Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 02 March 2015
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Proprioceptive Dysfunction, Related Motor Disorders and Their Neurological (Robotic) Rehabilitation View all 19 articles

Upper extremity proprioception in healthy aging and stroke populations, and the effects of therapist- and robot-based rehabilitation therapies on proprioceptive function

  • 1Robotics Research Centre, School of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
  • 2Interdisciplinary Graduate School, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore

The world’s population is aging, with the number of people ages 65 or older expected to surpass 1.5 billion people, or 16% of the global total. As people age, there are notable declines in proprioception due to changes in the central and peripheral nervous systems. Moreover, the risk of stroke increases with age, with approximately two-thirds of stroke-related hospitalizations occurring in people over the age of 65. In this literature review, we first summarize behavioral studies investigating proprioceptive deficits in normally aging older adults and stroke patients, and discuss the differences in proprioceptive function between these populations. We then provide a state of the art review the literature regarding therapist- and robot-based rehabilitation of the upper extremity proprioceptive dysfunction in stroke populations and discuss avenues of future research.

Introduction

Proprioceptive information is important for balance and postural control, the control and regulation of coordinated movements, motor learning, and error correction during movements (Jeannerod, 1988; Schmidt and Lee, 1988) and is generally composed of the modalities joint position sense and the sensation of limb movement (Gandevia et al., 2002). Joint position sense is defined as the ability of an individual to identify the static location of a body part, and is served by muscle spindle afferents and cutaneous afferents (Proske, 2006; Proske and Gandevia, 2009). Kinesthesia, a term introduced by Bastian (1887), refers to the perception of active and passive motion. Passive motion sense is served by slowly adapting mechanoreceptors (mainly secondary spindle endings and tendon organs in muscle), and tendon organs and Ruffini spray endings in other deep tissues, whereas active motion sense stems from the more rapidly adapting proprioceptors; mainly the muscle spindle primary endings, and lamellated corpuscles in other deep tissues (Grigg, 1994; Hogervorst and Brand, 1998).

The importance of proprioception in performing coordinated movements has been demonstrated in studies investigating motor control in individuals with proprioceptive deficits resulting from sensory neuropathy conditions or surgery (Rothwell et al., 1982; Ghez et al., 1995; Gordon et al., 1995; Messier et al., 2003; Sarlegna et al., 2006) and by disrupting proprioception in physically and neurologically healthy participants using tendon vibration (Cody et al., 1990; Cordo et al., 1995). Deficits in upper extremity proprioceptive function have also been reported in normally aging older adults (Adamo et al., 2007; Riberio and Oliveria, 2007) and individuals with stroke (Twitchell, 1951; Carey et al., 1993; Yekutiel, 2000), and have been found to negatively impact the quality of daily life and independence of the affected individual (Carey et al., 1997).

In this review, we first provide an overview of the behavioral research on upper extremity proprioceptive deficits in normally aging older adults, and then present an up-to-date overview of the proprioceptive declines in stroke patients. We conclude this review by reporting the state of the art in conventional and robotic rehabilitation of upper extremity proprioceptive function, and discuss the existing problems in this field and what may be proposed to move this area of science forward.

Proprioceptive Function in Healthy Older Adults

Proprioceptive declines in older adults are a result of anatomical and physiological changes in both the central and peripheral nervous systems (CNS and PNS, respectively), which negatively influence the ability to execute everyday tasks in the absence of vision. Changes in the peripheral nervous system that account for declines in proprioceptive function include increases in capsular thickness (Swash and Fox, 1972), decreases in muscle spindle sensitivity (Miwa et al., 1995; Burke et al., 1996; Kim et al., 2007) and diameter (Kararizou et al., 2005), and a lower total number of joint mechanoreceptors [especially for Ruffini, Pacinian, and Golgi-tendon type receptors, Morisawa (1998), Aydog et al. (2006)], intrafusal (Swash and Fox, 1972; Liu et al., 2005), and chain fibers (Liu et al., 2005).

Central nervous system changes include decreased gray matter volume in the anterior cingulate cortex, pre- and postcentral gyri, insula, and angula gyri (Good et al., 2001) and reduced activity in proprioceptive regions of the basal ganglia (Goble et al., 2012), both of which may contribute to declines in joint position sense in older adults. For example, Goble et al. (2012) used magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and tendon vibration to stimulate muscle spindle afferents in healthy young (mean age = 26.1 years; range = 19.9–32.4 years) and elderly adults (mean age = 68.9 years; range = 62.3–81.3 years), and reported a localized underactivation of the right putamen in elderly individuals. Using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), older (but not younger) adults with higher mean fractional anisotropy (FA) were found to have increased right putamen neural activity and better joint position sense performance. On the basis of these results, the authors argued that proprioceptive processing in the elderly is influenced by structural differences that limit activation within subcortical regions (i.e., putamen), which, in turn, influence performance in tests of joint position sense.

Age-related declines in cognitive and sensorimotor processing ability (D’Esposito et al., 1995; Seidler et al., 2010) are also thought to contribute to changes in proprioceptive function, especially in more cognitively demanding tasks. This is supported by studies demonstrating age-related changes in elbow joint position sense in tasks with greater memory demands (Adamo et al., 2007, 2009). In these studies, a contralateral remembered matching task, an ipsilateral remembered matching task, as well as a contralateral concurrent matching task was employed. In the former two tasks, the joint is passively moved to a target position and held for a few second before being returned to its starting angle. Participants are then required to match the memorized target joint angle using the ipsilateral (ipsilateral remembered matching task) or contralateral arm (contralateral remembered matching task). In the latter task (contralateral concurrent matching task), the hand is moved to a target position, and held there while the participant attempts to match the target position with the other hand. Overall, significantly poorer performance was observed in the older adults [mean age = 75.0 years in Adamo et al. (2007), 76.4 years in Adamo et al. (2009)] relative to their younger counterparts [mean age = 27.0 years in Adamo et al. (2007), 22.1 years in Adamo et al. (2009)], with the elderly group exhibiting greater matching errors for the task that required both memory-based matching and interhemispherical transfer of proprioceptive information (i.e., contralateral remembered). On the basis of these results, the authors postulated that the decrease in proprioceptive acuity in elderly individuals reflects age-related deterioration in cognitive function, and is exacerbated in tasks with greater memory demands.

Despite the well documented changes in the CNS, PNS, and neuromuscular systems, there is no general consensus regarding general proprioceptive abilities in elderly individuals. The majority of available research examining joint position sense has reported that individuals aged between 70 and 80 years perform much worse than individuals in their 20s and 30s, regardless of whether the movement is performed with the elbow (Adamo et al., 2007; Herter et al., 2014), arm (Stelmach and Sirica, 1986), wrist (Adamo et al., 2009), or finger (Ferrell et al., 1992; Kalisch et al., 2012).

