Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 March 2016
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Online and Offline Modulators of Motor Learning View all 15 articles

A Day Awake Attenuates Motor Learning-Induced Increases in Corticomotor Excitability

  • 1Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 2Child Development Center and Pediatric Sleep Disorders Center, University Children’s Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Neural Control of Movement Laboratory, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, Eidgenössische Technische Hochschule Zurich (ETH Zürich), Zurich, Switzerland

The “synaptic homeostasis hypothesis” proposes that the brain’s capacity to exhibit synaptic plasticity is reduced during the day but restores when sleeping. While this prediction has been confirmed for declarative memories, it is currently unknown whether it is also the case for motor memories. We quantified practice-induced changes in corticomotor excitability in response to repetitive motor sequence training as an indirect marker of synaptic plasticity in the primary motor cortex (M1). Subjects either practiced a motor sequence in the morning and a new motor sequence in the evening, i.e., after a 12 h period of wakefulness (wake group); or they practiced a sequence in the evening and a new sequence in the morning, i.e., after a 12 h period including sleep (sleep group). In both wake and sleep groups motor training improved movement performance irrespective of the time of day. Learning a new sequence in the morning triggered a clear increase in corticomotor excitability suggesting that motor training triggered synaptic adaptation in the M1 that was absent when a new sequence was learned in the evening. Thus, the magnitude of the practice-induced increase in corticomotor excitability was significantly influenced by time of day while the magnitude of motor performance improvements were not. These results suggest that the motor cortex’s potential to efficiently adapt to the environment by quickly adjusting synaptic strength in an activity-dependent manner is higher in the morning than in the evening.

Introduction

The synaptic homeostasis hypothesis (Tononi and Cirelli, 2003, 2006) assumes that a net increase in synaptic strength occurs when awake due to long-term potentiation (LTP) triggered by learning (Muellbacher et al., 2002; Silva, 2003; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a) or due to synaptic plasticity reflecting statistical regularities of the environment experienced during wakefulness (Cirelli and Tononi, 2000; Tononi and Cirelli, 2001, 2003; Huber et al., 2013). This increase in synaptic strength is believed to reduce neuronal selectivity, i.e., firing in response to a specific stimulus, but also limits the capacity to undergo further synaptic plasticity (saturation of learning capabilities; Tononi and Cirelli, 2003, 2006, 2014). The synaptic homeostasis hypothesis predicts that sleep “downscales” or renormalizes the overall synaptic strength hereby improving signal-to-noise ratio and restoring the brain’s energy balance and cellular homeostasis (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). Using a plausible computational model of sleep-dependent renormalization, it has been predicted that the human brain’s ability to form new memories is hereby renormalized in the morning following sleep (Olcese et al., 2010). In accordance with this latter prediction, behavioral studies testing the formation of declarative memories showed that sleep was beneficial for memory consolidation (Born et al., 2006; Gais et al., 2006) and that learning capacity was higher in the morning (i.e., after 12 h including sleep) than in the evening (i.e., after 12 h without sleep; Kvint et al., 2011). Moreover, sleep deprivation caused a substantial impairment in learning capacity (McDermott et al., 2003; Yoo et al., 2007; Mander et al., 2011). By contrast, for motor learning, and most notably for sequence learning, it has been shown that while sleep is beneficial for consolidation and retention performance, particularly when performance saturation was reached during prior training (Kvint et al., 2011), behavioral measurements of sequence learning capacity did not differ in the morning compared to the evening (Fischer et al., 2002; Walker et al., 2002; Brawn et al., 2010; Kvint et al., 2011; Sale et al., 2013).

Here, we test the prediction that motor learning-induced synaptic plasticity is attenuated after a period of wakefulness. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was used to estimate a person’s capacity to undergo synaptic plasticity in the primary motor cortex (M1) either after 12 h of wakefulness or after the same period including sleep. Synaptic plasticity was probed in response to repetitive training of a five-element motor sequence which has been shown to modify the functional organization of the motor system, a phenomenon known as use-dependent plasticity (Classen et al., 1998; Muellbacher et al., 2002; Ziemann et al., 2004; Rosenkranz and Rothwell, 2006; Stefan et al., 2006; Perez et al., 2007; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a,b; Huang et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2011; Bisio et al., 2015).

In humans, use-dependent plasticity within M1 is indicated by larger motor-evoked potentials (MEP) after training than at baseline (Classen et al., 1998; Perez et al., 2007; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a,b). This increase in corticomotor excitability most likely results from training-induced synaptic plasticity leading to strengthening of intracortical neuronal ensembles (Rioult-Pedotti et al., 1998, 2000; Bütefisch et al., 2000), it is N-Methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-dependent and it is strongly reduced by γ-aminobutyric acid type A (GABAA) receptor mediated inhibition (Bütefisch et al., 2000). Moreover, use-dependent plasticity occluded subsequent induction of LTP via paired-associative stimulation (PAS) protocols in accordance to principles of homeostatic metaplasticity as predicted by the Bienenstock-Cooper-Munro theory (Kirkwood et al., 1996; Stefan et al., 2006; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a), thus suggesting that this type of learning saturates synaptic plasticity. Together, these findings strongly suggest that use-dependent plasticity activates LTP-like mechanisms in humans (Bütefisch et al., 2000; Stefan et al., 2006; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a) which are reflected by changes in corticomotor excitability.

