Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 24 July 2017
Sec. Speech and Language

Increase in Beta-Band Activity during Preparation for Overt Speech in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease

  • 1Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany
  • 2Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, University Hospital Karl-Jaspers-Klinik, University of Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
  • 3Neuroimaging Unit, University of Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
  • 4Research Center Neurosensory Science, University of Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 6Institute of Psychology II, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Speech impairment is a frequent and often serious symptom of Parkinson’s disease (PD), characterized by a disorder of phonation, articulation and prosody. While research on the pathogenesis of the prominent limb motor symptoms has made considerable progress in recent years, the pathophysiology of PD speech impairment is still incompletely understood. To investigate the neural correlates of speech production in PD, EEG was recorded in 14 non-demented patients with idiopathic PD and preserved verbal fluency on regular dopaminergic medication (8 women; mean age ± SD: 69.5 ± 8.0 years). The control group consisted of 15 healthy age-matched individuals (7 women; age: 69.7 ± 7.0 years). All participants performed a visually-cued, overt speech production task; required utterances were papapa and pataka. During the preparatory phase of speech production, in a time window of 200–400 ms after presentation of the visual cue, β-power was significantly increased in PD patients compared to healthy controls. Previous research has shown that the physiological decrease of β-power preceding limb movement onset is delayed and smaller in PD patients off medication and normalizes under dopaminergic treatment. By contrast, our study demonstrates that β-power during preparation for speech production is higher in patients on dopaminergic therapy than controls. Thus, our results suggest that the mechanisms that regulate β-activity preceding limb movement and speech production differ in PD. The pathophysiological role of this increase in β-power during speech preparation needs to be determined.

Introduction

Impairment of speech production, first described by James Parkinson in his classical Essay on the Shaking Palsy (Parkinson, 1817), is an often serious sequela of Parkinson’s disease (PD), characterized by a disorder of phonation, articulation and prosody (Sapir, 2014). During the course of the illness, a majority of patients experiences difficulties of speech production. At the baseline visit of a large cohort study including 419 PD patients, 51% of patients reported at least slight impairment of speech production (Perez-Lloret et al., 2012). In a group of 125 patients with PD, speech intelligibility of PD patients as rated by listeners unfamiliar with dysarthric speech was significantly worse compared to unaffected age-matched controls (Miller et al., 2007). With progression of the disorder, speech impairment increases in frequency and intensity (Skodda et al., 2013). In the early stage of the disease, a disorder of phonation is often the leading symptom of PD speech impairment, presenting with a breathy and harsh voice of reduced loudness (Ho et al., 1998; Holmes et al., 2000). In a later stage, difficulties of articulation may develop. The articulatory precision in the production of consonants—in particular, stop consonants such as /k/, /p/ and /t/—is typically reduced (Ackermann and Ziegler, 1991) and speech rate declines (Martínez-Sánchez et al., 2016). Prosody, the natural variations in loudness, pitch and rhythm of fluent speech, is frequently impaired as well (Darkins et al., 1988).

PD is an adult-onset neurodegenerative disorder associated, at its core, with a loss of dopaminergic neurons within the substantia nigra that project to the striatum and, hence, changes of the functional connectivity within basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits (Wichmann et al., 2011; Göttlich et al., 2013). Multiple lines of evidence suggest that neural oscillations within these circuits differ between healthy individuals and patients with PD (Oswal et al., 2013; Brittain and Brown, 2014; Brittain et al., 2014). Subcortical recordings of local field potentials, mainly from the subthalamic nucleus, identified excessive neural oscillations in the β-band in PD patients (Bronte-Stewart et al., 2009; Hirschmann et al., 2011). This increase in rhythmic brain activity in basal ganglia-cortical circuits is now regarded as a key concept in the pathophysiology of motor and cognitive deficits in PD (Oswal et al., 2013). Several studies have shown that task-related modulation of cortical β-band activity is reduced in PD patients compared with healthy controls (Pollok et al., 2012; Heinrichs-Graham et al., 2014; te Woerd et al., 2014).

While research on the pathogenesis of the prominent limb motor symptoms in PD has made considerable progress (Wichmann et al., 2011), the pathophysiology of PD speech impairment is still little understood (Sapir, 2014). PD speech impairment has traditionally been characterized as a pure speech motor disorder—a hypokinetic dysarthria—due to hypokinesia, bradykinesia and rigidity of laryngeal and orofacial muscles. This pathophysiological model has been challenged by several experimental and therapeutic observations, though. Studies of orofacial muscle tone in patients with PD suggested that rigidity alone does not fully explain the characteristics of PD speech disorder (Sapir, 2014). Moreover, dopaminergic stimulation with L-dopa or apomorphine, highly effective in the treatment of limb motor symptoms in PD, does not consistently improve PD speech disorder (Kompoliti et al., 2000; Schulz and Grant, 2000; Ho et al., 2008). Finally, deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus may even result in a deterioration of PD speech disorder (Skodda et al., 2014; Tsuboi et al., 2015).