In contrast, age-related declines in the perception of active and passive motion (kinesthesia) have been found in some studies (Ferrell et al., 1992; Wright et al., 2011), but not in others (Kokmen et al., 1978; Wang et al., 2012). For example, Kokmen et al. (1978) reported that passive motion perception at the metacarpophalangeal (MCP) sensitivity was impaired in older individuals (age range = 61–84 years) compared to young adults (age range = 19–34 years), but that when the older cohort was separated into semidecades all differences of motion threshold perception lost significance. Similarly, Wang et al. (2012) reported that older participants (mean age = 66.5 years, range = 61–70 years) showed nearly identical performance to the younger cohort (mean age = 25.0 years, range = 22–29 years) when performing whole arm passive or active movements in the absence of visual feedback. Wright et al. (2011), however, found that the ability to perceive passive wrist movement is altered in older individuals, with passive movement detection thresholds were reportedly twice as high in older adults (mean age = 79.5 years) compared to younger individuals (mean age = 24.0 years).

Considering all available data, there is strong evidence that joint position sense acuity decreases with age. In contrast, the presence of age-related declines in kinesthesia depends on a number of factors (e.g., experimental paradigm and task, how proprioception was measured, whether the task required movement at a single joint or multiple joints). For example, whole arm movements require the integration of sensory signals from a greater number of muscles spanning different joints of different segment lengths in order to accurately estimate joint position sense. Thus, movements that require the whole arm [e.g., Wang et al. (2012)] are likely to be more complex than those than involve a single joint [e.g., Wright et al. (2011)]. More focused examination of task differences may be helpful in elucidating changes in kinesthesia across the life span.

Despite the wealth of research demonstrating that upper extremity joint position sense is worse in individuals aged between 70 and 80 years compared to individuals aged between 20 and 30 years, and that a sedentary lifestyle accelerates loss of joint position sense acuity in older individuals (Adamo et al., 2009), we were unable to find any studies that evaluated the effects of upper extremity proprioceptive training on healthy elderly individuals or any commercial robotic proprioceptive assessment and training systems that cater to elderly individuals. This is intriguing the ample evidence demonstrating the importance of proprioceptive feedback on many daily activities, and that lower extremity proprioception interventions (e.g., posture training and Tai Chi) are associated with increased proprioceptive acuity in active older adults (Sinaki and Lynn, 2002; Li et al., 2008). The examination of upper extremity training on proprioceptive declines is an avenue of research certainly worthwhile pursuing as the early diagnosis and effective management of sensorimotor control and dysfunction provides the opportunity for older adults to enjoy their later years as functional, active, independent members of the community. Researchers and clinicians should consider age-related declines in tactile discrimination and haptic sensation when designing training interventions for the elderly. For example, Stevens and Choo (1996) has reported that tactile acuity thresholds (as measured by two-point discrimination test) in the finger are on average about 80% higher in elderly adults (65 years of age and older) compared to younger adults (between the ages of 18 and 28 years), and that the ability to discriminate tactile gaps, orientation of lines, and the length of lines drawn on the skin (i.e., graphesthesia) deteriorates with age, with approximately 1% per annum increase in threshold between the ages of 20 and 80 years (Stevens and Patterson, 1995; Stevens and Cruz, 1996). Moreover, intervention programs should consider the factors that affect proprioception in elderly and stroke populations, keeping in mind that proprioceptive function may not be fully restored. As such, programs should also facilitate the development of compensatory strategies to ensure safety in both familiar (e.g., home and work) and novel (i.e., shopping center, public transport depot) environments.

Proprioceptive Function in Stroke Patients

Sensory and proprioceptive deficits are particularly common following stroke, and are correlated with length of hospitalization, likelihood of discharge, and increased mortality rates (Zeman and Yiannikas, 1989; Carey et al., 1993; Carey, 1995; Yekutiel, 2000; Sommerfeld and von Arbin, 2004), and have detrimental effects on personal safety and leisure activity levels after hospital discharge (Carey et al., 1997).

Impairments can range from disruption of one type of sensation modalities (e.g., primary tactile senses such as light touch, pressure and localization to more discriminatory senses, sharp/dull discrimination, temperature discrimination, and proprioception) to impairments of multiple or all somatosensory modalities (Jongbloed, 1986; Carey, 1995; Winward et al., 1999). It has been reported that tactile impairment is more frequent than proprioceptive impairment (Winward et al., 2002; Tyson et al., 2007). For example, Tyson et al. (2007) measured tactile and proprioceptive sensation in 93 acute stroke patients (range: 2–4 weeks post-stroke) using the Rivermead assessment of somatosensory performance (RASP), and reported that tactile impairment (66%) was more common than proprioceptive (27%), and impairment of discrimination was more common than detection (65 vs. 45%). Winward et al. (2002) also used the RASP as a measurement tool in their sample of 100 chronic stroke patients (range: 4.7–6.1 weeks post stroke), and reported impaired surface detection, surface localization, and impaired proprioception rates of 65, 31, and 52%.

In contrast to these two studies, there are reports that prevalence rates of upper extremity proprioceptive impairment are similar (Carey and Matyas, 2011) or greater (Connell et al., 2008) to those obtained for tactile impairment. Carey and Matyas (2011) reported impairments in tactile discrimination and wrist position sense (47 and 49%, respectively) in 51 post-acute and chronic stroke patients (mean days post stroke: 49.5 days). Connell et al. (2008) used the Nottingham sensory assessment (NSA) and found proprioceptive impairment to be more frequent than tactile impairment in a number of upper extremities in their sample of 70 patients (median days post stroke: 15 days), and argued that the discrepancy between studies is due to the use of the RASP in prior studies, which measures joint movement and movement direction discrimination, but not joint position sense, and therefore is less likely to detect proprioceptive impairment compared to the NSA.

Focusing on the modalities joint position sense and kinesthesia, neuroimaging studies have reported proprioceptive impairments after stroke with lesions to the thalamus (Sacco et al., 1987; Ja Gutrecht et al., 1992; Kim, 1992; Lee et al., 2012), posterior limb of the internal capsule [i.e., PLIC, e.g., Shintani et al. (2000)], and somatosensory (S1) and posterior parietal cortices (PPC) (Derouesne et al., 1984; Shintani et al., 2000; Kim, 2007). These brain areas are involved in numerous functions related to sensory processing in the healthy brain (Riehle and Vaadia, 2004). Specifically, the ventral posterolateral (VPL) nucleus of the thalamus projects somatosensory information from the extremities to the cortex, the PLIC transmits ascending sensory signals from the thalamus and descending motor commands from the cortex, S1 receives somatosensory information via VPL, and the superior parietal lobule (SPL) of PPC receives heavy input from S1 and projects to all areas of premotor cortex (with the exception of the ventral bank of the caudal cingulate motor area).

Proprioceptive deficits after stroke have been found to correlate with visuospatial neglect (Vallar et al., 1993, 1995; Semrau et al., 2013), with this combination of deficits, resulting in longer recovery times and poorer functional outcome (Smith et al., 1983). The observed proprioceptive deficits in patients with visuospatial neglect lead to the inference that spatial aspects of both vision and proprioception are damaged, and are supported by recent neurophysiological work demonstrating that the PPC is a common site for processing aspects of both sensory modalities (Buneo and Andersen, 2012).

