive. We excluded studies investigating the perception of own's body under dynamic condition (Valenzuela-Moguillansky et al., 2017) and the related therapeutic strategies proposed (Horwitz et al., 2003, 2004; Wand et al., 2012), because they are not considered in the theoretical framework of Longo et al. (Longo et al., 2010; Longo, 2016) that we have adopted as reference. Moreover, we did not consider other kinds of perceptual information as those related to the perceived stiffness (Haigh et al., 2003; Stanton et al., 2017).

Conclusions

Alterations of the implicit and explicit body experience have been preliminary found through this literature review. Despite the unclarity about the association or causation with chronic pain in MDRDs, perceptual dysfunctions could be reasonably considered as having a potential impact on clinical outcomes. If confirmed in future methodological robust studies, they may be potentially considered as one of the dimensions involved in clinical presentations of MDRDs, on a par with pain perception, functional limitations and restrictions in participation. Since an effective treatment depends on an effective diagnostic procedure, before conducting new treatment studies, future research should prioritize the objectification of perceptual dysfunctions subjectively referred by patients or reported through qualitative methods.

Some important questions remain open to be addressed: (a) explicit and implicit dysfunctions found in CLBP and CNP may constitute a cause, a consequence or an epiphenomenon respect to pain perception?; (b) the sub-groups highlighted in CLBP and CNP are consistently present even in other kind of MDRDs and also in the implicit domain of SoP; and (c) considering that in healthy subjects, a degree of distorted implicit somatoperception has been found in recent neuropsychological studies with respect to an intact explicit mechanism (Longo, 2017), we have to expect a further deterioration of the implicit somatoperception in those patients found to have yet an altered conscious perception of their own's body?

In order to answer to these questions, we conclude by suggesting three future research lines:

a) Longitudinal studies providing pain and multiple body perception outcome measures, along an enough large period of observation, may constitute an appropriate answer to the first point;

b) Implicit and explicit methodology of assessment should be administered in parallel to patients in order to test the potential divergences between conscious and unconscious mechanisms of body perception within and between MDRDs: this suggestion answer both to the second and the third critical points highlighted;

c) Moreover, the adoption of case-cohort studies with control groups of healthy subjects, further answer to the third point.

Overall, in consideration of the amount of literature already published since now we sustain and propose that the theoretical concept to split between implicit and explicit mechanisms of SoP may constitute an important starting point for future research agenda.

As suggested by extensive literature in neuropsychological field, pain perception appears to interact with a range of factors, among which implicit and explicit SoP, SpP and BO. For this reason, for future research agenda we encourage researchers to combine experimental lines on body experience with those studying chronic pain in MDRDs.

In consideration of findings emerged and the quality of studies found, at the state of art, the conduction of a future systematic review is appropriate only to synthesize the psychometric properties of the FreBAQ.

Author Contributions

AV contributed to conception of the study, managed the database, extracted data from studies, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AV and DR contributed to the design and performed the first search strategy. DR and AG performed peer-review of electronic search strategies. EC, DL, and MP performed calibration phases, screening, and selection steps. DP and AV performed pilot-trials of data extraction. DP performed crosschecking of data extraction. DP and GR performed data analysis. GR, ML, and MT performed data interpretation. AG, GR, ML, and MT wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revisions, read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Mirko Job for contribution in database management, and Tommaso Geri for his valuable help in statistical assistance.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00083/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

2-PET, 2-Point Estimation Task; BO, Body Ownership; BID, Body Image Drawing; CLBP, Chronic Low Back Pain; CNP, Chronic Neck Pain; CRAF, Computerized Rod And Frame test; CRPS, Complex Regional Pain Syndrome; FreBAQ, Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire; FreKAQ, Fremantle Knee Awareness Questionnaire; MDRDs, Musculoskeletal Disorders and Rheumatic Diseases; MNss, Motor-Neglect sub-scale; NLSQ, Neglect-Like Symptoms Questionnaire; PTP, Point-to-Point; RHI, Rubber Hand Illusion; SoP, Somatoperception; SpP, Space Perception; SVV, Subjective Visual Verticality; SVH, Subjective Visual Horizontality; WAD, Whiplash Associated Disorders.