The synaptic homeostasis hypothesis predicts that the brain’s capacity to undergo synaptic plasticity is reduced after a prolonged period awake, while this ability is restored after a night of sleep. In line with this theory, we hypothesize that inducing use-dependent plasticity in the morning by practicing one motor sequence will result in larger increases in corticomotor excitability than practicing a new motor sequence in the evening because overall synaptic strengthening during the waking day will diminish the potential to further increase synaptic efficiency.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Nineteen naïve (no musicians, no prior experience with the task), healthy, right-handed (Oldfield, 1971) subjects (1 female, mean ± SD age; 21.9 ± 1.1 years) participated in this experiment. All subjects signed a written informed consent prior to participation and were screened for adverse reactions to TMS when they complied with the inclusion criteria. The experimental procedure was approved by the local Ethics Committee for Biomedical Research at the Catholic University of Leuven in accordance to The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Helsinki, 1964).

General Setup

Participants were seated in a comfortable chair with their right forearm resting in a neutral position and performed the behavioral task on a laptop positioned in front of them (for details see below). Subjects wore a tight fitting swimming cap which allowed to outline the TMS coil position and helped placing the TMS coil appropriately in each session.

Electromyographic Recordings (EMG) and TMS

EMG recordings and TMS acquisition were performed in accordance to a standard protocol described in Alaerts et al. (2011). Focal TMS was applied with a 70 mm figure-of-eight coil connected to a Magstim 200 stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, Dyfed, UK). The coil was positioned over M1 of the left hemisphere, tangential to the scalp with the handle pointing backwards and laterally at 45° away from the mid-sagittal line (Pascual-Leone et al., 2002). The optimal scalp position (“hotspot”) for stimulating the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and its rest motor threshold (rMT; lowest stimulus intensity evoking MEPs with an amplitude of at least 50 μV in 5 out of 10 consecutive stimuli) were determined (Rossini et al., 1994; Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject data.

Disposable Ag-AgCl surface electrodes (Blue sensor SP Surface) were used to record EMG from the FDI. The first electrode was placed on the belly, the second on the tendon of the muscle and a third on a bony prominence (reference electrode). The signals were sampled at 5000 Hz (CED Power 1401, Cambridge Electronic Design, UK), amplified, band-pass filtered (5–1000 Hz), and stored on a PC for offline analysis. Pre-stimulus EMG recordings were used to assess the presence of unwanted background EMG activity in the 110–10 ms time interval preceding the magnetic pulse.

Corticomotor excitability was quantified by measuring input-output curves (IO curve) using 90, 115, 140, 165 and 190% of rMT. One IO curve consisted of 20 MEPs per intensity. They were acquired in two blocks of 50 MEPs so that per block 10 stimulations were acquired for each of the five intensities. In between blocks a rest period of approximately 2 min was provided. Within one block, the interstimulation interval ranged from 5–9 s resulting in a total block time of 6 min 30 s.

Behavioral Task

Subjects performed a computerized sequence tapping task (presented with E-Prime; Psychology Software Tools, Inc. Sharpsburg, PA, USA) adapted from Karni et al. (1998). The sequence to be executed was depicted on top of the laptop screen using a numbering system, with 1, 2, 3, and 4 corresponding to the index, middle, ring and little fingers of the right hand respectively. Throughout the experiment three different yet equally difficult sequences were used (A: 4–1–3–2–4; B: 2–3–1–4–2; C: 3–4–2–1–3). While tapping the sequence a black dot appeared on the screen below the current number every time the subject pressed a key indicating that a response was recorded without giving any accuracy feedback (Figure 1A). When a sequence was completed, the screen was refreshed so that the same sequence appeared on top without any black dots present. One experimental trial consisted of typing the given sequence for 30 s as many times as possible followed by a rest period of 30 s to prevent fatigue.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the finger sequencing task and experimental protocol. (A) The motor task is performed with the right dominant hand on a laptop keyboard. Three different five-element sequences are used throughout the experiment, each consisting of four numeric keys (A: 4–1–3–2–4; B: 2–3–1–4–2; C: 3–4–2–1–3). Each number represented a finger; with “1” being the index finger, “2” the middle finger, etc. The sequence to be executed (e.g., sequence A) was shown on the computer screen using the same numbering system to reduce the likelihood of the task including a working memory component. While performing the task, a black dot appeared on the screen indicating that a key had been pressed. Key presses were recorded and no feedback was provided regarding task accuracy. An experimental trial consisted of 30 s of sequence tapping followed by a rest period of 30 s to prevent fatigue. During the practice sessions, this experimental trial is repeated 12 times so that subjects are mass trained for 12 min in total. Participants were instructed to type the sequences as quickly and as accurately as possible and were motivated continuously throughout the experiment. (B) In both groups, we first determined the rest motor threshold (rMT) and hotspot for each subject. In the wake group, subjects are first tested at 8 a.m. and IO curves are measured before (yellow) and after training (red) the finger sequencing task for 12 min (e.g., sequence B). At 8 p.m., following a normal day, subjects are tested in an identical manner as during the morning session, however they are trained on a different sequence (e.g., sequence C). In the sleep group the first session takes place at 8 p.m. when IO curves are measured before (light blue) and after (dark blue) they practiced the finger sequencing task (e.g., sequence B). After a night’s sleep, they are tested again using the same procedure at 8 a.m. yet practicing a novel sequence (e.g., sequence C).

Experimental Protocol

Subjects participated in a familiarization session, first practice session and second practice session. Each session required the acquisition of a new motor sequence (i.e., A, B or C with the order randomized across participants) which was repeatedly practiced within that session. During the familiarization session TMS was used to determine the FDI hotspot and rMT. Afterwards three experimental trials were performed (i.e., 30 s tapping of e.g., sequence A, followed by 30 s rest) which lasted 3 min in total. Subjects were then randomly assigned to one of two experimental groups.

The first experimental group, the wake group, started their first session at 8 a.m. (Figure 1B). The FDI hotspot and rMT were determined and corticomotor excitability was measured in the form of an IO curve. Subjects then performed motor training, i.e., they practiced a new motor sequence (e.g., sequence B) for 12 experimental trials (i.e., 30 s tapping followed by 30 s rest) which lasted 12 min in total. Subjects then left the lab and followed their typical daily routine and returned for their second session at 8 p.m., which followed the identical procedure but, importantly, a new motor sequence was acquired (e.g., C).

