Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 25 June 2019
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Sensory Neuroscience Editor's Pick 2021 View all 10 articles

Analysis of Perceptual Expertise in Radiology – Current Knowledge and a New Perspective

Updated
  • 1Department of Radiology, SUNY Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Ophthalmology, SUNY Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Neurology, SUNY Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, United States
  • 4Department of Physiology/Pharmacology, SUNY Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, United States

Radiologists rely principally on visual inspection to detect, describe, and classify findings in medical images. As most interpretive errors in radiology are perceptual in nature, understanding the path to radiologic expertise during image analysis is essential to educate future generations of radiologists. We review the perceptual tasks and challenges in radiologic diagnosis, discuss models of radiologic image perception, consider the application of perceptual learning methods in medical training, and suggest a new approach to understanding perceptional expertise. Specific principled enhancements to educational practices in radiology promise to deepen perceptual expertise among radiologists with the goal of improving training and reducing medical error.

Introduction

Optimizing perceptual expertise in radiology has great practical importance. One of the primary goals of radiology education is to train novices to develop advanced or ‘expert’ search methods to enhance abnormality recognition (Wood, 1999). The principles underlying radiologic expertise are also important beyond the immediate field as courts and policy makers rely on radiologists to provide testimony and educate juries on applicable standards of medical care (Andrew, 2006; Berlin et al., 2006). Despite continual efforts to refine radiology education, however, the error rate in radiological readings has not improved in the last seven decades (Garland, 1949; Berlin, 2007), persisting at a rate of approximately 33% for abnormal studies (Waite et al., 2017b). This problem—compounded by increasing imaging volumes and examination complexity—mandates a deeper understanding of the nature of radiological expertise, to improve both student training as well as the accuracy of practicing clinicians.

In this review we put forward that radiology’s error rate has been recalcitrant to improvement secondary to a lack of knowledge about the mechanisms of expertise. We further propose that no principled theories for improvement will be developed until we understand the precise nature of expertise in radiology. We discuss the definition of perceptual expertise in radiology, search strategies employed by experts, and current training methods. We also examine the limitations of extant eye tracking studies and visual processing theories regarding radiological expertise. Finally, we delineate a new approach to achieve a principled understanding of radiologic expertise, and how it will promote new heuristics (i.e., in designing individualized educational plans for each radiological trainee), with the ultimate goal of reducing radiological error (Gegenfurtner et al., 2017; Sheridan and Reingold, 2017; Waite et al., 2017b).

What Does Radiologic Expertise Mean?

Radiologists are physicians who specialize in diagnosing and treating disease using a variety of medical imaging techniques such as x-rays, computed tomography (CT), magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), Positron emission tomography (PET), and ultrasound. In addition to a continually growing body of ‘fact-based’ knowledge regarding anatomy, radiological pathology, physics, and clinical medicine, expertise in radiology is considered largely perceptual in nature, defined by refined visual search patterns and diagnostic accuracy (Kelly et al., 2016). Thus, expert radiologists not only perceive abnormalities that non-experts do not, but they also better understand what to attend to and what to ignore (Gunderman and Patel, 2019).

Expertise in diagnostic imaging is usually inferred from the physician’s rank within the medical hierarchy—their title, level of training (i.e., intern, resident, attending, specialist), and years of experience—rather than by objective metrics. These indirect measures are presumably relied on because is difficult to accurately measure expertise. A principled understanding of perceptual expertise in specific tasks such as abnormality detection in radiology does not exist. If we could assess expertise with a biomarker instead —irrespective of physician’s rank—it would not only provide the basis to maximize accuracy and optimize heuristics, but it would also help determine the importance of training versus that of natural aptitude.

Analysis of Medical Imaging

At a fundamental level, image analysis involves two basic processes: visual inspection of the image and interpretation (Krupinski, 2010). Broadly, diagnostic radiology entails (1) detection—noting a potentially significant finding is present that merits further analysis; (2) recognition—deciding that the finding is pathologic; (3) discrimination—characterizing the lesion as a specific type; and (4) diagnosis. The first task, detection, has primary importance, because all following steps leading to diagnosis rely on detection efficacy (Gray et al., 1978).

Perception is paramount in radiologic diagnosis because if a radiologist misses an abnormality, no amount of factual knowledge can remedy such a lapse; the diagnostic process may be prematurely terminated, resulting in misdiagnosis and subsequent harm to the patient. At the other end of the spectrum, false positives can also be detrimental to patient health. In both cases, until we better understand the nature of radiologic expertise, we will not be able to dissociate the contributions of perception and cognition to diagnostic accuracy.

Error Rates in Radiology

Garland (1949) found that radiologists incurred an error rate of 33% in the interpretation of positive films (films that contain an abnormality), measured against the consensus of a group of experts. In a typical clinical practice (comprised of both normal and abnormal studies), the diagnostic error rate is approximately 4% (Siegle et al., 1998), a rate that translates into approximately 40 million interpretive errors per year worldwide (Bruno et al., 2015). Since Garland’s pioneering study, significant error rates have been noted in varied plain film modalities including mammography, chest X-rays (CXR), and bone X-rays, involving radiologists not only in private practice (Siegle et al., 1998), but also in academic settings, where interpretive error rates range from 13 to 90% depending on experimental conditions and the functional definition of error (Garland, 1949; Lehr et al., 1976; Forrest and Friedman, 1981; Muhm et al., 1983; Berlin, 2007; Brady, 2017). Recent studies of new technologies in radiology have determined that high error rates also exist in CT, MRI, and Ultrasound interpretation (Berlin, 2014; Herzog et al., 2017; Banaste et al., 2018).

Because of the subjective nature of radiologic interpretation, the definition of what is erroneous is established by expert opinion (Waite et al., 2017b). Thus, in a conclusive ‘error’ (as opposed to acceptable variation across observers), there is a substantial discrepancy with respect to peer consensus (Waite et al., 2017b). Although radiologic error can be classified in a number of ways (Kim and Mansfield, 2014), two broad categories of interpretive error are usually identified: perceptual errors and cognitive errors (Bruno et al., 2015). Cognitive errors are considered to occur when a correct positive finding is followed by misclassification due to faulty reasoning or lack of knowledge (Renfrew et al., 1992). Communication errors are an additional important cause of error, outside of interpretive or perceptual categorization (Waite et al., 2017c, 2018).

Omission or false negative errors occur when a radiologist fails to notice a perceptible lesion (as opposed to a fundamentally ambiguous lesion). A major source of diagnostic error and litigation, omission errors have been divided into three categories based on fixation times on missed lesions: search, recognition, and decision-making. Search errors are scanning errors in which the observer never fixates the lesion. Recognition errors are omission errors where the radiologist fixates the lesion for a duration shorter than the threshold dwell time (from 500 to 1000 ms depending on modality) considered necessary to recognize lesion features, and therefore fails to identify it. Decision-making errors are omission errors where the radiologist fixates the lesion for long enough to extract relevant lesion features, but dismisses the lesion as inconsequential (Kundel et al., 1978; Krupinski, 2010). Although only search and recognition errors are technically perceptual in etiology (Krupinski, 2010; Waite et al., 2017b), given the lack of eye tracking metrics during routine clinical imaging and most research studies, in practice (and in this review) all omission errors are usually termed ‘perceptual’ (Bruno et al., 2015).

Renfrew et al. (1992) classified 182 cases presented at a problem case conference and found that 43% were secondary to false negative or false positive readings. Kim and Mansfield analyzed 656 radiologic examinations with delayed diagnosis secondary to radiologic error and found that 42% were secondary to missed diagnosis (‘under-reading’) without an identifiable cause. An additional 42% were also secondary to missed diagnosis, but were felt to be attributable to a variety of causes, including satisfaction of search—where lesions remain undetected after the discovery of an initial lesion, alliterative error—where an error is made secondary to overreliance on a prior report, poor/misleading history, location of a finding outside the field of interest or at the corner of a film, and failure to consult prior radiologic studies/reports (Kim and Mansfield, 2014). In an analysis of 496 suits leading to malpractice claims, failure to diagnose was overwhelmingly the most common reason for initiating a malpractice suit against radiologists, comprising 78% of the cases (Baker et al., 2013). Funaki et al. (1997) and Rosenkrantz and Bansal (2016) found that missed findings accounted for 60–80% of interpretive error. Thus, faulty detection—failure to identify salient findings—is considered the most important source of interpretive error in radiology (Degnan et al., 2018).

Less discussed in the literature, false positive errors are also important to recognize. A major problem in screening examinations, false positive errors cause patient anxiety and often engender further unnecessary studies and procedures (Castells et al., 2016).

Given their ubiquity, interpretive errors are unlikely to be entirely due to bad radiologists (Brady, 2017). Indeed, given the high incidence of interpretive errors in essentially all radiologic scenarios—across multiple imaging modalities, and in both private practice and academic settings—a more probable explanation is that the methods to select potential radiology trainees, and resident education, are not better nowadays than 70 years ago.

Attention and Perception

Voluntary attention—the selective processing of information at a given location—is deployed with specific targets in mind and guided to target locations by their prominent visual features (Wolfe et al., 1989; Wolfe, 1994; Alexander and Zelinsky, 2009, 2011; Carrasco, 2011, 2014; Eckstein, 2011; Anton-Erxleben and Carrasco, 2013; Carrasco and Barbot, 2014; Nobre et al., 2014). Whereas color, brightness, and motion are known to attract attention in a bottom–up manner (Wolfe et al., 2016; Wolfe and Horowitz, 2017), the observer’s intended target representation also directs attention in a top–down manner. Learning, memory, and expectations shape this process (Carrasco et al., 1998; Chen and Zelinsky, 2006; Wolfe et al., 2016). Thus, radiological search likely reflects a combination of bottom–up and top–down attention (Jampani et al., 2012; Wen et al., 2017).

By developing better target representations—for example, with increasing expertise in nodule detection—radiologists learn to remove many irrelevant areas from consideration, based on their understanding of the structure and content of radiological images (Wolfe et al., 2016). Thus, expert radiologists do not scrutinize all regions of the image equally but direct their attention and eye movements more precisely to relevant areas. This heightens efficiency but can also mask unexpected findings.

Eye Movements and Search Patterns in Radiology

Eye Movements and Expertise

The study of eye movements can reveal not only the cognitive processes behind expertise, but also the mechanisms involved in acquiring these skills. In addition, studies of expert gaze patterns can identify common perceptual errors leading to strategies to mitigate them– improving training and reducing error (Fox and Faulkner-Jones, 2017). As such, eye tracking technology has been increasingly used to understand the nature and acquisition of radiological expertise.

During interpretation, expert radiologists generally fixate on abnormalities faster than novices, and their total image search time decreases with increasing levels of expertise (van der Gijp et al., 2017a). Experts also have fewer total fixations than novices. These differences may be due to novices spending more time looking at irrelevant but salient structures [such as the heart on a chest x-ray (CXR), when the lungs are more important to analyze in a nodule detection task] with experts demonstrating more effective search secondary to refined scene guidance (Wolfe et al., 2016; van der Gijp et al., 2017a). Other gaze metrics, such as saccade length and image coverage, demonstrate less consistent experience-based differences across studies (van der Gijp et al., 2017a) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Reprinted from Goodman and Felson (2007) with permission. (A,B) Proposed lung search pattern. (A) This commonly taught search pattern for examination of the lungs during chest radiograph (CXR) interpretation involves starting at the right base () (the costophrenic angle) and examining the right and then left lungs. (B) A second look is then performed in order to compare the right and left lungs as bilateral symmetry is assumed to be useful in recognizing abnormalities (Carmody et al., 1984). Reprinted from Waite et al. (2017a) with permission. Typical scanpaths of a novice (C) and an expert (D) radiologist, both searching a CXR which has a nodule at the left base (arrow in D). This free search pattern (D) is typically employed by experts and differs from the formal radiologic training given in residency. Instead, it indicates the flexible use of search strategies as a function of immediate visual information. The expert radiologist (D) has more efficient scanpaths (red lines) than the novice (C), with fewer fixations (circles), less coverage of the image, fewer saccades, and faster arrival at the abnormality.

Although most studies have examined the effects of expertise on plain film interpretation, expert radiologists are also more accurate and faster than novices during interpretation of volumetric imaging (Nakashima et al., 2016). In a study evaluating accuracy and interpretation in stroke CT and MRI cases, Cooper et al. (2009) found that attending radiologists were more accurate and had decreased time to fixation on lesions compared with novices. In addition, novices spent more time than experts examining normal anatomy, such as the ventricles (Cooper et al., 2009). Mallett et al. (2014) found that experts viewing a CT colonography video had shorter time to first pursuit on polyps (defined as the time from the beginning of video to fixation on the polyp for longer than 100 ms) compared with novices. A study of abdominal CT similarly found that attendings had higher accuracy than residents (Bertram et al., 2016). Another study found that experts had increased sensitivity toward medically important findings (such as nodules) compared with unimportant findings (such as bullae), suggesting specialization of their perceptual skills to detection of relevant findings (Nakashima et al., 2015).

Bertram et al. (2016) found that, whereas attendings and advanced residents performed better than less experienced residents when detecting low-contrast lesions on CT, detection of high-contrast lesions was comparable across groups. This finding suggests that expertise results in an increased ability to detect less salient abnormalities (Bertram et al., 2016).

