- Charitable Medical Healthcare Foundation, Augusta, GA, United States
Aside from the nature of consciousness itself, there are still many unsolved problems in the neurosciences. Despite the vast and quickly growing body of work in this field, we still find ourselves perplexed at seemingly simple qualities of our mental being such as why we need to sleep. The neurosciences are at least beginning to take a hold on these mysteries and are working toward solving them. We hold a perspective that metastable consciousness models, specifically the Default Space Model (DSM), provide insights into these mysteries. In this perspective article, we explore some of these curious questions in order to elucidate the interesting points they bring up. The DSM is a dynamic, global theory of consciousness that involves the maintenance of an internal, 3D simulation of the external, physical world which is the foundation and structure of consciousness. This space is created and filled by multiple frequencies of membrane potential oscillations throughout the brain and body which are organized, synchronized and harmonized by the thalamus. The veracity of the DSM is highlighted here in its ability to further understanding of some of the most puzzling problems in neuroscience.
Introduction
The most complex object in the known universe is essentially ourselves, our own brains. However, this three-pound mass of buttery consistency is an enigma in that it is “us,” and yet it may seem to be an utterly alien system. Due to its unimaginable complexity, understanding the brain is a very challenging task and there are still many hurdles to overcome before we truly understand our own nature. In order to gain this understanding, we need to ask the right questions. As neuroscientific academia grows, it seems the answers provided for seemingly simple questions become less satisfactory. In this perspective article, we present the perspective that a metastable theory of consciousness which includes physiology of the brain and body is necessary to clarify these questions.
In the neuroscience of consciousness, theories of metastability propose that consciousness arises from the global integration of several functional modules via neural oscillations acting in a cooperative and coordinated manner. In this perspective article, functional modules couple together while still expressing their own innate, independent behavior (Kelso and Tognoli, 2007). Brain-centric metastable models of consciousness such as the Dynamic Core Hypothesis (DCH), Global Workspace Theory (GWT), and the Operational Architectonics Theory of brain-mind (OAT) offer great insight into mysteries of neuroscience, However, these theoretically fall short of the explanatory power provided by a metastable model that includes the brain and body in its theory which is provided by the Default Space Model (DSM; Jerath and Crawford, 2014).
The DCH and GWT both share the DSM’s concept of the thalamocortical system as the functional hub of global module integration (Tononi and Edelman, 1998; Baars et al., 2013; Jerath et al., 2015a), however, the DSM provides a dynamic architectural structure allowing orderly, functional integration which is the conscious, internal, 3D simulation of the external physical environment into a 3D matrix termed the default space. OAT shares this insight into the most fundamental quality of this “virtual reality” of subjective experience, its 3D coordinate system occurring spatio-temporally in tandem with the physical world, correlating this inner reality with temporal and spatial organizations of brain activity (Fingelkurts et al., 2010). This structural foundation of a 3D matrix as the basis of conscious phenomena is illustrated by the neurological condition of contralateral neglect syndrome (Figure 1; Fingelkurts et al., 2010; Jerath and Crawford, 2014). OAT also shares with DSM the engagement of a metaphor of a global, harmonious, musical symphony to describe the electrical oscillatory synchronization of distinct, autonomous neuronal assemblies in the creation of consciousness via large-scale, cooperative, and integrative functions (Fingelkurts et al., 2010; Jerath et al., 2017a). The DSM model however posits that every cell of the body and not just neuronal cells are involved in this oscillatory ensemble which illustrates the holistic nature of consciousness (Jerath et al., 2017a). Although OAT asserts there is a center to this space, DSM takes this view further by describing how the perspective of where the consciousness self is centered in the 3D inner space is based on the spatial location of the thalamus in the brain as all perceptual information is coordinated around it (Jerath et al., 2015a).
