Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 05 February 2019
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Learning and Memory View all 59 articles

Direct Stimulation of Human Hippocampus During Verbal Associative Encoding Enhances Subsequent Memory Recollection

  • 1Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Research Institute of Basic Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 3Department of Neurosurgery, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea

Previous studies have reported conflicting results regarding the effect of direct electrical stimulation of the human hippocampus on memory performance. A major function of the hippocampus is to form associations between individual elements of experience. However, the effect of direct hippocampal stimulation on associative memory remains largely inconclusive, with most evidence coming from studies employing non-invasive stimulation. Here, we therefore tested the hypothesis that direct electrical stimulation of the hippocampus specifically enhances hippocampal-dependent associative memory. To test this hypothesis, we recruited surgical patients with implanted subdural electrodes to perform a word pair memory task during which the hippocampus was stimulated. Our results indicate that stimulation of the hippocampus during encoding helped to build strong associative memories and enhanced recollection in subsequent trials. Moreover, stimulation significantly increased theta power in the lateral middle temporal cortex during successful memory encoding. Overall, our findings indicate that hippocampal stimulation positively impacts performance during a word pair memory task, suggesting that successful memory encoding involves the temporal cortex, which may act together with the hippocampus.

Introduction

The hippocampus plays a pivotal role in associative memory (Olsen et al., 2012; Yonelinas, 2013), serving as a hub that supports the binding of information (Battaglia et al., 2011); it is thus regarded as a core region for stimulation in attempts to manipulate the memory circuit (Eichenbaum et al., 2007). Some previous studies have reported that direct hippocampal stimulation exerts negative effects (Coleshill et al., 2004; Lacruz et al., 2010; Jacobs et al., 2016; Goyal et al., 2018) or no effect (Suthana et al., 2012) on memory, whereas others have reported positive effects (Berger et al., 2011; Hampson et al., 2012; Fell et al., 2013). However, several non-invasive stimulation studies have reported that hippocampal stimulation enhances paired associative memory (Wang et al., 2014; Wang and Voss, 2015). In one recent study, hippocampal-targeted TMS enhanced associative memory. In contrast, item memory was unaffected, demonstrating a selective influence on associative memory vs. item memory (Tambini et al., 2018). Specifically, multiple-day electromagnetic stimulation has been shown to enhance memory recollection, by exerting network-level effects on memory precision (Nilakantan et al., 2017). However, as current evidence mostly comes from indirect stimulation of the hippocampus, the effect of direct hippocampal stimulation on associative memory has yet to be determined.

Recollection is defined as the retrieval of contextual details associated with a previously experienced event (Yonelinas, 2013). The cognitive process of recollection involves a set of brain regions termed the “recollection network” (Rugg and Vilberg, 2013). Numerous studies have shown both critical and necessary roles for structures outside of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) in memory (Buzsáki, 1996; Eichenbaum, 2000; Poldrack et al., 2001; Ritchey et al., 2015; Moscovitch et al., 2016). In particular, the lateral temporal cortex plays a key role in episodic memory processing (Chao et al., 1999). A recent study using lateral temporal cortical stimulation demonstrated enhanced verbal memory performance, presenting it as a viable target for exploring memory enhancement (Kucewicz et al., 2018). Furthermore, functional imaging changes have been observed in this region during the encoding stage of explicit verbal memory (Casasanto et al., 2002; Fletcher and Tyler, 2002). Indeed, neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal cortex has been shown to increase during the learning of associations between word pairs, suggesting that human associative learning is related to the activity of a specific population of “association” neurons (Ojemann and Schoenfield-McNeill, 1998; Ojemann et al., 2002, 2009).

Previous human intracranial electroencephalogram (iEEG) studies have reported that neural oscillatory changes in memory-related neocortical regions are accompanied by successful memory formation relative to unsuccessful encoding (Burke et al., 2013; Watrous et al., 2013; Lega et al., 2016). Specifically, low-frequency oscillations entrain the rhythm of behavioral tasks to optimize energy-efficient performance (Lakatos et al., 2008; Daitch et al., 2013), while changes in theta power enhance episodic memory (Rutishauser et al., 2010; Addante et al., 2011; Lega et al., 2012; Backus et al., 2016; Sweeney-Reed et al., 2016). In the present study, we sought to investigate changes in activity within the neocortical region (i.e., the lateral temporal cortex) during encoding. We presumed that low-frequency activity in the temporal neocortex reflects the effects of stimulation-induced activity, indicating that verbal episodic memory encoding involves a network of neocortical structures that may act interdependently with the hippocampus.

To test this hypothesis, we applied direct hippocampal stimulation in surgical patients with epilepsy based on methods described in previous studies (Suthana et al., 2012; Jacobs et al., 2016; Hansen et al., 2018). However, we utilized a stimulation current with a higher amplitude (2 mA) than that used in the aforementioned studies (0.1–1.5 mA). In addition, we adopted a word pair memory task that involved recruitment of the hippocampus during encoding (Axmacher et al., 2008; Hamani et al., 2008). To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first of its kind utilizing a word pair memory task, which enabled us to compare the ability to remember a learning episode (recollection) with the capacity to judge items as familiar (familiarity; Manns et al., 2003; Mickes et al., 2010).