It has also been shown that both the ipsilateral and contralateral limb (with respect to the side of the lesion) is affected after unilateral hemisphere stroke (Connell et al., 2008; Niessen et al., 2008). The pathophysiological mechanisms, which result in deficits of the ipsilateral upper extremity, are largely unknown. One hypothesis is that damage to the ipsilesional uncrossed descending corticospinal pathways influence the ability to perceive and interpret somatosensory information (Ziemann et al., 1999). Alternatively, it has been suggested that ipsilateral proprioceptive deficits after unilateral hemisphere stroke arise from a disturbance of interhemispheric, transcallosal transfer (Shimizu et al., 2002; Stinear et al., 2007), which indicates that activation of the ipsilateral hemisphere during unilateral upper-limb movements might be related to excitatory or inhibitory effects in the contralateral hemisphere. From a clinical perspective, given the observed proprioceptive deficits to both limbs, the ipsilateral limb (with respect to the side of the lesion, i.e., non-paretic) should not be used as a benchmark during the assessment of proprioceptive function. With respect to the assessment of joint position sense, for example, the measured error in the contralateral remembered or contralateral concurrent matching task may arise from the reference arm, the matching arm, or both. As such, it may be more appropriate to use the ipsilateral remembered matching task to evaluate joint position sense in individuals with unilateral hemisphere brain damage.

Recovery of sensory function after stroke occurs within the first 6 months after the stroke incidence (Smith et al., 1983; Winward et al., 2002; Connell et al., 2008). For example, Winward et al. (2002) examined recovery in eight sensory modalities in acute and sub-acute patients (n = 9 in each group). Results of that study showed significant improvements in the recovery of proprioception in the first 6 months of recovery, with five out of nine acute patients achieving 100% on the proprioception sub-test by the end of the first month, and six out of nine sub-acute patients achieving 97–100% in proprioception movement by week 33. Similarly, Connell et al. (2008) reported significant recovery in upper-limb proprioceptive function in the first 4 months after stroke in acute stroke patients (n = 70, mean days post stroke = 15).

In contrast, the association between proprioceptive impairment and motor function during the recovery process is unclear, with some studies reporting a significant correlation between the two variables (Kuffosky et al., 1982; La Joie et al., 1982; Sheikh et al., 1983; Wade et al., 1983; Pavot et al., 1986; de Weerdt et al., 1987; Mercier and Bourbonnais, 2004), while other studies have not (Twitchell, 1951; Katrak et al., 1998; Rand et al., 1999). For example, Wade et al. (1983) evaluated 25 prognostic factors and reported significant correlations between motor function recovery of the hemiplegic upper limb and initial motor deficit, loss of position sense in the arm, and the Camden Mental Score. Likewise, Connell et al. (2008) found that initial somatosensory impairment and upper-limb tactile sensation predicted proprioceptive recovery 6 months post stroke, and Sheikh et al. (1983) reported that sensory deficit was a significant predictor of disability in 900 stroke patients that were discharged.

Katrak et al. (1998) on the other hand, found that sensory and proprioceptive function (as measured by light touch, sensory inattention, and proprioception in the hemiplegic upper limb) was not correlated with recovery of hand movement or function (n = 71, all <28 days post stroke) over a 3-month period. A later study (Rand et al., 1999) compared upper extremity motor and functional recovery in patients with pure motor hemiplegia with that of patients afflicted by combined motor and proprioception deficits (n = 20 in each group, mean days post stroke = 17) during the first 6 weeks of rehabilitation. Results showed that motor and functional recovery improved for both groups across the rehabilitation period, with no observable relationship between the loss of proprioception and motor and functional recovery. Moreover, despite some observable improvement the majority of patients with combined sensory and motor deficits still exhibited moderate to severe impaired proprioception, indicating that the recovery of upper extremity motor function is not affected by proprioceptive loss.

The mixed results regarding the relationship between proprioception and the recovery of upper extremity function may be due to differences in proprioception measures, heterogeneity in study populations, and the number of somatosensory modalities and body areas assessed. Further studies are needed to establish whether proprioceptive impairment influences the recovery of upper extremity function after stroke, as this may have important implications for rehabilitation programs.

Differences in Proprioceptive Function between Healthy Elderly and Stroke Patients

The biological process of aging contributes to the increased risk of stroke, with individuals aged 65 and over accounting for more than two-thirds of hospitalizations (Hall et al., 2012). As such, some of the mechanisms that underlie proprioceptive dysfunction in people with stroke may be attributable to the natural aging process.

As in healthy older adults, acute and chronic stroke patients exhibit impairments in joint position sense (Niessen et al., 2008; Dukelow et al., 2012; Kattenstroth et al., 2013) and kinesthesia (Niessen et al., 2008). For example, Kattenstroth et al. (2013) reported that hand position sense acuity was significantly lower for sub-acute stroke patients (n = 10, mean weeks post stroke = 2.3) compared to healthy age-matched controls. Niessen et al. (2008) also found that shoulder joint position sense performance was lower in both the contralateral and the ipsilateral shoulders of sub-acute stroke patients (n = 22) when compared with healthy control subjects, although this effect failed to reach significance (p’s ranged from 0.063 to 0.299). This latter study also revealed a significant decrease in contralateral and ipsilateral shoulder kinesthesia (as measured by TDPM) for patients compared with the control group. The authors argued that the kinesthetic deficits exhibited by stroke patients arose from gamma motoneuron control dysfunction due to hemiparesis as a result of a stroke. This dysfunction affects the sensitivity of the muscle spindles, which leads to delays in movement detection when the muscle is stretched passively, hence larger TDPM scores. On the other hand, the large degree of humeral rotation in the joint position sense test (10° of internal or external rotation, relative to the chosen start position) passively stretched and sensitized the muscle spindles prior to reaching the reference position. Sensitizing the muscle spindles in this way afforded the accurate detection of the reference position in both groups.

To date, relatively little is known about the specific association between the location of brain lesions and proprioceptive dysfunction. This is unfortunate given that elucidating this relationship could enhance our understanding of the neural circuitry involved in proprioception and could lead to advances in diagnosis, preventive interventions, and treatment. From the limited number of studies conducted so far it appears that there is no specific association between lesion location and either joint position sense (Niessen et al., 2008; Dukelow et al., 2012; Kattenstroth et al., 2013) or kinesthesia (Niessen et al., 2008). One study (Dukelow et al., 2012) compared upper extremity joint position sense in 100 inpatient stroke rehabilitation subjects (mean age = 63.0, range = 21–90) with that of 231 non-disabled controls (mean age = 48.0, range = 20–88) and reported that a large number of individuals with ischemic stroke of the left (n = 17/31) and right (n = 24/29) middle cerebral artery (MCA) displayed proprioceptive deficits. However, proprioceptive deficits in participants with MCA lesions did not differ statistically from patients with lesions in other locations (i.e., left pontine artery, left basilar artery, left anterior cerebral artery). That said these results should be interpreted with caution due to lower power (small sample size, heterogeneity of patient groups, the high variability in patient performance), which may influence the ability to detect clinically meaningful effects. Given the importance that elucidating the lesion–symptom relationship could have for rehabilitation outcomes it is recommended that future studies employ sophisticated neuroimaging analysis techniques (Voxel-based morphometry, Voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping) to identify lesion characteristics associated with proprioceptive dysfunction in stroke patients.