Footnotes

References

Adamczyk, W. M., Luedtke, K., Saulicz, O., and Saulicz, E. (2018a). Sensory dissociation in chronic low back pain: two case reports. Physiother. Theor. Pract. 34, 643–651. doi: 10.1080/09593985.2017.1423431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adamczyk, W. M., Slugocka, A., Mehlich, K., Saulicz, E., and Luedtke, K. (2018b). Preliminary validation of a two-point estimation task for the measurement of sensory dissociation in patients with chronic low back pain. Pain Med. 20, 2472–2478 doi: 10.1093/pm/pny220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akkaya, N., Akkaya, S., Atalay, N. S., Balci, C. S., and Sahin, F. (2012). Relationship between the body image and level of pain, functional status, severity of depression, and quality of life in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Clin. Rheumatol. 31, 983–988. doi: 10.1007/s10067-012-1965-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, S., Allen, P., Peckham, S., and Goodwin, N. (2008). Asking the right questions: Scoping studies in the commissioning of research on the organisation and delivery of health services. Health Res. Policy Syst. 6:7. doi: 10.1186/1478-4505-6-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azañón, E., and Haggard, P. (2009). Somatosensory processing and body representation. Cortex 45, 1078–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, K. L., Elliott, C. J., Sackley, C. M., and Fairbank, J. C. (2008). Treatment of chronic back pain by sensory discrimination training. A phase I RCT of a novel device (FairMed) vs. TENS. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 9:97. doi: 10.1186/1471-2474-9-97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beales, D., Lutz, A., Thompson, J., Wand, B. M., and O'Sullivan, P. (2016). Disturbed body perception, reduced sleep, and kinesiophobia in subjects with pregnancy-related persistent lumbopelvic pain and moderate levels of disability: an exploratory study. Man. Ther. 21, 69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.math.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlucchi, G., and Aglioti, S. M. (2010). The body in the brain revisited. Exp. Brain Res. 200, 25–35. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1970-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bevan, S. (2015). Economic impact of musculoskeletal disorders (MSDs) on work in Europe. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 29, 356–373. doi: 10.1016/j.berh.2015.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blyth, F. M., Briggs, A. M., Schneider, C. H., Hoy, D. G., and March, L. M. (2019). The global burden of musculoskeletal pain—where to from here? Am. J. Public Health 109, 35–40. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2018.304747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bombardier, C., Hayden, J., and Beaton, D. E. (2001). Minimal clinically important difference. Low back pain: outcome measures. J. Rheumatol. 28, 431–438.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M. (2004). Probing the neural basis of body ownership. Science 305, 782–783. doi: 10.1126/science.1101836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel' touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756. doi: 10.1038/35784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, A. M., Woolf, A. D., Dreinhöfer, K., Homb, N., Hoy, D. G., Kopansky-Giles, D., et al. (2018). Reducing the global burden of musculoskeletal conditions. Bull. World Health Organ. 96, 366–368. doi: 10.2471/BLT.17.204891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canadian Institutes of Health Research (2010). A Guide to Knowledge Synthesis - CIHR. Available online at: http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/41382.html (accessed July 31, 2017).

PubMed Abstract

Carruthers, G. (2008). Types of body representation and the sense of embodiment. Conscious Cogn. 17, 1302–1316. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catley, M. J., O'Connell, N. E., Berryman, C., Ayhan, F. F., and Moseley, G. L. (2014). Is tactile acuity altered in people with chronic pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Pain 15, 985–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2014.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catley, M. J., Tabor, A., Wand, B. M., and Moseley, G. L. (2013). Assessing tactile acuity in rheumatology and musculoskeletal medicine–how reliable are two-point discrimination tests at the neck, hand, back and foot? Rheumatology 52, 1454–1461. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/ket140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (2009). Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (CRD) Guidance for Undertaking Systematic Reviews. York: The University of York.

Christophe, L., Delporte, L., Revol, P., dePaepe, A., Rode, G., Jacquin-Courtois, S., et al. (2016). Complex regional pain syndrome associated with hyperattention rather than neglect for the healthy side: a comprehensive case study. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 59, 294–301. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colquhoun, H. L., Jesus, T. S., O'Brien, K. K., Tricco, A. C., Chui, A., Zarin, W., et al. (2017). Study protocol for a scoping review on rehabilitation scoping reviews. Clin. Rehabil. 31, 1249–1256. doi: 10.1177/0269215516688514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colquhoun, H. L., Levac, D., O'Brien, K. K., Straus, S., Tricco, A. C., Perrier, L., et al. (2014). Scoping reviews: time for clarity in definition, methods, and reporting. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 67, 1291–1294. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Copay, A. G., Glassman, S. D., Subach, B. R., Berven, S., Schuler, T. C., and Carreon, L. Y. (2008). Minimum clinically important difference in lumbar spine surgery patients: a choice of methods using the oswestry disability index, medical outcomes study questionnaire short form 36, and pain scales. Spine J. 8, 968–974. doi: 10.1016/j.spinee.2007.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daneshmandi, H., Choobineh, A. R., Ghaem, H., Alhamd, M., and Fakherpour, A. (2017). The effect of musculoskeletal problems on fatigue and productivity of office personnel: a cross-sectional study. J. Prev. Med. Hyg. 58, E252–E258.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