Materials and Methods

Patients

The present study included six patients (four women; mean age: 33.6 ± 10.8 years) with drug-resistant epilepsy who had been implanted with intracranial electrodes to determine the area of the seizure onset zone. Human subjects: this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University Hospital (H-1407-115-596). All subjects provided written informed consent to participate in the study. A single mid-hippocampal electrode had been implanted in each patient based on the expertise of neuroradiologists experienced in neuroanatomy (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient demographics, clinical characteristics, electrode locations, and stimulation parameters.

Electrode Localization

Electrodes were implanted for clinical purposes only. Depending on clinical need, electrodes (AdTech Medical Instrument Corporation, Racine, WI, USA) were either placed at depth within the MTL (platinum, surface area of 0.059 cm2, placed 6 mm apart) or positioned for subdural electrocorticography (ECoG) on the cortical surface (diameter of 4 mm, placed 10 mm apart) with stainless steel contacts. Prior to electrode implantation, each patient underwent an magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan. Additional MRI and CT scans were performed following electrode implantation. Patients underwent preoperative MR imaging in a Magnetom Trio, Magnetim Verio 3-tesla (Siemens, München, Germany) or Signa 1.5-tesla scanner (GE, Boston, MA, USA). CT images were recorded using a Somatom sensation device (64 eco; Siemens München, Germany). Each patient had at least one hippocampal electrode in the region of interest. Target hippocampal electrodes for stimulation (anode) were inserted into the mid-body of the hippocampus gray matter using a temporo-lateral approach. Given the electrode contact space within the MTL, the pairing electrode (cathode) of stimulation was identified in the temporal white matter (Figure 1A). A neuroradiologist and neurosurgeon experienced in neuroanatomical localization identified bipolar pairs of electrodes within medial lobe sites, including the hippocampus, based on thin-section post-implantation CT scans and cross-sectional images. For visualization, individual preoperative MR images and postoperative CT images were co-registered as previously described (Avants et al., 2008), using CURRY software version 7.0 (Compumedics Neuroscan, Charlotte, NC, USA). In addition, the hippocampal subregions were localized using a manual segmentation process (Boccardi et al., 2015; Figure 1B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Location of stimulation. The green crosshair denotes the location of the stimulation electrode in the right mid-hippocampus (sagittal and coronal, respectively) in Patient 1 (left two panels, anode and cathode, respectively) and in the left mid-hippocampus in Patient 2 (right two panels). (B) The hippocampal subregions in each patient and one hippocampal electrode in the region of interest.

Stimulation Procedure

Stimulation was only administered during the encoding phase, as in previous studies (Suthana et al., 2012; Jacobs et al., 2016). During the task, the stimulation was configured to provide a bipolar stimulation between a pair of neighboring electrodes. Stimulation was delivered with a Grass S12X cortical stimulator (Natus, Warwick, RI, USA), using the following parameters: a frequency of 50 Hz, a balanced biphasic squared-wave pulse of 300 μs per phase, a 2-mA current, and total energy between 30 and 57 μC/cm2/phase. These parameters have been demonstrated to be safe and well-tolerated in patients with epilepsy (Kuncel and Grill, 2004; Boon et al., 2007), and the energy level was kept well below the safe maximum used for long-term and short-term stimulation (30 and 57, respectively; Agnew and McCreery, 1990; Gordon et al., 1990). The impedance of the depth electrode was always between 1 and 10 KΩ. Patients could not indicate when stimulation was applied.

Verbal Associative Memory Task

All stimuli were presented on a laptop computer, with Stim 2 Gentask (Neuroscan, Charlotte, NC, USA) used to present the word stimuli. We used a word pair memory task (Figure 2), which has been associated with recruitment of the MTL during memory encoding (Axmacher et al., 2008; Hamani et al., 2008). All word pairs consisted of two concrete Korean nouns with a mean frequency value of 105.11 (SD = 3.35, IQR = 122.5) according to the Korean Category Norms: Survey on Exemplar Frequency Norm, Typicality, and Features (Rhee, 1991) and the 2nd version of the Modern Korean Words database (Kim, 2005). Prior to the experimental session, a brief practice block of trials was administered to ensure that patients understood the task. For encoding, patients were asked to study 120 pairs over two sessions. Each word-pair trial began with a fixation cross appearing on the screen for 1 s, followed by the pair that was displayed for 4 s. Each session consisted of two blocks. The stimulation was randomized to one of the two blocks in each session. To ensure deep encoding, patients were instructed to report, by pressing a keyboard button with their index finger, whether they judged the word on the screen as “pleasant” or “unpleasant” (de Vanssay-Maigne et al., 2011). Following the encoding period, patients rested for 10 min and watched a 4-min video for distraction, to prevent inner rehearsal. During the retrieval period, patients were presented with 160 word pairs, including 40 new word pairs, 60 intact word pairs, and 60 rearranged word pairs. No word appeared twice, and patients were not exposed to the same experimental task more than once. Patients were asked to respond, by pressing one of three keyboard buttons, as accurately and quickly as possible depending on the presentation of the pairing options: same pairing (“intact”), altered word pairing (“rearranged”), or new pairing (“new”).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Timeline of the memory paradigm. The whole task consisted of three study periods: encoding, rest (distractor), and retrieval. Stimulation was delivered on-and-off at 50 Hz for 5 s during the encoding phase only and was randomly assigned to one of the two blocks in a single session. Lightning bolts denote periods of stimulation. In the encoding phase, the first word pair on the screen denotes “glass” (left) and “mirror” (right).