The aim of the present study was to investigate β-band rhythmic activity in the preparatory phase of overt non-lexical speech production in PD patients and in a control group of age-matched healthy individuals using EEG. We chose two target utterances, papapa and pataka. Both trisyllabic utterances are challenging because they require fast alternating (diadochokinetic) motions of the articulators. Pataka is particularly difficult because the three stop consonants demand a swift and precise movement of the tongue from the front to the back of the oral cavity. Actual speaking is associated with electromyographic activity in many different muscles, including the temporalis muscle that covers large parts of the temporal and parietal bone (Tuller et al., 1981). To minimize contamination of our EEG recordings by muscle artifacts, we only chose to analyze the preparatory phase, between the onset of the visual cue and the onset of vocalization.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-three patients with idiopathic PD (12 women) were recruited at the Outpatient Clinic, Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck. For details of recruitment, see Figure 1. One patient was excluded because the Parkinson Neuropsychometric Dementia Assessment (PANDA, Kalbe et al., 2008) indicated cognitive impairment. EEG recordings from seven patients had to be excluded because of excessive muscle activity during preparation for speaking. Another patient was excluded because the audio recording was missing due to a technical failure. The data sets of the remaining 14 patients (8 women) were analyzed for this study. Mean age ± standard deviation was 69.5 ± 8.0 years (age range: 52–81 years).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Recruitment of patients (A) and healthy controls (B). PANDA, Parkinson Neuropsychometric Dementia Assessment. BDI, Beck Depression Inventory.

All patients were diagnosed by an experienced neurologist; the initial diagnosis was made between 3 years and 16 years before this study was performed. All patients received antiparkinsonian medication at the time of the study (mean equivalent L-dopa dose: 677 mg/day). To assess the severity of motor symptoms, part III of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) was performed in all patients (Fahn and Elton, 1987). The mean UPDRS III score ± SD was 17 ± 11 (minimum: 3, maximum: 44).

Twenty-seven healthy individuals (13 women) were recruited for an age-matched control group. Twenty-five participants were recruited through the subject database of the Department of Neurology, two participants were healthy spouses of the patients recruited for this study. EEG recordings from 10 healthy participants had to be excluded because of excessive muscle activity during preparation for speaking. One healthy participant was excluded because of an exceptionally delayed speech onset. In this participant, the time between the onset of the visual cue and the onset of speaking was on average 1965 ms (for the rest of the control group, speech latency was 659 ± 153 ms). Another healthy participant was excluded because of an exceptionally high number of incorrect speech responses (72%). The data sets of the remaining 15 healthy participants (7 women) were analyzed for this study. Mean age ± SD was 69.7 ± 7.0 years (age range: 55–80 years).

All included patients and control participants were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of neurological or psychiatric disorder (except PD in the patient group). The five cognitive tasks of the PANDA (Kalbe et al., 2008) showed no cognitive impairment. The Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961) indicated no depressive symptoms. To assess verbal fluency at the behavioral level, all participants were asked to produce as many words beginning with “m” as possible within 2 min as part of the Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (Aschenbrenner et al., 2000).

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Research Ethics Board of the University of Lübeck with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the REB. Participants received a remuneration of 40 Euro.

Experimental Paradigm

A visually cued overt speech production task was performed during the EEG recording. Participants were seated in a comfortable chair with their eyes about 80 cm in front of a 20 inch LCD computer screen. To cue speech production, the syllables papapa or pataka were shown in the middle of the screen for 2000 ms. Patients were instructed to speak aloud the required utterance as soon as the cue appears. The onset-to-onset time interval between cues was 5000 ms. To reduce task-switching effects and to facilitate the task, a blocked presentation was chosen. Each block was preceded by the instruction “Jetzt kommt papapa (Now we’ll present papapa)” or “Jetzt kommt pataka (Now we’ll present pataka)”. Each block consisted of five papapa and five pataka trials. Five blocks were presented in pseudo-randomized order for each condition. In total, 25 papapa and 25 pataka trials were recorded. All stimuli were presented using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Berkeley, CA, USA)1.

All utterances were recorded using a microphone and stored for off-line analysis with the open source audio software Audacity2. All utterances were examined by the same listener (CW) and double-checked by another researcher (PS). Trials without a correct, intelligible utterance were excluded from further data analysis. In the patient group, on average 0.9 trials per participant were excluded because a correct verbal response was missing (minimum: 0 trials, maximum: 5 trials). In the control group, on average 1.7 trials per participant were excluded due to a missing correct response (minimum: 0 trials, maximum: 9 trials). Speech latency (the time difference between onset of the instruction and onset of overt speech) was determined for all correct responses and all participants. On an individual basis, speech latencies were z-transformed and trials with exceptionally short or long latencies (z < −3 or z > 3) excluded. On average, 0.9 trials per participant in the patient group (minimum: 0, maximum: 2) and 0.7 trials per participant in the control group (minimum: 0, maximum: 2) were excluded after z-transformation of latencies. The average individual speech latency was finally calculated for every participant.

EEG Acquisition

EEG data were recorded in a sound-dampened and electromagnetically-shielded room using an elastic 32 electrode cap (Electro-Cap International, Eaton, OH, USA)3 and a Synamps amplifier (Compumedics Neuroscan, Singen, Germany)4. To record brain electric activity, 29 tin electrodes were mounted on the scalp according to the 10/20 system with modified combinatorial nomenclature (American Electroencephalographic Society, 1994). Scalp electrodes were referenced online against the left mastoid. To monitor vertical and horizontal eye movements, electrodes were placed above and below the left eye and on the outer canthus of each eye. Data was sampled at 250 Hz, with a bandpass filter of 0.01–50 Hz. Electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ.