Relationship Between Fatigue, Expert Performance, and Eye Movements

Expertise may limit the effects of fatigue-related decreases in performance and changes in eye movements. Krupinski et al. (2012) found that residents had reduced detection accuracy in a CT nodule detection task after a day of reading, but the accuracy of attending radiologists was unaffected (Waite et al., 2017a). Similarly, Bertram et al. (2016) found that increased time at work resulted in decreased performance with CT scans for junior residents (with less than 2 years’ experience), suggesting that enhanced mental effort is more robust to fatigue in expert readers.

Hanna et al. (2018) found that after an overnight shift fatigued radiologists demonstrated worse diagnostic performance (with increased false negatives and positives) and increased time to fixate on fractures when examining bone radiographs. Although total viewing time per case was longer for all radiologists when fatigued, the effect was significantly more pronounced with residents compared to faculty members. In effect, in their fatigued states, faculty members had eye-tracking parameters more characteristic of non-fatigued residents (Hanna et al., 2018).

Do High-Performing Radiologists Have Greater Preexisting Visuospatial Skills?

Given the possibility that ingrained aptitude could play a role in radiological success, measuring the perceptual abilities of radiology applicants and residents could be of great practical importance. The excellence of a group of professionals may be optimized through selection of individuals with the best requisite skill set. Radiology residency applicants are usually selected on the basis of academic records, letters of recommendation, and a short interview (Smith and Berbaum, 1991), none of which directly pertain to perceptual abilities. As Birchall notes, the existing model of training assumes that almost all trainees will eventually reach an acceptable standard with practice and semantic knowledge. Yet, it is possible that trainees with higher preexisting skills (i.e., in visual-spatial processing) may reach a higher level of expertise—or may achieve the highest level more quickly—than trainees with lower preexisting skills (Birchall, 2015). The identification of a relevant perceptual test might therefore help determine how much individual residents may benefit from training, and, ultimately, how they will perform as radiologists (Sunday et al., 2017).

The fact that radiology’s high rate of error has remained unchanged for over half a century may be partly explained by selecting applicants without regards to their perceptual (or ‘potential’ perceptual) abilities. It is unknown whether high-performing radiologists had greater baseline visuospatial skills at the start of their training or whether their ability was secondary to learned expertise (Corry, 2011). The persistence of high interpretive error rates and the occasional ‘spectacular failure’ suggest the necessity of reevaluating selection methods (Smith and Berbaum, 1991).

To this end, investigators have attempted to identify perceptual requirements for both learning and practicing radiology, as well as whether practicing radiologists have superior perceptual skills outside of imaging. Nodine and colleagues conducted a series of experiments comparing the performance of radiologists and laypeople in searching Where’s Waldo images from the popular children’s books (in which the challenge for readers is to find the character Waldo amongst a crowd of people). Radiologists and laypeople performed similarly, indicating that radiological expertise in visual search and/or perceptual discrimination did not carry over to non-radiological tasks (Nodine and Krupinski, 1998).

Bass and Chiles performed a series of visual and perceptual tests (including tests of visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, visual memory, visual completion, gestalt closure, identification of hidden figures in a picture, and three-dimensional construction ability) in medical students, residents, and faculty (Bass and Chiles, 1990). They found a correlation between performance on the hidden figures test and the ability to identify pulmonary nodules on a CXR for medical students, but not for residents or board-certified radiologists, suggesting that any innate abilities are quickly superseded by training (Bass and Chiles, 1990).

Similarly, Kelly and colleagues found no difference in performance or gaze dynamics across radiologists of varying levels of experience (ranging from interns to attendings) when asked to identify hospitals on a series of maps, or to find an anomalous shape within a group of similar shapes. These results reinforce the premise that radiologic expertise is a learned task-specific skill (Kelly et al., 2018).

Smoker et al. (1984) found a correlation between radiology residents’ ability to assemble Lego blocks by replicating a diagram and semiannual faculty ratings of their film reading performance. Because the results of the construction tests did not differ between residents and faculty with varied experience, the researchers concluded that the tests measured inherent aptitude rather than expertise (Smoker et al., 1984). Follow up studies confirming these preliminary results have not been performed, however, and no visuospatial ability test currently exists to determine whether someone is likely to become an ‘expert’ radiologist (Smith and Berbaum, 1991; Krupinski, 2011). Indeed, residency training programs do not objectively measure perception either before or during residency (Brazeau-Lamontagne et al., 2004). Unfortunately, taught declarative knowledge about what to look for does not ensure either expert perceptual abilities or act as a safeguard against ‘creative reading’ such as interpreting composite shadows as real nodules (Brazeau-Lamontagne et al., 2004).

A recent study by Sunday et al. (2017) found a modest correlation in naïve subjects between performance on the Vanderbilt Chest Radiograph Test (VCRT, where observers mark in which lung a nodule is when looking at two CXR’s) and on the Novel Object Memory Test (a domain-general test of novel object recognition). Further research may determine whether some individuals learn to recognize radiologic abnormalities faster than others, and whether preexisting abilities, such as measured by the VCRT, place a limit on one’s ultimate level of performance (Sunday et al., 2017).

Search Pattern Instruction for Plain Film Imaging in Radiology

Research findings concerning human perception of medical images have not been widely translated into practical heuristics that improve training (Auffermann et al., 2016). Although there are published guidelines on how to interpret various radiologic examinations (Puddy and Hill, 2007; James and Kelly, 2013), they tend to be unprincipled and subjective, with few studies demonstrating their efficacy. When systematically analyzed, most of these educational tools have had mixed results.

Because novices lack the ability to generate a rapid and accurate global impression of an image, they may benefit from an orderly and comprehensive search pattern/order (Goodman and Felson, 2007; Auffermann et al., 2016) (Figure 1). If readers adhere to a specific order or search pattern in the inspection of anatomical structures, they may achieve more complete coverage of the image, reducing the number of overlooked abnormalities. Although full coverage could also be achieved by inspecting anatomical structures in a random order, keeping a specific order of inspection provides readers with a mental checklist (Kok et al., 2015). Thus, one commonly taught technique, “systematic viewing,” is to inspect anatomical areas in a fixed order.

Evidence to support the value of “systematic viewing” is wanting, however. Whereas, van Geel et al. (2017) found that students trained in systematic viewing methods inspected a larger portion of images than untrained students, they found no difference in their performance in chest radiographic interpretation. Kok et al. (2015) demonstrated similar findings. The combined data indicate that an emphasis on systematic viewing may not be justified.

Auffermann et al. (2016) found that physician assistants that were taught specific eye movements for analyzing CXR’s improved their ability to identify nodules and made less identification errors than those without such training. However, this study did not track the eye movements of participants. Thus, the effects of eye-movement training on search patterns or image coverage remain unknown (van Geel et al., 2017).

Expert Search Patterns

One potential reason that pre-defined search patterns fail to consistently improve resident accuracy is that experts themselves do not read plain films in a consistent, standardized manner. Therefore, the various advocated search methods are not consistently used in practice (Kundel and La Follette, 1972) (Figure 1). Instead, experts use a variety of non-systematic search patterns, so-called ‘free search,’ when looking at images (Auffermann et al., 2016). An evolution of search patterns from medical students to attending radiologists was noted in one study but attending search patterns were not systematic; eye movements were more affected by the findings on the radiograph than any preplanned search pattern (Kundel and La Follette, 1972). A pioneering study by Tuddenham and Calvert (1961) found a wide variation in search patterns among readers. Ironically, the only observer with a reproducible search pattern failed to report findings noted by observers with inconsistent patterns (Tuddenham and Calvert, 1961). These findings are remarkably consistent with those from studies conducted over 50 years later, suggesting that consistent search patterns do not help, and indeed might be detrimental, to accurate diagnosis (Kok et al., 2015). Carmody et al. (1984) also found that, although radiologists are taught to compare both lungs (to look for asymmetric findings), less than 4% of their eye movements indicated such comparison scans, further illustrating the gap between radiologic instruction and practice.

Search Patterns in Volumetric (3D) Imaging

Modern medical imaging increasingly includes not only static image viewing (e.g., mammography and plain film radiography), but also dynamic imaging, such as with sequential viewing of multiple slices of CT and MRI, often in different orientations. When reading a CT or MRI, contemporary radiologists must scroll through a stack of images-thin slices of the 3-D volume of an organ–a process known as “stack viewing” (Nakashima et al., 2016) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Conventional radiography (2D medical imaging) such as CXR, mammography, and plain film bone X-rays, is based on the fact that tissue will absorb photons from an X-ray beam in relation to the electron density of the tissue. The number of photons passing through the region of interest will be detected by image detectors that convert the body’s direct attenuation of the photons into digital images. The resulting images are a two-dimensional projection of a three-dimensional structure. (B) In volumetric imaging such as CT and MRI, cross-sectional images of the body are obtained to represent a “slice” of the person being imaged, such as the slices in a loaf of bread. Once a number of successive slices are generated, they can be digitally “stacked” to form a three-dimensional image of the patient. On the CT scan above there is a nodule in the right upper lobe (yellow circle). As a radiologist scrolls through the stack, the nodule will present as a suddenly appearing (and then disappearing) lesion, simulating motion.

Although numerous studies have examined search strategies utilized in viewing single 2-D medical images, relatively little is known about those employed during interpretation of 3-D volumetric medical images (Nakashima et al., 2016).

In many ways, stack viewing is a different perceptual task than reading static images, and it may be regarded as a type of visual search conducted in a dynamic display. During stack viewing, radiologists search for the onset signal of a suddenly appearing lesion that stands out among blood vessels and organs that appear to move (simulated motion) (Nakashima et al., 2016). Thus, the fundamental characteristics found in the search of static images do not necessarily apply to the search of dynamic displays.

Drew and colleagues identified two different global strategies adopted by radiologists when searching through volumetric images—specifically during a nodule detection task in chest CT. “Scanners” searched each slice widely, before moving on to the next depth. “Drillers” held their eyes relatively still in the x and y plane, limiting their search to a single lung quadrant while quickly scrolling—drilling—through slices in depth. The data revealed a higher true positive rate for drillers than for scanners: drillers identified 60%, and scanners identified 48%, of all available nodules (Drew et al., 2013b).

Whereas drillers had more experience than scanners in the above dataset (Drew et al., 2013b), a follow-up study showed that instruction in drilling techniques led to improved performance in residents, compared with instruction in scanning techniques (van der Gijp et al., 2017b). These combined results support the superiority of drilling vs. scanning strategies. It is also worth noting that, in contrast with of Drew et al. (2013b) findings, none of the participants in the van der Gijp et al. (2017b) study reported (or had eye movements suggestive of) ‘scanning’ when reading images under normal work conditions. This is consistent with one of the authors’(SW) experience.

More recently, Kelahan et al. (2018) found evidence that the ‘drillers’ vs. ‘scanners’ model is likely imperfect when applied to CT scans of the abdomen and pelvis, given there are more organs in the abdomen and pelvis than in the thorax, and radiologists looked for multiple possible imaging findings (vs. only nodules in the Drew et al., 2013b experiments). It is likely that different search techniques are used depending on imaging modality and the part of the body imaged (e.g., thoracic vs. abdominal or neurologic imaging).

In order to get a more granular understanding of scroll behavior beyond the more general concepts of drilling and scanning (Drew et al., 2013b), Venjakob and Mello-Thoms (2016) and den Boer et al. (2018) defined different types of scroll behavior during CT interpretation. Quantification of the number of slice transitions performed before radiologists change their scrolling direction can provide insight into the desired image content (Venjakob and Mello-Thoms, 2016). den Boer et al. (2018) temporally related scroll movements to cognitive data via a think-aloud strategy, whereupon radiology residents verbalized their thoughts while reading CT scans. Half runs and oscillations (‘local’ movements covering less than 50% of the stack slices) were often associated with analysis [defined as cognitive activities including characterization of findings (van der Gijp et al., 2014)]. Runs (movements forward or backward covering more than 50% of slices) (Venjakob and Mello-Thoms, 2016) and image manipulation (where readers changed contrast levels or stack orientation) were more frequently associated with perception—search strategies and the global search for abnormalities (den Boer et al., 2018). Interruptions (where scrolling was paused) mainly coincided with synthesis related to the integration of information (e.g., generating a differential diagnosis) (den Boer et al., 2018). It is unclear whether these findings generalize to expert search or whether the relationship between scroll behavior and cognition changes with experience, however, studies of this kind can engender further insights into how radiologists interpret volumetric examinations.

Because of the large data inherent to volumetric imaging (often 1000s of images per study) (Andriole et al., 2011), radiologists do not exhaustively foveate all regions of interest, but rely on detection of signals with their peripheral vision (Eckstein et al., 2017). Drew et al. (2013b) found that radiologists covered, on average, 69% of the lung tissue on a CT nodule detection task. In a similar nodule detection task, Rubin et al. (2015) found that observers covered, on average, only 26.7% of the lung tissue. The difference between the results from these two studies could reflect their respective operational definitions of central field size (Kundel, 2015). Yet, despite their discrepancies, the main common finding in both studies is that a large amount of the lung tissue is never examined with foveal vision. Interestingly, Rubin et al. (2015) found that although observers foveated less than 33% of the lung volume, their search volumes encompassed an average of 75% of all nodules, showcasing the efficiency of peripheral detection (Kundel, 2015).