de Vignemont, F. (2007). How many representations of the body? Behav. Brain Sci. 30, 204–205. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vignemont, F. (2010). Body schema and body image–pros and cons. Neuropsychologia 48, 669–680. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diers, M., Zieglgänsberger, W., Trojan, J., Drevensek, A. M., Erhardt-Raum, G., and Flor, H. (2013). Site-specific visual feedback reduces pain perception. Pain 154, 890–896. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Docherty, S., Schärer, R., Bagust, J., and Humphreys, B. K. (2012). Perception of subjective visual vertical and horizontal in patients with chronic neck pain: a cross-sectional observational study. Man. Ther. 17, 133–138. doi: 10.1016/j.math.2011.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrenbrusthoff, K., Ryan, C. G., Grüneberg, C., Wand, B. M., and Martin, D. J. (2018). The translation, validity and reliability of the German version of the fremantle back awareness questionnaire. PLoS ONE 13:e0205244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekedahl, H., Jönsson, B., and Frobell, R. B. (2012). Fingertip-to-floor test and straight leg raising test: validity, responsiveness, and predictive value in patients with acute/subacute low back pain. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 2210–2215. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2012.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eshkevari, E., Rieger, E., Longo, M. R., Haggard, P., and Treasure, J. (2012). Increased plasticity of the bodily self in eating disorders. Psychol. Med. 42, 819–828. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eshkevari, E., Rieger, E., Longo, M. R., Haggard, P., and Treasure, J. (2014). Persistent body image disturbance following recovery from eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 400–409. doi: 10.1002/eat.22219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, W., Zhang, R., Zhao, Y., Wang, L., and Zhou, Y.-D. (2019). Attenuation of pain perception induced by the rubber hand illusion. Front. Neurosci. 13:261. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrar, J. T., Young, J. P., LaMoreaux, L., Werth, J. L., and Poole, R. M. (2001). Clinical importance of changes in chronic pain intensity measured on an 11-point numerical pain rating scale. Pain 94, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00349-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flor, H. (2003). Cortical reorganisation and chronic pain: implications for rehabilitation. J. Rehabil. Med. 41(Suppl), 66–72. doi: 10.1080/16501960310010179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flor, H., Braun, C., Elbert, T., and Birbaumer, N. (1997). Extensive reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in chronic back pain patients. Neurosci. Lett. 224, 5–8. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)13441-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Förderreuther, S., Sailer, U., and Straube, A. (2004). Impaired self-perception of the hand in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS). Pain 110, 756–761. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frettlöh, J., Hüppe, M., and Maier, C. (2006). Severity and specificity of neglect-like symptoms in patients with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) compared to chronic limb pain of other origins. Pain 124, 184–189. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes, C. T., Longo, M. R., and Haggard, P. (2013). Body image distortions in healthy adults. Acta Psychol. 144, 344–351. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furlan, A. D., Pennick, V., Bombardier, C., van Tulder, M., and Editorial Board Cochrane Back Review Group (2009). 2009 updated method guidelines for systematic reviews in the cochrane back review group. Spine 34, 1929–1941. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181b1c99f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gadsby, S. (2017). Distorted body representations in anorexia nervosa. Conscious. Cogn. 51, 17–33. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gadsby, S. (2019). Body representations and cognitive ontology: drawing the boundaries of the body image. Conscious. Cogn.74:102772. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galer, B. S., and Jensen, M. (1999). Neglect-like symptoms in complex regional pain syndrome: results of a self-administered survey. J. Pain Symptom. Manage. 18, 213–217. doi: 10.1016/S0885-3924(99)00076-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallace, A., and Bellan, V. (2018). The parietal cortex and pain perception: a body protection system. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 151, 103–117. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63622-5.00005-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallace, A., and Spence, C. (2008). The cognitive and neural correlates of “tactile consciousness”: a multisensory perspective. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 370–407. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallace, A., Torta, D. M. E., Moseley, G. L., and Iannetti, G. D. (2011). The analgesic effect of crossing the arms. Pain 152, 1418–1423. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (1986). Body image and body schema: a conceptual clarification. J. Mid. Behav. 7, 541–554.

Google Scholar

Geri, T., Viceconti, A., Minacci, M., Testa, M., and Rossettini, G. (2019). Manual therapy: exploiting the role of human touch. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 44:102044. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2019.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilpin, H. R., Moseley, G. L., Stanton, T. R., and Newport, R. (2015). Evidence for distorted mental representation of the hand in osteoarthritis. Rheumatology 54, 678–682. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/keu367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenhalgh, T., and Peacock, R. (2005). Effectiveness and efficiency of search methods in systematic reviews of complex evidence: audit of primary sources. BMJ 331, 1064–1065. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38636.593461.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grod, J. P., and Diakow, P. R. (2002). Effect of neck pain on verticality perception: a cohort study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 83, 412–415. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.29660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P., Iannetti, G. D., and Longo, M. R. (2013). Spatial sensory organization and body representation in pain perception. Curr. Biol. 23, R164–R176. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P., and Wolpert, D. M. (2005). “Disorders of body schema,” in Higher-Order Motor Disorders: from Neuroanatomy and Neurobiology to Clinical Neurology, eds H. J. Freund, M. Jeannerod, M. Hallett, and R. Leiguarda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 261–271.

Google Scholar

Haigh, R. C., McCabe, C. S., Halligan, P. W., and Blake, D. R. (2003). Joint stiffness in a phantom limb: evidence of central nervous system involvement in rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatology 42, 888–892. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/keg243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvie, D. S., Edmond-Hank, G., and Smith, A. D. (2018). Tactile acuity is reduced in people with chronic neck pain. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 33, 61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2017.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Head, H., and Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34, 102–254. doi: 10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., and Green, S. (2008). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Google Scholar