Data Acquisition and Analysis

We recorded iEEG data including depth and ECoG using a 64-channel digital video monitoring system (Telefactor Beehive Horizon with an AURA LTM 64- and 128-channel amplifier system; Natus Neurology, West Warwick, RI, USA) digitized at a sampling rate of 1,600 Hz and filtered from 0.1 to 60 Hz. These iEEG data were analyzed using MATLAB software (version 2015b, Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). We first performed manual artifact rejection of the signal for every electrode. Channels affected by artifacts were excluded from subsequent analyses. For example, individual stimulus response trials were precluded if there were any motion artifacts. Signals exhibiting stimulation artifacts and epileptic-form spikes were also excluded from further analyses. The recorded data were re-referenced to the common average reference (CAR). To quantify specific changes in theta rhythm during stimulation for the encoding period of the memory task, we applied time-frequency analysis and used Morlet wavelet transformation (wave number: 2.48) to obtain a continuous-time complex value representation of the signal. Transformed data were squared to calculate power and normalized by the mean and standard deviation of the pre-stimulus baseline power (i.e., resting periods prior to the task) of each frequency. Data were then epoched with a window of 0–4 s from the onset of paired-word trials and aligned with 50-Hz stimulation, beginning at the onset of the memory task. In our experiment, stimulation “on” and “off” blocks were conducted separately. To avoid direct stimulation artifacts, the OFF periods of trials in the stimulation “on” block were used for the stimulation trials (Figure 2). Stimulation trials thus included 30 stimulation trials from the total 60 word pairs across the two stimulation “on” blocks, while the non-stimulation trials included 60 word pairs across the two stimulation “off” blocks. We compared the averaged power of each condition across a frequency range of 3–7 Hz for correctly and incorrectly encoded memory items. For visualization, normalized data were averaged across all trials for correct and incorrect trials, according to each condition. We extracted t-scores to perform independent two-sample t-tests.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical tests were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences v12.0 K (SPSS) and MATLAB (Mathworks). Our primary measurement of memory performance was the percentage of correctly recognized trials in each block. Paired non-parametric rank-sum t-tests were used to compare behavioral performance between conditions. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. For activity in the lateral temporal cortex, independent two-sample t-statistics (***p < 0.01 or **p < 0.05) were used to compare the average power amplitudes of iEEG waveforms between correctly and incorrectly recognized trials during the stimulation “on” and “off” periods. Prior to significance testing, normality was assessed using the Lilliefors test (p > 0.01, for all datasets). For multiple comparisons among theta power levels, the Bonferroni correction procedure was employed.

Results

Enhancement of Memory Recollection

In the behavioral analysis, we quantified the effect of stimulation on memory performance in two ways: first, we determined the hit rate of associative memory; second, we examined whether stimulation affected the ability to remember a learning episode (recollection) or the capacity to judge items as familiar (familiarity). At the behavioral level, individual memory performance was measured during the encoding phase of the word pair memory task during “on” and “off” hippocampal stimulation. The proportion of intact word pairs correctly identified as intact and rearranged word pairs correctly identified as rearranged were regarded as correct responses. The average accuracy values for all patients suggested that stimulation induced overall improvements in pair recognition memory (Figure 3A, Wilcoxon rank sum test, p = 0.027). Consistent increases in total raw scores and word accuracy were observed during the “on” period. The stimulation order was randomized across the six patients and we could not find any behavioral tendency regarding stimulation order (Table 2).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Memory performance. (A) Lines connect each patient’s “on” and “off” data point (left); data from all six patients were averaged (right). Accuracy refers to the proportion of correctly recognized words during the stimulation “on” and stimulation “off” periods. (B) Lines connect each patient’s “on” and “off” data for the recollection index (left); data were averaged across all six patients (right). (C) Familiarity index (D). Difference scores of the stimulation effect of the two conditions. FAM indicates the familiarity index and REC indicates the recollection index. *p < 0.05. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean (SEM). n.s., not significant.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Word pair memory task behavioral results.

As word-to-word associations may reflect one of two independent types of retrieval (recollection of a specific experience or a sense of familiarity; Giovanello et al., 2006; Quamme et al., 2007), we used a similar procedure to measure hippocampal-dependent memory to obtain pure estimates of general memory and familiarity (Yonelinas and Jacoby, 1995; Cohn and Moscovitch, 2007; Hamani et al., 2008).

We calculated the recollection index, which reflects the hit rate of associative memory (i.e., intact pairs correctly identified as intact and rearranged pairs correctly identified as rearranged) minus the false alarm rate in associative memory (i.e., rearranged pairs identified as intact). The familiarity index was calculated by dividing the false alarm rate in associative memory (i.e., rearranged pair identified as intact) by 1 minus the recollection index. Comparisons between the recollection and familiarity indices revealed that only recollection was markedly and consistently increased in all patients during the stimulation “on” period (Figures 3B,C, Wilcoxon rank sum test, p = 0.027 and p = 0.068, respectively; Table 3). We observed significant differences in recollection but not familiarity indices; based on the stimulation, the difference index for the two conditions showed statistical significance (Wilcoxon rank sum test, p = 0.028, Figure 3D).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Recollection vs. familiarity index for stimulation “on” and “off”.