EEG Analysis

Raw data were re-referenced to the average activity of both mastoid electrodes. An independent component analysis (ICA) was performed using an extended information maximization algorithm (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995) as implemented in EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004)5 to identify and remove components related to eye blinks and ocular movements (Hoffmann and Falkenstein, 2008).

To calculate time-frequency spectra, single-trial data were convolved with a complex Morlet wavelet using the open source MATLAB toolbox FieldTrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011)6. The width of the wavelet was set at seven cycles. To avoid contamination of EEG data by speech-related muscle artifacts, individual EEG epochs were truncated at speech onset. Thus, the number of analyzed trials and hence the signal-to-noise ratio decreased after approximately 400 ms. Figure 2 illustrates the number of trials (mean ± SD) available at a given time point for the papapa and the pataka conditions, separately.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Number of trials over time. The analysis of EEG data focused on the preparatory phase of speech production. Thus, individual EEG epochs were truncated at speech onset. The Figure shows the mean number of trials at a given time (bold blue line) for the papapa condition (A) and the pataka condition (B) across all participants. The error bars represent the standard deviation.

After wavelet transform, oscillatory power in the studied frequencies (1–50 Hz, linear increase) was computed for each trial in the time window −1000 ms to speech onset and averaged for each subject time-locked to the onset of the visual cue before calculating a grand average. The interval −100 ms to 0 ms (onset of visual cue) served as baseline for all computations. A baseline close to the onset of the visual cue was chosen to minimize contamination by muscle artifacts related to the preceding trial.

After inspection of the time course of β-activity (Figure 7), we decided to test changes in β-power in the time window of 200–400 ms after onset of the visual cue.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Individual EEG data. The figure illustrates EEG recordings of two patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD), showing the first four papapa trials of the experiment. The onset of the visual cue (papapa) is represented by a red line. Epochs of EEG data have been extracted between −1000 and 1000 ms relative to the onset of the cue. The upper recording (A) displays artifacts related to overt speech, starting around 500 ms after onset of the cue (marked by arrows). As these artifacts started with speech onset, the recording was included in the final data analysis. The lower recording of another patient with PD (B) shows massive high-frequency artifacts due to muscular activity. This recording was excluded from the final data analysis.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Time-frequency spectra for the papapa condition. The figure represents averaged changes in spectral power relative to a baseline period of −100 ms to 0 ms during the preparation for speech production. Time (in seconds) is shown on the x-axis; 0 s is the onset of the visual cue (papapa). Frequency (1–50 Hz) is shown on the y-axis. Time-frequency spectra are shown for controls (A) and patients (B). The boxes represent the frequency range (16–31 Hz) and time window (200–400 ms) of the ANOVA.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Time-frequency spectra for the pataka condition. The figure represents averaged changes in spectral power relative to a baseline period of −100 ms to 0 ms during the preparation for speech production. Time (in seconds) is shown on the x-axis; 0 s is the onset of the visual cue (pataka). Frequency (1–50 Hz) is shown on the y-axis. Time-frequency spectra are shown for controls (A) and patients (B). The boxes represent the frequency range (16–31 Hz) and time window (200–400 ms) of the ANOVA.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Differences in time-frequency spectra between controls and patients. For this figure, averaged changes in spectral power in controls, relative to a baseline period of −100 ms to 0 ms, were subtracted from changes in spectral power in patients. Yellow and red colors indicate higher spectral power in patients vs. controls. Time (in seconds) is shown on the x-axis; 0 s is the onset of the visual cue. Frequency (1–50 Hz) is shown on the y-axis. (A) displays the difference in spectral power for the papapa condition, (B) for the pataka condition. The boxes represent the frequency range (16–31 Hz) and time window (200–400 ms) of the ANOVA.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Time courses, individual changes and topographies of spectral power in the β-band. (A) shows averaged time courses of spectral power in the β-band (16–31 Hz) relative to a baseline period of −100 to 0 ms for controls (blue lines) and patients (red lines). Dashed lines depict the papapa condition, solid lines the pataka condition. Time (in seconds) is shown on the x-axis; the vertical line at 0 s represents the onset of the visual cue. Relative change (in % of the baseline value) is shown on the y-axis. The boxes represent the time window (200–400 ms) of the ANOVA. (B) demonstrates the changes in β-band power relative to the baseline for each individual participant, averaged across the time window of 200–400 ms. Healthy controls are shown in blue (1: papapa condition; 2: pataka condition), patients with PD are shown in red (3: papapa condition; 4: pataka condition). Data were plotted for the central electrodes Fz, Cz and Pz. The black diamonds represent the group mean, the error bars the standard deviation. (C) displays the topographical distribution of changes in spectral power in the β-band relative to the baseline in controls and patients. For each topological plot, the average of the papapa and pataka condition was calculated.