Hillier, S., Immink, M., and Thewlis, D. (2015). Assessing proprioception: a systematic review of possibilities. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 29, 933–949. doi: 10.1177/1545968315573055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirakawa, Y., Hara, M., Fujiwara, A., Hanada, H., and Morioka, S. (2014). The relationship among psychological factors, neglect-like symptoms and postoperative pain after total knee arthroplasty. Pain Res. Manag. 19, 251–256. doi: 10.1155/2014/471529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, E. B., Theorell, T., and Anderberg, U. M. (2003). Fibromyalgia patients' own experiences of video self-interpretation: a phenomenological-hermeneutic study. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 17, 257–264. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-6712.2003.00223.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, E. B., Theorell, T., and Anderberg, U. M. (2004). New technique for assessment of self-perception in fibromyalgia patients: a pilot study with video-interpretation. Arts Psychother. 31, 153–164. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2004.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hotz-Boendermaker, S., Marcar, V. L., Meier, M. L., Boendermaker, B., and Humphreys, B. K. (2016). Reorganization in secondary somatosensory cortex in chronic low back pain patients. Spine 41, E667–E673. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0000000000001348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1, New York, NY: Henry Holt and Co.

Google Scholar

Janssens, L., Goossens, N., Wand, B. M., Pijnenburg, M., Thys, T., and Brumagne, S. (2017). The development of the dutch version of the fremantle back awareness questionnaire. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 32, 84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2017.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnsen, L. G., Hellum, C., Nygaard, Ø. P., Storheim, K., Brox, J. I., Rossvoll, I., et al. (2013). Comparison of the SF6D, the EQ5D, and the oswestry disability index in patients with chronic low back pain and degenerative disc disease. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 14:148. doi: 10.1186/1471-2474-14-148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keizer, A., Smeets, M. A. M., Dijkerman, H. C., van den Hout, M., Klugkist, I., van Elburg, A., et al. (2011). Tactile body image disturbance in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 190, 115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keizer, A., Smeets, M. A. M., Postma, A., van Elburg, A., and Dijkerman, H. C. (2014). Does the experience of ownership over a rubber hand change body size perception in anorexia nervosa patients? Neuropsychologia 62, 26–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khadilkar, A., Milne, S., Brosseau, L., Wells, G., Tugwell, P., Robinson, V., et al. (2005). Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation for the treatment of chronic low back pain: a systematic review. Spine 30, 2657–2666. doi: 10.1097/01.brs.0000188189.21202.0f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khadilkar, A., Odebiyi, D. O., Brosseau, L., and Wells, G. A. (2008). Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) versus placebo for chronic low-back pain. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. CD003008. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003008.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauche, R., Cramer, H., Haller, H., Musial, F., Langhorst, J., Dobos, G. J., et al. (2012a). My back has shrunk: the influence of traditional cupping on body image in patients with chronic non-specific neck pain. Forsch. Komplementmed. 19, 68–74. doi: 10.1159/000337688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauche, R., Cramer, H., Hohmann, C., Choi, K.-E., Rampp, T., Saha, F. J., et al. (2012b). The effect of traditional cupping on pain and mechanical thresholds in patients with chronic nonspecific neck pain: a randomised controlled pilot study. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2012:429718. doi: 10.1155/2012/429718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, J., Cot, D., McCabe, C., and RGN (2010a). Body Perception Disturbance (BPD) in CRPS. Practical Pain Management. Available online at: https://www.practicalpainmanagement.com/pain/neuropathic/crps/body-perception-disturbance-bpd-crps (accessed August 19, 2018).

Google Scholar

Lewis, J. S., Kellett, S., McCullough, R., Tapper, A., Tyler, C., Viner, M., et al. (2019). Body perception disturbance and pain reduction in longstanding complex regional pain syndrome following a multidisciplinary rehabilitation program. Pain Med. 20, 2213–2219. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnz176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, J. S., Kersten, P., McCabe, C. S., McPherson, K. M., and Blake, D. R. (2007). Body perception disturbance: a contribution to pain in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS). Pain 133, 111–119. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, J. S., Kersten, P., McPherson, K. M., Taylor, G. J., Harris, N., McCabe, C. S., et al. (2010b). Wherever is my arm? Impaired upper limb position accuracy in complex regional pain syndrome. Pain 149, 463–469. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J., Halaki, M., Rajan, P., and Leaver, A. (2019). Relationship between proprioception and pain and disability in people with non-specific low back pain: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Spine 44, E606–E617. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0000000000002917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R. (2015). Implicit and explicit body representations. Eur. Psychol. 20, 6–15. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R. (2016). “Types of body representation,” in Foundations of embodied cognition: Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment (New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group), 117–134.