Rehabilitation of Proprioceptive Function in Stoke Populations

Therapist-Based Interventions

Sensory interventions to improve function or remediate sensory and proprioceptive impairments of the upper limb following stroke can occur via either passive or activity sensory training. Passive sensory training involves the application of electrical stimulation to produce activation of cutaneous nerves in the absence of muscle contraction, whereas active sensory training involves a series of exercises designed specifically to train sensory function.

There is preliminary evidence that passive sensory training (i.e., via the application of electrical stimulation to produce activation of cutaneous nerves in the absence of muscle contraction) leads to significant improves in tactile sensation (Cambier et al., 2003), kinesthetic sensation (Cambier et al., 2003), light touch detection (Acerra et al., 2005), pressure and temperature pain sensation (Acerra et al., 2005), mechanical sensation (Chen et al., 2005), and arm function (Cambier et al., 2003; Chen et al., 2005) in individual with stroke. For example, Cambier et al. (2003) examined the effects of intermittent pneumatic compression of the hemiplegic upper limb (n = 12, mean time since stroke = 83 days) compared to sham therapy (n = 11, mean time since stroke = 114 days). Results indicated that somatosensory function improved for both groups over the course of treatment, but the improvements were greater for the group that received standard physiotherapy combined with intermittent pneumatic compression treatment (experimental group) compared to the control group that received sham treatment (81.1 vs. 30.9% improvement). That study also demonstrated between-group differences in tactile (37.10) and kinesthetic sensation (26.20), but not for two-point discrimination (0.31) and stereognosis (5.60), in favor of the experimental group. In sum, intervention protocols utilizing passive sensory training have the potential to improve motor and sensory impairment of the upper limb after stroke. However, this corpus work is still in its infancy and further high quality studies are needed in order to properly evaluate their effectiveness in post-stroke populations.

Researchers have also examined the effects of active sensory training programs on stroke recovery (see Table 1). The first known study to examine the effects of re-education and training on upper extremity proprioceptive function after stroke was conducted by Carey et al. (1993) (Exp 2). In that study four patients (time since stroke = 5–26.5 weeks) completed a 30 session training program, which consisted of graded texture (tactile discrimination test, TDT) and limb position discrimination training (proprioceptive discrimination test, PDT). Results indicated that patients showed significant improvements of trained abilities, with discrimination capabilities of the affected hand reaching levels comparable to those of the unaffected hand, which were maintained at 2- and 5-month follow-up. Unfortunately, the clinical relevance of this study was undermined by the fact that the stimuli used in the training program and assessment were identical, and outcome measures were limited to two trained submodalities.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of studies that have examined the effects of upper extremity proprioceptive interventions on stroke function.

More recent studies (Yekutiel and Guttman, 1993; Smania et al., 2003) used experimental protocols in which the training activities differed from those used to assess somatosensory function, and also assessed a number of submodalities. For example, in Yekutiel and Guttman (1993) 20 patients (all >2 years post stroke) with chronic hemiplegia and somatic deficits received retaining of sensory function three times/week for 6 weeks (with each session lasting approximately 45 min). The training program consisted of the modalities letter tactile recognition, shape, weight, and texture discrimination, kinesthesia, and passive drawing. Overall, the treated group showed large and significant gains in all sensory tests, compared to a control group of similar age. However, gains in elbow position sense were not correlated with those of any of the other three tests, and were also negatively correlated with sensory score at outset.

Smania et al. (2003) evaluated the effects of a training program that targeted somatic sensation as well as sensory-related deficits in motor control in four unilateral chronic stroke patients (time since stroke = 5–20 months). The training program consisted of thirty treatment sessions each lasting 50 min. At the beginning of each training session patients performed a series of 25 manual exercises, after which the therapist selected the more challenging manual exercises that were then performed with the impaired hand. Despite differences in the degree of improvement between patients, significant improvements in a number of sensory and motor tasks were reported, which were maintained at 6-month follow-up. Results of this study indicate that proprioceptive function can be improved through training programs targeting somatosensory and related deficits of motor control, and that training generalizes to activities not targeted by the training program.

Carey et al. (2011) recently compared the effectiveness of two training programs in fifty chronic stroke patients (all >6 weeks post stroke) with impaired texture discrimination, limb position sense, and/or tactile object recognition. In the sensory discrimination program (SD group), participants received generalized texture discrimination (TDT and fabric matching test), limb position sense (wrist and finger PDT), tactile object recognition (functional tactile object recognition test), and temperature discrimination training (Roylan hot and cold temperature discrimination kit). In contrast, the non-specific training program (NS group) consisted of non-specific repeated exposure to stimuli varying in texture, shape, size, weight, hardness, and temperature, via grasping of common objects, and passive movements of the upper limb. The training program consisted of 10 intervention sessions, each of approximately 60-min duration, conducted three times a week. Results indicated that there was very little change in functional somatosensory discrimination capacity scores (a composite score comprised of the modalities: texture discrimination, limb position sense, and tactile object recognition) for the majority of patients in the NS group, and those with moderate or severe impairment showed little or no improvement. In comparison, almost all (n = 22/25) patients in the SD group showed a positive change in functional somatosensory discrimination capacity due to sensory retraining, and there was no observable difference in the magnitude of change between different levels of impairment severity. Proprioceptive improvement was maintained and slightly increased at 6-week and 6-month follow-up. The results of this randomized controlled trial [National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) level II], therefore, indicate that chronic stroke patients with proprioceptive deficits can benefit from sensory discrimination training.

The first study to evaluate the effects of a sensory training on kinesthesia in stroke patients was conducted by Byl et al. (2003). In that study, chronic stroke patients (n = 18, time since stroke = 6 months to 7 years post stroke) were randomly assigned to Group A (n = 8, sensory training 4 weeks, motor training 4 weeks) or Group B (n = 10, motor training 4 weeks, sensory training 4 weeks). The motor training program consisted of practicing repetitive specific fine motor tasks, while the sensory retraining program consisted of graded and repetitive sensory discrimination activities [e.g., replicating drawings of letters, numbers, or figures drawn on the skin (graphesthesia), touching subject on the digit and hand and have subject put finger on spot where touched (touch localization), grasping and interpreting information about the objects properties (stereognosis), indicating limb position following passive movement (kinesthesia)]. Significant improvements in functional independence and performance parameters of the affected upper limb were reported, with improvement >20% for motor (motor reaction time, Purdue Pegboard Task), sensory discrimination (kinesthesia, graphesthesia, stereognosis), and functional independence (California functional evaluation) scores (27, 21, and 41%, respectively) performance in 83% of patients. The results of this study demonstrate that 12 h of supervised learning based sensory motor training programs are sufficient to achieve significant improvements in function in chronic stroke patients that are maintained at 6-month follow-up.

In sum, the relatively few studies with treatment interventions that specifically target somatosensory recovery after stroke have reported positive improvements in upper-limb proprioceptive function in acute (Carey et al., 1993) and chronic stroke patients (Yekutiel and Guttman, 1993; Byl et al., 2003; Smania et al., 2003; Carey et al., 2011). However, a number of these studies suffer from small sample size issues (n < 20 patients; Carey et al., 1993; Smania et al., 2003), which decreases statistical power, and thus negatively affect the ability of detecting a true effect, and only one study was classified as randomized controlled trial (Carey et al., 2011). As such, more high quality (NHMRC level II) studies with homogenous and large sample sizes are needed in order to determine (via meta-analysis and effect size calculations) the overall effectiveness of proprioceptive retraining programs on stroke.