As peripheral vision cannot provide the kind of fine spatial discriminations that characterizes foveal vision, detectability of certain lesions can differ in 3D vs. 2D image searches (Eckstein et al., 2017). For instance, Eckstein et al. (2017) found higher detectability for calcifications in 2D single slice images—a Gaussian noise field tuned to be similar to the noise present in mammograms, and relatively improved detection of masses in 3D volumetric imaging– a stack of 2D images where the user could scroll up and down in the stack of images in a similar manner to the way a clinician explores a digital breast tomosynthesis case (Eckstein et al., 2017).

Models of saliency further illustrate differences between 2D and 3D search, predicting that different radiologic examinations are approached in distinct ways to optimize performance. Wen et al.,(2016) made a ‘dynamic saliency map’ where higher saliency is ascribed to motion flows that deviate from normal dominant flows, expected to reflect the observation that nodules pop out from anatomical backgrounds during volumetric interpretation. Alternatively, the Graph-Based Visual Saliency (GBVS) model predicts saliency due to the comparison of static image features within a given slice. Wen and colleagues demonstrated that driller fixations were aligned better with the dynamic saliency map and scanners with GBVS. This suggests that topographic maps representing conspicuity of objects and locations differ between radiological viewing methods, and more specifically, that scanners tend to use primarily 2-D information in their search, whereas drillers (the great majority of radiologists) use more dynamic information when interpreting cross-sectional imaging (Wen et al., 2016).

Both Wen’s and Eckstein’s studies lend support to the notion that 2D search and volumetric search are different perceptual tasks, and thus observer performance in one may not generalize to the other (Wen et al., 2016; Eckstein et al., 2017).

Search Patterns and Spatial Frequency Analysis

The ability to detect a lesion is not only dependent on lesion characteristics, but also on the relation between the lesion and the background. As described in the holistic model of medical image perception, a target is compared to its background (which may camouflage the target, causing recognition error) to determine whether it is noteworthy (Nodine and Kundel, 1987). The relation between the lesion and the background therefore determines whether any given lesion will be above or below the detection threshold for a given decision criterion (Mello-Thoms et al., 2003). Mello-Thoms (2003, 2006), Mello-Thoms et al. (2003), and Mello-Thoms and Chapman (2004) performed a number of studies using image processing in order to understand the interplay between lesions and the parenchyma during mammographic interpretation. Using wavelet packet decomposition, they measured the log of the energy (energy being the integral of the signal strength) of different spatial frequency bands across a range of orientations and made a profile of measurements for each fixated region (Mello-Thoms, 2003). Because radiologists make comparative judgments between the background and any potential lesions, Mello-Thoms et al. (2003) considered not just the profile in each region (termed the local profile) but also a combined profile (the global profile) across all fixated regions in an image. The local profile is related to the conspicuity of local features and the global profile is a measure, specific to each observer, of the searched background to which local features are compared (Taylor, 2007).

Both residents and mammographers were visually attracted to similar areas of mammograms, despite mammographers having significantly superior accuracy (Mello-Thoms et al., 2003). The data showed that the computed profiles did not discriminate between mammographers’ true- and false-positive decisions, suggesting that they possess mental schema (well-modeled by the spatial frequency representation) about how malignant lesions should look, and they do not deviate from that schema even when they make a mistake (a false positive). The profiles did, however, discriminate between mammographer’s true-positive and false-negative decision outcomes. For residents, however, there were statistical differences between false and true positives, and the global profile seemed to play a much smaller role in their decision-making, implying that conspicuity of local elements is strongly related to their decisions. Residents were less able to contrast local findings with global features, which misguided their judgments (Mello-Thoms, 2003; Mello-Thoms et al., 2003). Further understanding of what imaging features attract visual attention in conjunction with decision making can afford more specific training- for example by concentrating on residents’ improving their comparison skills during training (Mello-Thoms, 2003).

Challenges to Eye Tracking Studies

Simple eye movement measurements may only provide meaningful data on radiologic expertise in certain imaging scenarios (Jarodzka and Boshuizen, 2017). For example, time to first fixation and the proportion of time spent on relevant findings, is only informative in the case of localized diseases, but not for diseases that are diffuse in nature (and may thus require more complex metrics) (Kok, 2012).

Kok (2012) found that radiologists examine diffuse and focal diseases on chest X-rays differently. In addition to ‘standard’ eye tracking measures they examined the ‘global/local ratio’ of saccades—computed by dividing the number of ‘long’ by ‘short’ saccades (respectively defined as greater and less than 1.6 degrees of visual angle). A higher ratio indicated a global, dispersed viewing pattern, and a lower ratio indicated local clusters of fixations in specific regions (Kok, 2012). Although there were significant differences in interpretation accuracy (determining the likely diagnosis for the examination) between attending radiologists and medical students (no difference was found in accuracy between residents and attendings), the global/local ratio between groups was similar in diseased images. The fact that all groups changed their viewing pattern according to the type of disease, but that medical students had significantly worse interpretation accuracy, is evidence that perceptual aspects of image interpretation- detecting abnormalities- develop before the ability to correctly interpret abnormalities or integrate them into a correct diagnosis (Kok, 2012). Further studies with more complex oculomotor metrics will be needed to unravel the relationship between eye movements and diagnostic expertise.

A recent study by Kasprowski et al. (2018) challenges the presumed relationship between visual search efficiency and diagnostic accuracy. They analyzed various eye movement metrics while four participant groups viewed CXRs preceded by proposed answers: observers with no medical experience, radiology technicians (‘radiographers’- skilled in performing X-ray examinations but not involved in diagnosis), radiology residents, and radiology attendings. As expected, attendings had the highest average diagnostic accuracy, followed by residents, radiographers, and laypeople. Interestingly, although there were significant differences in the eye movement metrics of residents and attendings, there were no significant differences between the visual patterns of radiology technicians and attendings, despite residents being significantly more accurate. The authors surmise that this finding may reflect general experience analyzing X-rays. Their participant radiographers had over 10 years’ experience checking the technical quality of imaging, compared to participant residents, who had less than 6 years’ experience, albeit for a different purpose—diagnosis. Thus, the ability to reproduce characteristics of experts eye movements does not guarantee improved diagnostic performance (Kasprowski et al., 2018). Expertise in radiology requires not only efficient, task-oriented, eye movements but accurate recognition of abnormalities.

An additional confound is that eye tracking metrics are not only dependent on imaging findings and reader expertise, but are also influenced by clinical history (reviewed in Waite et al., 2017b) and reader expectations of abnormal findings. In an analysis of 16 studies comparing the accuracy of tests with and without clinical information, Loy and Irwig found that clinical information improves interpretive accuracy through improved sensitivity without a loss of specificity, consistent with readers being alerted to additional imaging features, rather than by merely altering their level of suspicion (Loy and Irwig, 2004). Although many of these studies were not performed with associated eye tracking metrics, search patterns likely also vary in conjunction with changes in accuracy. Moreover prevalence expectation changes search patterns. Reed and colleagues studied performance during a CXR-based nodule detection task under conditions where readers were told that the images contained a specific number of abnormal findings. Higher prevalence expectations were linked to increased reader fixations and total analysis time (Reed et al., 2011). A follow-up study demonstrated that this ‘prevalence effect’ was particularly evident in normal examinations, with readers exhibiting decreased confidence that normal images were in fact normal (Reed et al., 2014).

A more fundamental challenge to assessing the findings of many eye tracking studies’ relates to their experimental design. Gur et al. (2008) and Gur (2009) found that radiologic performance during the interpretation of mammograms was different in real-life vs. laboratory settings.

Lastly, Van der Gijp and colleagues noted that studies conducted over the last two decades have largely focused on the differences between experts and novices, while theory-driven research on how to improve detection has been relatively neglected. Thus, there is a need for the field to move beyond the description of differences between experts and novices, to the development of more efficient strategies and methods to accelerate and improve training (van der Gijp et al., 2017a).

Holistic ‘Gist’ Processing Theory

Prevailing models of medical image perception rest on the premise that expert observers process a medical image holistically at the first glimpse. These holistic processing accounts are encompassed by a couple of different theoretical frameworks, the oldest described being the global-focal search or ‘holistic’ model (Nodine and Kundel, 1987; Litchfield and Donovan, 2016; Sheridan and Reingold, 2017).

According to the global-focal search model, medical experts rapidly extract a global impression of an image. This impression consists of a comparison between the contents of the image and the expert’s prior knowledge about the appearance of normal and abnormal medical images (i.e., the expert’s schemata). This enables experts to identify perturbations (deviations from their schemata that indicate possible abnormalities) and direct their eyes toward their corresponding locations for further (i.e., foveal) examination (Nodine and Kundel, 1987; Sheridan and Reingold, 2017). Features are subsequently scrutinized and tested against schemata to determine whether a finding is suspicious, in which case diagnostic decisions are made (Waite et al., 2017b) (for a more complete review see, Kundel et al., 2008). This process, lasting seconds to minutes, is capacity-limited by the bottleneck of attention (Waite et al., 2017b). Similar time constraints apply to all models that rely on global processing as a core component of expertise in medical image perception (Sheridan and Reingold, 2017). Another popular model posits that initial global processing (consisting of bottom–up “global image statistics” like average orientation and average size of objects) signals if there is an abnormality (establishing its likelihood) without providing location information or constraining the subsequent serial search. The searcher can then change their strategy to a slower, more complete search for the abnormality (Evans et al., 2010, 2016; Drew et al., 2013a; Chin et al., 2018) (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Figure modified from Wolfe et al. (2011) with permission. Two-pathway architecture for visual processing. The selective pathway can bind features and recognize objects but is capacity-limited. At its bottleneck, preference for further processing is given to items with certain basic attributes (such as color, orientation, and size) when those attributes match the appearance of a target object. However, these attributes do not fully explain the efficiency of search in the real world, where elements are arranged in a rule-governed manner—for example, people generally appear on horizontal surfaces. The regularity of scenes provides two kinds of scene-based guidance—semantic guidance, referring to the knowledge of the probability of the presence of an object in a scene and its probable location, and episodic guidance, referring to the memory of a specific previously encountered scene. In conjunction with the selective pathway, the non-selective pathway extracts statistics such as velocity, direction of motion, and size, rapidly from the entire image. Although the non-selective pathway does not support precise object recognition, it provides information used in scene-based guidance to direct attention to important locations (such as the probable locations of nodules on CXR’s). Conscious experience of the visual world is comprised of the products of both pathways (Wolfe et al., 2011).

One of the global-focal model’s principal predictions is that rapid initial global processing constrains search to suspicious areas in the image (Carrigan et al., 2018). This strategy may be available to experts but not novices, explaining why expert observers search medical images with higher efficiency—finding more abnormalities in a shorter timeframe, and with fewer eye movements—than novices do. Support for this hypothesis has been provided by studies showing that expert radiologists can identify subtle abnormalities on mammography and chest radiography displayed for only 250 ms (Kundel and Nodine, 1975; Oestmann et al., 1988; Krupinski, 2011; Evans et al., 2013; Sheridan and Reingold, 2017; Carrigan et al., 2018).

Using time to first fixation on lesion data during mammographic interpretation, Kundel and colleagues found that 67% of cancers were fixated on within the 1st second of viewing. The remainder of the cancer locations were fixated on later in search. This has been interpreted as further supporting a two-component system- rapid initial holistic processing guides initial search with subsequent slower processing representing discovery of cancers from search and discovery (Kundel et al., 2008).

The idea that holistic processing is integral to expert performance is also supported by experiments designed to disrupt it. Oestmann et al. (1993) found decreased detection performance for subtle lung cancers in upside-down images, even with unlimited viewing times. In a related study, Carmody et al. (1980) compared nodule detection performance in two viewing conditions: segmented search—in which the CXR was presented in six sections and viewed piecemeal—versus global search—in which the entire film was presented—and found an increased false positive rate in the segmented search scenario, which they attributed to an impaired gestalt.

Face perception is considered a prime example of holistic processing, where recognition is based on the synthesis of facial features that yields a unique face more than the summed recognition of each individual facial feature. The ‘gold standard’ for testing holistic face processes is the face inversion task, whereupon inversion disproportionately impairs the recognition of faces relative to other object classes. Turning a face upside down is thought to disrupt normal holistic face processing, forcing participants to use a less optimal strategy based on analysis of specific features (e.g., wide-set eyes, square jaw). Processing in mammographic interpretation appears to share similar characteristics to this holistic perception. In a recent study, Chin et al. (2018) tested the effects of image inversion during interpretation of normal and abnormal mammograms-inverted and normally oriented-by experienced radiologists and radiology residents (Figure 4). Participants were also asked to judge the facial expressions (e.g., neutral/happy) of briefly presented upright and inverted faces (Chin et al., 2018). Both groups demonstrated better expression discrimination of faces in the upright compared to the inverted orientations, as expected. However, detection rates for upright and inverted mammograms depended on expertise level. Whereas radiology residents were unaffected by image orientation, experienced radiologists performed better on upright images than on inverted ones. In addition, although accuracy in the upright position increased with years of experience, the magnitude of the inversion effect also increased, demonstrating that use of holistic strategies increases as a function of domain-specific perceptual experience. In short, expert holistic processing helped in detection of an upright stimulus but provided no advantage when the image was inverted (Chin et al., 2018).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Reprinted from Chin et al. (2018) with permission. Examples of mammogram and face stimuli in upright and inverted orientations. (A) Upright and inverted abnormal mammogram. (B) Upright and inverted smiling face. The inversion effect posits that stimuli are processed as an integrated whole rather than a sum of its parts; therefore, the inverted image is harder to recognize and process (Chin et al., 2018).

