Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 21 November 2017
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience

Commentary: Brain-to-Brain Synchrony Tracks Real-World Dynamic Group Interactions in the Classroom and Cognitive Neuroscience: Synchronizing Brains in the Classroom

  • Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva y Social, Facultad de Psicología, Diego Portales University, Santiago, Chile

A commentary on
Brain-to-Brain Synchrony Tracks Real-World Dynamic Group Interactions in the Classroom

by Dikker, S., Wan, L., Davidesco, I., Kaggen, L., Oostrik, M., McClintock, J., et al. (2017). Curr. Biol. 27, 1375–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002

Cognitive Neuroscience: Synchronizing Brains in the Classroom

by Bhattacharya, J. (2017). Curr. Biol. 27, R346–R348. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.071

Earlier this year, Dikker et al. (2017) published a research report opening an important new chapter in the ongoing dialogue between Neuroscience and Education, namely, the possibility of successfully acquiring and analyzing diachronic human neurophysiological data during semi-structured, real-world classroom interactions using low-cost data-acquisition technologies. Taken as disciplinary work [i.e. hyperscanning (e.g., Astolfi et al., 2010) or neuroeducational (e.g., Mercier and Charland, 2013) studies], the article surely deserves praise; however, as noted by Bhattacharya (2017) it also signifies a milestone for the entire field of Cognitive Neuroscience as it represents a big step in settling a paradigm shift, which has been slowly gaining grounds in our discipline. Given the excitement that the concept of a “paradigm shift” can bring out along with the new research avenues that low-cost technologies offer, in this commentary we offer a perspective about the challenges and implications of the ever-increasing possibility of studying cognition in its “natural state.”

Social Neuroscience is an exciting new field that has provided in-depth insights, expanded the range of topics, and added an unprecedented layer of complexity to the understanding of the neural basis of human behavior. Thanks to this multidisciplinary effort, researchers interested in the neural basis of human behavior have explored not only how traditionally-conceptualized “lower-level” and “higher-level” cognitive processes relate to specific brain structures or concerted neural dynamics, but also have turned to explore how the brain supports other mental processes that are key in the interaction between individuals. However, the field still presents some important limitations at the theoretical and methodological levels. Most social cognition studies investigate behavior and concomitant brain activity of isolated individuals while exposed to stimuli of social relevance (e.g., facial and/or bodily gestures as in Thompson et al., 2007), social nature (e.g., facial expression of emotions as in Vuilleumier and Pourtois, 2007), or immerse in a social context (e.g., increased number of individuals within the stimuli as in Akitsuki and Decety, 2009; Puce et al., 2013) without actually involving a real interaction with another person (i.e., simulated interactions as in Caruana et al., 2016). These approaches are commonly referred to as “one-person-neuroscience.” Most experiments use experimental paradigms probing “offline” social cognition, where isolated participants have to think about other people's mental states while being detached from a real social interaction (Pfeiffer et al., 2013).

Without a doubt, these approaches have advanced the field tremendously, but have also limited the exploration of real life social cognition; when information is diachronically and mutually exchanged within an appropriate temporal frame between individuals. It is within this context that it has been suggested that in order to further develop social neuroscience as a field and gain more knowledge about the neural basis of social interaction, researchers should move toward quantifying the interpersonal co-regulated coupling between interacting partners (Goldman and de Vignemont, 2009), while they mutually and continuously affect one another (Varela et al., 1991; Clark, 2008, 2013), either by mutual coordination or cooperation (Konvalinka and Roepstorff, 2012). The work by Dikker et al. (2017) embodies the aforementioned spirit.

Thus, in order to better study the neural basis of situated and embodied social interaction, we suggest that each study contributing to the referred paradigmatic shift should be evaluated (a priori or a posteriori) considering two main challenges depicted in Figure 1:

(i) The theoretical need of developing a coherent framework for interpreting results and mapping the whole spectrum of relations between personal, social, and neural dynamics.

(ii) The methodological need, referring to the relationship between the ever-improving data-acquisition technologies and the actual usage plausibility of such technologies at a single-subject and/or group level. The latter can be further divided into a technical requirement of developing novel experimental setups to observe and measure interactions between two or more people and an analytical requirement of generating novel analysis procedures to quantify mutual interactions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. When studying the embodied nature of social cognition, regardless of how close or far away a research design is from “synthetic laboratory settings,” it must consider theoretical and methodological needs.