Robot-Based Interventions

Current conventional stroke rehabilitation therapies are a labor intensive process, which involves daily one-on-one interactions with therapists that can last for several weeks. The significant burden placed on therapists and healthcare systems has motivated a number of researchers to develop robotic devices targeting post-stroke upper extremity motor rehabilitation [cf. Maciejasz et al. (2014) for an overview for currently developed robotic devices for upper-limb motor rehabilitation]. The advantages of using robots is that they can deliver high-dosage and high-intensity training, interact with human motion, physically assist movement, accurately measure performance, and can provide continual assessment of changes in motor function through performance measures.

The prevalence of proprioceptive impairment in stroke populations has led a number of scientists to develop robotic systems for the quantification and rehabilitation of proprioception function in stroke populations (Casadio et al., 2009; Cordo et al., 2009; Sangueniti et al., 2009; Vergaro et al., 2010; Cho et al., 2014). For example, Cordo et al. (2009) utilized a device fitted with tendon vibrators at the flexor and extensor tendons of the wrist and finger joints to examine the efficacy of the assisted movement with enhanced sensation (AMES) approach for proprioceptive rehabilitation. In that study, 18 chronic stroke patients (all >1 year post stroke) performed 30-min daily therapy sessions over a 6-month treatment period. During each session, the robotic device cycled the wrist and fingers in flexion and extension while the patient assisted the motion imposed by device by exerting a flexion force on the device during imposed flexion, and an extension force during imposed extension. At each reversal of movement direction, tendon vibration switched between the flexor and extensor tendons, always applying vibration to the lengthening tendon (i.e., to the muscle antagonistic to the assisted joint motion). Every second day, a joint position test was conducted in which the patient was instructed to follow a graphically presented target while staying inside the target zone. Results showed improvements in the joint positioning task across the 6-month period for all patients (average improvement of 77%), with a recovery trajectory for the majority of patients (n = 15/18) following a negative exponential trajectory that reached 90% of asymptote at day 111 (approximately 3.6 months). The results of this study, thus, indicate that stimulating proprioceptive afferents in the lengthening muscles during voluntary contraction improves joint position performance after a 6-month training period.

Serious games are a fundamental component of many robotic rehabilitation devices, which while useful for patient motivation and retention, also allow patients to compensate for proprioceptive deficits by relying on vision. To counteract such compensatory strategies the Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences Laboratory at the Italian Institute of Technology (Casadio et al., 2009; Sangueniti et al., 2009; Vergaro et al., 2010) have developed a training framework that manipulates the amount of visual information available during task performance in order to force the patient to rely on proprioceptive feedback. For example, in Vergaro et al. (2010) ten chronic stroke patients (all <1 year post stroke) were asked to grasp the handle of the Braccio di Ferro robot and track a moving target that drew a figure-of-eight shaped trajectory on the computer screen. In the visuo-haptic (VH) condition, the position of the target was presented to the subjects visually by means of a circle on the computer screen, and haptically via an attractive force field to the target. During the pure haptic (PH) condition, the patient was blindfolded and had to rely on the robot-generated force field her to detect the direction the target was moving. In general, there was a significant decrease in the level of assistive force required, a decrease in tracking error, and an increase in movement smoothness across sessions. Based on these results, Vergaro et al. (2010) argued that training lead to a recalibration of sensory channels, and that patients were capable of performing continuous tracking tasks using only proprioceptive cues.

A recent study utilized virtual reality (VR) technology to assess the effect of proprioceptive feedback in upper extremity rehabilitation in stroke patients (Cho et al., 2014). In that study, 10 patients (all >10 weeks post stroke) interacted with a living room VR environment that featured a semi-transparent cylinder that represented the position of the hand, and an opaque cylinder that represented the target position. In the visual feedback virtual environment (VTFE) condition, patients moved the semi-transparent cylinder (current hand position) to the position of the opaque cylinder (target position), and pressed a mouse button with the unaffected hand once they believed the affected hand reached the target position. In the proprioception feedback virtual environment (PFVE) condition, both cylinders were visible at the start of the trial. However, as soon as the patient initiated the reaching movement the semi-transparent cylinder (reflecting the current hand position) disappeared, forcing the patient to rely on proprioceptive feedback to estimate the current and final position. As with the VFVE condition, patients pressed the mouse button once they believed the semi-transparent cylinder corresponded to the position of the opaque cylinder. Results of this study showed a significant improvement in performance after PFVE training, compared to VFVE training, which suggests that improvements in proprioceptive function post-stroke are greater when rehabilitation training systems force patients to rely on proprioceptive feedback.

Future Work

This article reviewed the literature concerning upper extremity proprioceptive deficits in normally aging older adults and individuals with stroke. Despite the advances in this field, there are still important areas where greater progress is needed. In the first instance, a clear understanding of the effects of increasing age proprioception is important clinically for identifying the impact of stroke on proprioceptive function. As such, it is important that future studies include age-matched controls of appropriate sample size. In the long term, this data can be used to develop a database of normative proprioceptive function that enables the comparison of measurement values during initial assessment and across the training period.

In addition, more accurate clinical assessment of proprioception is vital. Currently, clinical evaluation of proprioceptive function is typically undertaken in a subjective, non-standardized and unreliable manner, and suffers from very poor inter-rater reliability and sensitivity and poor or absent normal value criteria (Lincoln et al., 1991). In contrast, rehabilitation robots capable of proprioceptive assessment have better diagnostic and prognostic precision and are more likely to discriminate subtle differences of deficit and changes over time compared to current clinical measures (Dukelow et al., 2010, 2012). Moreover, robotic tools affords the opportunity to compare the measurement values obtained immediately after assessment, and allow clinicians to decide the most appropriate treatment options, which should lead to improvements in rehabilitation treatment and lower long term healthcare costs.

With respect to stroke populations, there is preliminary evidence to suggest that conventional and robot-assisted sensory re-education training lead to positive improvement in upper-limb proprioceptive function in acute (Carey et al., 1993) and chronic stroke patients (Yekutiel and Guttman, 1993; Byl et al., 2003; Smania et al., 2003; Casadio et al., 2009; Cordo et al., 2009; Carey et al., 2011; Cho et al., 2014). Despite these results, the number of studies examining proprioceptive dysfunction is small, and future work with large sample sizes is needed to corroborate the observed positive effects. Therapist-based training programs directed at age-related and stroke induced functional losses are limited by manpower and healthcare and insurance policies issues, and as such, rehabilitation robots can be used to alleviate the burden on therapists and healthcare systems. Unfortunately, the examination of proprioceptive recovery after a stroke incidence is still in its infancy, and as such the comparison of proprioceptive recovery rates between therapist-based and robot-based training, and the treatment programs that yield the most effective outcomes, remains an open question.