Google Scholar

Longo, M. R. (2017). Distorted body representations in healthy cognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 378–388. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1143956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Azañón, E., and Haggard, P. (2010). More than skin deep: body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex. Neuropsychologia 48, 655–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Cardozo, S., and Haggard, P. (2008a). Visual enhancement of touch and the bodily self. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 1181–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., and Haggard, P. (2010). An implicit body representation underlying human position sense. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 11727–11732. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003483107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., and Haggard, P. (2012). Implicit body representations and the conscious body image. Acta Psychol. 141, 164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Mancini, F., and Haggard, P. (2015). Implicit body representations and tactile spatial remapping. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 160, 77–87. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M., Tsakiris, M., and Haggard, P. (2008b). What is embodiment? A psychometric approach. Cognition 107, 978–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Louw, A., Puentedura, E. J., Reese, D., Parker, P., Miller, T., and Mintken, P. E. (2017). Immediate effects of mirror therapy in patients with shoulder pain and decreased range of motion. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 98, 1941–1947. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magni, N. E., McNair, P. J., and Rice, D. A. (2018). Sensorimotor performance and function in people with osteoarthritis of the hand: a case-control comparison. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 47, 676–682. doi: 10.1016/j.semarthrit.2017.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, F., Longo, M. R., Kammers, M. P. M., and Haggard, P. (2011). Visual distortion of body size modulates pain perception. Psychol. Sci. 22, 325–330. doi: 10.1177/0956797611398496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, F., Steinitz, H., Steckelmacher, J., Iannetti, G. D., and Haggard, P. (2015). Poor judgment of distance between nociceptive stimuli. Cognition 143, 41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

March, L., Smith, E. U. R., Hoy, D. G., Cross, M. J., Sanchez-Riera, L., Blyth, F., et al. (2014). Burden of disability due to musculoskeletal (MSK) disorders. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 28, 353–366. doi: 10.1016/j.berh.2014.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez, E., Aira, Z., Buesa, I., Aizpurua, I., Rada, D., and Azkue, J. J. (2018). Embodied pain in fibromyalgia: disturbed somatorepresentations and increased plasticity of the body schema. PLoS ONE 13:e0194534. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, C. S., Haigh, R. C., Shenker, N. G., Lewis, J., and Blake, D. R. (2004). Phantoms in rheumatology. Novartis Found. Symp. 260, 154–174. discussion 174–178, 277–279. doi: 10.1002/0470867639.ch11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGowan, J., Sampson, M., Salzwedel, D. M., Cogo, E., Foerster, V., and Lefebvre, C. (2016). PRESS peer review of electronic search strategies: 2015 guideline statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 75, 40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2016.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J., and Coslett, H. B. (2016a). Understanding body representations. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 33, 1–4. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1197195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J., and Coslett, H. B. (2016b). What can errors tell us about body representations? Cogn Neuropsychol 33, 5–25. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1188065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mibu, A., Nishigami, T., Kishishita, S., Son, K., Yamamoto, S., Tanaka, K., et al. (2015). The relationships between the distorted body image of neck and clinical profile in patients with chronic nonspecific neck pain. Pain Res. 30, 30–36. doi: 10.11154/pain.30.30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, H. M., Zimmermann, J., Röder, C., Lenz, C., Overbeck, G., and Grabhorn, R. (2010). Separating two components of body image in anorexia nervosa using fMRI. Psychol. Med. 40, 1519–1529. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira, C., Bassi, A. R., Brandão, M. P., and Silva, A. G. (2017). Do patients with chronic neck pain have distorted body image and tactile dysfunction? Eur. J. Physiother. 19, 215–221. doi: 10.1080/21679169.2017.1334818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morone, G., Iosa, M., Paolucci, T., Fusco, A., Alcuri, R., Spadini, E., et al. (2012). Efficacy of perceptive rehabilitation in the treatment of chronic nonspecific low back pain through a new tool: a randomized clinical study. Clin. Rehabil. 26, 339–350. doi: 10.1177/0269215511414443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moseley, G. L. (2008). I can't find it! Distorted body image and tactile dysfunction in patients with chronic back pain. Pain 140, 239–243. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moseley, G. L., Gallace, A., and Spence, C. (2012). Bodily illusions in health and disease: physiological and clinical perspectives and the concept of a cortical “body matrix.” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 34–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moseley, G. L., Parsons, T. J., and Spence, C. (2008). Visual distortion of a limb modulates the pain and swelling evoked by movement. Curr. Biol. 18, R1047–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murata, A., and Ishida, H. (2007). “Representation of bodily self in the multimodal parieto-premotor network,” in Representation and Brain, ed S. Funahashi (Tokyo: Springer Japan), 151–176. doi: 10.1007/978-4-431-73021-7_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murata, A., Wen, W., and Asama, H. (2016). The body and objects represented in the ventral stream of the parieto-premotor network. Neurosci. Res. 104, 4–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mussap, A. J., and Salton, N. (2006). A “rubber-hand” illusion reveals a relationship between perceptual body image and unhealthy body change. J. Health Psychol. 11, 627–639. doi: 10.1177/1359105306065022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Guideline Centre (UK) (2016). Low back pain and sciatica in over 16s: assessment and management. London: National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (UK). Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK401577/ (accessed July 30, 2019).