Although the field has been energetically revisiting and discussing about the theoretical need (e.g., Konvalinka and Roepstorff, 2012; De Jaegher et al., 2016; Krakauer et al., 2017), at this early stage, it is important to avoid the idea that the methodological need can be considered a “won battle” thanks to economically-accessible consumer-grade technologies (Bhattacharya, 2017). Recent evidence indicates that although the current technical landscape is very promising, we must proceed with caution (Melnik et al., 2017). Thus, we would like to point out that “more naturalistic” data acquisition can be equally undertaken using both consumer-grade, as in Dikker et al. (2017), as well as research-grade technologies, each providing advantages and disadvantages. On the one hand, consumer-grade equipment allows to concomitantly acquiring data from larger samples of subjects while decreasing signal quality and hindering analysis possibilities. On the other hand, research-grade technologies allow far more complex analyses but at a major set-up complexity and economical cost. Finally, we would like to stress that, contrary to Bhattacharya (2017), we believe consumer-grade technologies are not the natural next step outside “synthetic laboratory settings,” but a parallel path facing similar, if not larger, technical and analytical requirements that research-grade technologies confront (Figure 1). Therefore, in times of paradigmatic change, with new and exciting theoretical vistas encompassed within the embodied social cognition framework and new available technologies, social neuroscience wants to be methodologically cautious and remember that research questions and epistemological views guide our work. We want to extend the laboratory toward the real world, not escape from it.

Author Contributions

FP conceptualized and wrote the commentary. AR discussed the original idea and edited the commentary.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Christian Salas, Dr. Daniel Rojas, Dr. Claudio Martinez, and Dr. Alemka Tomicic for helpful ongoing discussion of ideas.

References

Akitsuki, Y., and Decety, J. (2009). Social context and perceived agency affects empathy for pain: an event-related fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 47, 722–734. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astolfi, L., Toppi, J., De Vico, F. F., Vecchiato, G., Salinari, S., Mattia, D., et al. (2010). Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans. Brain Topogr. 23, 243–256. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0147-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhattacharya, J. (2017). Cognitive neuroscience: synchronizing brains in the classroom. Curr. Biol. 27, R346–R348. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caruana, N., McArthur, G., Woolgar, A., and Brock, J. (2016). Simulating social interactions for the experimental investigation of joint attention. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension, 1st Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 181–204. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., and Adolphs, R. (2016). What does the interactive brain hypothesis mean for social neuroscience? A dialogue. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 371:20150379. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikker, S., Wan, L., Davidesco, I., Kaggen, L., Oostrik, M., McClintock, J., et al. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Curr. Biol. 27, 1375–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, A., and de Vignemont, F. (2009). Is social cognition embodied? Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed). 13, 154–159. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konvalinka, I., and Roepstorff, A. (2012). The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A., and Poeppel, D. (2017). Neuroscience needs behavior: correcting a reductionist Bias. Neuron 93, 480–490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melnik, A., Legkov, P., Izdebski, K., Kärcher, S. M., Hairston, W. D., Ferris, D. P., et al. (2017). Systems, subjects, sessions: to what extent do these factors influence EEG data? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:150. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercier, J., and Charland, P. (2013). An agenda for neuroeducation: relating psychophysiological and behavioral data across time scales of learning. Neuroeducation 2, 71–86. Available online at: http://www.associationneuroeducation.org/neuroeducation/mercier2013

Google Scholar

Pfeiffer, U. J., Timmermans, B., Vogeley, K., Frith, C. D., and Schilbach, L. (2013). Towards a neuroscience of social interaction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:22. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puce, A., McNeely, M. E., Berrebi, M. E., Thompson, J. C., Hardee, J., and Brefczynski-Lewis, J. (2013). Multiple faces elicit augmented neural activity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:282. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. C., Hardee, J. E., Panayiotou, A., Crewther, D., and Puce, A. (2007). Common and distinct brain activation to viewing dynamic sequences of face and hand movements. Neuroimage 37, 966–973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind. Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., and Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia 45, 174–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG, hyperscanning, research technology, data acquisition, embodied cognition, social neuroscience

Citation: Parada FJ and Rossi A (2017) Commentary: Brain-to-Brain Synchrony Tracks Real-World Dynamic Group Interactions in the Classroom and Cognitive Neuroscience: Synchronizing Brains in the Classroom. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:554. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00554

Received: 11 September 2017; Accepted: 01 November 2017;
Published: 21 November 2017.

Edited by:

Klaus Gramann, Technische Universität Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Joydeep Bhattacharya, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2017 Parada and Rossi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francisco J. Parada, francisco.parada@udp.cl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.