A similar procedure was followed in the second experimental group, the sleep group, but the first practice session took place in the evening at 8 p.m. After this first session subjects went home for a night of sleep and returned to the lab at 8 a.m. the following morning for the second session. The presentation of sequences A, B, and C was randomly assigned to familiarization, session 1 and session 2, and differed across subjects.

Before and during the testing day(s), subjects did not perform strenuous exercise, had no more than two cups of coffee a day and followed their normal sleep rhythm without taking additional naps during the day (as instructed and verified via self-report; Table 1).

Data Analysis and Statistics

Key presses were recorded and accuracy (%) was calculated as the number of correct sequences divided by all completed sequences during each 30 s trial. Performance speed was measured as the time (s) between key presses, i.e., the inter-tap interval (ITI). A performance score was calculated for each subject and trial by dividing the accuracy percentage by the ITI, with higher scores indicating better performance (also see de Beukelaar et al., 2014). A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on performance scores with the between-subject factor group (wake, sleep) and the within-subject factors session (1st, 2nd) and training block (trial 1–12).

Corticomotor excitability was quantified by MEP peak-to-peak amplitude. MEP amplitude is known to be modulated by EMG background activation since slight voluntary contractions of the target muscle might increase MEP amplitude (Barker et al., 1986, 1987; Hess et al., 1987; Rothwell et al., 1987; Devanne et al., 1997; Nollet et al., 2003). Therefore pre-stimulus EMG recordings were used to assess the presence of unwanted background EMG activity in the 110–10 ms preceding the magnetic pulse and were quantified via root mean square scores (RMS) across this interval. For each subject and over all trials we calculated the mean and standard deviations of the background EMG so that values over + 2.5 standard deviation were removed from the analysis. Furthermore we considered MEP peak-to-peak amplitudes which exceeded Q3 + 1.5 × (Q3 & Q1) as outliers (3.1%) that were removed from further analysis, with Q1 denoting the first quartile and Q3 the third quartile computed over the whole set of trials for each subject. MEP amplitudes were averaged for each stimulation intensity of each IO curve that was recorded and these averages where then subjected to group statistics.

We first tested whether motor practice changed corticomotor excitability as quantified by the IO curve and whether these changes would differ between the first and second session. This analysis was performed separately for each experimental group using a repeated measures ANOVA (rmANOVA) with the within-subject factors session (1st, 2nd), pre-post (pre, post) and intensity (90, 115, 140, 165 and 190%). Next we tested whether baseline corticomotor excitability (i.e., measured prior to motor practice) changed from the first to the second session and calculated for each group a rmANOVA for the IO curve measured at pre, using the factors session (1st, 2nd) and intensity (90, 115, 140, 165 and 190%).

Finally we directly compared whether changes in corticomotor excitability induced by motor practice differed between sessions and groups. Therefore, we calculated the integral underneath the IO curve measured before and after motor practice (Carson et al., 2013), and calculated a facilitation index (FacInd) by:

FacInd = Intensity1−5MEPpost/Intensity1−5MEPpre

FacInd > 1 indicates that an increase in corticomotor excitability is observed from pre to post training, while a FacInd < 1 represents a decrease. The FacInds were calculated for the two sessions and the two groups and were entered into a repeated measures ANOVA with the between-subject factor group (wake, sleep) and within-subject factor session (1st, 2nd).

The alpha level for all statistical tests was set to 0.05 and significant interactions were further analyzed by the use of a Fisher’s LSD post hoc analysis. All statistical analyses were performed with Statistica 8 (StatSoft, OK, USA).

Results

Behavioral Results

Both groups improved motor sequence performance during each of the practice sessions to a similar extent (Figure 2). Accordingly, statistics revealed a main effect for training block (F(11,187) = 34.61; p < 0.001) but no main effect for group or a higher interaction containing the factor group (F < 2.56; p > 0.12). Additionally, performance was generally better in session 2 (310.52 ± 91.74) than in session 1 (297.97 ± 100.79) as indicated by a significant session main effect (F(1,17) = 5.01; p < 0.05). However, there was no statistical evidence to suggest that learning gains were differential influenced be waking or sleeping since the session × trial interaction failed to reach significance (F(11,187) = 0.78; p = 0.66).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Behavioral data of the wake and sleep group during both practice sessions. For both groups and in each session a separate learning curve is shown for the sequence tapping task. There was a significant main effect for training block (F(11,187) = 34.61; p < 0.001), yet no main effect for group or a higher interaction containing this group factor was found (F < 2.56; p > 0.12). A main effect for session (F(1,17) = 5.01; p < 0.05) indicates that during the second session a higher performance is achieved in both groups. §Represents a main training block effect (p < 0.001). Vertical bars indicate standard errors (SEs).