Figure 1. The 3D matrix of consciousness and contralateral neglect syndrome. Contralateral neglect syndrome, resulting from damage to the right parietal lobe, illustrates the independent presence of a subconscious 3D space which is the structural foundation of experience. In the healthy individual who has an intact spatial framework of consciousness, the right parietal lobe maps spatial information on sensory data from both sides of the body, while the left parietal lobe is able to map information from the right side of the body. In this healthy person, sensation from every spatial perspective can be perceived, such as the full image of a house. In patients with contralateral neglect (also termed hemispatial neglect) suffering right parietal lobe damage, information from both sides of the body is fed to this lobe but cannot be spatially mapped to the lobe damage. The intact left parietal lobe can still map information from the right side of the body, but sensation from the left side has no cortex to spatially orient it. These patients not only lack consciousness of such sensory stimuli, but the entire left side of space ceases to consciously exist, illustrating the fundamental necessity of the spatial 3D matrix in the occurrence of consciousness. These patients are not aware of any “missing space,” and experience the remaining right side as the full physical world. The actions of these patients are also restricted to the remaining side, eating food from only one side of their plate and only dressing one side of their body. Research shows that stimuli from the “missing” side is fully processed, but however remains unconscious (Figure by Lynsey Ekema, MSMI. © 2014 Dr. Ravinder Jerath. All rights reserved).
DSM and OAT theories propound that functionally integrated, microscopic, mesoscopic and macroscopic bioelectric fields constitute the spatially organized, 3D matrix of consciousness (Fingelkurts et al., 2010; Jerath et al., 2015a). OAT however fails to sufficiently identify biological mechanisms for the distal, macroscopic coordination of global reentrant and oscillatory activity of which DCH, GWT and DSM show with substantial physiological support that the thalamocortical system is responsible for Edelman et al. (2011); Baars et al. (2013); Jerath et al. (2015a). Both DSM and OAT correlate the highly stable oscillatory activity of the default mode network with the conscious sense of self centered in the externalized, perceptual reality of a 3D space (Fingelkurts et al., 2010). The DSM advances these concepts by binding bioelectric operations of the brain such as the default mode network with bioelectric operations of the body such as respiration and cardio activities (Jerath and Crawford, 2015b). The DSM consists of the strongest points of metastable theories of consciousness along with the bioelectric connection of the body and brain which provide the greatest insight into the biggest mysteries of neuroscience.
Insight Into the Top Mysteries of the Mind
Consciousness is commonly thought to arise from computational processes solely within the brain (Edelman, 2008). Previously, we have reviewed evidential support from studies on respiration physiology and electrophysiology to show that consciousness is a body-wide process and not limited to individual cortical sites (Jerath et al., 2017a) which suggest that it is actually an interactive process between the brain and body. The brain-body theory of the DSM provides insight into current mysteries of the mind and has been supported by physiological research. Through providing insight into unresolved quandaries of neuroscience, we show the veracity of this model and other metastable models and the power they have to improve and understand ourselves.
What Is the Relationship Between Subjective Experience and the Physical World?
Through optical illusions and other illusions such as the rubber-hand illusion, it is evident that we do not see reality as it truly is, however, we perceive a subjective version of it. This nature is illuminated by the founding notion of the DSM and other “world simulation” models, that the external physical world is subjectively simulated by an internally generated experiential reality (Hesslow, 2002; Revonsuo, 2006; Trehub, 2007). According to the DSM, this internal reality biologically consists of macroscopic, synchronized bioelectric oscillations throughout the brain and body (Jerath et al., 2015a). This bioelectric architecture provides the conscious phenomenon of a 3D matrix framework for the qualities of the external world to be defaulted into (Jerath et al., 2015a). A common human experience is that we directly experience the external world through our senses, however, the DSM explains along with evidence from illusions, neurology and psychology that what we experience is actually heavily influenced by our executive expectations, past experiences, and biases instead of a direct experience of external stimuli (Driver and Vuilleumier, 2001; Smythies, 2003; Grush, 2004; Revonsuo, 2006; Maldonato and Dell’Orco, 2012).
This internal simulation of the external world is therefore a best guess of reality which highlights the aspects that are most important to our tasks at hand which improves survival chances by reducing processing requirements (Maldonato and Dell’Orco, 2012). Both the DSM and OAT suppose that global, multisensory integration and unification into a singular conscious experience is functionally, spatially and temporally reflected in the dynamic structure of the bioelectric fields of the brain (Fingelkurts et al., 2010; and body in the DSM; Jerath et al., 2016). Further research into this mystery should investigate identifying an isomorphism in the space-time of subjective experience with the physical, bioelectric space-time of the brain.