A recent study by Brennan et al. (2018) suggests an intriguing relationship between expertise and gist processing. They found that not only could radiologists detect cancer above chance in abnormal mammograms viewed for only ½ second, but they were able to differentiate between normal mammograms in patients that remained cancer free after 2 years from normal mammograms in patients that subsequently developed cancer. These results suggests that mammograms without overt signs of cancer can contain information that may predict future malignancy. The readers that most rapidly differentiated between positive and negative mammograms were also better at differentiating between normal mammograms in patients that remained cancer free and normal mammograms in patients where cancer developed later. Their results suggest that expertise may increase radiologists’ capacity to perceive the ‘gist of the abnormal,’ an ability to detect a globally elevated risk of cancer in studies without overt radiographic signs (Brennan et al., 2018).

In a related experiment, Evans and colleagues found that expert mammographers demonstrated above-chance classification of a study as abnormal when shown 500 ms images from the normal breast in patients with overt signs of cancer in the opposite breast. This suggests that a widely distributed ‘global signal of abnormality’ was present in the normal breast parenchyma (Evans et al., 2016). Evans et al. proposed that such a signal, if present before the appearance of a clinical lesion, could be used as a warning sign suggesting greater vigilance (Evans et al., 2016). One possible way to accomplish this would be to flash an image for a half-second, record the readers’ gist response before usual presentation, and then use this signal to predict future breast cancer (Brennan et al., 2018). In conjunction, given a high gist response correlates with cancer in the current image, it might be cost-effective to send an image to a second reader for double reading when a case is classified as abnormal from the gist response, but reported as normal after usual presentation (Brennan et al., 2018).

Challenges to Holistic Processing Theory

Volumetric Imaging

Holistic processing theory is at best incomplete as a model of perceptual expertise in the era of modern imaging. The studies that buttressed the holistic processing model were conducted with plain 2-D imaging such as CXR (Kundel and Nodine, 1975; Oestmann et al., 1988) and mammography (Evans et al., 2013), but the nature of 3D volumetric imaging (with the necessity to scroll through images in the z-plane to detect abnormalities) is such that no single image can provide meaningful global image statistics or afford knowledge of image perturbations throughout the entire dataset. Therefore, there is no rapid ‘global signal’ that can be extracted from any single image to either organize subsequent fixations or contribute to the reader’s conviction that a subsequent search will uncover an abnormality.

Flash Preview Moving Window (FPMW) Experiments

One of the problems with assessing the validity of the holistic model in radiology is that the corresponding studies were conducted under free-viewing conditions. Because observers had constant access to the whole scene via peripheral vision, it is difficult to isolate the specific contribution of the initial scene preview on subsequent eye movement behavior (Litchfield and Donovan, 2016).

The flash preview moving window (FPMW) protocol draws from both ‘flash’ methodology and a ‘moving window’ paradigm. A brief preview of a scene is shown to observers, who must then search for a target within the scene while their peripheral vision is restricted (Litchfield and Donovan, 2016). To control how much of the scene is accessible for visual processing, a mask of variable size is tied to the observer’s central fixation, occluding the rest of the scene outside the fixation window. In this way, observers remain free to make eye movements, while researchers systematically control how much foveal, parafoveal, and peripheral information is available for visual processing. As a result, observers may examine a scene solely with high-resolution foveal vision, isolated from parafoveal or peripheral contributions (Litchfield and Donovan, 2017) (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Reprinted from Litchfield and Donovan (2016) with permission. Two different experience groups—expert radiologists and psychology students—searched for lung nodules from CXR images using the flash-preview moving window (FPMW) paradigm. Participants looked at the target word for 15 s (“lung nodule”). They then saw a fixation cross for 200 ms, then either a mask preview (random array of colored pixels) or a CXR (a ‘scene’ preview) for 250 ms. Next, participants saw a mask for 50 ms and then a second fixation cross for 400 ms. Following a second presentation of the fixation cross, they conducted a windowed search, with a 2.5-degree radius window restricting the field of view (Litchfield and Donovan, 2016).

Using this protocol, Litchfield and colleagues found that, whereas experts (consultant radiologists) identified more nodules on CXR than novices (psychology undergraduates), denying experts an initial glimpse of the image did not impact their performance. In contrast, novices performed better in the mask preview condition than with the scene preview. Further, the first eye movements following the scene preview and the mask preview had comparable speeds and amplitudes (Litchfield and Donovan, 2016). Thus, contrary to predictions from the holistic processing theory, the provision of an initial glimpse of the scene did not contribute to expert performance—and indeed reduced novice performance.

A follow-up experiment compared the detection performance of novices and experienced radiographers in three different imaging modalities—CT scan of the head, skeletal x-rays, and CXRs—for specific target abnormalities. As expected, the detection skills of experienced radiographers surpassed those of novices. However, access to a scene preview not only did not benefit, but actually impaired the performance of observers in both groups (Litchfield and Donovan, 2016). These combined findings argue against the hypothesis that processing the initial glimpse of a scene is beneficial to performance (Litchfield and Donovan, 2017) (but see Sheridan and Reingold, 2017 for a counterpoint).

Scene Processing Without Attention?

We also note that flash preview experiments (Kundel and Nodine, 1975; Krupinski, 2011; Evans et al., 2013) are thought to support the holistic processing hypothesis based on the assumption that searchers only use information available while images are displayed (Sheridan and Reingold, 2017). Yet, there is reason to believe that image processing can continue after images are no longer displayed, even in the presence of a mask (Carrasco and McElree, 2001; Carrasco et al., 2003).

More generally, gist processing is thought of as a type of scene processing that occurs in the absence or near absence of attention (Li et al., 2002; Rousselet et al., 2002; Fei-Fei et al., 2005; Otsuka and Kawaguchi, 2007), in parallel across the visual field (Rousselet et al., 2002). But even if the initial glimpse of a scene is demonstrably helpful, it does not necessarily follow that searchers process information without attention. Instead, it may be that searchers rapidly extract visual information during a single brief fixation that provides high-resolution information at the fovea, and lower-resolution information in the visual periphery (Livingstone and Hubel, 1988; Strasburger et al., 2011), facilitated by attention. Attentional allocation to the periphery might involve one or more serial shifts of selective processing, either covertly during the first fixation or while continuing to process the image in memory, before the observer is required to provide a response. Carrasco et al. (2003, 2004, 2006) showed that peripheral stimuli continues to be processed after being removed from view, with the temporal dynamics depending on where stimuli are in the visual field, within the timeframe allowed by many gist experiments. Either scenario would be contrary to the concept of gist as occurring merely in parallel across the visual field and in the absence or near absence of attention.

The Development of Perceptual Expertise in Radiology

Kelly and colleagues found that certain ocular metrics (such as time to first fixation) improved at an earlier stage of training than diagnostic accuracy in pneumothorax detection. This same study found significant differences in diagnostic accuracy, but not in the ocular metrics, of consultants (equivalent USA rank: attending) vs. registrars (equivalent USA rank: fellow), suggesting that expert gaze dynamics are learned at a faster pace than diagnostic abilities, and that they plateau at a relatively early stage of formal residency training (Kelly et al., 2016).

Ravesloot et al. (2017) found that scores from image interpretation questions (image-based questions testing interpretation skills) improved faster than knowledge-based questions (text-based factual questions) for the first 3 years of residency when residents took the Dutch Radiology Progress Test, a mandatory semiannual test taken by all Dutch radiology residents (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Reprinted from Ravesloot et al. (2017) with permission. Graph estimating image interpretation skill development during residency, as measured by the Dutch Radiology Progress test. Image score measures performance on image interpretation skills as opposed to factual knowledge. The score represents percentage from the maximum possible score and is calculated by subtracting the number of incorrect answers from the number of correct answers to account for guessing (making a negative value possible). The slope represents the speed of skill development and measures 16.8% during the first year of training. The slope decreases by 50% every year until it reaches 2.0% at the end of training. Note that the maximum image-score is estimated at 55.8%. Dotted lines represent the middle 95% of performances (Ravesloot et al., 2017).

Both eye tracking metrics and performance on image interpretation-based questions show that the ability to analyze images develops at a faster rate than factual knowledge. Whereas radiologists’ perceptual skills begins to grow from the start of exposure to imaging, radiology-specific factual knowledge contributes little to this initial development (Ravesloot et al., 2017).

Together, these studies also indicate that perceptual expertise plateaus early with a high level of error. Worse, this plateau likely persists beyond a radiologists formal training period, an important issue for residency programs to address.

Challenges to the Functional Definition of Expertise

The functional definition of expertise in the literature is limited for a number of reasons. Radiologic learners are often classified into the broad categories of experts versus novices, dramatically oversimplifying reality while disregarding intermediate training stages (Gunderman et al., 2001). Even studies that include intermediate stages in their analyses base their categorization on the professional level of training (Kundel et al., 2007). Indeed, a large meta-analysis of eye tracking research in professional domains found that in 6 of 8 radiology-based studies, expertise was determined solely according to professional levels of training and/or years of experience (Gegenfurtner et al., 2011). Fox and Faulkner-Jones (2017) note that experimental designs tend to include at most three participants groups, and bin medical students as novices, radiology residents as intermediate-level trainees, and attendings as experts.

The assignment of the ‘expert’ label purely based on professional degrees, rank, or experience is problematic for reasons both theoretical and practical. Although a specific attending may be considered to be an ‘expert’ by peers because of an extensive knowledge base, this does not necessarily directly translate into perceptual expertise. As Gunderman notes, expertise in one radiologic domain does not necessarily apply to every domain (Gunderman et al., 2001). For instance, Beam et al. (2006) and Elmore et al. (2016) note that mammographers have two distinct tasks: (1) Interpretation of screening mammograms and (2) Interpretation of abnormal screening mammographic findings. The first task requires evaluation of standard images from a large population of individuals without specific signs or symptoms (considered ‘perceptual’), whereas the second task requires careful analysis of specific abnormalities (considered more ‘cognitive’) (Beam et al., 2006; Elmore et al., 2016). Both studies found only a moderate correlation between radiologists’ performance in the two domains. In other words, proficiency in one area did not guarantee proficiency in the other (Beam et al., 2006; Elmore et al., 2016). The presence of wide disparities in screening and diagnostic interpretive skills in the same individual has been described as ‘expertise disequilibrium’ (Beam et al., 2006). In addition, assuming perceptual expertise based purely on experience negates any potential effects from age-related declines in contrast sensitivity, known to be most marked for high spatial frequencies (Owsley and Burton, 1991; Davies et al., 1994).

It follows that, even if attendings are considered ‘experts’ by their peers (or by rank or by years of training), they are not all de-facto perceptual experts and indeed may be highly capable in only a subclass of the complete radiology skillset. This heterogeneity is problematic if, in fact, homogeneity of expertise is assumed when evaluating radiologists’ perceptual performance (i.e., such as in a nodule detection task study).

Bearing out these theoretical concerns, several research studies have demonstrated that residents in the ‘intermediate bin’ can outperform ‘expert’ attendings. Rubin et al. (2015) found no significant correlation between experience and detection of artificially placed nodules in CT examinations. Indeed, at least one resident in their first year of training outperformed a sub-specialist thoracic radiologist in their specialty, despite an extensive experience differential (Rubin et al., 2015). Similarly, Kelly et al. (2016) found that less experienced radiologists occasionally outperformed more experienced radiologists in pneumothorax detection. Kundel et al. (2007) found a wide range of diagnostic performance across residents, fellows, and attendings when looking at a test set of mammograms. Hanna et al. (2018) likewise found that residents could outperform attendings in a fracture detection task under both fatigued and non-fatigued conditions. Lesgold et al. (1988) discovered that more advanced residents were occasionally less likely to make a correct interpretation on several complex x-rays compared to more junior residents. Finally, Nakashima et al. (2016) found no difference in performance in a nodule and bullae detection task on CT amongst radiologists with experience ranging from 3 to 15 years.

In summary, experience is a necessary, but insufficient, indicator of expert performance (van der Gijp et al., 2017a). At best, experience is an uncertain predictor of expertise level, and at worse, it reflects little more than seniority (Shanteau et al., 2002). Likewise, the use of certification (such as conferred from the American Board of Radiology) as a marker of expertise is limited in that it is most often tied to years on the job, rather than to objective performance. To compound the problem, certification suffers from the ‘ratchet up effect,’ whereupon individuals move up but not down the ladder, even if their skill level suffers a serious decline over time (Shanteau et al., 2002). Ericsson has noted that there is little empirical evidence for the traditional view that expertise is acquired through extended experience alone (Ericsson, 2004). Instead, the development of expertise more likely arises from domain-specific ‘deliberate practice,’ with accurate and detailed immediate feedback and opportunities for repetition (Causer et al., 2014; Ericsson, 2017). It follows that if a first year resident outperforms a fellowship-trained thoracic radiologist in nodule detection, the resident should be labeled as an ‘expert’ (in studies of search tasks), irrespective of their lack of experience. Defining expertise by performance, rather than by titles, is therefore liable to produce more accurate data.