Stimulation Induces Theta Increases in the Lateral Middle Temporal Cortex During Correctly Remembered Encoding Trials

To assess whether neural population signals from the temporal neocortex regions change depend on the episodic memory changes, we performed time-frequency analyses for each stimulation condition. We tested whether the lateral middle temporal cortex displayed stimulation-induced theta activities; as indication of the role of theta activation in memory performance, we compared oscillatory power between correctly encoded trials and incorrectly encoded trials.

We found that the baseline corrected theta power (3–7 Hz) in the middle temporal cortex averaged across trials for each patient was greater during the “on” period than during the “off” period. Corrections for multiple comparisons revealed that the theta amplitude in the lateral temporal cortex significantly increased during the presentation of word stimuli (4 s) during the “on” period in all five patients for which data from lateral temporal recording electrodes were available (two-sample t-test, Bonferroni-corrected: p = 0.035; p = 0.002; p = 0.032; p = 0.008; p = 0.042, for patients 1–5, respectively, Figure 4A). Patient 6 did not have lateral temporal electrodes. We also compared incorrectly remembered encoding trials between “on” and “off” stimulation periods. There was no significant increase in theta power in the lateral temporal cortex during the stimulation “on” period (p = 0.040; p = 0.089; p = 0.075; p = 0.051; p = 0.067, for patients 1–5, respectively, Figure 4B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Individual differences in theta power in the middle temporal cortex over 4 s for correctly remembered words during the “on” and “off” periods. (B) Individual differences in theta power in the middle temporal cortex over 4 s for incorrectly remembered words during the “on” and “off” periods. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, corrected. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean (SEM).

Next, we confirmed that these theta power differences could only be detected in the temporal cortex. We extracted the theta power from all electrodes during the memory encoding phase of the “on” and “off” stimulation periods, and then tested for differences (Figure 5A). Topographical maps were constructed to visualize differences in theta power between the correct and incorrect responses for both the “on” and “off” conditions for each patient’s electrodes. The results show that prominent theta power differences are only found in the middle temporal cortex. The individual normalized time-frequency topographical maps for the stimulation “on” period showed an overall increase in theta power in the middle temporal cortex (two-sample t-test, Bonferroni-corrected: p < 0.05, Figure 5B, upper panel). Conversely, no significant increase in theta power was observed for the contrast map in the middle temporal cortex during the stimulation “off” period (two-sample t-test, Bonferroni-corrected: p > 0.05, Figure 5B; bottom panel).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Location of the target in the lateral temporal cortex in the sagittal plane following co-registration of preoperative high-resolution MRI and postoperative CT images (not illustrated), for patients 1–5, respectively. Spheres indicate the location of the recording site. Topographical maps of differences in theta power between the correct and incorrect responses during both the “on” and “off” condition. The yellow sphere denotes locations in which significant increases in theta power were observed in the “on” condition that were higher than those observed in the “off” condition. The yellow sphere indicates the region for the time-frequency map in panel B. Some electrode grids/strips were excluded because they were not visible. (B) Time-frequency map comparing correctly and incorrectly remembered pairs during the “on” and “off” period for five patients. On the x-axis, 0 s indicates the onset of the memory task. During the “on” condition, theta (3–7 Hz) power was significantly increased during the 4 s of word presentation (p < 0.05, corrected) for successfully recognized trials. In contrast, no significant increases in theta power were observed during the “off” condition (p > 0.05, corrected).

Discussion

In the present study, we examined the effect of hippocampal stimulation on a word pair memory task. Our findings indicate that such stimulation enhanced memory performance in individual patients, and that this increase in pair recognition was reflected in recollection estimates rather than familiarity estimates. Furthermore, our findings show that theta power increased in the middle temporal cortex during the stimulation condition (i.e., during encoding)—an effect that was not observed in the stimulation “off” condition, suggesting that direct stimulation involves the middle temporal cortex during successful memory encoding.

Although the present study utilized stimulation parameters (i.e., frequency and pulse characteristics) similar to those used in previous studies (Suthana et al., 2012; Jacobs et al., 2016; Hansen et al., 2018), there are some discrepancies between the present and earlier work; these can probably be attributed to minor differences in stimulation parameters (i.e., increased duration and intensity) as well as different memory paradigms. While our study tested associative word-pair memory, an earlier study tested single-word item memory (Jacobs et al., 2016).

There is thus the possibility that task-related differences in hippocampal activity have affected the behavioral outcome of the hippocampal stimulation. A robust body of evidence indicates that the hippocampus supports the encoding of associative or relational information (Davachi, 2006; Diana et al., 2007; Mayes et al., 2007; Battaglia et al., 2011; Olsen et al., 2012; Yonelinas, 2013). Moreover, selective hippocampal lesions severely impaired associative memory rather than item memory itself in humans (Turriziani et al., 2004) as well as in primates (Zola-Morgan et al., 1986; Pascalis and Bachevalier, 1999). Differences in excitability of neurons could also play a key role in determining the behavioral outcome of stimulation effects. Indeed, neuronal activity in the human hippocampus is significantly higher for associative paired-item memory than for single-item memory (Cameron et al., 2001).