How Do We So Quickly Process and Interpret the External World?
Of the massive amount of sensory information bombarding our senses at any time, we are only aware of a small percentage, with estimates at 99% of externally derived sensory data being discarded upon entering the brain (Gazzaniga, 1998). The massive amount of computation required to process all of the information inundating our senses at any given time is unnecessary and beyond what the brain is capable of (Kveraga et al., 2007). Computational and theoretical work has shown the top-down predictions and expectations of the mind are integrated with external sensory input to minimize processing demands of perception (Engel et al., 2001). The DSM explains how attention and expectations derived from memory and other cognitive functions synchronize directly with the sensory organs in order to quickly influence what is integrated into the internal replication of the external world (Jerath et al., 2015a).
The bioelectric framework of the default space replicates the external space continuously in time. This space and the “virtual” objects contained within it therefore prime the filling of external sensory stimuli into this space based on expectations derived from the state of the simulation in the previous moment in time (Jerath et al., 2015a). These internal predictions directly influence the sensory organs via oscillatory synchronization with the cortex (Jerath et al., 2016). Through this synchronization, lateral inhibition at a distance influences incoming sensory information so that expected stimuli essentially “fall” into place upon the sensory receptors at which they are predicted through filtering and amplifying, thereby drastically reducing processing required and allowing for increased sensory acuity (Jerath et al., 2017b). Commonly, lateral inhibition indicates cells inhibiting physically adjacent cells. However, in our denotation, cortical inhibitions of sensory organ cells are “lateral” in that they are “adjacent” through synchronization via membrane potential oscillations (Jerath et al., 2016). By bringing the processing power of the cortex to the sensory organs through the synchronization of the current “world simulation” with the sensory organs, the next “frame” of the simulation can be almost immediately updated as the sensory organs are part of the body-wide, bioelectric framework of consciousness (Jerath et al., 2016). Further research may utilize EEG monitoring on the sensory organs and cortex to identify top-down regulation of the sensory organs.
How Do All of Our Sensations Unify Into One Experience Seamlessly?
The “binding” problem is a mystery of the neurosciences of how a unified conscious experience arises from the distributed activities of the central nervous system (Revonsuo and Newman, 1999). For instance with vision, how is information from thousands of cones in the retina receiving different parts of a visual stimulus integrated into a neural representation of a single object with different characteristics such as form, color, motion, brightness, and depth? Metastable theories such as OAT and the DSM explain how oscillations between distributed modules synchronize together to create the infrastructure of an internal 3D space that functionally binds objects to it and binds sensory information to those objects (Fingelkurts et al., 2010; Jerath et al., 2016). The DSM furthers this notion by asserting the synchrony not only between cortical modules, but also between those cortical modules and the sensory organs (Jerath et al., 2016).
Research has been conducted on exploring the “binding” problem through dynamic oscillations and synchrony which support metastable models (Kelso, 2002; Fingelkurts et al., 2003). There are several neural theories that support the functional importance of top-down mechanisms in synchronistic binding (Engel et al., 2001). The temporal binding model assumes that neural synchrony with precision in the millisecond range is crucial for object representation, and that this synchrony enhances the neural activity important to the task at hand while discarding less valuable activity (Roelfsema et al., 1996). This model predicts that neurons that respond to the same sensory object likely fire in synchrony within the millisecond range and that this synchronization should not exist between neurons that are stimulated by different objects in sensory space (Singer and Gray, 1995). While this supports metastable concepts, the notion of binding is furthered by the DSM not only through the assertion that the sensory receptors are bound to the virtual representations in the 3D default space, but that a harmonious synchrony of oscillations throughout the body and brain provide a united bioelectric structure that creates the internal 3D matrix to which individual modules may bind their perceptual processes to.