Google Scholar

Nishigami, T., Mibu, A., Osumi, M., Son, K., Yamamoto, S., Kajiwara, S., et al. (2015). Are tactile acuity and clinical symptoms related to differences in perceived body image in patients with chronic nonspecific lower back pain? Man. Ther. 20, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.math.2014.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishigami, T., Mibu, A., Tanaka, K., Yamashita, Y., Shimizu, M. E., Wand, B. M., et al. (2018). Validation of the Japanese version of the fremantle back awareness questionnaire in patients with low back pain. Pain Pract. 18, 170–179. doi: 10.1111/papr.12586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishigami, T., Mibu, A., Tanaka, K., Yamashita, Y., Yamada, E., Wand, B. M., et al. (2017). Development and psychometric properties of knee-specific body-perception questionnaire in people with knee osteoarthritis: the fremantle knee awareness questionnaire. PLoS ONE 12:e0179225. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishigami, T., Okuno, H., Nakano, H., Omura, Y., Osumi, M., Michele, S., et al. (2012). Effects of a hardness discrimination task in failed back surgery syndrome with severe low back pain and disturbed body image: case study. J. Nov. Physiother. S1:008. doi: 10.4172/2165-7025.S1-008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishigami, T., Wand, B. M., Newport, R., Ratcliffe, N., Themelis, K., Moen, D., et al. (2019). Embodying the illusion of a strong, fit back in people with chronic low back pain. A pilot proof-of-concept study. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 39, 178–183. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2018.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostelo, R. W. J. G., Deyo, R. A., Stratford, P., Waddell, G., Croft, P., Von Korff, M., et al. (2008). Interpreting change scores for pain and functional status in low back pain: towards international consensus regarding minimal important change. Spine 33, 90–94. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0b013e31815e3a10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pamment, J., and Aspell, J. E. (2017). Putting pain out of mind with an “out of body” illusion. Eur. J. Pain 21, 334–342. doi: 10.1002/ejp.927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paolucci, T., Baldari, C., Di Franco, M., Didona, D., Reis, V., Vetrano, M., et al. (2016). A new rehabilitation tool in fibromyalgia: the effects of perceptive rehabilitation on pain and function in a clinical randomized controlled trial. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2016:7574589. doi: 10.1155/2016/7574589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paolucci, T., Fusco, A., Iosa, M., Grasso, M. R., Spadini, E., Paolucci, S., et al. (2012). The efficacy of a perceptive rehabilitation on postural control in patients with chronic nonspecific low back pain. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 35, 360–366. doi: 10.1097/MRR.0b013e328356427c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paolucci, T., Piccinini, G., Iosa, M., Piermattei, C., De Angelis, S., Zangrando, F., et al. (2017). The importance of trunk perception during brace treatment in moderate juvenile idiopathic scoliosis: What is the impact on self-image? J. Back Musculoskelet. Rehabil. 30, 203–210. doi: 10.3233/BMR-160733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peltz, E., Seifert, F., Lanz, S., Müller, R., and Maihöfner, C. (2011). Impaired hand size estimation in CRPS. J. Pain 12, 1095–1101. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, M. D. J., Godfrey, C. M., Khalil, H., McInerney, P., Parker, D., and Soares, C. B. (2015). Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int. J. Evid. Based Healthc. 13, 141–146. doi: 10.1097/XEB.0000000000000050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pia, L., Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Burin, D., and Berti, A. (2016). Sensing the body, representing the body: Evidence from a neurologically based delusion of body ownership. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 33, 112–119. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1185404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pia, L., Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Fornia, L., and Berti, A. (2013). Pain and body awareness: evidence from brain-damaged patients with delusional body ownership. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:298. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picelli, A., Negrini, S., Zenorini, A., Iosa, M., Paolucci, S., and Smania, N. (2016). Do adolescents with idiopathic scoliosis have body schema disorders? A cross-sectional study. J. Back Musculoskelet. Rehabil. 29, 89–96. doi: 10.3233/BMR-150602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitron, V., Alsmith, A., and de Vignemont, F. (2018). How do the body schema and the body image interact? Conscious. Cogn. 65, 352–358. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pool, J. J. M., Ostelo, R. W. J. G., Hoving, J. L., Bouter, L. M., and de Vet, H. C. W. (2007). Minimal clinically important change of the neck disability index and the numerical rating scale for patients with neck pain. Spine 32, 3047–3051. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0b013e31815cf75b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preston, C., and Newport, R. (2011). Analgesic effects of multisensory illusions in osteoarthritis. Rheumatology 50, 2314–2315. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/ker104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramakonar, H., Franz, E. A., and Lind, C. R. P. (2011). The rubber hand illusion and its application to clinical neuroscience. J. Clin. Neurosci. 18, 1596–1601. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2011.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinersmann, A., Landwehrt, J., Krumova, E. K., Ocklenburg, S., Güntürkün, O., and Maier, C. (2012). Impaired spatial body representation in complex regional pain syndrome type 1 (CRPS I). Pain 153, 2174–2181. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinersmann, A., Landwehrt, J., Krumova, E. K., Peterburs, J., Ocklenburg, S., Güntürkün, O., et al. (2013). The rubber hand illusion in complex regional pain syndrome: preserved ability to integrate a rubber hand indicates intact multisensory integration. Pain 154, 1519–1527. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.03.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiswich, J., Krumova, E. K., David, M., Stude, P., Tegenthoff, M., and Maier, C. (2012). Intact 2D-form recognition despite impaired tactile spatial acuity in complex regional pain syndrome type I. Pain 153, 1484–1494. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riva, G. (2011). The key to unlocking the virtual body: virtual reality in the treatment of obesity and eating disorders. J. Diabetes Sci. Technol. 5, 283–292. doi: 10.1177/193229681100500213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riva, G. (2018). The neuroscience of body memory: from the self through the space to the others. Cortex 104, 241–260. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, C., Harland, N., Drew, B., and Martin, D. (2014). Tactile acuity training for patients with chronic low back pain: a pilot randomised controlled trial. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 15:59. doi: 10.1186/1471-2474-15-59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpina, F., Castelnuovo, G., and Molinari, E. (2014). Tactile mental body parts representation in obesity. Psychiatry Res. 220, 960–969. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwoebel, J., and Coslett, H. B. (2005). Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the human body. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 543–553. doi: 10.1162/0898929053467587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serino, S., Pedroli, E., Keizer, A., Triberti, S., Dakanalis, A., Pallavicini, F., et al. (2016a). Virtual reality body swapping: a tool for modifying the allocentric memory of the body. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 19, 127–133. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serino, S., Scarpina, F., Keizer, A., Pedroli, E., Dakanalis, A., Castelnuovo, G., et al. (2016b). A novel technique for improving bodily experience in a non-operable super-super obesity case. Front. Psychol. 7:837. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solcà, M., Ronchi, R., Bello-Ruiz, J., Schmidlin, T., Herbelin, B., Luthi, F., et al. (2018). Heartbeat-enhanced immersive virtual reality to treat complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology 91, e479–e489. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitoni, G. F., Serino, A., Cotugno, A., Mancini, F., Antonucci, G., and Pizzamiglio, L. (2015). The two dimensions of the body representation in women suffering from Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 230, 181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, T. R., Gilpin, H. R., Edwards, L., Moseley, G. L., and Newport, R. (2018). Illusory resizing of the painful knee is analgesic in symptomatic knee osteoarthritis. PeerJ. 6:e5206. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, T. R., Leake, H. B., Chalmers, K. J., and Moseley, G. L. (2016). Evidence of impaired proprioception in chronic, idiopathic neck pain: systematic review and meta-analysis. Phys. Ther. 96, 876–887. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20150241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, T. R., Moseley, G. L., Wong, A. Y. L., and Kawchuk, G. N. (2017). Feeling stiffness in the back: a protective perceptual inference in chronic back pain. Sci. Rep. 7:9681. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-09429-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stratford, P., Riddle, D., Binkley, J., Spadoni, G., Westaway, M., and Padfiled, B. (2009). Using the neck disability index to make decisions concerning individual patients. Physiother. Can. 1999, 107–119.