Conclusion

This review summarized the most significant research conducted on upper extremity proprioceptive function in healthy older adults and those affected by stroke, with results indicating that proprioceptive function declines as a result of normal aging, and affects between 11 and 75% of stroke patients. The number of stroke patients aged 65 and over accounts for more than two-thirds of hospitalizations, and as such early diagnosis and effective management of sensorimotor control and dysfunction may reduce the risk of stroke. The observed deficits in proprioceptive function in stroke populations have lead a number of researchers to develop therapist-based and robotic proprioceptive training programs. Although this area of research is still in its infancy, these developments should lead to improvements in the evaluation, rehabilitation, and treatment of proprioceptive function.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was partly supported by the “H-Man” project (NMRC/BnB/0006b/2013), Ministry of Health, Singapore.

References

Acerra, N. E., Souvlis, T., and Moseley, G. (2005). Does mirror-box therapy improve sensory and motor changes in the early post-stroke population? A randomised controlled trial. Aust. J. Physiother. 51(4-e Suppl.), S7.

Google Scholar

Adamo, D. E., Alexander, N. B., and Brown, S. H. (2009). The influence of age and physical activity on upper limb proprioceptive ability. J. Aging Phys. Act. 17, 272–293.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Adamo, D. E., Martin, B. J., and Brown, S. H. (2007). Age-related differences in upper limb proprioceptive acuity. Percept. Mot. Skills 104, 1297–1309. doi: 10.2466/pms.104.4.1297-1309

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aydog, S. T., Korkusuz, P., Doral, M. N., Tetik, O., and Demirel, H. A. (2006). Decrease in the numbers of mechanoreceptors in rabbit ACL: the effects of ageing. Knee Surg. Sports Traumatol. Arthrosc. 14, 325–329. doi:10.1007/s00167-005-0673-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastian, H. C. (1887). The “muscular sense”: its nature and cortical localisation. Brain 10, 1–88. doi:10.1093/brain/10.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buneo, C. A., and Andersen, R. A. (2012). Integration of target and hand position signals in the posterior parietal cortex: effects of workspace and hand vision. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 187–199. doi:10.1152/jn.00137.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, J. R., Schutten, M. C., Koceja, D. M., and Kamen, G. (1996). Age-dependent effects of muscle vibration and the Jendrassik maneuver on the patellar tendon reflex response. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 77, 600–604. doi:10.1016/S0003-9993(96)90302-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byl, N., Roderick, J., Mohamed, O., Hanny, M., Kotler, J., Smith, A., et al. (2003). Effectiveness of sensory and motor rehabilitation of the upper limb following principles of neuroplasticity: patients stable poststroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 17, 76–91. doi:10.1177/0888439003257137

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cambier, D. C., De Corte, E., Danneels, L. A., and Witvrouw, E. E. (2003). Treating sensory impairments in the post-stroke upper limb with intermittent pneumatic compression. Results of a preliminary trial. Clin. Rehabil. 17, 14–20. doi:10.1191/0269215503cr580oa

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, L. M. (1995). Somatosensory loss after stroke. Crit. Rev. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 7, 51–91. doi:10.1615/CritRevPhysRehabilMed.v7.i1.40

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, L. M., Macdonell, R., and Matyas, T. A. (2011). SENSe: study of the effectiveness of neurorehabilitation on sensation: a randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 25, 304–313. doi:10.1177/1545968310397705

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, L. M., and Matyas, T. A. (2011). Frequency of discriminative sensory loss in the hand after stroke in a rehabilitation setting. J. Rehabil. Med. 43, 257–263. doi:10.2340/16501977-0662

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, L. M., Matyas, T. A., and Oke, L. E. (1993). Sensory loss in stroke patients: effective training of tactile and proprioceptive discrimination. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 74, 602–611. doi:10.1016/0003-9993(93)90158-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, L. M., Oke, L. E., and Matyas, T. A. (1997). Impaired touch discrimination after stroke: a quantitative test. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 11, 219–232. doi:10.1177/154596839701100404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casadio, M., Giannoni, P., Morasso, P., and Sanguineti, V. (2009). A proof of concept study for the integration of robot therapy with physiotherapy in the treatment of stroke patients. Clin. Rehabil. 23, 217–228. doi:10.1177/0269215508096759

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. C., Liang, C. C., and Shaw, F. Z. (2005). Facilitation of sensory and motor recovery by thermal intervention for the hemiplegic upper limb in acute stroke patients. Stroke 36, 2665–2669. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000189992.06654.ab

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, S., Ku, J., Cho, Y. K., Kim, I. Y., Kang, Y. J., Jang, D. P., et al. (2014). Development of virtual reality proprioceptive rehabilitation system for stroke patients. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 113, 258–265. doi:10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.09.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cody, F. W., Schwartz, M. P., and Smit, G. P. (1990). Proprioceptive guidance of human voluntary wrist movements studied using muscle vibration. J. Physiol. 427, 455–470. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connell, L., Lincoln, N., and Radford, K. (2008). Somatosensory impairment after stroke: frequency of different deficits and their recovery. Clin. Rehabil. 22, 758–767. doi:10.1177/0269215508090674

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordo, P., Gurfinkel, V. S., Bevan, L., and Kerr, G. K. (1995). Proprioceptive consequences of tendon vibration during movement. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 1675–1688.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordo, P., Lutsep, H., Cordo, I., Wright, W. G., Cacciatore, T., and Skoss, R. (2009). Assisted movement with enhanced sensation (AMES): coupling motor and sensory to remediate motor deficits in chronic stroke patients. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 23, 67–77. doi:10.1177/1545968308317437

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derouesne, C., Mas, J. L., Bolgert, F., and Castaigne, P. (1984). Pure sensory stroke caused by a small cortical infarct in the middle cerebral artery territory. Stroke 15, 660–662. doi:10.1161/01.STR.15.4.660

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., Shin, R. K., Atlas, S., and Grossman, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 378, 279–281. doi:10.1038/378279a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Weerdt, W., Lincoln, N. B., and Harrison, M. A. (1987). Prediction of arm and hand function recovery in stroke patients. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 10, 110–112. doi:10.1097/00004356-198700105-00022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dukelow, S. P., Herter, T. M., Bagg, S. D., and Scott, S. H. (2012). The independence of deficits in position sense and visually guided reaching following stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 9, 72. doi:10.1186/1743-0003-9-72

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dukelow, S. P., Herter, T. M., Moore, K. D., Demers, M. J., Glasgow, J. I., Bagg, S. D., et al. (2010). Quantitative assessment of limb position sense following stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 24, 178–187. doi:10.1177/1545968309345267

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrell, W. R., Crighton, A., and Sturrock, R. D. (1992). Age-dependent changes in position sense in human proximal interphalangeal joints. Neuroreport 3, 259–261. doi:10.1097/00001756-199203000-00011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandevia, S. C., Refshauge, K. M., and Collins, D. F. (2002). Proprioception: peripheral inputs and perceptual interactions. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 508, 61–68. doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-0713-0_8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghez, C., Gordon, J., and Ghilardi, M. F. (1995). Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 361–372.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Goble, D. J., Coxon, J. P., Van Impe, A., Geurts, M., Van Hecke, W., Sunaert, S., et al. (2012). The neural basis of central proprioceptive processing in older versus younger adults: an important sensory role for right putamen. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 895–908. doi:10.1002/hbm.21257

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0857

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., and Ghez, C. (1995). Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 347–360.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigg, P. (1994). Peripheral neural mechanisms in proprioception. J. Sport Rehabil. 3, 2–17.