An ANOVA was calculated for the averaged power in the β-band (16–31 Hz) and the 200–400 ms time window using the between-subjects factor group (patients vs. controls) and the within-subjects factors condition (papapa vs. pataka), anteriority of EEG electrodes (with 3 levels: F7/F3/Fz/F4/F8; T7/C3/Cz/C4/T8; P7/P3/Pz/P4/P8) and laterality of EEG electrodes (with 5 levels: F7/T7/P7; F3/C3/P3; Fz/Cz/Pz; F4/C4/P4; F8/T8/P8) using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 23)7. Greenhouse-Geisser correction was performed when necessary. To estimate the effect size of ANOVA results, partial η2 was calculated.

Results

Behavioral Data

No significant difference between patients and controls regarding the number of words produced in the verbal fluency test (Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test, Aschenbrenner et al., 2000) was found (16 ± 6 vs. 17 ± 4, p = 0.69). During the EEG experiment, speech latency of included trials was not significantly different between patients and controls (651 ± 145 ms vs. 659 ± 153 ms; p = 0.89).

EEG Data

Raw EEG data of two PD patients are shown in Figure 3. Figure 3A displays the first four papapa trials of a patient whose data were included in the final analysis. By contrast, Figure 3B displays the first four papapa trials of another patient whose data were excluded due to massive muscle artifacts.

Figure 4 illustrates the time-frequency spectra for the papapa condition in the control (A) and the patient group (B). Figure 5 illustrates the time-frequency spectra for the pataka condition in the control (A) and the patient group (B). The time-frequency spectra in Figure 6 show the differences of spectral power between patients and controls for the papapa condition (A) and the pataka condition (B). Figure 7A depicts the averaged time courses of spectral power in the β-band for both experimental conditions (papapa vs. pataka) and groups (patients vs. controls). Figure 7B demonstrates individual changes in β-band power (averaged across the time window of 200–400 ms) for patients vs. controls and for both speech production conditions. The topological distribution of changes in β-power in the time window of 200–400 ms relative to the baseline (−100 to 0 ms) for controls and patients is illustrated in Figure 7C.

In the ANOVA, β-power was significantly higher in PD patients than in controls in the time window of 200–400 ms after the visual cue (significant effect of group; F(1,27) = 10.90, p = 0.003, partial η2 = 0.288). The main effect of condition and the interactions condition × group, condition × anteriority, condition × group × anteriority, condition × laterality, condition × group × laterality, condition × anteriority × laterality and condition × group × anteriority × laterality were not significant (p > 0.05).

Discussion

The present study demonstrates significant differences in the time course of oscillatory brain activity in the β-band (16–31 Hz) between PD patients and healthy controls during the preparation for overt speech. In a time window of 200–400 ms after presentation of the visual cue, β-power was significantly increased in PD patients compared to healthy controls (Figure 7A).

Oscillatory Brain Activity in Speech Production

Overt speech production is a highly complex task, accomplished by a distributed bilateral cortical and subcortical neural network (for a review, see Kemmerer, 2015). The brain areas that are crucial for the different stages of speech production are well known. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), activation was detected in the primary motor cortex, supplementary motor area, cingulate motor area, thalamus, basal ganglia, insula, temporal lobe and cerebellum during non-lexical speech production (Sörös et al., 2006). The information flow between these areas of the speech production network, in contrast, is only partially understood. Models of speech production propose detailed motor plans that activate dozens of muscles in a well-defined order on a millisecond time-scale and that are constantly updated by auditory and somatosensory feedback (Hickok, 2012).

Multiple lines of evidence, using electroencephalography, magnetoencephalography and electrocorticography, have demonstrated that electrical brain oscillations are crucial for information flow within distributed brain networks (Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999; Fries, 2005; Schnitzler and Gross, 2005; van Wijk et al., 2012; Cheyne, 2013). One of the most widely replicated finding is that preparation for and execution of limb movements are correlated with a gradual decrease of oscillatory power in the β-band, starting about 1 s before movement onset (Jasper and Penfield, 1949; Pfurtscheller, 1981; Leocani et al., 1997; for a review, see Hari and Salmelin, 1997; Cheyne, 2013; Kilavik et al., 2013).

Similar to voluntary limb movements, the preparation of speech production is also associated with a decrease of β-power (Gehrig et al., 2012; Jenson et al., 2014; Mersov et al., 2016). Mersov et al. (2016) studied brain rhythms of fluent speakers and adults who stutter using magnetoencephalography before and during cued overt reading of words in a carrier phrase. During speech preparation, a decrease of β-power (in this study: 15–25 Hz) was found in the bilateral cuneus of fluent individuals. In addition, a decrease of β-power was found in the mouth motor cortex (Mersov et al., 2016). Similarly, Gehrig et al. (2012) used magnetoencephalography to investigate the preparatory phase of overt and covert reading in healthy individuals. The authors found a decrease of β-power in the bilateral parietal lobe and the left articulatory motor region during preparation, beginning about 350 ms after the onset of a visual preparation cue (Gehrig et al., 2012).

Although the dynamics of β-power have been investigated for decades, the functional significance of event-related fluctuations of oscillatory brain rhythms has not been fully elucidated (Cheyne, 2013). The pre-movement decrease of β-power is thought to reflect the preparation for the motor response (Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999; Cheyne, 2013; Kilavik et al., 2013). In contrast, an increase of β-power in the motor cortex preserves the current motor state and inhibits the initiation of new motor plans (Neuper and Pfurtscheller, 2001; Engel and Fries, 2010). Several studies suggest a causal relationship between β-oscillations and motor functions (van Wijk et al., 2012). Stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus at 20 Hz slowed the development of maximal grip force in PD patients relative to healthy controls (Chen et al., 2011). Even in healthy adults, non-invasive transcranial alternating-current stimulation at 20 Hz over the contralateral motor cortex slows voluntary hand movements (Pogosyan et al., 2009).