Google Scholar

Strauman, T. J., Vookles, J., Berenstein, V., Chaiken, S., and Higgins, E. T. (1991). Self-discrepancies and vulnerability to body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 946–956. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumitani, M., Rossetti, Y., Shibata, M., Matsuda, Y., Sakaue, G., Inoue, T., et al. (2007a). Prism adaptation to optical deviation alleviates pathologic pain. Neurology 68, 128–133. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000250242.99683.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumitani, M., Shibata, M., Iwakura, T., Matsuda, Y., Sakaue, G., Inoue, T., et al. (2007b). Pathologic pain distorts visuospatial perception. Neurology 68, 152–154. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000250335.56958.f0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takasaki, H., Treleaven, J., Johnston, V., and Jull, G. (2012). Minimum repetitions for stable measures of visual dependency using the dot version of the computer-based rod-frame test. Man. Ther. 17, 466–469. doi: 10.1016/j.math.2012.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor-Clarke, M., Jacobsen, P., and Haggard, P. (2004). Keeping the world a constant size: object constancy in human touch. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 219–220. doi: 10.1038/nn1199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Joanna Briggs Institute (2015). Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewers' Manual: 2015 edition/supplement. Adelaide, SA.

Tong, M. H., Mousavi, S. J., Kiers, H., Ferreira, P., Refshauge, K., and van Dieën, J. (2017). Is there a relationship between lumbar proprioception and low back pain? A systematic review with meta-analysis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 98, 120–136.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2016.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treleaven, J., and Takasaki, H. (2015). High variability of the subjective visual vertical test of vertical perception, in some people with neck pain - should this be a standard measure of cervical proprioception? Man. Ther. 20, 183–188. doi: 10.1016/j.math.2014.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O'Brien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., et al. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 169, 467–473. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trojan, J., Diers, M., Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C., and Torta, D. M. E. (2014). Body, space, and pain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:369. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., and Fotopoulou, A. (2008). Is my body the sum of online and offline body-representations? Conscious. Cogn. 17, 1317–1320. discussion 1321–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., Prabhu, G., and Haggard, P. (2006). Having a body versus moving your body: how agency structures body-ownership. Conscious. Cogn. 15, 423–432. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsay, A., Allen, T. J., Proske, U., and Giummarra, M. J. (2015). Sensing the body in chronic pain: a review of psychophysical studies implicating altered body representation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 52, 221–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turton, A. J., Palmer, M., Grieve, S., Moss, T. P., Lewis, J., and McCabe, C. S. (2013). Evaluation of a prototype tool for communicating body perception disturbances in complex regional pain syndrome. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:517. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uematsu, H., Sumitani, M., Yozu, A., Otake, Y., Shibata, M., Mashimo, T., et al. (2009). Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) impairs visuospatial perception,whereas post-herpetic neuralgia does not: possible implications for supraspinal mechanism of CRPS. Ann. Acad. Med. Singap. 38, 931–936.