Google Scholar

Hall, M. J., Levant, S., and DeFrances, C. J. (2012). Hospitalization for Stroke in U.S. Hospitals, 1989–2009. NCHS Data Brief, no 95. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Google Scholar

Herter, T. M., Scott, S. H., and Dukelow, S. P. (2014). Systematic changes in position sense accompany normal aging across adulthood. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11, 43. doi:10.1186/1743-0003-11-43

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogervorst, T., and Brand, R. (1998). Current concepts review: mechanoreceptors in joint function. J. Bone Joint Surg. 80A, 1365–1378.

Google Scholar

Ja Gutrecht, J. A., Zamani, A. A., and Deepak, N. P. (1992). Lacunar thalamic stroke with pure cerebellar and proprioceptive deficits. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 55, 854–856. doi:10.1136/jnnp.55.9.854

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1988). The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal-Directed Movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Jongbloed, L. (1986). Prediction of function after stroke: a critical review. Stroke 17, 765–776. doi:10.1161/01.STR.17.4.765

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalisch, T., Kattenstroth, J. C., Kowalewski, R., Tegenthoff, M., and Dinse, H. R. (2012). Age-related changes in the joint position sense of the human hand. Clin. Interv. Aging 7, 499–507. doi:10.2147/CIA.S37573

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kararizou, E., Manta, P., Kalfakis, N., and Vassilopoulos, D. (2005). Morphometric study of the human muscle spindle. Anal. Quant. Cytol. Histol. 27, 1–4.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Katrak, P., Bowring, G., Conroy, P., Chilvers, M., Poulos, R., and McNeil, D. (1998). Predicting upper limb recovery after stroke: the place of early shoulder and hand movement. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 79, 758–761. doi:10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90352-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kattenstroth, J., Kalisch, T., Kowalewski, R., Tegenthoff, M., and Dinse, H. R. (2013). Quantitative assessment of joint position sense recovery in subacute stroke patients: a pilot study. J. Rehabil. Med. 45, 1004–1009. doi:10.2340/16501977-1225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, G. H., Suzuki, S., and Kanda, K. (2007). Age-related physiological and morphological changes of muscle spindles in rats. J. Physiol. 582, 525–538. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2007.130120

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. S. (1992). Pure sensory stroke. Clinical – radiological correlates of 21 cases. Stroke 23, 983–987. doi:10.1161/01.STR.23.7.983

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. S. (2007). Patterns of sensory abnormality in cortical stroke. Neurology 68, 174–180. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000251298.12763.9b

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokmen, E., Bossemeyer, R. W., and Williams, W. J. (1978). Quantitative evaluation of joint motion sensation in an aging population. J. Gerontol. 33, 62–67. doi:10.1093/geronj/33.1.62

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuffosky, A., Wadell, I., and Nilsson, B. Y. (1982). The relationship between sensory impairment and motor recovery in patients with hemiplegia. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 14, 27–32.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

La Joie, W. J., Reddy, N. M., and Melvin, J. L. (1982). Somatosensory evoked potentials: their predictive value in right hemiplegia. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 63, 223–226.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. Y., Kim, S. H., Choi, B. Y., Chang, C. H., Ahn, S. H., and Jang, S. H. (2012). Functional MRI finding by proprioceptive input in patients with thalamic hemorrhage. NeuroRehabilitation 30, 131–136. doi:10.3233/NRE-2012-0736

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. X., Xu, D. Q., and Hong, Y. (2008). Effects of 16-week Tai Chi intervention on postural stability and proprioception of knee and ankle in older people. Age Ageing 37, 575–578. doi:10.1093/ageing/afn109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lincoln, N. B., Crow, J. L., Jackson, J. M., Waters, G. R., Adams, S. A., and Hodgson, P. (1991). The unreliability of sensory assessments. Clin. Rehabil. 5, 273–282. doi:10.1177/026921559100500403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J. X., Eriksson, P. O., Thornell, L. E., and Pedrosa-Domellof, F. (2005). Fiber content and myosin heavy chain composition of muscle spindles in aged human biceps brachii. J. Histochem. Cytochem. 53, 445–454. doi:10.1369/jhc.4A6257.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maciejasz, P., Eschweiler, J., Gerlach-Hahn, K., Jansen-Troy, A., and Leonhardt, S. (2014). A survey on robotic devices for upper limb rehabilitation. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11, 3, doi:10.1186/1743-0003-11-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercier, C., and Bourbonnais, D. (2004). Relative shoulder flexor and handgrip strength is related to upper limb function after stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 18, 215–221. doi:10.1191/0269215504cr724oa

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messier, J., Adamovich, S., Berkinblit, M., Tunik, E., and Poizner, H. (2003). Influence of movement speed on accuracy and coordination of reaching movements to memorized targets in threedimensional space in a deafferented subject. Exp. Brain Res. 150, 399–416. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1413-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miwa, T., Miwa, Y., and Kanda, K. (1995). Dynamic and static sensitivities of muscle spindle primary endings in aged rats to ramp stretch. Neurosci. Lett. 201, 179–182. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(95)12165-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morisawa, Y. (1998). Morphological study of mechanoreceptors on the coracoacromial ligament. J. Orthop. Sci. 3, 102–110. doi:10.1007/s007760050029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niessen, M. H., Veeger, D. H., Koppe, P. A., Konijnenbelt, M. H., van Dieën, J., and Janssen, T. W. (2008). Proprioception of the shoulder after stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 333–338. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2007.08.157

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavot, A. P., Ignacio, D. R., Kuntavanish, A., and Lightfoote, W. E. II (1986). The prognostic value of somatosensory evoked potentials in cerebrovascular accidents. Electromyogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 26, 333–340.

Google Scholar

Proske, U. (2006). Kinesthesia: the role of muscle receptors. Muscle Nerve 34, 545–558. doi:10.1002/mus.20627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proske, U., and Gandevia, S. C. (2009). The kinaesthetic senses. J. Physiol. 587, 4139–4146. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2009.175372

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rand, D., Weiss, P. L. T., and Gottlieb, D. (1999). Does proprioceptive loss influence recovery of the upper extremity after stroke? Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 13, 15–21. doi:10.1177/154596839901300104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riberio, F., and Oliveria, J. (2007). Aging effects on joint proprioception: the role of physical activity on proprioception preservation. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 4, 71–76. doi:10.1007/s11556-007-0026-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riehle, A., and Vaadia, E. (eds) (2004). Motor Cortex in Voluntary Movements: A Distributed System for Distributed Functions. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Rothwell, J. C., Traub, M. M., Day, B. L., Obeso, J. A., Thomas, P. K., and Marsden, C. D. (1982). Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain 105, 515–542. doi:10.1093/brain/105.3.515

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacco, R. L., Bello, J. A., Traub, R., and Brust, J. C. (1987). Selective proprioceptive loss from a thalamic lacunar stroke. Stroke 18, 1160–1163. doi:10.1161/01.STR.18.6.1160