A Way Forward—Ways to Promote Perceptual Expertise

The Role of Subspecialty Training

Given increasing specialization in medicine, some authors have advocated that radiology groups pursue a subspecialization model (Strax, 2012; Gunderman and Stevens, 2014; Arenson, 2018). Indeed, several studies have shown that subspecialists have better accuracy in their relative subfields than general radiologists (Sickles et al., 2002; Briggs et al., 2008; Bell and Patel, 2014; Kligerman et al., 2018), perhaps due to limiting the field of scope and enhanced networking with subspecialty medical experts whom provide prompt feedback. Thus, concentration on a narrow field may be a way to achieve the case volume and feedback needed to ensure expertise.

Perceptual Learning

Perceptual learning may be defined as “an increase in the ability to extract information from the environment, as a result of experience and practice with stimulation coming from it” (Gibson, 1969). In general terms, this refers to how experience can change the way we perceive sights, sounds, smells, tastes, and touch. Such continuous learning is the foundation of perceptual competence and eventual expertise. As such, it is a topic of intense and increasing scientific study. For recent reviews see (Lu et al., 2011; Gilbert and Li, 2012; Sasaki et al., 2012; Kawato et al., 2014; Li, 2016; Seitz, 2017).

The developing brain calibrates its perceptual systems through interaction with the environment; for instance, tuning and updating neural representations of our body and sensory organs as we grow (i.e., the length of our limbs changes with age). Much of this learning occurs during critical periods in early infancy, when the brain is most plastic, and its processes and connections are easily molded by experience. It is during such critical periods that changes in visual experience can have profound impact on the functional organization of the brain regions underlying perception. Yet, even though neural plasticity is diminished in adults, it is not lost. Indeed, with proper training, adults can exhibit an impressive degree of perceptual learning. A variety of stimuli including texture, orientation, contrast, and motion are used in visual perceptual learning research (Figure 7).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Reprinted from Sasaki et al. (2010) with permission. Texture-discrimination tasks such as depicted here are frequently used in visual perceptual learning (VPL) studies. The subject is first asked to report whether a ‘T’ (as in this example) or an ‘L’ is presented in the center of the display to ensure fixation, and then whether the orientation of the target (which is comprised of the three elements with orientations that depart from those of the other elements in the display) is vertical (as in this example) or horizontal. VPL of the target orientation is examined.

Perceptual learning studies most often focus on a specific visual feature, such as orientation, and train participants with exemplars. Typically, performance thresholds improve over time, so that by the end of training participants can accurately discriminate stimuli that would have been indistinguishable to them at the study’s onset. This type of learning differs from more conventional instruction methods where training is understood as consisting of facts, concepts, and procedures that are stored in one’s mind and later retrieve as needed for performance. This “container” model of the mind has come into question, however, in light of persistent problems in learning and instruction (Kellman, 2013). Students who have been faithfully taught and diligently absorbed declarative and procedural inputs occasionally fail to recognize key structures and patterns in real-world tasks. In addition, they may know how to perform procedures but fail to understand their application, especially to new problems or situations. Lastly, learners may understand concepts but process them slowly, with a high cognitive load, which leads to impairment in demanding, complex, or time-limited tasks (Kellman, 2013). One of us (SW) has encountered medical students who, after reviewing basic CXR principles from textbooks for a month, did not know how to apply their declarative knowledge in any practical sense, to interpret novel examinations. In other words, students may be able to memorize isolated, inert facts, but lack the ability to use them constructively to solve problems in radiologic diagnosis (Gunderman et al., 2001).

Whereas novice trainees tend to engage in more feature-based ‘analytical’ processes, experts perform more rapid pattern recognition, which relies in part on experience-based perceptual (a form of ‘implicit’) learning (Rimoin et al., 2015). Two broad categories of perceptual learning effects have been described: discovery and fluency (Kellman et al., 2008; Kellman, 2013). Discovery effects involve finding information relevant to classifications, extracting it selectively and distinguishing important structures from irrelevant variations. Fluency effects involve improvements in the extraction and encoding of relevant information. Effects include speed, parallel processing, and automaticity (experts demonstrate lower cognitive load than novices), allowing efficient perceptual classification to coexist with other cognitive processes in complex tasks (Krasne et al., 2013).

One of the major problems in radiology education is the lack of formalization and verbalization of what exactly happens during visual information extraction (i.e., “How do you teach to see a nodule?”) (Kellman and Garrigan, 2009; Krasne et al., 2013). Instead, it is assumed that advanced pattern recognition and automaticity will eventually arise from long apprenticeships, leaving crucial aspects of learning to occur in an unsystematic fashion, over an unspecified time, with unquantified results (Krasne et al., 2013).

Perceptual training, aimed at developing perceptual skills, has been applied to domains of human activity ranging from cricket play to language learning to airplane navigation (Tallal et al., 1996; Hopwood et al., 2011; Kellman and Kaiser, 2016). In the case of visual-based medical specialties, such as pathology, dermatology, and radiology, perceptual learning may be used to teach trainees how to visually recognize abnormalities, rather than how to interpret medical imaging based on a formal set of explicit rules (Chen et al., 2017). Yet, although this learning is a ubiquitous process in the adult brain, its implementation can require 10s of 1000s of trials of practice—without the certainty of learning success. Mere exposure to visual stimuli can produce perceptual learning, but it is often insufficient to yield robust results in the absence of additional factors, such as attention and reinforcement (Seitz, 2017).

Perceptual learning can be aided by training with attention. Covert attention, our ability to selectively process information at a given location without directing our gaze to that location, improves performance in many visual tasks mediated by basic visual dimensions, e.g., contrast sensitivity, spatial resolution and orientation (for reviews see Carrasco, 2011; Anton-Erxleben and Carrasco, 2013; Carrasco and Barbot, 2014). Research from the Carrasco lab has shown that covert attention can enable learning, in situations in which learning does not occur with training for the same time without attention (Szpiro and Carrasco, 2015), and facilitates transfer of learning to untrained locations (Donovan et al., 2015; Donovan and Carrasco, 2018). Possibly accounted for by plasticity in intermediate perceptual and higher decision-making brain regions, perceptual learning likely involves changes across a distributed collection of cortical areas (for review, Maniglia and Seitz, 2018). Although much of the research thus far has focused on low level perceptual discriminations, there is considerable evidence that perceptual learning is equally applicable to high-level, complex tasks (Kellman, 2013; Krasne et al., 2013).

Kellman and colleagues combined perceptual learning and adaptive learning methods (training that adapts in real-time to the trainees’ activity, and adjusts to their performance) to develop a web-based instruction program dubbed Perceptual and Adaptive Learning Modules (PALM), which does not rely on explicit instructions (Kellman et al., 2008; Rimoin et al., 2015). The PALM database includes multiple categories of diseases, and a typical module presents the learner with the task of discriminating or classifying a structure over a variety of instances, allowing the learner to discover the invariant properties of the given structure (Kellman et al., 2010). Mastery is gauged based on both accuracy and response time, given that experts are not only more accurate, but also able to complete tasks in a shorter time (and with less cognitive effort) than novices (Gunderman et al., 2001).

PALM has been successfully applied in medical perceptual training, including teaching histopathologic and dermatologic diagnosis to medical students, and echocardiography interpretation to anesthesiology residents. Learners demonstrated improvements in accuracy and fluency (sequential accurate responses made with short response times) even 6 months after training. Their training gains surpassed those typically obtained in traditional teaching (Krasne et al., 2013; Rimoin et al., 2015; Romito et al., 2016).

Sowden et al. (2000) conducted one of the first perceptual learning studies in radiologic imaging. They found that novice film readers improved their discriminations of clusters of microcalcifications in mammograms, and reduced their decision speeds, after following a perceptual learning regime where they viewed 60 images, three times each day, for 4 days. Negative feedback was provided in the form of a computer beep when the wrong cluster location was selected (Sowden et al., 2000). Remarkably, this work showed that, whereas radiologists in training may have already seen 1000s of images, even small amounts of practice in a relatively short interval, can produce significant improvements in sensitivity (Sowden et al., 2000; Krasne et al., 2013).

Chen et al. (2017) examined the efficacy of perceptual learning on the performance of novices with no prior knowledge of plain film interpretation, on the detection of hip fractures. They found that top performing novices achieved comparable accuracy to that of board-certified radiologists after 1280 images and 52 min of training. Whereas it is not known to what degree these findings might extrapolate to different pathology or imaging modalities, the data speaks to the potential of perceptual learning in radiology training (Chen et al., 2017) (Figure 8).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Reprinted from Chen et al. (2017) with permission. (A) Example of an image shown during perceptual training of hip fracture identification. (B) Arrows represent the feedback provided in case of a wrong answer. Top novices achieved expert level accuracy in hip fracture detection in under 1 h of perceptual training (Chen et al., 2017).

Books and digital resources abound to generate differential diagnoses for problematic findings (with pictures of representative abnormalities), but none of these resources address the first step of interpretation: perception. The fact that observational errors constitute the bulk of interpretive error in radiology (and that error rates have not changed in over half a century) (Waite et al., 2017b), highlights the need for (a) new educational methods to teach radiologists in training, and (b) the reassessment of present didactic and question-and-answer instruction techniques.

A New Research Paradigm

As outlined in this review, there have been important strides in understanding the nature of a radiologists’ perceptual expertise. In short, experts possess more refined and complex search strategies, organize information more efficiently (Lesgold et al., 1988), are faster and in general make less perceptual errors than novices, partially because they are better able to discern lesions from the background. Yet, there is no concrete evidence that radiologists have superior perceptual skills outside of imaging, and as such there is no accepted or principled basis to choose radiology trainees that will be most likely to attain mastery. In addition, research has demonstrated that perceptual expertise peaks earlier than factual knowledge and begins very early in training (Ravesloot et al., 2017). Unfortunately, perceptual expertise also appears to plateau at a high level of error, accounting for the persistent high levels of error noted since Garland’s pioneering work in the 1940s (Garland, 1949).

Even with our current understanding of the nature of radiologic expertise, and how it develops during residency, significant gaps in knowledge remain. Importantly, these include translation of the knowledge base into concrete methods for radiologists to decrease interpretive error. Peak expertise is currently achieved by radiologists only after years of trial-and-error, resulting in the learning of hidden principles that have not yet been articulated, and as such are not explicitly recognized or consistently applied. Until we understand the precise perceptual criteria that radiologists apply to discriminate abnormalities in medical images, the field as a whole will not achieve peak performance.

The ability of a radiologist to see abnormalities largely depends on their skill to recognize subtle shapes and textures embedded in a noisy background. Radiologic expertise may therefore constitute the solution to a complex texture discrimination problem. If so, expert radiologists may learn to rapidly detect abnormalities in their peripheral vision, and then use this information to target central vision for deeper analysis. This idea differs from previous suggestions about the perception of textures in visual performance in radiology (Balas et al., 2009; Whitney and Yamanashi Leib, 2018) in that here we refer to texture in the most general possible sense. Although classical image statistics, such as contrast, entropy, and the correlation between central and nearby pixel intensities, are thought to guide ocular fixation targeting, these statistics are not necessarily task relevant and therefore do not provide a complete picture of the relationship between informativeness and ocular targeting (McCamy et al., 2014).

We hypothesize that it is the correlated combination of the cardinal dimensions—the basis of visual texture (Bergen and Adelson, 1988; Bergen, 1991)—that expert radiologists may use to detect abnormalities. That is, just as both woody and granite textures can share the same cardinal image statistics, they nevertheless appear dissimilar due to their different textures (they are visual metamers with respect to their first-order statistics, but not in their second-order statistics). Prior work on texture statistics in radiological diagnosis has focused on individual texture features (i.e., “co-occurrence”), or specific (non-general) types of potentially relevant textures to radiology, while ignoring or actively removing information from other aspects of texture (i.e., orientation) (Hu et al., 2016). Previous studies also suggest that adding specific textures to objects in films can increase their visibility (Vittitoe et al., 1997). However, these previous studies necessarily chose what they felt were relevant textures, and their choices were to some extent arbitrary (as there is no established principle to know which textures are critical). Here, we propose that the field of radiology should determine the relevant textures from the entire space of second-order statistics that the human visual system can perceive. This approach entails the employment of a general model of texture statistics (Heeger and Bergen, 1995; Portilla and Simoncelli, 2000; Freeman and Simoncelli, 2011) to ascertain empirically, from the entire space of possible textures, which features are diagnostically informative. Such a strategy is likely to result in a principled and non-arbitrary set of informative textures that represent the difference in perceptual abilities between experts and novices (Armato et al., 2011). Once known, specific perceptual learning heuristics could be designed to train for enhanced detection of those particular textures.

The proposed model would fit all known results to the best of our knowledge, and is supported by studies showing that experienced radiologists tend to have longer saccadic lengths between fixations, suggesting that they are better able to see targets further out in their peripheral vision (and use them to target subsequent fixations) than novices (Alzubaidi et al., 2010). Extensive training may enable radiologists to perceptually learn to detect abnormal textures at increased retinal eccentricities, earlier in their instruction and more consistently. It follows that analysis of fixation consistency across radiologists (a measure of image ‘informativeness’) may account for both bottom–up and top–down influences on image exploration (McCamy et al., 2014).