Google Scholar

Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C. (2013). Pain and body awareness: an exploration of the bodily experience of persons suffering from fibromyalgia. Constructivist Foundations 8, 339–350.

Google Scholar

Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C., Reyes-Reyes, A., and Gaete, M. I. (2017). Exteroceptive and interoceptive body-self awareness in fibromyalgia patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci.11:117. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Heijde, D., Daikh, D. I., Betteridge, N., Burmester, G. R., Hassett, A. L., Matteson, E. L., et al. (2018). Common language description of the term rheumatic and musculoskeletal diseases (RMDs) for use in communication with the lay public, healthcare providers and other stakeholders endorsed by the European League Against Rheumatism (EULAR) and the American College of Rheumatology (ACR). Ann. Rheum. Dis. 77, 829–832. doi: 10.1136/annrheumdis-2017-212565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vartanian, L. R. (2012). “Self-discrepancy theory and body image,” in Encyclopedia of Body Image and Human Appearance (Oxford, UK: Elsevier), 711–717.

Google Scholar

Vetrano, M., Pournajaf, S., Vulpiani, M., Santoboni, F., Paolucci, T., Spadini, E., et al. (2013). Perceptive rehabilitation of patients with non-specific chronic low back pain: the role of body midline. A randomized controlled trial. Eur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 49, 517–525.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Viceconti, A., Camerone, E. M., Luzzi, D., Pardini, M., Ristori, D., Gallace, A., et al. (2018). Explicit and implicit experience of own's body in painful musculoskeletal disorders and rheumatic diseases: a scoping review protocol of available quantitative and qualitative evidence. SAGE Open Med. 6:2050312118820026. doi: 10.1177/2050312118820026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vos, T., Abajobir, A. A., Abate, K. H., Abbafati, C., Abbas, K. M., Abd-Allah, F., et al. (2017). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. Lancet 390, 1211–1259. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32154-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., Abbaszadeh, S., Smith, A. J., Catley, M. J., and Moseley, G. L. (2013a). Acupuncture applied as a sensory discrimination training tool decreases movement-related pain in patients with chronic low back pain more than acupuncture alone: a randomised cross-over experiment. Br. J. Sports Med. 47, 1085–1089. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., Catley, M. J., Rabey, M. I., O'Sullivan, P. B., O'Connell, N. E., and Smith, A. J. (2016). Disrupted self-perception in people with chronic low back pain. Further evaluation of the fremantle back awareness questionnaire. J. Pain 17, 1001–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., Di Pietro, F., George, P., and O'Connell, N. E. (2010). Tactile thresholds are preserved yet complex sensory function is impaired over the lumbar spine of chronic non-specific low back pain patients: a preliminary investigation. Physiotherapy 96, 317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., Elliott, R. L., Sawyer, A. E., Spence, R., Beales, D. J., O'Sullivan, P. B., et al. (2017). Disrupted body-image and pregnancy-related lumbopelvic pain. A preliminary investigation. Musculoskelet.Sci.Pract. 30, 49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.msksp.2017.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., James, M., Abbaszadeh, S., George, P. J., Formby, P. M., Smith, A. J., et al. (2014). Assessing self-perception in patients with chronic low back pain: development of a back-specific body-perception questionnaire. J. Back Musculoskelet. Rehabil. 27, 463–473. doi: 10.3233/BMR-140467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., Keeves, J., Bourgoin, C., George, P. J., Smith, A. J., O'Connell, N. E., et al. (2013b). Mislocalization of sensory information in people with chronic low back pain: a preliminary investigation. Clin. J. Pain 29, 737–743. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e318274b320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., O'Connell, N. E., Di Pietro, F., and Bulsara, M. (2011). Managing chronic nonspecific low back pain with a sensorimotor retraining approach: exploratory multiple-baseline study of 3 participants. Phys. Ther. 91, 535–546. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20100150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wand, B. M., Tulloch, V. M., George, P. J., Smith, A. J., Goucke, R., O'Connell, N. E., et al. (2012). Seeing it helps: movement-related back pain is reduced by visualization of the back during movement. Clin. J. Pain 28, 602–608. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31823d480c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: musculoskeletal disorders, rheumatic diseases, chronic pain, somatoperception, body perception, body representation, body ownership, scoping review

Citation: Viceconti A, Camerone EM, Luzzi D, Pentassuglia D, Pardini M, Ristori D, Rossettini G, Gallace A, Longo MR and Testa M (2020) Explicit and Implicit Own's Body and Space Perception in Painful Musculoskeletal Disorders and Rheumatic Diseases: A Systematic Scoping Review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:83. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00083

Received: 29 October 2019; Accepted: 24 February 2020;
Published: 09 April 2020.

Edited by:

Jeffrey M. Yau, Baylor College of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Jared Medina, University of Delaware, United States
Daniel Elliott Harper, Emory University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Viceconti, Camerone, Luzzi, Pentassuglia, Pardini, Ristori, Rossettini, Gallace, Longo and Testa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonello Viceconti, antonello.viceconti@gmail.com; Marco Testa, mtesta.unige@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.