Further research may address how global coherence is created while allowing individual cortical models to have their own processing activities and synchronize with other associated modules. This indicates that if total unity/synchronization or total segregation exists among all neural assemblies of the global bioelectric field then a loss of consciousness should occur (Fingelkurts et al., 2010). The main insight metastable models provide to this mystery is that bioelectric synchrony among the processing of distributed assemblies (and sensory organs in the DSM) represent the binding mechanism, and such global binding dynamics leads to the integrated, unified, conscious experience. Patients with conditions that disrupt the abundant, diverse, and complex synchrony among many cortical modules such as schizophrenia (Bressler, 2003; Borisov et al., 2005) should show a disruption in perceptual binding, and this is indeed the case (Stephens and Graham, 2000; Cuesta and Peralta, 2001). Further research should examine other patients with disrupted synchronization to investigate the broadness of this mechanism in perceptual binding.
Why Do We Sleep?
The function of sleep is still one of the most perplexing mysteries in biology (Frank, 2006). Multiple functions have been proposed such as restoration via the removal of metabolic waste products (Xie et al., 2013), memory consolidation (Walker and Stickgold, 2006), and even energy conservation during dark nights when food cannot be found and safety is an issue (Zepelin et al., 2005). According to the DSM, cardiorespiratory synchronization (Bianchi et al., 2006) and corresponding changes in the autonomic nervous system during slow-wave sleep toward the parasympathetic state (Frostig et al., 1990; Cabiddu et al., 2012) promote the restorative hyperpolarization of neurons throughout the brain (Jerath et al., 2014).
Parasympathetic activation can synchronize multiple physiologic processes, regulating the homeostasis of sleep states (Lurie, 2011; Nasi et al., 2011). Research support for the DSM’s hypothesis on sleep is needed, however, plausibility that increased parasympathetic activity during slow-wave sleep via cardiorespiratory synchronization leads to hyperpolarization of cellular membrane potentials throughout the body is drawn from studies on various cellular membrane potentials during prolonged parasympathetic activation (Sato et al., 2006; Hanson et al., 2012). Additionally, studies have demonstrated hyperpolarization of neural membrane potentials during slow-wave sleep (Hirsch et al., 1983; Steriade et al., 2001) and that sleep-deprived brains show increased membrane excitability (Winters et al., 2011; Yan et al., 2011). The restorative function of membrane hyperpolarization via oscillations may work by combating overly excitable membranes that may disturb action potential precision and stimulus discrimination (Schaefer et al., 2006). Further research should investigate the nature of global membrane hyperpolarization during sleep and how this may provide a restorative function.
How and By What Mechanisms Can Emotions Be Regulated?
Negative emotions and psychological stress have been shown to play significant roles in the development of health problems such as hypertension and coronary heart disease (Treiber et al., 2001; Snieder et al., 2002; Todaro et al., 2003). By what methods can we learn to accept our negative emotions and move toward a more positive mental atmosphere? Under stress, a sympathetic response occurs and cardiorespiratory synchronization decreases (Zhang et al., 2010). The parasympathetic state has been identified as a predictor for the success of instructed mood repair (Yaroslavsky et al., 2016). In modern society, our autonomic nervous systems, evolutionary developed for a much different environment, are often chronically stuck in the stress-induced “fight or flight” response of the sympathetic nervous system (Nesse et al., 2016). Although our ancestors lives were likely more stressful overall, the sympathetic nervous system was activated only when physical danger was present, whereas today, our tendencies to commit ourselves to goals and responsibilities we cannot achieve in part leads the sympathetic response to be activated for greatly extended periods (Nesse et al., 2016).
Through mind-body interventions such as mindfulness and meditative breathing exercises in which a synchrony between cardiac and respiratory rates is created, a parasympathetic response (Cysarz and Büssing, 2005; Tang et al., 2009; Chang and Lo, 2013), as well as inhibition of amygdala activity (Creswell et al., 2007) which is stimulated during distress (Davis, 1992), can occur. Through practice of such breathing techniques, one may be able to reduce anxiety, distress, neuroticism, and emotional disorders, and increase positive well-being (Jerath et al., 2006, 2015b; Rosenkranz et al., 2013; Goyal et al., 2014). The physiologically supported perspective of the DSM on the autonomic nervous system and its relation to distress can be illustrated graphically (Figure 2; Jerath et al., 2015a).