Brain Oscillations during the Preparation for Speech in PD Patients

With the advent of deep brain stimulation for the treatment of PD and the possibility to record local field potentials through the stimulation leads, an increase of β-band oscillations in cortico-basal ganglia circuits at rest (Brittain and Brown, 2014), even at an early stage of the disease (Pollok et al., 2012), has been described. Excessive β-band oscillations are believed to be directly linked to chronic dopamine depletion (Mallet et al., 2008) and are now regarded as neurophysiological signature of PD (Oswal et al., 2013). In PD patients who underwent bilateral implantation of deep brain stimulation electrodes in the subthalamic nucleus and who were off dopaminergic medication, a significant correlation was found between the power of local field potentials in a 8–35 Hz frequency band and clinical severity of PD as assessed by the total UPDRS III score (Neumann et al., 2016). Treatment with dopamine or deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus has been shown to decrease or even normalize excessive β-band oscillations (Giannicola et al., 2010; Jenkinson and Brown, 2011; Quinn et al., 2015). In PD patients off medication, the onset of pre-movement β-power decrease (Pollok et al., 2012; Meziane et al., 2015) was delayed during the preparation for limb movements. Heinrichs-Graham et al. (2014) used magnetoencephalography to study brain oscillations during planning, execution and termination of a tap of the right index finger in unmedicated PD patients and healthy age-matched controls. PD patients had a significantly smaller decrease of β-power prior and during movement than controls (Heinrichs-Graham et al., 2014).

While cortical and subcortical activity associated with limb movement has been widely investigated in PD, only a small number of studies have examined the neural correlates of speech production in PD. Functional neuroimaging research using positron emission tomography (PET; Pinto et al., 2004) and fMRI (Rektorova et al., 2007; Arnold et al., 2014) found increased activation, mainly in motor areas, in PD patients compared to controls. Overt speech production was associated with increased activation in the supplementary motor area (Pinto et al., 2004), primary orofacial sensorimotor cortex (Rektorova et al., 2007) as well as left dorsal premotor cortex and inferior frontal gyrus (Arnold et al., 2014) in patients vs. controls. For the study of brain oscillations, Hebb et al. (2012) recorded local field potentials in the subthalamic nucleus of PD patients during a continuous speech-language task, including naming the months of the year and counting upward from one. The authors observed a bilateral decrease of β-power preceding and during overt speech production in the subthalamic nucleus (Hebb et al., 2012). A recent study by Wojtecki et al. (2017) recorded subthalamic nucleus activity during a silent word generation paradigm, followed by overt pronunciation of the generated word. During silent word generation, an entirely cognitive task, the authors found a significant increase in α- and θ-power, but not in β- and γ-power. During overt speech, the recordings indicated a small, but insignificant decrease of β-power (Wojtecki et al., 2017).

To the best of our knowledge, an increase of β-power during the preparatory phase preceding movement has not been described before, neither in limb motor nor in speech control. The present study cannot provide definite answers regarding the functional significance of β-activity and its relationship to speech production, but may stimulate hypotheses for future research. Hyperactivity in speech motor areas, as demonstrated by PET and fMRI, has been interpreted as cortical mechanisms to compensate for basal ganglia dysfunctions. After all we know of the function of β-oscillations, an increase of β-power has an anti-kinetic effect (Jenkinson and Brown, 2011) and cannot be regarded as compensatory in nature.

It is important to note that the PD patients studied here were non-demented individuals with preserved verbal fluency. In addition, only fast, correct and intelligible responses have been included in the EEG analysis. Thus, the increase in β-power seen here does not necessarily cause speech production deficits. Moreover, the increase in β-power was seen in PD patients on medication, receiving a mean equivalent L-dopa dose of 677 mg/day. Importantly, the increase of β-band activity in our study was observed in the time window 200–400 ms after presentation of the visual cue, but not immediately preceding speech onset. Overt speech production is a multi-stage process that involves cognitive processes (mental retrieval of phonemes and combination of phonemes to syllables and words) and motor processes (developing an articulatory plan and execution of articulatory movements; Indefrey, 2011). Behavioral and electrophysiological studies delineated the temporal evolution of this cascade of events (Indefrey, 2011). The time window of the β-band increase observed here is dominated by a cognitive process, the retrieval of the phonological code. Planning of the articulatory movements starts later, about 150 ms before onset of articulation (Indefrey, 2011). This time course may explain why our patients had no symptoms of dysarthric speech with normal verbal fluency in the neuropsychological assessment and normal speech latency during the EEG recording. Our results would be compatible with an impairment of phonological processing, which has not been expected before and which was not tested explicitly in this study.