Conclusion

Despite recent improvements in computer aided detection (CAD) and machine learning algorithms, radiologic interpretation is likely to remain a human task for the foreseeable future. Although many radiologists are concerned about artificial intelligence (AI) displacing them, most scholars are now of the opinion that AI will augment rather than replace radiologists (Liew, 2018). In a future where radiologists are mandatory as component human authorities (Liew, 2018), educational and practical interventions to improve human perceptual and decision-making skills continue to be needed to improve accuracy and reduce medical error (Waite et al., 2017b; Ekpo et al., 2018).

Radiology must move past the declarative knowledge paradigm to advance its training models and decrease its long-standing high error rate. In short, simply informing radiologists about potential errors does not improve their perception.

We propose that one way to improve radiologic instruction is, first, to determine precisely what makes abnormalities different from normal tissue—as in which textures are most informative—and then specifically train physicians to detect these textures with their peripheral vision, to enhance their ability to find abnormalities in medical images. This knowledge could afford focused perceptual learning, thereby supplementing conceptual knowledge and providing the exposure to abnormalities required for sensitivity improvements to occur during residency, rather than waiting (and hoping) for them to develop during routine radiologic practice. Our review of the literature moreover indicates the need for a deeper understanding of expertise-related individual differences in oculomotor-behavior, especially with regard to the informativeness of image regions. By precisely characterizing the contributions from each of these skills to the radiologist’s toolkit (and any potential overlap), we may be able to optimize heuristics for training on each of them.

As a field dominated by a primarily perceptual task, radiology needs a more refined understanding of perceptual expertise to improve accuracy, reduce error, and improve patient care.

Author Contributions

SW conceived and wrote the manuscript. AG wrote an initial section of the manuscript and contributed to the figures and permissions. RA wrote a section of the manuscript. SM contributed to the editing of the manuscript. SM, MC, DH, and SM-C contributed to the conception of the work. SM-C was responsible for extensive editing. All authors contributed to the revising, reading, and approving the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Anthony Mancuso, MD, for general feedback and Jami Jadidi, MD, for figure assistance.

References

Alexander, R. G., and Zelinsky, G. J. (2009). The frankenbear experiment: looking for part-based similarity effects on search guidance with complex objects. J. Vis. 9, 1184–1184. doi: 10.1167/9.8.1184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, R. G., and Zelinsky, G. J. (2011). Visual similarity effects in categorical search. J. Vis. 11:9. doi: 10.1167/11.8.9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzubaidi, M., Balasubramanian, V., Patel, A., Panchanathan, S., and Black, J. A. (2010). “What catches a radiologist’s eye? A comprehensive comparison of feature types for saliency prediction,” in Proceedings of the Medical Imaging 2010: Computer-Aided Diagnosis, (Bellingham: SPIE).

Google Scholar

Andrew, L. B. (2006). Expert witness testimony: the ethics of being a medical expert witness. Emerg. Med. Clin. North Am. 24, 715–731. doi: 10.1016/j.emc.2006.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andriole, K. P., Wolfe, J. M., Khorasani, R., Treves, S. T., Getty, D. J., and Jacobson, F. L. (2011). Optimizing analysis, visualization, and navigation of large image data sets: one 5000-section CT scan can ruin your whole day. Radiology 259, 346–362. doi: 10.1148/radiol.11091276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anton-Erxleben, K., and Carrasco, M. (2013). Attentional enhancement of spatial resolution: linking behavioural and neurophysiological evidence. nature reviews. Neuroscience 14, 188–200. doi: 10.1038/nrn3443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arenson, R. L. (2018). Factors affecting interpretative accuracy: how can we reduce errors? Radiology 287, 213–214. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2017172781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armato, S. G., McLennan, G., Bidaut, L., McNitt-Gray, M. F., Meyer, C. R., and Reeves, A. P. (2011). The lung image database consortium (LIDC) and image database resource initiative (IDRI): a completed reference database of lung nodules on CT scans. Med. Phys. 38, 915–931. doi: 10.1118/1.3528204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auffermann, W. F., Little, B. P., and Tridandapani, S. (2016). Teaching search patterns to medical trainees in an educational laboratory to improve perception of pulmonary nodules. J. Med. Imaging 3:011006. doi: 10.1117/1.JMI.3.1.011006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, S. R., Patel, R. H., Yang, L., Lelkes, V. M., and Castro, A. III (2013). Malpractice suits in chest radiology: an evaluation of the histories of 8265 radiologists. J. Thorac. Imaging. 28, 388–391. doi: 10.1097/RTI.0b013e3182a21be2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balas, B., Nakano, L., and Rosenholtz, R. (2009). A summary-statistic representation in peripheral vision explains visual crowding. J. Vis. 9:13. doi: 10.1167/9.12.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banaste, N., Caurier, B., Bratan, F., Bergerot, J. F., Thomson, V., and Millet, I. (2018). Whole-body CT in patients with multiple traumas: factors leading to missed injury. Radiology 289, 374–383. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2018180492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bass, J. C., and Chiles, C. (1990). Visual skill. correlation with detection of solitary pulmonary nodules. Invest. Radiol. 25, 994–998.

Google Scholar

Beam, C. A., Conant, E. F., and Sickles, E. A. (2006). Correlation of radiologist rank as a measure of skill in screening and diagnostic interpretation of mammograms. Radiology 238, 446–453. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2382042066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, M. E., and Patel, M. D. (2014). The degree of abdominal imaging (AI) subspecialization of the reviewing radiologist significantly impacts the number of clinically relevant and incidental discrepancies identified during peer review of emergency after-hours body CT studies. Abdom. Imaging 39, 1114–1118. doi: 10.1007/s00261-014-0139-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergen, J. R. (1991). “Theories of visual texture perception,” in Vision and Visual Dysfunction, ed. D. Regan (New York, NY: Macmillan), 114–134.

Google Scholar

Bergen, J. R., and Adelson, E. H. (1988). Early vision and texture perception. Nature. 333, 363–364. doi: 10.1038/333363a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlin, L. (2007). Accuracy of diagnostic procedures: has it improved over the past five decades? Am. J. Roentgenol. 188, 1173–1178. doi: 10.2214/ajr.06.1270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlin, L. (2014). Radiologic errors, past, present and future. Diagnosis 1, 79–84. doi: 10.1515/dx-2013-0012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlin, L., Hoffman, T. R., Shields, W. F., and Cox, J. (2006). American college of radiology. when does expert witness testimony constitute a violation of the ACR code of ethics? the role of the ACR committee on ethics. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 3, 252–258. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2005.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertram, R., Kaakinen, J., Bensch, F., Helle, L., Lantto, E., Niemi, P., et al. (2016). Eye movements of radiologists reflect expertise in CT study interpretation: a potential tool to measure resident development. Radiology 281, 805–815. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2016151255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birchall, D. (2015). Spatial ability in radiologists: a necessary prerequisite? Br. J. Radiol. 88:20140511. doi: 10.1259/bjr.20140511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, A. P. (2017). Error and discrepancy in radiology: inevitable or avoidable? Insights Imaging 8, 171–182. doi: 10.1007/s13244-016-0534-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazeau-Lamontagne, L., Charlin, B., Gagnon, R., Samson, L., and van der Vleuten, C. (2004). Measurement of perception and interpretation skills during radiology training: utility of the script concordance approach. Med. Teach. 26, 326–332. doi: 10.1080/01421590410001679000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, P. C., Gandomkar, Z., Ekpo, E. U., Tapia, K., Trieu, P. D., and Lewis, S. J. (2018). Radiologists can detect the ‘gist’ of breast cancer before any overt signs of cancer appear. Sci. Rep. 8:8717. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26100-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, G. M., Flynn, P. A., Worthington, M., Rennie, I., and McKinstry, C. S. (2008). The role of specialist neuroradiology second opinion reporting: is there added value? Clin. Radiol. 63, 791–795. doi: 10.1016/j.crad.2007.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruno, M. A., Walker, E. A., and Abujudeh, H. H. (2015). Understanding and confronting our mistakes: the epidemiology of error in radiology and strategies for error reduction. RadioGraphics 35, 1668–1676. doi: 10.1148/rg.2015150023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmody, D. P., Kundel, H. L., and Toto, L. C. (1984). Comparison scans while reading chest images. Taught, but not practiced. Invest. Radiol. 19, 462–466. doi: 10.1097/00004424-198409000-00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmody, D. P., Nodine, C. F., and Kundel, H. L. (1980). Global and segmented search for lung nodules of different edge gradients. Invest. Radiol. 15, 224–233. doi: 10.1097/00004424-198005000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vis. Res. 51, 1484–1525. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M. (2014). “Spatial attention: perceptual modulation,” in The Oxford Handbook of Attention, eds S. Kastner and A. C. Nobre (New York: Oxford University Press), 183–230.

Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., and Barbot, A. (2014). How attention affects spatial resolution. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 79, 149–160. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2014.79.024687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., Giordano, A. M., and McElree, B. (2004). Temporal performance fields: visual and attentional factors. Vis. Res. 44, 1351–1365. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., Giordano, A. M., and McElree, B. (2006). Attention speeds processing across eccentricity: feature and conjunction searches. Vis. Res. 46, 2028–2040. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., and McElree, B. (2001). Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 5363–5367. doi: 10.1073/pnas.081074098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., McElree, B., Denisova, K., and Giordano, A. M. (2003). Speed of visual processing increases with eccentricity. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 669–670.

Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., Ponte, D., Rechea, C., and Sampedro, M. J. (1998). “Transient structures”: the effects of practice and distractor grouping on within-dimension conjunction searches. Percept. Psychophys. 60, 1243–1258. doi: 10.3758/bf03206173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrigan, A. J., Wardle, S. G., and Rich, A. N. (2018). Finding cancer in mammograms: if you know it’s there, do you know where? Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 3:10. doi: 10.1186/s41235-018-0096-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castells, X., Torá-Rocamora, I., Posso, M., Román, M., Vernet-Tomas, M., Rodríguez-Arana, A., et al. (2016). Risk of breast cancer in women with false-positive results according to mammographic features. Radiology 280, 379–386. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2016151174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Causer, J., Barach, P., and Williams, A. M. (2014). Expertise in medicine: using the expert performance approach to improve simulation training. Med. Educ. 48, 115–123. doi: 10.1111/medu.12306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, W., HolcDorf, D., McCusker, M. W., Gaillard, F., and Howe, P. D. L. (2017). Perceptual training to improve hip fracture identification in conventional radiographs. PLoS One 12:e0189192. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., and Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Real-world visual search is dominated by top-down guidance. Vis. Res. 46, 4118–4133. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, M. D., Evans, K. K., Wolfe, J. M., Bowen, J., and Tanaka, J. W. (2018). Inversion effects in the expert classification of mammograms and faces. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 3:31. doi: 10.1186/s41235-018-0123-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, L., Gale, A., Darker, I., Toms, A., and Saada, J. (2009). “Radiology image perception and observer performance: How does expertise and clinical information alter interpretation? Stroke detection explored through eye-tracking,” in Proceedings of SPIE-The International Society for Optical Engineering, (Bellingham: SPIE).

Google Scholar

Corry, C. A. (2011). The future of recruitment and selection in radiology. Is there a role for assessment of basic visuospatial skills? Clin. Radiol. 66, 481–483. doi: 10.1016/j.crad.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, R. L. I., Sowden, P. T., Hammond, S. M., and Ansell, J. (1994). “Expertise in categorizing mammograms: a perceptual or conceptual skill?,” in Proceedings of SPIE - The International Society for Optical Engineering, (Bellingham: SPIE).

Google Scholar

Degnan, A. J., Ghobadi, E. H., Hardy, P., Krupinski, E., Scali, E. P., Stratchko, L., et al. (2018). Perceptual and interpretive error in diagnostic radiology-causes and potential solutions. Acad. Radiol. 26, 833–845. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2018.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

den Boer, L., van der Schaaf, M. F., Vincken, K. L., Mol, C. P., Stuijfzand, B. G., and van der Gijp, A. (2018). Volumetric image interpretation in radiology: scroll behavior and cognitive processes. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 23, 783–802. doi: 10.1007/s10459-018-9828-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, I., and Carrasco, M. (2018). Endogenous spatial attention during perceptual learning facilitates location transfer. J. Vis. 18:7. doi: 10.1167/18.11.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, I., Szpiro, S., and Carrasco, M. (2015). Exogenous attention facilitates location transfer of perceptual learning. J. Vis. 15:11. doi: 10.1167/15.10.11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drew, T., Evans, K., Võ, M. L., Jacobson, F. L., and Wolfe, J. M. (2013a). Informatics in radiology: what can you see in a single glance and how might this guide visual search in medical images? Radiographics 33, 263–274. doi: 10.1148/rg.331125023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drew, T., Vo, M. L., Olwal, A., Jacobson, F., Seltzer, S. E., and Wolfe, J. M. (2013b). Scanners and drillers: characterizing expert visual search through volumetric images. J. Vis. 13:3. doi: 10.1167/13.10.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckstein, M. P. (2011). Visual search: a retrospective. J. Vis. 11:14. doi: 10.1167/11.5.14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckstein, M. P., Lago, M. A., and Abbey, C. K. (2017). The role of extra-foveal processing in 3D imaging. Proc. SPIE Int. Soc. Opt. Eng. 10136:101360E. doi: 10.1117/12.2255879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekpo, E. U., Alakhras, M., and Brennan, P. (2018). Errors in mammography cannot be solved through technology alone. Asian Pac. J. Cancer Prev. 19, 291–301.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Elmore, J. G., Cook, A. J., Bogart, A., Carney, P. A., Geller, B. M., and Taplin, S. H. (2016). Radiologists’ interpretive skills in screening vs. diagnostic mammography: are they related? Clin. Imaging 40, 1096–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.clinimag.2016.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. Acad. Med. 79(10 Suppl.), S70–S81.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ericsson, K. A. (2017). Expertise and individual differences: the search for the structure and acquisition of experts’ superior performance. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 8:e1382. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K., Birdwel, R. L., Georgian-Smith, D., and Wolfe, J. M. (2010). “Discrimination and Localization of Abnormalities in Mammograms from a Global Signal,” in Proceedings of the Radiological Society of North America 2010 Scientific Assembly and Annual Meeting, Chicago IL.