Neural Results

Wake Group

In the wake group, corticomotor excitability increased due to practice in the morning session while no such increase is seen in the evening session (Figure 3A). This is supported by a significant session × prepost × intensity interaction (F(4,32) = 3.31; p < 0.05) and by follow up analyses revealing a significant prepost × intensity interaction (F(4,32) = 4.23; p < 0.01) for the morning session, while significance was not reached in the evening session (F(4,32) = 2.54; p = 0.06). This indicates that motor practice changed corticomotor excitability more strongly in the morning than in the evening.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Neural data. (A) Input-0utput (IO) curves pre- and post-training in the first and the second practice session for both wake and sleep group. In the wake group (left panel) we show an increase in corticomotor excitability from pre-training (yellow curve) to post-training (red curve) in the morning session at 8 a.m. (F(4,32) = 4.23; p < 0.01) but not in the evening session at 8 p.m. A Fisher LSD post hoc analysis shows that this effect in the morning is found for suprathreshold stimulation with intensities ≥ 115% rMT. In the sleep group (right panel) we show no increase in corticomotor excitability from pre-training (light blue curve) to post-training (dark blue curve) in their first session being the evening session at 8 p.m. During the second session on the consecutive morning at 8 a.m., a significant increase in excitability is seen from pre- to post-training (F(4,36) = 2.68; p < 0.05), especially for supratreshold stimulation with intensities ≥ 140% rMT. (B) IO curves obtained pre-training in both sessions for both wake and sleep group. For the wake group an increase during the day is observed since the pre-training curve obtained in the evening session is increased compared to the pre curve in the morning (F(4,32) = 5.96; p < 0.01), especially for supratreshold intensities ≥ 165% rMT. For the sleep group no difference between both pre curves is observed (p = 0.27). #Indicates a prepost × intensity interaction effect (p < 0.05); §Indicates a session × intensity interaction effect (p < 0.01); significant Fisher LSD post hoc analyses are represented by *p < 0.001 and **p < 0.0001. Vertical bars indicate SEs.

When comparing the pre-training IO curves between the two experimental sessions, baseline excitability increased over a 12 h-day awake as indicated by a significant session × intensity interaction for the pre curves of both sessions (F(4,32) = 5.96; p < 0.01; Figure 3B).

Sleep Group

In the sleep group, motor practice did not cause a significant increase of corticomotor excitability in the evening session (prepost × intensity interaction: F(4,36) = 0.45; p = 0.77) while a significant increase was observed during the following morning session (F(4,36) = 2.68; p < 0.05), i.e., after a night of sleep (Figure 3A). However, the session × prepost × intensity interaction did not reach significance most likely due to large inter-individual variability (F(4,36) = 1.94; p = 0.13).

When investigating the evolution of the pre-training IO curves, we found no session × intensity interaction in the sleep group indicating that there was no significant change in baseline excitability overnight (F(4,36) = 1.34; p = 0.27; Figure 3B).

FacInd

The FacInd was calculated to directly test whether the potential to undergo changes in corticomotor excitability differed when practice sessions were either separated by 12 h awake (wake group) or 12 h including sleep (sleep group). Figure 4 shows that the FacInd of the wake group was higher in the morning (indicating that excitability changed by approximately 14.4 ± 18.6% in response to motor practice) than in the evening (excitability changes were only 4.4 ± 13.5%). The sleep group, by contrast, exhibited the opposite pattern with a lower FacInd in the evening (−1.3 ± 8.1%) than the next morning (22.7 ± 31.8%), i.e., after a night of sleep. Importantly, statistics revealed a significant group × session interaction (F(1,8) = 5.36; p < 0.05) suggesting that wakefulness decreases the ability to change corticomotor excitability in response to motor practice whereas the ability to exhibit use-dependent neural changes was re-established at the next morning after a night of sleep.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Facilitation Index (FacInd). The FacInd is calculated as a general measurement quantifying the potential to undergo learning-induced changes of corticomotor excitability. We divide the integrated motor-evoked potentials (MEP) amplitudes collected for each intensity after practice (i.e., area under the IO curve post-training) by the integrated MEP amplitudes of those collected before (i.e., area under the IO curve pre-training). This calculation shows that the potential to exhibit training-induced neural changes is dependent on time of day and is higher in the morning compared to the evening in both experimental groups *p = 0.052. #Indicates a group × session interaction effect (p < 0.05); marginally significant Fisher LSD post hoc analysis is represented by *p = 0.052. Vertical bars indicate SEs.

Discussion

In the present study, we tested the prediction that motor learning-induced synaptic plasticity is attenuated after a period of wakefulness. The capacity to undergo synaptic plasticity was probed by measuring changes in corticomotor excitability in response to acquiring a finger sequence tapping task, a learning paradigm that is well-known to induce use-dependent plasticity in M1. Our main finding is that the capacity to increase corticomotor excitability in response to motor practice (a marker of training-induced synaptic strengthening) is reduced after 12 h of wakefulness during the day.

Behavioral Data

In this study, we used a sequence tapping task to induce use-dependent plasticity. The advantage of this task is that a similar learning process can be induced twice, a requirement of our experimental design. In order to account for potential differences in complexity, we randomized the presentation of sequences in a balanced order. Furthermore, a familiarization session was performed, so that subjects knew the general paradigm in order to minimize novelty effects.

Unlike the neural measurements, the behavioral data did not show differential performance gains in the morning compared to the evening sessions. It is important to note that task performance is not a “pure” measurement of memory formation because it is strongly influenced by fatigue, attention, alertness and motivation (Karni et al., 1998; Robertson et al., 2004a,b). For the sequence tapping task used here it is well-known that time of day does not result in differential motor learning gains (Fischer et al., 2002; Walker et al., 2003; Brawn et al., 2010), and that the beneficial effect of sleep has only been demonstrated for retention performance (i.e., an indirect marker of consolidation) but not for restoring motor learning capacity. Thus, the absence of behavioral differences between morning and evening sessions when estimating sequence learning is highly consistent with previous findings.

Furthermore, the lack of differential behavioral findings between morning and evening sessions could be explained by the overall simplicity of the sequence tapping task in combination with the relative short acquisition phase in relation to the total training time. More specifically, Walker et al. (2002) reported an overall performance increase for sequence tapping of 59.3% over 12 × 30 s training trials with the largest increase occurring during the first 3 training trials (38.8%). Therefore, shortening the training period (e.g., less and/or shorter training trials) to prevent subjects to reach a performance plateau by the end of training could potentially be a more sensitive procedure to reflect more subtle time of day effects on behavior. Note also that the behavioral performance measurements are likely to reflect different learning processes: initially, skill acquisition ensures that the sequence is correctly represented at the neural level and that it is fluently performed which might cause large gains early in learning. By contrast, in a later phase, repetitive practice of the sequence is likely to activate mechanisms related to use-dependent plasticity; i.e., neural changes that are induced by extensively repeating movements within a specified time window (Classen et al., 1998; Bütefisch et al., 2000; Stefan et al., 2006; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a). This might explain why memory specific neurophysiological processes are not always accurately reflected by behavioral changes (Urbain et al., 2013).