Our study has important limitations. In order to design a short experimental paradigm, feasible for patients with PD, we included only 25 trials per condition. In a future study, we would prefer 50 or more trials per condition to increase the signal-to-noise ratio of the EEG recordings. Moreover, a relatively high number of participants had to be excluded because of ongoing muscle artifacts. In a future study, we would take more time to familiarize the participants with the speech production paradigm and train them to relax their facial and jaw muscles. Regarding the analysis of EEG data, we decided to reference to the average activity of both mastoid electrodes. As the choice of reference may influence spectral power, the use of a neutral reference may have advantages (Yao, 2001; Yao et al., 2005).

In conclusion, the changes of oscillatory brain activity preceding limb movement and speech production differ in PD. While the physiological decrease of β-power preceding movement onset is delayed and smaller in PD patients off medication and normalizes under dopaminergic treatment, speech production is preceded by an increase of β-power in our patients who were on dopaminergic therapy compared to controls. This fundamental difference in rhythmic brain activity may contribute to the poor efficacy of dopamine and deep brain stimulation in PD speech disorder. Future research is warranted to determine the generators and the pathophysiological role of β-oscillations in PD speech disorder. First of all, a comparison of rhythmic brain activity between PD patients with and without speech impairment is needed to establish the relationship between behavioral speech performance and brain oscillations. Second, speech-related brain oscillations should be studied in patients on and off treatment (dopaminergic medication or deep brain stimulation) to gain further insights into the effects of antiparkinsonian treatment on speech-related β-power. Finally, a dedicated study on phonological processing in PD should find out whether the abnormal brain activity seen here influences cognitive processes in speech production and contributes to the pathophysiology of PD speech disorder. The results of these investigations may help to improve the currently often unsatisfactory therapy of this frequent and debilitating symptom of PD, e.g., through modification of deep brain stimulation protocols or further development of non-invasive stimulation techniques.

Author Contributions

PS and TFM conceptualized and designed the study. CW and MA-K collected the data. ND, CW and PS analyzed the data. PS, ND and CW prepared the figures. PS wrote the manuscript. ND, CW, MA-K, NB and TFM interpreted the data, provided important feedback and revised the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Marcus Heldmann for setting up the experimental paradigm, Susanne Schellbach for expert help with the EEG recordings and Marcel Schulze for assistance with the preparation of Figure 3. Dr. Nuria Doñamayor is currently an honorary research fellow at the University of Cambridge, funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DO1915/1-1).

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.neurobs.com
  2. ^ web.audacityteam.org
  3. ^ http://electro-cap.com
  4. ^ http://compumedicsneuroscan.com
  5. ^ http://sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab
  6. ^ http://www.fieldtriptoolbox.org
  7. ^ https://www.ibm.com/analytics/us/en/technology/spss/

References

Ackermann, H., and Ziegler, W. (1991). Articulatory deficits in parkinsonian dysarthria: an acoustic analysis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 54, 1093–1098. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.54.12.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Electroencephalographic Society. (1994). Guideline thirteen: guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J. Clin. Neurophysiol.sss 11, 111–113. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199401000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Arnold, C., Gehrig, J., Gispert, S., Seifried, C., and Kell, C. A. (2014). Pathomechanisms and compensatory efforts related to Parkinsonian speech. Neuroimage Clin. 4, 82–97. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aschenbrenner, S., Tucha, O., and Lange, K. (2000). RWT—Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test. Handanweisung. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, A. J., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 7, 1129–1159. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brittain, J. S., and Brown, P. (2014). Oscillations and the basal ganglia: motor control and beyond. Neuroimage 85, 637–647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brittain, J. S., Sharott, A., and Brown, P. (2014). The highs and lows of beta activity in cortico-basal ganglia loops. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 1951–1959. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronte-Stewart, H., Barberini, C., Koop, M. M., Hill, B. C., Henderson, J. M., and Wingeier, B. (2009). The STN beta-band profile in Parkinson’s disease is stationary and shows prolonged attenuation after deep brain stimulation. Exp. Neurol. 215, 20–28. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C. C., Lin, W. Y., Chan, H. L., Hsu, Y. T., Tu, P. H., Lee, S. T., et al. (2011). Stimulation of the subthalamic region at 20 Hz slows the development of grip force in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Neurol. 231, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheyne, D. O. (2013). MEG studies of sensorimotor rhythms: a review. Exp. Neurol. 245, 27–39. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darkins, A. W., Fromkin, V. A., and Benson, D. F. (1988). A characterization of the prosodic loss in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Lang. 34, 315–327. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(88)90142-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., and Fries, P. (2010). Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahn, S., Elton, R. L., and Members of the UPDRS Development Committee. (1987). “Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale,” in Recent Developments in Parkinson’s Disease, eds S. Fahn, C. D. Marsden, D. B. Caine and A. Lieberman (Florham Park, NJ: Macmillan Health Care Information), 153–163.