Google Scholar

Evans, K. K., Georgian-Smith, D., Tambouret, R., Birdwell, R. L., and Wolfe, J. M. (2013). The gist of the abnormal: above-chance medical decision making in the blink of an eye. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 1170–1175. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0459-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. K., Haygood, T. M., Cooper, J., Culpan, A. M., and Wolfe, J. M. (2016). A half-second glimpse often lets radiologists identify breast cancer cases even when viewing the mammogram of the opposite breast. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 10292–10297. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606187113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fei-Fei, L., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2005). Why does natural scene categorization require little attention? exploring attentional requirements for natural and synthetic stimuli. Vis. Cogn. 12, 893–924. doi: 10.1080/13506280444000571

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forrest, J. V., and Friedman, P. J. (1981). Radiologic errors in patients with lung cancer. West J. Med. 134, 485–490.

Google Scholar

Fox, S., and Faulkner-Jones, B. (2017). Eye-tracking in the study of visual expertise: methodology and approaches in medicine. Front. Learn. Res. 5, 43–54. doi: 10.14786/flr.v5i3.258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, J., and Simoncelli, E. P. (2011). Metamers of the ventral stream. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1195–1201. doi: 10.1038/nn.2889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funaki, B., Szymski, G., and Rosenblum, J. (1997). Significant on-call misses by radiology residents interpreting computed tomographic studies: perception versus cognition. Emerg. Radiol. 4, 290–294. doi: 10.1007/bf01461735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, L. H. (1949). On the scientific evaluation of diagnostic procedures. Radiology 52, 309–328.

Google Scholar

Gegenfurtner, A., Kok, E., van Geel, K., de Bruin, A., Jarodzka, H., Szulewski, A., et al. (2017). The challenges of studying visual expertise in medical image diagnosis. Med. Educ. 51, 97–104. doi: 10.1111/medu.13205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gegenfurtner, A., Lehtinen, E., and Säljö, R. (2011). Expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations: a meta-analysis of eye-tracking research in professional domains. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 523–552. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9174-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, C. D., and Li, W. (2012). Adult visual cortical plasticity. Neuron 75, 250–264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, L. R., and Felson, B. (2007). Felson’s Principles of Chest Roentgenology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Gray, J. E., Taylor, K. W., and Hobbs, B. B. (1978). Detection accuracy in chest radiography. Am. J. Roentgenol. 131, 247–253. doi: 10.2214/ajr.131.2.247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunderman, R., Williamson, K., Fraley, R., and Steele, J. (2001). Expertise: implications for radiological education. Acad. Radiol. 8, 1252–1256.

Google Scholar

Gunderman, R. B., and Patel, P. (2019). Perception’s crucial role in radiology education. Acad. Radiol. 26, 141–143. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2018.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunderman, R. B., and Stevens, D. (2014). Radiology’s decathlete: the generalist. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 11, 1014–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2014.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, D. (2009). Imaging technology and practice assessments: what next? Acad. Radiol. 16, 638–640. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2008.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, D., Bandos, A. I., Cohen, C. S., Hakim, C. M., Hardesty, L. A., and Ganott, M. A. (2008). The “laboratory” effect: comparing radiologists’ performance and variability during prospective clinical and laboratory mammography interpretations. Radiology 249, 47–53. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2491072025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanna, T. N., Zygmont, M. E., Peterson, R., Theriot, D., Shekhani, H., and Johnson, J. O. (2018). The effects of fatigue from overnight shifts on radiology search patterns and diagnostic performance. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 15, 1709–1716. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2017.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heeger, D. J., and Bergen, J. (1995). “Pyramid-Based texture analysis/synthesis,” in Proceedings of the International Conference on Image Processing, Washington, DC.

Google Scholar

Herzog, R., Elgort, D. R., Flanders, A. E., and Moley, P. J. (2017). Variability in diagnostic error rates of 10 MRI centers performing lumbar spine MRI examinations on the same patient within a 3-week period. Spine J. 17, 554–561. doi: 10.1016/j.spinee.2016.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopwood, M. J., Mann, D. L., Farrow, D., and Nielsen, T. (2011). Does visual-perceptual training augment the fielding performance of skilled cricketers? Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 6, 523–535. doi: 10.1260/1747-9541.6.4.523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Liang, Z., Song, B., Han, H., Pickhardt, P. J., and Zhu, W. (2016). Texture feature extraction and analysis for polyp differentiation via computed tomography colonography. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 35, 1522–1531. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2016.2518958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, B., and Kelly, B. (2013). The abdominal radiograph. Ulst. Med. J. 82, 179–187.

Google Scholar

Jampani, V., Vaidya, V., Ujjwal, and Sivaswamy, J. (2012). “Assessment of computational visual attention models on medical images,” in Proceedings of the Eighth Indian Conference on Computer Vision, Graphics and Image Processing, (Mumbai: ACM), 1–8.

Google Scholar

Jarodzka, H., and Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2017). Unboxing the black box of visual expertise in medicine. Front. Learn. Res. 5:167–183. doi: 10.14786/flr.v5i3.332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasprowski, P., Harezlak, K., and Kasprowska, S. (2018). “Development of diagnostic performance & visual processing in different types of radiological expertise,” in Proceedings of the 2018 ACM Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications, (Warsaw: ACM), 1–6.

Google Scholar

Kawato, M., Lu, Z. L., Sagi, D., Sasaki, Y., Yu, C., and Watanabe, T. (2014). Perceptual learning–the past, present and future. Vis. Res. 99, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Kelahan, L. C., Fong, A., Blumenthal, J., Kandaswamy, S., Ratwani, R. M., and Filice, R. W. (2018). The radiologist’s gaze: mapping three-dimensional visual search in computed tomography of the abdomen and pelvis. J. Digit. Imaging 32, 234–240. doi: 10.1007/s10278-018-0121-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellman, P. J. (2013). Adaptive and perceptual learning technologies in medical education and training. Mil. Med. 178(10 Suppl.), 98–106. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-13-00218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellman, P. J., and Garrigan, P. (2009). Perceptual learning and human expertise. Phys. Life Rev. 6, 53–84. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellman, P. J., and Kaiser, M. K. (2016). Perceptual learning modules in flight training. Proc. Hum. Fact. Ergon. Soc. Ann. Meet. 38, 1183–1187. doi: 10.1177/154193129403801808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellman, P. J., Massey, C. M., and Son, J. Y. (2008). Perceptual learning and the technology of expertise studies in fraction learning and algebra. Pragmat. Cogn. 16, 356–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01053.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellman, P. J., Massey, C. M., and Son, J. Y. (2010). Perceptual learning modules in mathematics: enhancing students’ pattern recognition, structure extraction, and fluency. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2, 285–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01053.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, B., Rainford, L. A., McEntee, M. F., and Kavanagh, E. C. (2018). Influence of radiology expertise on the perception of nonmedical images. J. Med. Imaging 5:031402. doi: 10.1117/1.JMI.5.3.031402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, B. S., Rainford, L. A., Darcy, S. P., Kavanagh, E. C., and Toomey, R. J. (2016). The development of expertise in radiology: in chest radiograph interpretation, “expert” search pattern may predate “expert” levels of diagnostic accuracy for pneumothorax identification. Radiology 280, 252–260. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2016150409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. W., and Mansfield, L. T. (2014). Fool me twice: delayed diagnoses in radiology with emphasis on perpetuated errors. Am. J. Roentgenol. 202, 465–470. doi: 10.2214/AJR.13.11493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kligerman, S. J., Mitchell, J. W., Sechrist, J. W., Meeks, A. K., Galvin, J. R., and White, C. S. (2018). Radiologist performance in the detection of pulmonary embolism: features that favor correct interpretation and risk factors for errors. J. Thorac. Imaging 33, 350–357. doi: 10.1097/RTI.0000000000000361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, E. M. (2012). Looking in the same manner but seeing it differently: bottom-up and expertise effects in radiology. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 854–862. doi: 10.1002/acp.2886

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, E. M., Jarodzka, H., de Bruin, A. B., BinAmir, H. A., Robben, S. G., and van Merriënboer, J. J. (2015). Systematic viewing in radiology: seeing more, missing less? Adv. Health Sci. Educ. Theory Pract. 21, 189–205. doi: 10.1007/s10459-015-9624-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasne, S., Hillman, J. D., Kellman, P. J., and Drake, T. A. (2013). Applying perceptual and adaptive learning techniques for teaching introductory histopathology. J. Pathol. Inform. 4:34. doi: 10.4103/2153-3539.123991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krupinski, E. A. (2010). Current perspectives in medical image perception. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72, 1205–1217. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.5.1205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krupinski, E. A. (2011). The role of perception in imaging: past and future. Semin. Nucl. Med. 41, 392–400. doi: 10.1053/j.semnuclmed.2011.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krupinski, E. A., Berbaum, K. S., Caldwell, R. T., Schartz, K. M., Madsen, M. T., and Kramer, D. J. (2012). Do long radiology workdays affect nodule detection in dynamic CT interpretation? J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 9, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2011.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kundel, H. L. (2015). Visual search and lung nodule detection on CT scans. Radiology 274, 14–16. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14142247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kundel, H. L., and La Follette, P. S. Jr. (1972). Visual search patterns and experience with radiological images. Radiology 103, 523–528. doi: 10.1148/103.3.523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kundel, H. L., and Nodine, C. F. (1975). Interpreting chest radiographs without visual search. Radiology 116, 527–532. doi: 10.1148/116.3.527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kundel, H. L., Nodine, C. F., and Carmody, D. (1978). Visual scanning, pattern recognition and decision-making in pulmonary nodule detection. Invest. Radiol. 13, 175–181. doi: 10.1097/00004424-197805000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kundel, H. L., Nodine, C. F., Conant, E. F., and Weinstein, S. P. (2007). Holistic component of image perception in mammogram interpretation: gaze-tracking study. Radiology 242, 396–402. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2422051997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kundel, H. L., Nodine, C. F., Krupinski, E. A., and Mello-Thoms, C. (2008). Using gaze-tracking data and mixture distribution analysis to support a holistic model for the detection of cancers on mammograms. Acad. Radiol. 15, 881–886. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2008.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehr, J. L., Lodwick, G. S., Farrell, C., Braaten, M. O., Virtama, P., Kolvisto, E. L., et al. (1976). Direct measurement of the effect of film miniaturization on diagnostic accuracy. Radiology 118, 257–263. doi: 10.1148/118.2.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesgold, A., Rubinson, H., Feltovich, P., Glaser, R., Klopfer, D., and Wang, Y. (1988). “Expertise in a complex skill: Diagnosing x-ray pictures,” in The Nature of Expertise, eds M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, and M. J. Farr (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 311–342.

Google Scholar

Li, F. F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2002). Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 9596–9601. doi: 10.1073/pnas.092277599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. (2016). Perceptual learning: use-dependent cortical plasticity. Ann. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2, 109–130. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, C. (2018). The future of radiology augmented with Artificial Intelligence: a strategy for success. Eur. J. Radiol. 102, 152–156. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2018.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litchfield, D., and Donovan, T. (2016). Worth a quick look? Initial scene previews can guide eye movements as a function of domain-specific expertise but can also have unforeseen costs. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 982–994. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litchfield, D., and Donovan, T. (2017). The flash-preview moving window paradigm: unpacking visual expertise one glimpse at a time. Front. Learn. Res. 5, 66–80. doi: 10.14786/flr.v5i3.269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingstone, M. S., and Hubel, D. H. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science 240, 740–749. doi: 10.1126/science.3283936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loy, C. T., and Irwig, L. (2004). Accuracy of diagnostic tests read with and without clinical information: a systematic review. JAMA 292, 1602–1609.

Google Scholar

Lu, Z. L., Hua, T., Huang, C. B., Zhou, Y., and Dosher, B. A. (2011). Visual perceptual learning. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 95, 145–151. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mallett, S., Phillips, P., Fanshawe, T. R., Helbren, E., Boone, D., and Gale, A. (2014). Tracking eye gaze during interpretation of endoluminal three-dimensional CT colonography: visual perception of experienced and inexperienced readers. Radiology 273, 783–792. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14132896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maniglia, M., and Seitz, A. R. (2018). Towards a whole brain model of perceptual learning. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 20, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCamy, M. B., Otero-Millan, J., Di Stasi, L. L., Macknik, S. L., and Martinez-Conde, S. (2014). Highly informative natural scene regions increase microsaccade production during visual scanning. J. Neurosci. 34, 2956–2966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4448-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mello-Thoms, C. (2003). Perception of breast cancer: eye-position analysis of mammogram interpretation. Acad. Radiol. 10, 4–12.