Influence of Waking vs. Sleep on the Potential for Increasing Corticomotor Excitability in Response to Motor Training

We found that the potential for increasing corticomotor excitability, measured as the difference between IO curves recorded before and after motor practice (similar to Lotze et al., 2003; Perez et al., 2004; Jensen et al., 2005; Stefan et al., 2006; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a,b; Zhang et al., 2011), was higher in the morning than the evening. It is important to keep in mind that we infer synaptic plasticity based on changes of corticomotor excitability as measured by single pulse TMS. This assumption is based on a large body of evidence reporting a robust increase of corticomotor excitability in response to extensive motor practice inducing use-dependent plasticity (Classen et al., 1998; Perez et al., 2007; Rosenkranz et al., 2007a,b), or other plasticity inducing protocols using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS; Romero Lauro et al., 2014), PAS (Stefan et al., 2000; Ridding and Uy, 2003) or theta-burst stimulation (Jacobs et al., 2012). Importantly, this rise in corticomotor excitability after extensive motor training has been shown to be of cortical origin (rather than reflecting changes at e.g., the spinal level; Rioult-Pedotti et al., 1998, 2000; Bütefisch et al., 2000), is specific for motor learning rather than for motor performance (Rosenkranz et al., 2007a), and it is abolished when synaptic plasticity is reduced either by blocking NMDA receptors or by increasing GABAergic inhibition with pharmacological agents (Bütefisch et al., 2000). Although there is compelling evidence that learning a finger sequence tapping task typically results in increased corticomotor excitability early after training, this might not be the case for all motor tasks (e.g., Tunovic et al., 2014) reported a delayed increase in corticomotor excitability). An alternative approach to probe neuroplasticity of human M1 is to experimentally induced LTP (typically by a PASLTP protocol) after motor training has been performed. According to models of homeostatic metaplasticity, the effect of LTP-inducing PAS (i.e., PASLTP) is either reduced or even reversed to long-term depression (LTD) depending on the extent to which synaptic plasticity has been induced by prior motor practice. Combining motor training and PAS protocols is an elegant approach to test synaptic plasticity, however, the efficiency of PASLTP has been shown to be dependent on corticosteroid levels which are typically lowest early in the morning. Accordingly, PASLTP effects have been shown to be significantly smaller in the morning than in the evening (Sale et al., 2008). In the context of our paradigm this represents a potential confound and could therefore not be applied. Note however, that only the response to PASLTP was influenced by corticosteroid levels whereas MEP amplitudes were comparable over the day. Moreover, we controlled other confounding factors like the background EMG across the pre and the post session excluding the possibility that excitability changes were caused by pre-contraction. Therefore, we argue that the increase in corticomotor excitability in response to a standardized practice protocol as quantified by the FacInd is a surrogate marker of a person’s ability to undergo neuroplastic changes at the synaptic level (see also Rosenkranz et al., 2007b). Under this assumption, our data suggest that a day awake decreases the potential to show neural changes due to motor learning.

Our data indicate that there is no causal link between practice-induced changes in corticomotor excitability and practice-induced changes of motor behavior (Bestmann and Krakauer, 2015). It has been suggested that there is no straightforward relationship between MEP size (i.e., IO curve) and behavioral output following learning (Muellbacher et al., 2000, 2001; McDonnell and Ridding, 2006; Bagce et al., 2013). From our data, it is apparent that behavior can improve significantly even though corticomotor excitability remains virtually unchanged (as observed after a day of wakefulness). These short-term changes in corticomotor excitability as obtained in the morning sessions appear to indicate that M1 underwent adaptive changes resulting in increased efficiency of the activated neural network (Bestmann and Krakauer, 2015). In other words, a change in corticomotor excitability is not essential to learn a novel motor task, however, in order to efficiently learn the task the neural system needs to adapt. However, we showed that this capacity to learn and to efficiently adapt to the changing world around us is attenuated after a day of wakefulness. This interpretation is in line with the predictions of the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis proposing that one central function of sleep is to downscale overall synaptic strength, thus maintaining the brain’s efficiency by ensuring that neurons fire sparsely but selectively for important inputs. In this manner energy consumption is maintained at a sustainable level, and most importantly for our study the ability to learn is restored (Yoo et al., 2007).

Potentiation of Synaptic Strength During Wakefulness

Our findings also support the notion that synaptic strength is potentiated during the day (Tononi and Cirelli, 2003, 2006, 2014) since corticomotor excitability measured prior to motor training increased from the morning to the evening in the wake group consistent with previous findings in humans (Huber et al., 2013) and animal models (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008).