Figure 2. Representation of mind-body techniques on the autonomic nervous system. This figure correlates a spectrum of emotional states with a spectrum of physiological states. The state of distress is correlated with the sympathetic state of the autonomic nervous system. The large outer oval represents the homeostatic state of the body, while the size the large inner oval represents a chaotic-calm spectrum of bodily afferent signaling. The body-brain connection is stronger in the parasympathetic state illustrating increased synchronization and coherence. The size of the brain represents overall cognitive ability and the level of control it exerts on overall homeostasis. Other indications in this image further illustrate the benefits of parasympathic dominace which can be cultured through meditative techniques (Figure by Michael Jensen MSMI, CMI).
Through intentional modification of one’s afferent signaling, the nervous system can be regulated in a way that leads to regulation of emotion (Jerath and Crawford, 2015a). This assertion is made from research indicating that internal feedback from physiological changes influence emotional states (Critchley, 2005). Although breathing may be the most powerful, or at least most studied mechanisms of affective change, other forms of affective signaling modification have shown to be effective in emotional regulation. For instance, forced facial expressions can result in the emotion indicated by the facial expression. Forced smiling and laughter has been shown to significantly improve mood in participants (Neuhoff and Schaefer, 2002). Other non-respiratory mediation techniques have been shown to induce positive affective change. For instance, Transcendental Meditation, which does not involve breath control, has been indicated to reduce sympathetic tone and stress reactivity (Barnes and Orme-Johnson, 2012).
Depending on the breathing technique, the autonomic nervous system can be voluntarily influenced in different ways. Cyclic hyperventilation followed by breath retention has been demonstrated to result in activation of the sympathetic nervous system (Kox et al., 2014). Sympathetic, “flight or fight,” arousal states are associated with rapid breathing (Nyklíček et al., 1997). Such respiratory rhythms are generated in regions of the brainstem (Feldman and Del Negro, 2006) which illustrate the connection between respiration and emotion as these regions have reentrant connections with the limbic system (Evans, 2010). Cardiovascular afferent signaling has also been shown to evoke autonomic responses (Gray et al., 2012). This research has formed a basis for the proposition that the phase synchronization of the heartbeat with respiration (cardiorespiratory coherence) can have a greatly expanded effect on the autonomic nervous system (Jerath and Crawford, 2015a). Through the practice of meditative and deep breathing techniques, one can modulate visceral afferent signaling which in turn can modulate the autonomic nervous system, brainstem, and cortical areas of the brain leading to improvements in anxiety, chronic stress, and mood disorders (Jerath and Crawford, 2015a). Although a great deal of research has been conducted on stress and meditation, further research into meditation may yield greater insights by focusing on mind-body synchronization and the effects of generating parasympathetic dominance.
Conclusion
The neurosciences have made tremendous progress in deciphering the activities of almost 100 billion neurons in a single human brain, however there are many more mysteries that await elucidation. In light of metastable models of consciousness such as the DSM, we have provided insight into some of the more tantalizing questions that mystify neuroscientists. Although some of these questions may seem obvious, current scientific understanding of their essence is limited. The insights we have provided illustrate the veracity of the DSM and the power of metastable models in illuminating the shadows of neuroscience.
Author Contributions
RJ has developed the theory with some contribution to the writing of the manuscript. CB has made the major contribution in the writing of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was funded by Charitable Medical Healthcare Foundation of Augusta, GA, USA.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Dr. Vernon Barnes for editing the manuscript.