Google Scholar

Mello-Thoms, C. (2006). How does the perception of a lesion influence visual search strategy in mammogram reading? Acad. Radiol. 13, 275–288. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2005.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mello-Thoms, C., and Chapman, B. (2004). A preliminary report on the role of spatial frequency analysis in the perception of breast cancers missed at mammography screening. Acad. Radiol. 11, 894–908. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2004.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mello-Thoms, C., Dunn, S. M., Nodine, C. F., and Kundel, H. L. (2003). The perception of breast cancers–a spatial frequency analysis of what differentiates missed from reported cancers. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 22, 1297–1306. doi: 10.1109/tmi.2003.817784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muhm, J. R., Miller, W. E., Fontana, R. S., Sanderson, D. R., and Uhlenhopp, M. A. (1983). Lung cancer detected during a screening program using four-month chest radiographs. Radiology 148, 609–615. doi: 10.1148/radiology.148.3.6308709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakashima, R., Komori, Y., Maeda, E., Yoshikawa, T., and Yokosawa, K. (2016). Temporal characteristics of radiologists’ and novices’ lesion detection in viewing medical images presented rapidly and sequentially. Front. Psychol. 7:1553.

Google Scholar

Nakashima, R., Watanabe, C., Maeda, E., Yoshikawa, T., Matsuda, I., Miki, S., et al. (2015). The effect of expert knowledge on medical search: medical experts have specialized abilities for detecting serious lesions. Psychol. Res. 79, 729–738. doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0616-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nobre, A. C., Kastner, S., and Carrasco, M. (2014). Spatial Covert Attention Perceptual Modulation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Nodine, C. F., and Krupinski, E. A. (1998). Perceptual skill, radiology expertise, and visual test performance with NINA and WALDO. Acad. Radiol. 5, 603–612. doi: 10.1016/s1076-6332(98)80295-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nodine, C. F., and Kundel, H. L. (1987). Using eye movements to study visual search and to improve tumor detection. Radiographics 7, 1241–1250. doi: 10.1148/radiographics.7.6.3423330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oestmann, J. W., Greene, R., Bourgouin, P. M., Linetsky, L., and Llewellyn, H. J. (1993). Chest “gestalt” and detectability of lung lesions. Eur. J. Radiol. 16, 154–157. doi: 10.1016/0720-048x(93)90015-f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oestmann, J. W., Greene, R., Kushner, D. C., Bourgouin, P. M., Linetsky, L., and Llewellyn, H. J. (1988). Lung lesions: correlation between viewing time and detection. Radiology 166, 451–453. doi: 10.1148/radiology.166.2.3336720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otsuka, S., and Kawaguchi, J. (2007). Natural scene categorization with minimal attention: evidence from negative priming. Percept. Psychophys. 69, 1126–1139. doi: 10.3758/bf03193950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owsley, C., and Burton, K. B. (1991). “Aging and Spatial Contrast Sensitivity: Underlying Mechanisms and Implications for Everyday Life,” in The Changing Visual System: Maturation and Aging in the Central Nervous System, eds P. Bagnoli and W. Hodos (Boston, MA: Springer), 119–136. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-3390-0_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portilla, J., and Simoncelli, E. P. (2000). A parametric texture model based on joint statistics of complex wavelet coefficients. Int. J. Comput. Vis. 40:23.

Google Scholar

Puddy, E., and Hill, C. (2007). Interpretation of the chest radiograph. Cont. Educ. Anaesth. Crit. Care Pain 7, 71–75.

Google Scholar

Ravesloot, C. J., van der Schaaf, M. F., Kruitwagen, C. L. J. J., van der Gijp, A., Rutgers, D. R., and Haaring, C. (2017). Predictors of knowledge and image interpretation skill development in radiology residents. Radiology 284, 758–765. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2017152648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, W. M., Chow, S. L., Chew, L. E., and Brennan, P. C. (2014). Can prevalence expectations drive radiologists’ behavior? Acad. Radiol. 21, 450–456. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2013.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, W. M., Ryan, J. T., McEntee, M. F., Evanoff, M. G., and Brennan, P. C. (2011). The effect of abnormality-prevalence expectation on expert observer performance and visual search. Radiology 258, 938–943. doi: 10.1148/radiol.10101090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renfrew, D. L., Franken, E. A. Jr., Berbaum, K. S., Weigelt, F. H., and Abu-Yousef, M. M. (1992). Error in radiology: classification and lessons in 182 cases presented at a problem case conference. Radiology 183, 145–150. doi: 10.1148/radiology.183.1.1549661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimoin, L., Altieri, L., Craft, N., Krasne, S., and Kellman, P. J. (2015). Training pattern recognition of skin lesion morphology, configuration, and distribution. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 72, 489–495. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2014.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romito, B. T., Krasne, S., Kellman, P. J., and Dhillon, A. (2016). The impact of a perceptual and adaptive learning module on transoesophageal echocardiography interpretation by anaesthesiology residents. Br. J. Anaesth. 117, 477–481. doi: 10.1093/bja/aew295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenkrantz, A. B., and Bansal, N. K. (2016). Diagnostic errors in abdominopelvic CT interpretation: characterization based on report addenda. Abdom. Radiol. 41, 1793–1799. doi: 10.1007/s00261-016-0741-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousselet, G. A., Fabre-Thorpe, M., and Thorpe, S. J. (2002). Parallel processing in high-level categorization of natural images. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 629–630. doi: 10.1038/nn866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, G. D., Roos, J. E., Tall, M., Harrawood, B., Bag, S., and Ly, D. L. (2015). Characterizing search, recognition, and decision in the detection of lung nodules on CT scans: elucidation with eye tracking. Radiology 274, 276–286. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14132918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasaki, Y., Nanez, J. E., and Watanabe, T. (2010). Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 53–60. doi: 10.1038/nrn2737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasaki, Y., Náñez, J. E., and Watanabe, T. (2012). Recent progress in perceptual learning research. Wiley Interdiscipl. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 3, 293–299. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seitz, A. R. (2017). Perceptual learning. Curr. Biol. 27, R631–R636. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shanteau, J., Weiss, D. J., Thomas, R. P., and Pounds, J. C. (2002). Performance-based assessment of expertise: how to decide if someone is an expert or not. Eur. J. Operat. Res. 136, 253–263. doi: 10.1016/s0377-2217(01)00113-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheridan, H., and Reingold, E. M. (2017). The holistic processing account of visual expertise in medical image perception: a review. Front. Psychol. 8:1620. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sickles, E. A., Wolverton, D. E., and Dee, K. E. (2002). Performance parameters for screening and diagnostic mammography: specialist and general radiologists. Radiology 224, 861–869. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2243011482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegle, R. L., Baram, E. M., Reuter, S. R., Clarke, E. A., Lancaster, J. L., McMahan, C. A., et al. (1998). Rates of disagreement in imaging interpretation in a group of community hospitals. Acad. Radiol. 5, 148–154. doi: 10.1016/s1076-6332(98)80277-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, W. L., and Berbaum, K. S. (1991). Improving resident selection. discrimination by perceptual abilities. Invest. Radiol. 26, 910–912. doi: 10.1097/00004424-199110000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smoker, W. R., Berbaum, K. S., Luebke, N. H., and Jacoby, C. G. (1984). Spatial perception testing in diagnostic radiology. Am. J. Roentgenol. 143, 1105–1109. doi: 10.2214/ajr.143.5.1105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowden, P. T., Davies, I. R., and Roling, P. (2000). Perceptual learning of the detection of features in X-ray images: a functional role for improvements in adults’ visual sensitivity? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 379–390. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.26.1.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strasburger, H., Rentschler, I., and Juttner, M. (2011). Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: a review. J. Vis. 11:13. doi: 10.1167/11.5.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strax, R. (2012). Counterpoint: Generalism vs subspecialization–the ACR should develop a practice model to support general radiologists. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 9, 537–538. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2012.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunday, M. A., Donnelly, E., and Gauthier, I. (2017). Individual differences in perceptual abilities in medical imaging: the vanderbilt chest radiograph test. Cogn. Res. 2:36. doi: 10.1186/s41235-017-0073-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szpiro, S. F. A., and Carrasco, M. (2015). Exogenous attention enables perceptual learning. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1854–1862. doi: 10.1177/0956797615598976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., and Nagarajan, S. S. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science 271, 81–84. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5245.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, P. M. (2007). A review of research into the development of radiologic expertise: implications for computer-based training. Acad. Radiol. 14, 1252–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2007.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuddenham, W. J., and Calvert, W. P. (1961). Visual search patterns in roentgen diagnosis. Radiology 76, 255–256. doi: 10.1148/76.2.255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Gijp, A., Ravesloot, C. J., Jarodzka, H., van der Schaaf, M. F., van der Schaaf, I. C., and van Schaik, J. P. J. (2017a). How visual search relates to visual diagnostic performance: a narrative systematic review of eye-tracking research in radiology. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 22, 765–787. doi: 10.1007/s10459-016-9698-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Gijp, A., Vincken, K. L., Boscardin, C., Webb, E. M., Ten Cate, O. T. J., and Naeger, D. M. (2017b). The effect of teaching search strategies on perceptual performance. Acad. Radiol. 24, 762–767. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2017.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Gijp, A., van der Schaaf, M. F., van der Schaaf, I. C., Huige, J. C., Ravesloot, C. J., and van Schaik, J. P. (2014). Interpretation of radiological images: towards a framework of knowledge and skills. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 19, 565–580. doi: 10.1007/s10459-013-9488-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Geel, K., Kok, E. M., Dijkstra, J., Robben, S. G., and van Merriënboer, J. J. (2017). Teaching systematic viewing to final-year medical students improves systematicity but not coverage or detection of radiologic abnormalities. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 14, 235–241. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2016.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venjakob, A. C., and Mello-Thoms, C. R. (2016). Review of prospects and challenges of eye tracking in volumetric imaging. J. Med. Imaging 3:011002. doi: 10.1117/1.JMI.3.1.011002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vittitoe, N. F., Baker, J. A., and Floyd, C. E. (1997). Fractal texture analysis in computer-aided diagnosis of solitary pulmonary nodules. Acad. Radiol. 4, 96–101. doi: 10.1016/s1076-6332(97)80005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waite, S., Kolla, S., Jeudy, J., Legasto, A., Macknik, S. L., and Martinez-Conde, S. (2017a). Tired in the reading room: the influence of fatigue in radiology. J. Am. Coll. Radiol. 14, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2016.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waite, S., Scott, J., Gale, B., Fuchs, T., Kolla, S., Reede, D., et al. (2017b). Interpretive error in radiology. Am. J. Roentgenol. 208, 739–749. doi: 10.2214/ajr.16.16963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waite, S., Scott, J. M., Legasto, A., Kolla, S., Gale, B., Krupinski, E. A., et al. (2017c). Systemic error in radiology. Am. J. Roentgenol. 209, 629–639. doi: 10.2214/ajr.16.17719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waite, S., Scott, J. M., Drexler, I., Martino, J., Legasto, A., Gale, B., et al. (2018). Communication errors in radiology - Pitfalls and how to avoid them. Clin. Imaging 51, 266–272. doi: 10.1016/j.clinimag.2018.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, G., Aizenman, A., Drew, T., Wolfe, J. M., Haygood, T. M., and Markey, M. K. (2016). Computational assessment of visual search strategies in volumetric medical images. J. Med. Imaging 3:015501. doi: 10.1117/1.JMI.3.1.015501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, G., Rodriguez-Niño, B., Pecen, F. Y., Vining, D. J., Garg, N., and Markey, M. K. (2017). Comparative study of computational visual attention models on two-dimensional medical images. J. Med. Imaging 4:025503. doi: 10.1117/1.JMI.4.2.025503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitney, D., and Yamanashi Leib, A. (2018). Ensemble perception. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 69, 105–129. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0: a revised model of visual search. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 202–238. doi: 10.3758/bf03200774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., and Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 15, 419–433. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.15.3.419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, J. M., Evans, K. K., Drew, T., Aizenman, A., and Josephs, E. (2016). How do radiologists use the human search engine? Radiat. Prot. Dosim. 169, 24–31. doi: 10.1093/rpd/ncv501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfe, J. M., and Horowitz, T. S. (2017). Five factors that guide attention in visual search. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0058.

Google Scholar

Wolfe, J. M., Võ, M. L., Evans, K. K., and Greene, M. R. (2011). Visual search in scenes involves selective and nonselective pathways. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, B. P. (1999). Visual expertise. Radiology 211, 1–3.

Google Scholar

Keywords: visual perception, expertise, radiology, visual search, perceptual learning, attention, holistic processing, gist

Citation: Waite S, Grigorian A, Alexander RG, Macknik SL, Carrasco M, Heeger DJ and Martinez-Conde S (2019) Analysis of Perceptual Expertise in Radiology – Current Knowledge and a New Perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:213. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00213

Received: 18 February 2019; Accepted: 07 June 2019;
Published: 25 June 2019.

Edited by:

Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, United States

Reviewed by:

Craig Abbey, University of California, Santa Barbara, United States
Tim Donovan, University of Cumbria, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Waite, Grigorian, Alexander, Macknik, Carrasco, Heeger and Martinez-Conde. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen Waite, c3RlcGhlbi53YWl0ZUBkb3duc3RhdGUuZWR1

Senior author

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.