Contrary to the wake group, in the sleep group we found only a slight non-significant decrease in baseline corticomotor excitability after a night of sleep. One has to note, though, that the sleep group did not participate in extra motor training during the day and that the synapses of the muscular representation probed with TMS might not have been strongly potentiated prior to the evening motor training. This is a key difference to the wake group since these subjects were exposed to intensive motor training in the morning. Furthermore, TMS stimulates pyramidal neurons in layer 5 transsynaptically, i.e., via interneurons located in layer 2/3 (Di Lazzaro and Ziemann, 2013). Higher MEPs might not only result from synaptic strengthening occurring within M1 but also from potentiated inputs to these M1 interneurons in layer 2/3 deriving from other areas (Bestmann and Krakauer, 2015). One primary candidate area that might have been activated by the tapping task is the striatum which has been shown to be involved in sequence learning and has dense reciprocal connections with M1 (Doyon et al., 2003; Doyon and Benali, 2005). Other likely input areas to M1 that also undergo changes in response to motor practice are parieto-premotor networks (Doyon and Benali, 2005). Even though these areas outside of M1 contribute to all phases of sequence learning it has been suggested that the time course is slightly different: thus while M1 probably undergoes most prominent synaptic changes during and immediately after practice, the striatum is believed to become increasingly more important during memory consolidation, i.e., during the first minutes and hours after the training has finished (Shadmehr and Holcomb, 1997; Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002; Frankland and Bontempi, 2005; Censor et al., 2010). Consequently we propose that short-term changes of corticomotor excitability as observed when comparing pre to post-training measurements might be predominantly driven by fast neuroplastic changes (which certainly involve M1), while long-term changes in corticomotor excitability as observed when comparing baseline excitability between the morning and the evening test might additionally be influenced by slow neuroplastic changes that occurred during consolidation and potentially, also in areas outside of M1.

Interpretational Issues

The present study was designed in light of the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis, i.e., whether motor learning capacity is reduced after a day awake but restored in the morning after a night of sleep. Our results are in line with this prediction; however, the present study design does not allow us to dissociate the influence of sleep from the influence of circadian rhythms. Indeed it has been shown that performance of certain motor tasks show time of day effects (Miller et al., 1992; Wyse et al., 1994; Atkinson and Reilly, 1996; Edwards et al., 2007; Keisler et al., 2007) and it is possible that the ability to undergo changes in corticomotor excitability in response to repetitive motor training is also influenced by circadian rhythms. However, previous studies using plasticity inducing brain stimulation protocols would predict the opposite pattern of results than obtained in our present study (Sale et al., 2010). Future research is needed that objectively measures sleep quality by the use of electroencephalography (EEG) and experimentally modulates slow wave sleep which seems to be most related to synaptic downscaling and investigates whether, for example, slow wave sleep perturbation impacts on the renormalization of motor learning capacity. It is also important to note that we, tested two different groups of subjects. Even though, our groups were well matched regarding age, gender, over day activity and sleeping hours it might be advantageous to use a cross-over design in future studies.

Conclusion

In this study, we show that the learning-induced synaptic plasticity caused by acquiring a finger sequence tapping task decreases after a day awake. Our findings are in line with the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis which states that synaptic strength is potentiated during the day and sleep restores learning capacity by maintaining synaptic potentiation within an optimal range. Hence, sleep ensures that M1 circuits can undergo reorganization to perform the practiced movements with high efficiency; a mechanism which is attenuated with time spent awake. Although our findings are in accordance with this hypothesis, future studies should objectively measure sleep quality and vary sleep independently of time of day to provide more direct evidence regarding the restorative role of sleep in synaptic homeostasis.

Author Contributions

TTdeB designed the study; collected, analyzed and interpreted the data; drafted and revised the manuscript; gave final approval. JVS collected, analyzed and interpreted the data; drafted and revised the manuscript; gave final approval. RH interpreted the data; revised the manuscript; gave final approval. NW designed the study; interpreted the data; revised the manuscript; gave final approval.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Research Foundation—Flanders (grant numbers G.0401.12) and a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (project number 320030_149561). TTdeB is a predoctoral fellow of the Research Foundation-Flanders and was additionally supported by a JUMO grant (14/042).

References

Alaerts, K., Swinnen, S. P., and Wenderoth, N. (2011). Action perception in individuals with congenital blindness or deafness: how does the loss of a sensory modality from birth affect perception-induced motor facilitation? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1080–1087. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, G., and Reilly, T. (1996). Circadian variation in sports performance. Sports Med. 21, 292–312. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199621040-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagce, H. F., Saleh, S., Adamovich, S. V., Krakauer, J. W., and Tunik, E. (2013). Corticospinal excitability is enhanced after visuomotor adaptation and depends on learning rather than performance of error. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 1097–1106. doi: 10.1152/jn.00304.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, A. T., Freeston, I. L., Jalinous, R., and Jarratt, J. A. (1986). Clinical evaluation of conduction time measurements in central motor pathways using magnetic stimulation of human brain. Lancet 1, 1325–1326. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(86)91243-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, A. T., Freeston, I. L., Jalinous, R., and Jarratt, J. A. (1987). Magnetic stimulation of the human brain and peripheral nervous system: an introduction and the results of an initial clinical evaluation. Neurosurgery 20, 100–109. doi: 10.1097/00006123-198701000-00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bestmann, S., and Krakauer, J. W. (2015). The use and interpretations of the motor-evoked potential for understanding behavior. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 679–689. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4183-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisio, A., Avanzino, L., Gueugneau, N., Pozzo, T., Ruggeri, P., and Bove, M. (2015). Observing and perceiving: a combined approach to induce plasticity in human motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1212–1220. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Born, J., Rasch, B., and Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember. Neuroscientist 12, 410–424. doi: 10.1177/1073858406292647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brawn, T. P., Fenn, K. M., Nusbaum, H. C., and Margoliash, D. (2010). Consolidating the effects of waking and sleep on motor-sequence learning. J. Neurosci. 30, 13977–13982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3295-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bütefisch, C. M., Davis, B. C., Wise, S. P., Sawaki, L., Kopylev, L., Classen, J., et al. (2000). Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 3661–3665. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, R. G., Nelson, B. D., Buick, A. R., Carroll, T. J., Kennedy, N. C., and Cann, R. M. (2013). Characterizing changes in the excitability of corticospinal projections to proximal muscles of the upper limb. Brain Stimul. 6, 760–768. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Censor, N., Dimyan, M. A., and Cohen, L. G. (2010). Modification of existing human motor memories is enabled by primary cortical processing during memory reactivation. Curr. Biol. 20, 1545–1549. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.07.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2000). Differential expression of plasticity-related genes in waking and sleep and their regulation by the noradrenergic system. J. Neurosci. 20, 9187–9194.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Classen, J., Liepert, J., Wise, S. P., Hallett, M., and Cohen, L. G. (1998). Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 1117–1123.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