As mentioned earlier, previous studies using non-invasive stimulation have described the role of the hippocampus in associative memory (Wang et al., 2014; Wang and Voss, 2015) and memory precision (Nilakantan et al., 2017), and demonstrated its selective influence on associative vs. item memory (Tambini et al., 2018). However, these studies did not provide confirmatory evidence of hippocampal-dependent functions, since non-invasive stimulation is limited to delivering stimulation specifically to the hippocampus itself. Our current findings, however, provide direct evidence for a causal role of the human hippocampus in associative memory. Further expanding the knowledge provided by prior studies, our results suggest that direct hippocampal stimulation can enhance hippocampal-dependent associative binding.

Stimulation intensity and duration may be important regarding whether memory will be disrupted or enhanced by the activity in the hippocampus. A previous study suggested that optimal stimulation is based upon the current level, rather than the frequency of stimulation (Hescham et al., 2013). Furthermore, the duration of stimulation may lead to changes in the total energy delivered to the tissues (Moro et al., 2002). Additionally, different stimulation parameters have been associated with both increases and decreases in downstream brain activity in previous electrophysiology and functional MRI (fMRI) studies (Logothetis et al., 2010). We speculate that the positive effect of stimulation on behavior in the present study may be due to both the memory task and the precision of the stimulation parameters. As we were unable to directly address the controversy presented in the literature, further studies are required to explore the precise effect of stimulation on neural activity and variations in behavioral outcomes.

Our behavioral results indicate that hippocampal stimulation enhances memory performance, particularly recollection ability that retains specificity, and that the level of detail depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus integrates elements of memory representations associated with the qualitative aspects of an event during encoding (Yonelinas, 2013), and it is known to be crucial for recollection, but not for familiarity (Davachi and Wagner, 2002; Giovanello et al., 2003; Addis et al., 2004). Previous neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies have indicated that the hippocampus is an essential region for remembering context-specific details (recollection; Eldridge et al., 2000; Moscovitch and McAndrews, 2002). One non-invasive stimulation study reported that hippocampus-targeted cortical stimulation enhances highly specific memory recollection (Nilakantan et al., 2017). Taken together, these findings demonstrate that direct stimulation of the hippocampus during encoding boosts memory recollection.

Furthermore, our results reveal that hippocampal-dependent memory is correlated with the theta activity of the lateral temporal cortex during memory encoding in the stimulation “on” condition. A previous functional MRI (fMRI) study investigating associative memory tasks indicated that the hippocampus supports strong recollection, with additional contributions from several neocortices (Wais, 2011). There is growing consensus that direct electrical stimulation regulates the physiology across a network connected to the stimulation site (McIntyre and Hahn, 2010; Kim et al., 2016; Ezzyat et al., 2017). Stimulation may thus be mediated via cortico-hippocampal communication (Acheson and Hagoort, 2013; Wang et al., 2014).

The present study focused on activity changes in the low-frequency band (i.e., theta rhythm), for several reasons. For example, low-frequency power may maximize excitability and enhance subsequent memory encoding in cortical regions in the presence of word pairs (Lakatos et al., 2008; Haque et al., 2015). Theta rhythms generate large and synchronous membrane-potential fluctuations in many neurons throughout brain-wide networks (He et al., 2008; Buzsáki et al., 2013). Hence, the rhythm may be propagated to distant brain regions via long-range communication (Gloveli et al., 2005). Accordingly, our data revealed a concomitant increase in theta power in the temporal cortex during correctly remembered trials in stimulation “on” conditions. In particular, the hippocampal stimulation enhanced recollection ability. Although recollection was not specifically tested as in the present study, a previous study pointed out orchestration in theta phase-synchrony between the MTL and a distributed neocortical memory network for vividly remembered experiences (Fuentemilla et al., 2014).

Despite their critical role in human cognition, distributed networks of oscillatory activity in memory have remained largely uncharacterized. Our findings reveal stimulation-mediated theta oscillatory changes in the human temporal neocortex, thereby broadening our understanding of stimulation-induced neural correlates in one of the distributed networks that may act together with the hippocampus.

The present study has some limitations that should be noted, including the limited range of targets and parameters explored. Furthermore, as we only investigated six patients, we cannot claim with any certainty that the statistical power was sufficient. Despite these limitations, however, we observed a consistent positive effect of stimulation in all six patients, using a verbal associative memory paradigm. Further studies involving larger samples of patients are required and more in-depth analyses considering variable stimulation parameters are warranted.

Author Contributions

SJ, JK, and CC contributed to the study design and wrote the article. SJ and CC performed the study. SJ and JK analyzed the data. JK and CC obtained funding.

Funding

This research was supported by grants (2018M3C1B8013690 and 2018R1A4A1025616) from the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation (NRF), funded by the Ministry of Science & ICT, South Korea.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the patients and their families for their participation and support. We thank Kisun Kim, Woorim Jeong, and Dahye Kim for assistance with the iEEG experiments, and Seung-Hyun Jin for valuable comments on the analysis.