Google Scholar

Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehrig, J., Wibral, M., Arnold, C., and Kell, C. A. (2012). Setting up the speech production network: how oscillations contribute to lateralized information routing. Front. Psychol. 3:169. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giannicola, G., Marceglia, S., Rossi, L., Mrakic-Sposta, S., Rampini, P., Tamma, F., et al. (2010). The effects of levodopa and ongoing deep brain stimulation on subthalamic beta oscillations in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Neurol. 226, 120–127. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2010.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Göttlich, M., Münte, T. F., Heldmann, M., Kasten, M., Hagenah, J., and Krämer, U. M. (2013). Altered resting state brain networks in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One 8:e77336. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., and Salmelin, R. (1997). Human cortical oscillations: a neuromagnetic view through the skull. Trends Neurosci. 20, 44–49. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)10065-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebb, A. O., Darvas, F., and Miller, K. J. (2012). Transient and state modulation of beta power in human subthalamic nucleus during speech production and finger movement. Neuroscience 202, 218–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.11.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs-Graham, E., Wilson, T. W., Santamaria, P. M., Heithoff, S. K., Torres-Russotto, D., Hutter-Saunders, J. A., et al. (2014). Neuromagnetic evidence of abnormal movement-related beta desynchronization in Parkinson’s disease. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2669–2678. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2012). Computational neuroanatomy of speech production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 135–145. doi: 10.1038/nrn3158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirschmann, J., Özkurt, T. E., Butz, M., Homburger, M., Elben, S., Hartmann, C. J., et al. (2011). Distinct oscillatory STN-cortical loops revealed by simultaneous MEG and local field potential recordings in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage 55, 1159–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, A. K., Bradshaw, J. L., and Iansek, R. (2008). For better or worse: the effect of levodopa on speech in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 23, 574–580. doi: 10.1002/mds.21899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, A. K., Iansek, R., Marigliani, C., Bradshaw, J. L., and Gates, S. (1998). Speech impairment in a large sample of patients with Parkinson’s disease. Behav. Neurol. 11, 131–137. doi: 10.1155/1999/327643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, S., and Falkenstein, M. (2008). The correction of eye blink artefacts in the EEG: a comparison of two prominent methods. PLoS One 3:e3004. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, R. J., Oates, J. M., Phyland, D. J., and Hughes, A. J. (2000). Voice characteristics in the progression of Parkinson’s disease. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 35, 407–418. doi: 10.1080/136828200410654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Indefrey, P. (2011). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: a critical update. Front. Psychol. 2:255. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasper, H., and Penfield, W. (1949). Electrocorticograms in man: effect of voluntary movement upon the electrical activity of the precentral gyrus. Arch. Psychiatr. Z. Neurol. 183, 163–174. doi: 10.1007/bf01062488

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, N., and Brown, P. (2011). New insights into the relationship between dopamine, beta oscillations and motor function. Trends Neurosci. 34, 611–618. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenson, D., Bowers, A. L., Harkrider, A. W., Thornton, D., Cuellar, M., and Saltuklaroglu, T. (2014). Temporal dynamics of sensorimotor integration in speech perception and production: independent component analysis of EEG data. Front. Psychol. 5:656. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalbe, E., Calabrese, P., Kohn, N., Hilker, R., Riedel, O., Wittchen, H. U., et al. (2008). Screening for cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease with the Parkinson neuropsychometric dementia assessment (PANDA) instrument. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 14, 93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2007.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D. (2015). “Speech production,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Language, (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 145–188.

Kilavik, B. E., Zaepffel, M., Brovelli, A., MacKay, W. A., and Riehle, A. (2013). The ups and downs of beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortex. Exp. Neurol. 245, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kompoliti, K., Wang, Q. E., Goetz, C. G., Leurgans, S., and Raman, R. (2000). Effects of central dopaminergic stimulation by apomorphine on speech in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 54, 458–462. doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.2.458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leocani, L., Toro, C., Manganotti, P., Zhuang, P., and Hallett, M. (1997). Event-related coherence and event-related desynchronization/synchronization in the 10 Hz and 20 Hz EEG during self-paced movements. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 104, 199–206. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(96)96051-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mallet, N., Pogosyan, A., Sharott, A., Csicsvari, J., Bolam, J. P., Brown, P., et al. (2008). Disrupted dopamine transmission and the emergence of exaggerated beta oscillations in subthalamic nucleus and cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 4795–4806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0123-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Sánchez, F., Meilán, J. J., Carro, J., Gómez Íñiguez, C., Millian-Morell, L., Pujante Valverde, I. M., et al. (2016). Speech rate in Parkinson’s disease: a controlled study. Neurologia 31, 466–472. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2014.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mersov, A. M., Jobst, C., Cheyne, D. O., and De Nil, L. (2016). Sensorimotor oscillations prior to speech onset reflect altered motor networks in adults who stutter. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:443. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meziane, H. B., Moisello, C., Perfetti, B., Kvint, S., Isaias, I. U., Quartarone, A., et al. (2015). Movement preparation and bilateral modulation of beta activity in aging and Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One 10:e0114817. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N., Allcock, L., Jones, D., Noble, E., Hildreth, A. J., and Burn, D. J. (2007). Prevalence and pattern of perceived intelligibility changes in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 78, 1188–1190. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.110171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, W. J., Degen, K., Schneider, G. H., Brücke, C., Huebl, J., Brown, P., et al. (2016). Subthalamic synchronized oscillatory activity correlates with motor impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 31, 1748–1751. doi: 10.1002/mds.26759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuper, C., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2001). Event-related dynamics of cortical rhythms: frequency-specific features and functional correlates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 43, 41–58. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00178-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Oswal, A., Brown, P., and Litvak, V. (2013). Synchronized neural oscillations and the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 26, 662–670. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkinson, J. (1817). An Essay on the Shaking Palsy. London: Sherwood, Neely, and Jones.