References
Baars, B. J., Franklin, S., and Ramsoy, T. Z. (2013). Global workspace dynamics: cortical “binding and propagation” enables conscious contents. Front. Psychol. 4:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00200
Barnes, V. A., and Orme-Johnson, D. W. (2012). Prevention and treatment of cardiovascular disease in adolescents and adults through the transcendental meditation®program: a research review update. Curr. Hypertens. Rev. 8, 227–242. doi: 10.2174/157340212803530411
Bianchi, A. M., Ferini-Strambi, L., Castronovo, V., and Cerutti, S. (2006). Multivariate and multiorgan analysis of cardiorespiratory variability signals: the CAP sleep case. Biomed. Tech. 51, 167–173. doi: 10.1515/bmt.2006.029
Borisov, S. V., Kaplan, A. Y., Gorbachevskaya, N. L., and Kozlova, I. A. (2005). Analysis of EEG structural synchrony in adolescents with schizophrenic disorders. Fiziol. Cheloveka 31, 16–23. doi: 10.1007/s10747-005-0042-z
Bressler, S. L. (2003). Cortical coordination dynamics and the disorganization syndrome in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 35–39. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300145
Cabiddu, R., Cerutti, S., Viardot, G., Werner, S., and Bianchi, A. M. (2012). Modulation of the sympatho-vagal balance during sleep: frequency domain study of heart rate variability and respiration. Front. Physiol. 3:45. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00045
Chang, C. H., and Lo, P. C. (2013). Effects of long-term dharma-chan meditation on cardiorespiratory synchronization and heart rate variability behavior. Rejuvenation Res. 16, 115–123. doi: 10.1089/rej.2012.1363
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., and Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosom. Med. 69, 560–565. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e3180f6171f
Critchley, H. D. (2005). Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective and cognitive integration. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 154–166. doi: 10.1002/cne.20749
Cuesta, M. J., and Peralta, V. (2001). Integrating psychopathological dimensions in functional psychoses: a hierarchical approach. Schizophr. Res. 52, 215–229. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00190-0
Cysarz, D., and Büssing, A. (2005). Cardiorespiratory synchronization during Zen meditation. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 95, 88–95. doi: 10.1007/s00421-005-1379-3
Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 353–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002033
Driver, J., and Vuilleumier, P. (2001). Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction. Cognition 79, 39–88. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00124-4
Edelman, S. (2008). On the nature of minds, or: truth and consequences. J. Exp. Theor. Artif. Intell. 20, 181–196. doi: 10.1080/09528130802319086
Edelman, G. M., Gally, J. A., and Baars, B. J. (2011). Biology of consciousness. Front. Psychol. 2:4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00004
Engel, A. K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565
Evans, K. C. (2010). Cortico-limbic circuitry and the airways: insights from functional neuroimaging of respiratory afferents and efferents. Biol. Psychol. 84, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.02.005
Feldman, J. L., and Del Negro, C. A. (2006). Looking for inspiration: new perspectives on respiratory rhythm. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 232–242. doi: 10.1038/nrn1871
Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., Krause, C. M., Möttönen, R., and Sams, M. (2003). Cortical operational synchrony during audio-visual speech integration. Brain Lang. 85, 297–312. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00059-2
Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., and Neves, C. F. H. (2010). Natural world physical, brain operational, and mind phenomenal space-time. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 195–249. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.04.001
Frank, M. G. (2006). The mystery of sleep function: current perspectives and future directions. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 375–392. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2006.17.4.375
Frostig, R. D., Frysinger, R. C., and Harper, R. M. (1990). Recurring discharge patterns in multiple spike trains. II. Application in forebrain areas related to cardiac and respiratory control during different sleep-waking states. Biol. Cybern. 62, 495–502. doi: 10.1007/bf00205111
Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M. S., Gould, N. F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., et al. (2014). Meditation Programs for Psychological Stress and Well-Being. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.
Gray, M. A., Beacher, F. D., Minati, L., Nagai, Y., Kemp, A. H., Harrison, N. A., et al. (2012). Emotional appraisal is influenced by cardiac afferent information. Emotion 12, 180–191. doi: 10.1037/a0025083
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 377–442; discussion 396–442. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x04000093
Hanson, B., Gill, J., Western, D., Gilbey, M. P., Bostock, J., Boyett, M. R., et al. (2012). Cyclical modulation of human ventricular repolarization by respiration. Front. Physiol. 3:379. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00379
Hesslow, G. (2002). Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 242–247. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01913-7
Hirsch, J. C., Fourment, A., and Marc, M. E. (1983). Sleep-related variations of membrane potential in the lateral geniculate body relay neurons of the cat. Brain Res. 259, 308–312. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)91264-7
Jerath, R., and Crawford, M. W. (2014). Neural correlates of visuospatial consciousness in 3D default space: insights from contralateral neglect syndrome. Conscious. Cogn. 28, 81–93. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.06.008
Jerath, R., and Crawford, M. W. (2015a). How does the body affect the mind? role of cardiorespiratory coherence in the spectrum of emotions. Adv. Mind Body Med. 29, 4–16.