References

Acheson, D. J., and Hagoort, P. (2013). Stimulating the brain’s language network: syntactic ambiguity resolution after TMS to the inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1664–1677. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Addante, R. J., Watrous, A. J., Yonelinas, A. P., Ekstrom, A. D., and Ranganath, C. (2011). Prestimulus theta activity predicts correct source memory retrieval. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 10702–10707. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014528108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Addis, D. R., Moscovitch, M., Crawley, A. P., and McAndrews, M. P. (2004). Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus 14, 752–762. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnew, W. F., and McCreery, D. B. (1990). Considerations for safety with chronically implanted nerve electrodes. Epilepsia 31, S27–S32. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1990.tb05845.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avants, B. B., Epstein, C. L., Grossman, M., and Gee, J. C. (2008). Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med. Image Anal. 12, 26–41. doi: 10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axmacher, N., Schmitz, D. P., Weinreich, I., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2008). Interaction of working memory and long-term memory in the medial temporal lobe. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2868–2878. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Backus, A. R., Schoffelen, J. M., Szebenyi, S., Hanslmayr, S., and Doeller, C. F. (2016). Hippocampal-prefrontal theta oscillations support memory integration. Curr. Biol. 26, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battaglia, F. P., Benchenane, K., Sirota, A., Pennartz, C. M., and Wiener, S. I. (2011). The hippocampus: hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 310–318. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, T. W., Hampson, R. E., Song, D., Goonawardena, A., Marmarelis, V. Z., and Deadwyler, S. A. (2011). A cortical neural prosthesis for restoring and enhancing memory. J. Neural Eng. 8:046017. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/4/046017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boccardi, M., Bocchetta, M., Morency, F. C., Collins, D. L., Nishikawa, M., Ganzola, R., et al. (2015). Training labels for hippocampal segmentation based on the EADC-ADNI harmonized hippocampal protocol. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 175–183. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boon, P., Vonck, K., De Herdt, V., Van Dycke, A., Goethals, M., Goossens, L., et al. (2007). Deep brain stimulation in patients with refractory temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 48, 1551–1560. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01005.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, J. F., Zaghloul, K. A., Jacobs, J., Williams, R. B., Sperling, M. R., Sharan, A. D., et al. (2013). Synchronous and asynchronous theta and gamma activity during episodic memory formation. J. Neurosci. 33, 292–304. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2057-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1996). The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cereb. Cortex 6, 81–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., Logothetis, N., and Singer, W. (2013). Scaling brain size, keeping timing: evolutionary preservation of brain rhythms. Neuron 80, 751–764. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, K. A., Yashar, S., Wilson, C. L., and Fried, I. (2001). Human hippocampal neurons predict how well word pairs will be remembered. Neuron 30, 289–298. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00280-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casasanto, D. J., Killgore, W. D., Maldjian, J. A., Glosser, G., Alsop, D. C., Cooke, A. M., et al. (2002). Neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful verbal memory encoding. Brain Lang. 80, 287–295. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V., and Martin, A. (1999). Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 913–919. doi: 10.1038/13217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, M., and Moscovitch, M. (2007). Dissociating measures of associative memory: evidence and theoretical implications. J. Mem. Lang. 57, 437–454. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleshill, S. G., Binnie, C. D., Morris, R. G., Alarcon, G., van Emde Boas, W., Velis, D. N., et al. (2004). Material-specific recognition memory deficits elicited by unilateral hippocampal electrical stimulation. J. Neurosci. 24, 1612–1616. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4352-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daitch, A. L., Sharma, M., Roland, J. L., Astafiev, S. V., Bundy, D. T., Gaona, C. M., et al. (2013). Frequency-specific mechanism links human brain networks for spatial attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 19585–19590. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307947110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davachi, L. (2006). Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davachi, L., and Wagner, A. D. (2002). Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from relational and item-based learning. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 982–990. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vanssay-Maigne, A., Noulhiane, M., Devauchelle, A. D., Rodrigo, S., Baudoin-Chial, S., Meder, J. F., et al. (2011). Modulation of encoding and retrieval by recollection and familiarity: mapping the medial temporal lobe networks. Neuroimage 58, 1131–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 379–386. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 41–50. doi: 10.1038/35036213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Engel, S. A. (2000). Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1149–1152. doi: 10.1038/80671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ezzyat, Y., Kragel, J. E., Burke, J. F., Levy, D. F., Lyalenko, A., Wanda, P., et al. (2017). Direct brain stimulation modulates encoding states and memory performance in humans. Curr. Biol. 27, 1251–1258. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fell, J., Staresina, B. P., Do Lam, A. T., Widman, G., Helmstaedter, C., Elger, C. E., et al. (2013). Memory modulation by weak synchronous deep brain stimulation: a pilot study. Brain Stimul. 6, 270–273. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher, P., and Tyler, L. (2002). Neural correlates of human memory. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 8–9. doi: 10.1038/nn0102-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentemilla, L., Barnes, G. R., Duzel, E., and Levine, B. (2014). Theta oscillations orchestrate medial temporal lobe and neocortex in remembering autobiographical memories. Neuroimage 85, 730–737. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovanello, K. S., Keane, M. M., and Verfaellie, M. (2006). The contribution of familiarity to associative memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia 44, 1859–1865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovanello, K. S., Verfaellie, M., and Keane, M. M. (2003). Disproportionate deficit in associative recognition relative to item recognition in global amnesia. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 186–194. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.3.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gloveli, T., Dugladze, T., Rotstein, H. G., Traub, R. D., Monyer, H., Heinemann, U., et al. (2005). Orthogonal arrangement of rhythm-generating microcircuits in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 13295–13300. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506259102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, B., Lesser, R. P., Rance, N. E., Hart, J. Jr., Webber, R., Uematsu, S., et al. (1990). Parameters for direct cortical electrical stimulation in the human: histopathologic confirmation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 371–377. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90082-u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goyal, A., Miller, J., Watrous, A. J., Lee, S. A., Coffey, T., Sperling, M. R., et al. (2018). Electrical stimulation in hippocampus and entorhinal cortex impairs spatial and temporal memory. J. Neurosci. 38, 4471–4481. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3049-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamani, C., McAndrews, M. P., Cohn, M., Oh, M., Zumsteg, D., Shapiro, C. M., et al. (2008). Memory enhancement induced by hypothalamic/fornix deep brain stimulation. Ann. Neurol. 63, 119–123. doi: 10.1002/ana.21295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampson, R. E., Song, D., Chan, R. H., Sweatt, A. J., Riley, M. R., Gerhardt, G. A., et al. (2012). A nonlinear model for hippocampal cognitive prosthesis: memory facilitation by hippocampal ensemble stimulation. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 20, 184–197. doi: 10.1109/tnsre.2012.2189163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, N., Chaieb, L., Derner, M., Hampel, K. G., Elger, C. E., Surges, R., et al. (2018). Memory encoding-related anterior hippocampal potentials are modulated by deep brain stimulation of the entorhinal area. Hippocampus 28, 12–17. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haque, R. U., Wittig, J. H. Jr., Damera, S. R., Inati, S. K., and Zaghloul, K. A. (2015). Cortical low-frequency power and progressive phase synchrony precede successful memory encoding. J. Neurosci. 35, 13577–13586. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0687-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, B. J., Snyder, A. Z., Zempel, J. M., Smyth, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2008). Electrophysiological correlates of the brain’s intrinsic large-scale functional architecture. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 16039–16044. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807010105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hescham, S., Lim, L. W., Jahanshahi, A., Blokland, A., and Temel, Y. (2013). Deep brain stimulation in dementia-related disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2666–2675. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, J., Miller, J., Lee, S. A., Coffey, T., Watrous, A. J., Sperling, M. R., et al. (2016). Direct electrical stimulation of the human entorhinal region and hippocampus impairs memory. Neuron 92, 983–990. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. S. (2005). Modern Korean Words version 2. 780p (National Institute of Korean Language).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kim, K., Ekstrom, A. D., and Tandon, N. (2016). A network approach for modulating memory processes via direct and indirect brain stimulation: toward a causal approach for the neural basis of memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 134, 162–177. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kucewicz, M. T., Berry, B. M., Miller, L. R., Khadjevand, F., Ezzyat, Y., Stein, J. M., et al. (2018). Evidence for verbal memory enhancement with electrical brain stimulation in the lateral temporal cortex. Brain 141, 971–978. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuncel, A. M., and Grill, W. M. (2004). Selection of stimulus parameters for deep brain stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 2431–2441. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacruz, M. E., Valentin, A., Seoane, J. J., Morris, R. G., Selway, R. P., and Alarcon, G. (2010). Single pulse electrical stimulation of the hippocampus is sufficient to impair human episodic memory. Neuroscience 170, 623–632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A. D., Ulbert, I., and Schroeder, C. E. (2008). Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320, 110–113. doi: 10.1126/science.1154735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lega, B., Burke, J., Jacobs, J., and Kahana, M. J. (2016). Slow-theta-to-gamma phase-amplitude coupling in human hippocampus supports the formation of new episodic memories. Cereb. Cortex 26, 268–278. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lega, B. C., Jacobs, J., and Kahana, M. (2012). Human hippocampal theta oscillations and the formation of episodic memories. Hippocampus 22, 748–761. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logothetis, N. K., Augath, M., Murayama, Y., Rauch, A., Sultan, F., Goense, J., et al. (2010). The effects of electrical microstimulation on cortical signal propagation. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1283–1291. doi: 10.1038/nn.2631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manns, J. R., Hopkins, R. O., Reed, J. M., Kitchener, E. G., and Squire, L. R. (2003). Recognition memory and the human hippocampus. Neuron 37, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01147-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., and Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntyre, C. C., and Hahn, P. J. (2010). Network perspectives on the mechanisms of deep brain stimulation. Neurobiol. Dis. 38, 329–337. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2009.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mickes, L., Johnson, E. M., and Wixted, J. T. (2010). Continuous recollection versus unitized familiarity in associative recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 36, 843–863. doi: 10.1037/a0019755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moro, E., Esselink, R. J. A., Xie, J., Hommel, M., Benabid, A. L., and Pollak, P. (2002). The impact on Parkinson’s disease of electrical parameter settings in STN stimulation. Neurology 59, 706–713. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.5.706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovitch, M., Cabeza, R., Winocur, G., and Nadel, L. (2016). Episodic memory and beyond: the hippocampus and neocortex in transformation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 105–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscovitch, D. A., and McAndrews, M. P. (2002). Material-specific deficits in “remembering” in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Neuropsychologia 40, 1335–1342. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00213-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilakantan, A. S., Bridge, D. J., Gagnon, E. P., VanHaerents, S. A., and Voss, J. L. (2017). Stimulation of the posterior cortical-hippocampal network enhances precision of memory recollection. Curr. Biol. 27, 465–470. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojemann, G. A., and Schoenfield-McNeill, J. (1998). Neurons in human temporal cortex active with verbal associative learning. Brain Lang. 64, 317–327. doi: 10.1006/brln.1998.1982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojemann, G. A., Schoenfield-McNeill, J., and Corina, D. P. (2002). Anatomic subdivisions in human temporal cortical neuronal activity related to recent verbal memory. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 64–71. doi: 10.1038/nn785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojemann, G. A., Schoenfield-McNeill, J., and Corina, D. (2009). The roles of human lateral temporal cortical neuronal activity in recent verbal memory encoding. Cereb. Cortex 19, 197–205. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsen, R. K., Moses, S. N., Riggs, L., and Ryan, J. D. (2012). The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and comparison. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:146. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascalis, O., and Bachevalier, J. (1999). Neonatal aspiration lesions of the hippocampal formation impair visual recognition memory when assessed by paired-comparison task but not by delayed nonmatching-to-sample task. Hippocampus 9, 609–616. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-1063(1999)9:6<609::aid-hipo1>3.3.co;2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A., Clark, J., Pare-Blagoev, E. J., Shohamy, D., Creso Moyano, J., Myers, C., et al. (2001). Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature 414, 546–550. doi: 10.1038/35107080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quamme, J. R., Yonelinas, A. P., and Norman, K. A. (2007). Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia. Hippocampus 17, 192–200. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhee, K. Y. (1991). Korean Category norms: survey on exemplar frequency norm, typicality, and features. Korean J. Exp. Cogn. Psychol. 3, 131–160.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ritchey, M., Libby, L. A., and Ranganath, C. (2015). Cortico-hippocampal systems involved in memory and cognition: the PMAT framework. Prog. Brain Res. 219, 45–64. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rugg, M. D., and Vilberg, K. L. (2013). Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutishauser, U., Ross, I. B., Mamelak, A. N., and Schuman, E. M. (2010). Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking of single neurons. Nature 464, 903–907. doi: 10.1038/nature08860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suthana, N., Haneef, Z., Stern, J., Mukamel, R., Behnke, E., Knowlton, B., et al. (2012). Memory enhancement and deep-brain stimulation of the entorhinal area. N. Engl. J. Med. 366, 502–510. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1107212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeney-Reed, C. M., Zaehle, T., Voges, J., Schmitt, F. C., Buentjen, L., Kopitzki, K., et al. (2016). Pre-stimulus thalamic theta power predicts human memory formation. Neuroimage 138, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tambini, A., Nee, D. E., and D’Esposito, M. (2018). Hippocampal-targeted theta-burst stimulation enhances associative memory formation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1452–1472. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turriziani, P., Fadda, L., Caltagirone, C., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2004). Recognition memory for single items and for associations in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 42, 426–433. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wais, P. E. (2011). Hippocampal signals for strong memory when associative memory is available and when it is not. Hippocampus 21, 9–21. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. X., Rogers, L. M., Gross, E. Z., Ryals, A. J., Dokucu, M. E., Brandstatt, K. L., et al. (2014). Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks and associative memory. Science 345, 1054–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.1252900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. X., and Voss, J. L. (2015). Long-lasting enhancements of memory and hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity following multiple-day targeted noninvasive stimulation. Hippocampus 25, 877–883. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watrous, A. J., Tandon, N., Conner, C. R., Pieters, T., and Ekstrom, A. D. (2013). Frequency-specific network connectivity increases underlie accurate spatiotemporal memory retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 349–356. doi: 10.1038/nn.3315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonelinas, A. P. (2013). The hippocampus supports high-resolution binding in the service of perception, working memory and long-term memory. Behav. Brain Res. 254, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonelinas, A. P., and Jacoby, L. L. (1995). The relation between remembering and knowing as bases for recognition: effects of size congruency. J. Mem. Lang. 34, 622–643. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1995.1028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., and Amaral, D. G. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 6, 2950–2967. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.06-10-02950.1986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain stimulation, memory enhancement, recollection, hippocampus, theta power, lateral temporal cortex

Citation: Jun S, Kim JS and Chung CK (2019) Direct Stimulation of Human Hippocampus During Verbal Associative Encoding Enhances Subsequent Memory Recollection. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:23. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00023

Received: 07 June 2018; Accepted: 18 January 2019;
Published: 05 February 2019.

Edited by:

Thomas Wolbers, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Germany

Reviewed by:

Jesse Rissman, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
Joel Voss, Northwestern University, United States

Copyright © 2019 Jun, Kim and Chung. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: June Sic Kim, anNraW1AaGJmLnJlLmty
Chun Kee Chung, Y2h1bmdjQHNudS5hYy5rcg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.