de Beukelaar, T. T., Woolley, D. G., and Wenderoth, N. (2014). Gone for 60 seconds: reactivation length determines motor memory degradation during reconsolidation. Cortex 59, 138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devanne, H., Lavoie, B. A., and Capaday, C. (1997). Input-output properties and gain changes in the human corticospinal pathway. Exp. Brain Res. 114, 329–338. doi: 10.1007/pl00005641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Lazzaro, V., and Ziemann, U. (2013). The contribution of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the functional evaluation of microcircuits in human motor cortex. Front. Neural Circuits 7:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyon, J., and Benali, H. (2005). Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyon, J., Penhune, V., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2003). Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia 41, 252–262. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00158-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyon, J., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). “Functional anatomy of motor skill learning,” in Neuropsychology of Memory, eds L. R. Squire and D. L. Schacter (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 225–238.

Google Scholar

Edwards, B., Waterhouse, J., and Reilly, T. (2007). The effects of circadian rhythmicity and time-awake on a simple motor task. Chronobiol. Int. 24, 1109–1124. doi: 10.1080/07420520701795316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A. L., and Born, J. (2002). Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 99, 11987–11991. doi: 10.1073/pnas.182178199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., and Bontempi, B. (2005). The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 119–130. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gais, S., Lucas, B., and Born, J. (2006). Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learn. Mem. 13, 259–262. doi: 10.1101/lm.132106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, C. W., Mills, K. R., and Murray, N. M. (1987). Responses in small hand muscles from magnetic stimulation of the human brain. J. Physiol. 388, 397–419. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, V. S., Haith, A., Mazzoni, P., and Krakauer, J. W. (2011). Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms: model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal models. Neuron 70, 787–801. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Mäki, H., Rosanova, M., Casarotto, S., Canali, P., Casali, A. G., et al. (2013). Human cortical excitability increases with time awake. Cereb. Cortex 23, 332–338. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, M., Premji, A., and Nelson, A. J. (2012). Plasticity-inducing TMS protocols to investigate somatosensory control of hand function. Neural Plast. 2012:350574. doi: 10.1155/2012/350574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, J. L., Marstrand, P. C., and Nielsen, J. B. (2005). Motor skill training and strength training are associated with different plastic changes in the central nervous system. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 99, 1558–1568. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01408.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., et al. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 861–868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keisler, A., Ashe, J., and Willingham, D. T. (2007). Time of day accounts for overnight improvements in sequence learning. Learn. Mem. 14, 669–672. doi: 10.1101/lm.751807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkwood, A., Rioult, M. C., and Bear, M. F. (1996). Experience-dependent modification of synaptic plasticity in visual cortex. Nature 381, 526–528. doi: 10.1038/381526a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kvint, S., Bassiri, B., Pruski, A., Nia, J., Nemet, I., Lopresti, M., et al. (2011). Acquisition and retention of motor sequences: the effects of time of the day and sleep. Arch. Ital. Biol. 149, 303–312. doi: 10.4449/aib.v149i3.1244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotze, M., Braun, C., Birbaumer, N., Anders, S., and Cohen, L. G. (2003). Motor learning elicited by voluntary drive. Brain 126, 866–872. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mander, B. A., Santhanam, S., Saletin, J. M., and Walker, M. P. (2011). Wake deterioration and sleep restoration of human learning. Curr. Biol. 21, R183–R184. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, C. M., LaHoste, G. J., Chen, C., Musto, A., Bazan, N. G., and Magee, J. C. (2003). Sleep deprivation causes behavioural, synaptic and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 23, 9687–9695.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McDonnell, M. N., and Ridding, M. C. (2006). Transient motor evoked potential suppression following a complex sensorimotor task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1266–1272. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L. S., Lombardo, T. W., and Fowler, S. C. (1992). Time of day effects on a human force discrimination task. Physiol. Behav. 52, 839–841. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90359-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muellbacher, W., Ziemann, U., Boroojerdi, B., Cohen, L., and Hallett, M. (2001). Role of the human motor cortex in rapid motor learning. Exp. Brain Res. 136, 431–438. doi: 10.1007/s002210000614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muellbacher, W., Ziemann, U., Boroojerdi, B., and Hallett, M. (2000). Effects of low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on motor excitability and basic motor behavior. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1002–1007. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00284-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muellbacher, W., Ziemann, U., Wissel, J., Dang, N., Kofler, M., Facchini, S., et al. (2002). Early consolidation in human primary motor cortex. Nature 415, 640–644. doi: 10.1038/nature712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nollet, H., Van Ham, L., Deprez, P., and Vanderstraeten, G. (2003). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: review of the technique, basic principles and applications. Vet. J. 166, 28–42. doi: 10.1016/s1090-0233(03)00025-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olcese, U., Esser, S. K., and Tononi, G. (2010). Sleep and synaptic renormalization: a computational study. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 3476–3493. doi: 10.1152/jn.00593.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., Manoach, D. S., Birnbaum, R., and Goff, D. C. (2002). Motor cortical excitability in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 24–31. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01317-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, M. A., Lungholt, B. K., Nyborg, K., and Nielsen, J. B. (2004). Motor skill training induces changes in the excitability of the leg cortical area in healthy humans. Exp. Brain Res. 159, 197–205. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1947-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Perez, M. A., Wise, S. P., Willingham, D. T., and Cohen, L. G. (2007). Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in transfer of procedural knowledge. J. Neurosci. 27, 1045–1053. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4128-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridding, M. C., and Uy, J. (2003). Changes in motor cortical excitability induced by paired associative stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 1437–1444. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00115-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rioult-Pedotti, M. S., Friedman, D., Hess, G., and Donoghue, J. P. (1998). Strengthening of horizontal cortical connections following skill learning. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 230–234. doi: 10.1038/678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rioult-Pedotti, M. S., Friedman, D., Hess, G., and Donoghue, J. P. (2000). Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science 290, 533–536. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5491.533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Miall, R. C. (2004a). Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Press, D. Z. (2004b). Awareness modifies the skill-learning benefits of sleep. Curr. Biol. 14, 208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero Lauro, L. J., Rosanova, M., Mattavelli, G., Convento, S., Pisoni, A., Opitz, A., et al. (2014). TDCS increases cortical excitability: direct evidence from TMS-EEG. Cortex 58, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenkranz, K., Kacar, A., and Rothwell, J. C. (2007a). Differential modulation of motor cortical plasticity and excitability in early and late phases of human motor learning. J. Neurosci. 27, 12058–12066. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2663-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenkranz, K., Williamon, A., and Rothwell, J. C. (2007b). Motocortical excitability and synaptic plasticity is enhanced in professional musicians. J. Neurosci. 27, 5200–5206. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0836-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenkranz, K., and Rothwell, J. C. (2006). Differences between the effects of three plasticity inducing protocols on the organisation of the human motor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 822–829. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04605.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Barker, A. T., Berardelli, A., Caramia, M. D., Caruso, G., Cracco, R. Q., et al. (1994). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91, 79–92. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90029-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothwell, J. C., Thompson, P. D., Day, B. L., Dick, J. P. R., Kachi, T., Cowan, J. M. A., et al. (1987). Motor cortical stimulation in intact man. I. General characteristics of EMG responses in different muscles. Brain 110, 1173–1190. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.5.1173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Sale, M. V., Ridding, M. C., and Nordstrom, M. A. (2008). Cortisol inhibits neuroplasticity induction in human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 8285–8293. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1963-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sale, M. V., Ridding, M. C., and Nordstrom, M. A. (2010). Circadian modulation of neuroplasticity in humans and potential therapeutic implications. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 55–66. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2010.21.1.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sale, M. A., Ridding, M. C., and Nordstrom, M. A. (2013). Time of day does not modulate improvements in motor performance following a repetitive ballistic motor training task. Neural Plast. 2013:396865. doi: 10.1155/2013/396865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., and Holcomb, H. H. (1997). Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science 277, 821–825. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5327.821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, A. J. (2003). Molecular and cellular cognitive studies of the role of synaptic plasticity in memory. J. Neurobiol. 54, 224–237. doi: 10.1002/neu.10169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefan, K., Kunesch, E., Cohen, L. G., Benecke, R., and Classen, J. (2000). Induction of plasticity in the human motor cortex by paired associative stimulation. Brain 123, 572–584. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.3.572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefan, K., Wycislo, M., Gentner, R., Schramm, A., Naumann, M., Reiners, K., et al. (2006). Temporary occlusion of associative motor cortical plasticity by prior dynamic motor training. Cereb. Cortex 16, 376–385. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2001). Some considerations on sleep and neural plasticity. Arch. Ital. Biol. 139, 221–241.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2003). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res. Bull. 62, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 10, 49–62. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2014). Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tunovic, S., Press, D. Z., and Robertson, E. M. (2014). A physiological signal that prevents motor skill improvements during consolidation. J. Neurosci. 34, 5302–5310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3497-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urbain, C., Schmitz, R., Schmidt, C., Cleeremans, A., Van Bogaert, P., Maquet, P., et al. (2013). Sleep-dependent neurophysiological processes in implicit sequence learning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 2003–2014. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., Pfister-Genskow, M., Faraguna, U., and Tononi, G. (2008). Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 200–208. doi: 10.1038/nn2035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., and Stickgold, R. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425, 616–620. doi: 10.1038/nature01930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Morgan, A., Hobson, J. A., and Stickgold, R. (2002). Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron 35, 205–211. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00746-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wyse, J. P., Mercer, T. H., and Gleeson, N. P. (1994). Time-of-day dependence of isokinetic leg strength and associated interday variability. Br. J. Sports Med. 28, 167–170. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.28.3.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, S. S., Hu, P. T., Gujar, N., Jolesz, F. A., and Walker, M. P. (2007). A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 385–392. doi: 10.1038/nn1851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., de Beukelaar, T. T., Possel, J., Olaerts, M., Swinnen, S. P., Woolley, D. G., et al. (2011). Movement observation improves early consolidation of motor memory. J. Neurosci. 31, 11515–11520. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6759-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziemann, U., Iliać, T. V., Pauli, C., Meintzschel, F., and Ruge, D. (2004). Learning modifies subsequent induction of long-term potentiation-like and long-term depression-like plasticity in human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 1666–1672. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5016-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: synaptic homeostasis hypothesis, transcranial magnetic stimulation, finger sequence tapping, motor learning, sleep

Citation: de Beukelaar TT, Van Soom J, Huber R and Wenderoth N (2016) A Day Awake Attenuates Motor Learning-Induced Increases in Corticomotor Excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:138. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00138

Received: 30 October 2015; Accepted: 15 March 2016;
Published: 30 March 2016.

Edited by:

Julien Doyon, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal (IUGM), Canada

Reviewed by:

Bjoern Rasch, University of Fribourg, Switzerland
Shirley Fecteau, Laval University, Canada

Copyright © 2016 de Beukelaar, Van Soom, Huber and Wenderoth. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nicole Wenderoth, bmljb2xlLndlbmRlcm90aEBoZXN0LmV0aHouY2g=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.