Jerath, R., and Crawford, M. W. (2015b). Layers of human brain activity: a functional model based on the default mode network and slow oscillations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:248. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00248
Jerath, R., Crawford, M. W., and Barnes, V. A. (2015a). A unified 3D default space consciousness model combining neurological and physiological processes that underlie conscious experience. Front. Psychol. 6:1204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01204
Jerath, R., Crawford, M. W., Barnes, V. A., and Harden, K. (2015b). Self-regulation of breathing as a primary treatment for anxiety. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 40, 107–115. doi: 10.1007/s10484-015-9279-8
Jerath, R., Cearley, S. M., Barnes, V. A., and Junca, S. (2017a). The dynamic role of breathing and cellular membrane potentials in the experience of consciousness. World J. Neurosci. 7, 66–81. doi: 10.4236/wjns.2017.71007
Jerath, R., Cearley, S. M., Paladiya, R., and Barnes, V. A. (2017b). Sensory consciousness is experienced through amplification of sensory stimuli via lateral inhibition. World J. Neurosci. 7, 244–256. doi: 10.4236/wjns.2017.73020
Jerath, R., Cearley, S. M., Barnes, V. A., and Nixon-Shapiro, E. (2016). How lateral inhibition and fast retinogeniculo-cortical oscillations create vision: a new hypothesis. Med. Hypotheses 96, 20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2016.09.015
Jerath, R., Edry, J. W., Barnes, V. A., and Jerath, V. (2006). Physiology of long pranayamic breathing: neural respiratory elements may provide a mechanism that explains how slow deep breathing shifts the autonomic nervous system. Med. Hypotheses 67, 566–571. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.02.042
Jerath, R., Harden, K., Crawford, M., Barnes, V. A., and Jensen, M. (2014). Role of cardiorespiratory synchronization and sleep physiology: effects on membrane potential in the restorative functions of sleep. Sleep Med. 15, 279–288. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.10.017
Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). The complementary nature of coordination dynamics: self-organization and the origins of agency. J. Nonlinear Phenomena Complex Syst. 5, 364–371.
Kelso, J. A. S., and Tognoli, E. (2007). “Toward a complementary neuroscience: Metastable coordination dynamics of the brain,” in Neurodynamics of Cognition and Consciousness. Understanding Complex Systems, eds L. I. Perlovsky and R. Kozma (Heidelberg, Berlin: Springer), 39–59.
Kox, M., van Eijk, L. T., Zwaag, J., van den Wildenberg, J., Sweep, F. C. G. J., van der Hoeven, J. G., et al. (2014). Voluntary activation of the sympathetic nervous system and attenuation of the innate immune response in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, 7379–7384. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322174111
Kveraga, K., Ghuman, A. S., and Bar, M. (2007). Top-down predictions in the cognitive brain. Brain Cogn. 65, 145–168. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.06.007
Lurie, A. (2011). Hemodynamic and autonomic changes in adults with obstructive sleep apnea. Adv. Cardiol. 46, 171–195. doi: 10.1159/000325109
Maldonato, M., and Dell’Orco, S. (2012). The predictive brain. World Futures 68, 381–389. doi: 10.1080/02604027.2012.693846
Nasi, T., Virtanen, J., Noponen, T., Toppila, J., Salmi, T., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2011). Spontaneous hemodynamic oscillations during human sleep and sleep stage transitions characterized with near-infrared spectroscopy. PLoS One 6:e25415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025415
Nesse, R. M., Bhatnagar, S., and Ellis, B. J. (2016). “Evolutionary origins and functions of the stress response system,” in Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior, ed. G. Fink (Amsterdam: Academic Press), 95–100.
Neuhoff, C. C., and Schaefer, C. (2002). Effects of laughing, smiling, and howling on mood. Psychol. Rep. 91, 1079–1080. doi: 10.2466/pr0.91.7.1079-1080
Nyklíček, I., Thayer, J. F., and Van Doornen, L. J. P. (1997). Cardiorespiratory differentiation of musically-induced emotions. J. Psychophysiol. 11, 304–321.