Google Scholar

Perez-Lloret, S., Nègre-Pagès, L., Ojero-Senard, A., Damier, P., Destée, A., Tison, F., et al. (2012). Oro-buccal symptoms (dysphagia, dysarthria, and sialorrhea) in patients with Parkinson’s disease: preliminary analysis from the French COPARK cohort. Eur. J. Neurol. 19, 28–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2011.03402.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G. (1981). Central beta rhythm during sensorimotor activities in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 51, 253–264. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(81)90139-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., and Lopes da Silva, F. H. (1999). Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00141-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, S., Thobois, S., Costes, N., Le Bars, D., Benabid, A. L., Broussolle, E., et al. (2004). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation and dysarthria in Parkinson’s disease: a PET study. Brain 127, 602–615. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pogosyan, A., Gaynor, L. D., Eusebio, A., and Brown, P. (2009). Boosting cortical activity at Beta-band frequencies slows movement in humans. Curr. Biol. 19, 1637–1641. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollok, B., Krause, V., Martsch, W., Wach, C., Schnitzler, A., and Südmeyer, M. (2012). Motor-cortical oscillations in early stages of Parkinson’s disease. J. Physiol. 590, 3203–3212. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.231316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinn, E. J., Blumenfeld, Z., Velisar, A., Koop, M. M., Shreve, L. A., Trager, M. H., et al. (2015). Beta oscillations in freely moving Parkinson’s subjects are attenuated during deep brain stimulation. Mov. Disord. 30, 1750–1758. doi: 10.1002/mds.26376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rektorova, I., Barrett, J., Mikl, M., Rektor, I., and Paus, T. (2007). Functional abnormalities in the primary orofacial sensorimotor cortex during speech in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22, 2043–2051. doi: 10.1002/mds.21548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapir, S. (2014). Multiple factors are involved in the dysarthria associated with Parkinson’s disease: a review with implications for clinical practice and research. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 57, 1330–1343. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S-13-0039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnitzler, A., and Gross, J. (2005). Normal and pathological oscillatory communication in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 285–296. doi: 10.1038/nrn1650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, G. M., and Grant, M. K. (2000). Effects of speech therapy and pharmacologic and surgical treatments on voice and speech in parkinson’s disease. J. Commun. Disord. 33, 59–88. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(99)00025-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S., Grönheit, W., Mancinelli, N., and Schlegel, U. (2013). Progression of voice and speech impairment in the course of Parkinson’s disease: a longitudinal study. Parkinsons Dis. 2013:389195. doi: 10.1155/2013/389195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S., Grönheit, W., Schlegel, U., Südmeyer, M., Schnitzler, A., and Wojtecki, L. (2014). Effect of subthalamic stimulation on voice and speech in Parkinson’s disease: for the better or worse? Front. Neurol. 4:218. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörös, P., Sokoloff, L. G., Bose, A., McIntosh, A. R., Graham, S. J., and Stuss, D. T. (2006). Clustered functional MRI of overt speech production. Neuroimage 32, 376–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

te Woerd, E. S., Oostenveld, R., de Lange, F. P., and Praamstra, P. (2014). A shift from prospective to reactive modulation of beta-band oscillations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage 100, 507–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuboi, T., Watanabe, H., Tanaka, Y., Ohdake, R., Yoneyama, N., Hara, K., et al. (2015). Distinct phenotypes of speech and voice disorders in Parkinson’s disease after subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 856–864. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-308043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuller, B., Harris, K. S., and Gross, B. (1981). Electromyographic study of the jaw muscles during speech. J. Phon. 9, 175–188.

Google Scholar

van Wijk, B. C., Beek, P. J., and Daffertshofer, A. (2012). Neural synchrony within the motor system: what have we learned so far? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:252. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichmann, T., DeLong, M. R., Guridi, J., and Obeso, J. A. (2011). Milestones in research on the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 1032–1041. doi: 10.1002/mds.23695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wojtecki, L., Elben, S., Vesper, J., and Schnitzler, A. (2017). The rhythm of the executive gate of speech: subthalamic low-frequency oscillations increase during verbal generation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 1200–1211. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, D. (2001). A method to standardize a reference of scalp EEG recordings to a point at infinity. Physiol. Meas. 22, 693–711. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/22/4/305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, D., Wang, L., Oostenveld, R., Nielsen, K. D., Arendt-Nielsen, L., and Chen, A. C. (2005). A comparative study of different references for EEG spectral mapping: the issue of the neutral reference and the use of the infinity reference. Physiol. Meas. 26, 173–184. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/26/3/003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, speech production, brain rhythms, β-band, EEG

Citation: Sörös P, Doñamayor N, Wittke C, Al-Khaled M, Brüggemann N and Münte TF (2017) Increase in Beta-Band Activity during Preparation for Overt Speech in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:371. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00371

Received: 11 February 2017; Accepted: 03 July 2017;
Published: 24 July 2017.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, United States

Reviewed by:

Dezhong Yao, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China
Bahar Güntekin, School of International Medicine, Istanbul Medipol University, Turkey

Copyright © 2017 Sörös, Doñamayor, Wittke, Al-Khaled, Brüggemann and Münte. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter Sörös, peter.soros@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.