Revonsuo, A. (2006). Inner Presence: Consciousness as a Biological Phenomenon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Revonsuo, A., and Newman, J. (1999). Binding and consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 8, 123–127. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1999.0393
Roelfsema, P. R., Engel, A. K., König, P., and Singer, W. (1996). The role of neuronal synchronization in response selection: a biologically plausible theory of structured representation in the visual cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 603–625. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.603
Rosenkranz, M. A., Davidson, R. J., Maccoon, D. G., Sheridan, J. F., Kalin, N. H., and Lutz, A. (2013). A comparison of mindfulness-based stress reduction and an active control in modulation of neurogenic inflammation. Brain Behav. Immun. 27, 174–184. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.013
Sato, T., Nishishita, K., Kato, Y., Okada, Y., and Toda, K. (2006). Tonic activity of parasympathetic efferent nerve fibers hyperpolarizes the resting membrane potential of frog taste cells. Chem. Senses 31, 307–313. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj034
Schaefer, A. T., Angelo, K., Spors, H., and Margrie, T. W. (2006). Neuronal oscillations enhance stimulus discrimination by ensuring action potential precision. PLoS Biol. 4:e163. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040163
Singer, W., and Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 555–586. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.003011
Snieder, H., Harshfield, G. A., Barbeau, P., Pollock, D. M., Pollock, J. S., and Treiber, F. A. (2002). Dissecting the genetic architecture of the cardiovascular and renal stress response. Biol. Psychol. 61, 73–95. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00053-4
Stephens, L., and Graham, G. (2000). When Self-Consciousness Breaks: Alien Voices and Inserted Thoughts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Steriade, M., Timofeev, I., and Grenier, F. (2001). Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1969–1985. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969
Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Fan, Y., Feng, H., Wang, J., Feng, S., et al. (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 8865–8870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904031106
Todaro, J. F., Shen, B.-J., Niaura, R., Spiro, A. III., and Ward, K. D. (2003). Effect of negative emotions on frequency of coronary heart disease (The Normative Aging Study). Am. J. Cardiol. 92, 901–906. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9149(03)00967-6
Tononi, G., and Edelman, D. B. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science 282, 1846–1851. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5395.1846
Trehub, A. (2007). Space, self, and the theater of consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 310–330. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.004
Treiber, F. A., Davis, H., and Turner, J. R. (2001). “Cardiovascular responsivity to stress and preclinical manifestations of cardiovascular disease in youth,” in Health and Behavior in Childhood and Adolescence: Cross-Disciplinary Perspectives, eds L. Hayman, M. McMahon and J. R. Turner (New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.).
Walker, M. P., and Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 139–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070307
Winters, B. D., Huang, Y. H., Dong, Y., and Krueger, J. M. (2011). Sleep loss alters synaptic and intrinsic neuronal properties in mouse prefrontal cortex. Brain Res. 1420, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.078
Xie, L., Kang, H., Xu, Q., Chen, M. J., Liao, Y., Thiyagarajan, M., et al. (2013). Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science 342, 373–377. doi: 10.1126/science.1241224
Yan, J., Li, J. C., Xie, M. L., Zhang, D., Qi, A. P., Hu, B., et al. (2011). Short-term sleep deprivation increases intrinsic excitability of prefrontal cortical neurons. Brain Res. 1401, 52–58. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.032
Yaroslavsky, I., Rottenberg, J., Bylsma, L. M., Jennings, J. R., George, C., Baji, I., et al. (2016). Parasympathetic nervous system activity predicts mood repair use and its effectiveness among adolescents with and without histories of major depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 323–336. doi: 10.1037/abn0000149
Zepelin, H., Siegel, J. M., and Tobler, I. (2005). “Mammalian sleep,” in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, eds M. H. Kryger, T. Roth and W. C. Dement (Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders), 91–100.
Keywords: neuroscience mysteries, default space, consciousness, sleep, emotion regulation, binding problem, thalamus, metastability
Citation: Jerath R and Beveridge C (2018) Top Mysteries of the Mind: Insights From the Default Space Model of Consciousness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:162. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00162
Received: 08 February 2018; Accepted: 06 April 2018;
Published: 24 April 2018.
Edited by:
Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, United StatesReviewed by:
Beata Saniova, Comenius University, SlovakiaAndrew A. Fingelkurts, BM-Science, Finland
Copyright © 2018 Jerath and Beveridge. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ravinder Jerath, rj605r@aol.com