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sangueniti, V., Casadio, M., Vergaro, E., Squeri, V., Giannoni, P., and Morasso, P. G. (2009). Robot therapy for stroke survivors: proprioceptive training and regulation of assistance. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 145, 126–142. doi:10.3233/978-1-60750-018-6-126

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarlegna, F. R., Gauthier, G. M., Bourdin, C., Vercher, J. L., and Blouin, J. (2006). Internally driven control of reaching movements: a study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject. Brain Res. Bull. 69, 404–415. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A., and Lee, T. (1988). Motor Control and Learning, 5th Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, function, and biomechanical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 721–733. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semrau, J. A., Herter, T. M., Scott, S. H., and Dukelow, S. P. (2013). Robotic identification of kinaesthetic deficits after stroke. Stroke 44, 3414–3421. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.002058

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheikh, K., Brennan, P. J., Meade, T. W., Smith, D. S., and Goldenberg, E. (1983). Predictors of mortality and disability in stroke. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 37, 70–74. doi:10.1136/jech.37.1.70

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimizu, T., Hosaki, A., Hino, T., Sato, M., Komori, T., Hirai, S., et al. (2002). Motor cortical disinhibition in the unaffected hemisphere after unilateral cortical stroke. Brain 125, 1896–1907. doi:10.1093/brain/awf183

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shintani, S., Tsuruoka, S., and Shiigai, T. (2000). Pure sensory stroke caused by a cerebral hemorrhage: clinical-radiologic correlations in seven patients. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 21, 515–520.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinaki, M., and Lynn, S. G. (2002). Reducing the risk of falls through proprioceptive dynamic posture training in osteoporotic women with kyphotic posturing: a randomized pilot study. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 81, 241–246. doi:10.1097/00002060-200204000-00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smania, N., Montagnana, B., Faccioli, S., Fiaschi, A., and Aglioti, S. M. (2003). Rehabilitation of somatic sensation and related deficit of motor control in patients with pure sensory stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 84, 1692–1702. doi:10.1053/S0003-9993(03)00277-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. L., Akhtar, A. J., and Garraway, W. M. (1983). Proprioception and spatial neglect after stroke. Age Ageing 12, 63–69. doi:10.1093/ageing/12.1.63

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommerfeld, D. K., and von Arbin, M. H. (2004). The impact of somatosensory function on activity performance and length of hospital stay in geriatric patients with stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 18, 149–155. doi:10.1191/0269215504cr710oa

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stelmach, G. E., and Sirica, A. (1986). Aging and proprioception. Age 9, 99–103. doi:10.1007/BF02432281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. C., and Choo, K. K. (1996). Spatial acuity of the body surface over the life span. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 13, 153–166. doi:10.3109/08990229609051403

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. C., and Cruz, L. A. (1996). Spatial acuity of touch: ubiquitous decline with aging revealed by repeated threshold testing. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 13, 1–10. doi:10.3109/08990229609028907

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. C., and Patterson, M. Q. (1995). Dimensions of spatial acuity in the touch sense: changes over the life span. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 12, 29–47. doi:10.3109/08990229509063140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stinear, C. M., Barber, P. A., Smale, P. R., Coxon, J. P., Fleming, M. K., and Byblow, W. D. (2007). Functional potential in chronic stroke patients depends on corticospinal tract integrity. Brain 130, 170–180. doi:10.1093/brain/awl333

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swash, M., and Fox, K. P. (1972). The effect of age on human skeletal muscle. Studies of the morphology and innervation of muscle spindles. J. Neurol. Sci. 16, 417–432. doi:10.1016/0022-510X(72)90048-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twitchell, T. E. (1951). The restoration of motor function following hemiplegia in man. Brain 74, 443–480. doi:10.1093/brain/74.4.443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyson, S., Hanley, M., Chillala, J., Selley, A. B., and Tallis, R. C. (2007). Sensory loss in hospital-admitted people with stroke: characteristics, associated factors and relationship with function. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 22, 166–172. doi:10.1177/1545968307305523

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallar, G., Antonucci, G., Guariglia, C., and Pizzamiglio, L. (1993). Deficits of position sense, unilateral neglect and optokinetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia 31, 1191–1200. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90067-A

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallar, G., Guariglia, C., Nico, D., Magnotti, L., and Pizzamiglio, L. (1995). Optokinetic stimulation affects both vertical and horizontal deficits of position sense in unilateral neglect. Cortex 31, 669–683. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80019-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vergaro, E., Casadio, M., Squeri, V., Giannoni, P., Morasso, P., and Sanguineti, V. (2010). Self-adaptive robot training of stroke survivors for continuous tracking movements. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 7, 13. doi:10.1186/1743-0003-7-13

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, D. T., Langton-Hewer, R., Wood, V. A., Skilbeck, C. E., and Ismail, H. M. (1983). The hemiplegic arm after stroke: measurement and recovery. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 46, 521–524. doi:10.1136/jnnp.46.6.521

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Sutter, C., Müsseler, J., Dangel, R. J. Z., and Disselhorst-Klug, C. (2012). Perceiving one’s own limb movements with conflicting sensory feedback: the role of mode of movement control and age. Front. Psychol. 3:289. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00289

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winward, C. E., Halligan, P. W., and Wade, D. T. (1999). Current practice and clinical relevance of somatosensory assessment after stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 13, 48–55. doi:10.1191/026921599701532126

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winward, C. E., Halligan, P. W., and Wade, D. T. (2002). The Rivermead assessment of somatosensory performance (RASP): standardization and reliability data. Clin. Rehabil. 16, 523–533. doi:10.1191/0269215502cr522oa

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, M. L., Adamo, D. E., and Brown, S. H. (2011). Age-related declines in the detection of passive wrist movement. Neurosci. Lett. 500, 108–112. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.06.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yekutiel, M. (2000). Sensory Re-Education of the Hand After Stroke. London: Whurr Publishers Ltd.

Google Scholar

Yekutiel, M., and Guttman, E. (1993). A controlled trial of the retraining of the sensory function of the hand in stroke patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 56, 241–244. doi:10.1136/jnnp.56.3.241

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeman, B. D., and Yiannikas, C. (1989). Functional prognosis in stroke: use of somatosensory evoked potentials. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 52, 242–247. doi:10.1136/jnnp.52.2.242

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziemann, U., Ishii, K., Borgheresi, A., Yaseen, Z., Battaglia, F., Hallett, M., et al. (1999). Dissociation of the pathways mediating ipsilateral and contralateral motor-evoked potentials in human hand and arm muscles. J. Physiol. 518, 895–906. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0895p.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: proprioception, stroke, aging, rehabilitation robotics, upper extremity

Citation: Hughes CML, Tommasino P, Budhota A and Campolo D (2015) Upper extremity proprioception in healthy aging and stroke populations, and the effects of therapist- and robot-based rehabilitation therapies on proprioceptive function. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00120

Received: 04 August 2014; Accepted: 17 February 2015;
Published online: 02 March 2015.

Edited by:

John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Claudia Voelcker-Rehage, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
Diane Elizabeth Adamo, Wayne State University, USA

Copyright: © 2015 Hughes, Tommasino, Budhota and Campolo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Charmayne Mary Lee Hughes, Robotics Research Centre, School of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, 50 Nanyang Avenue, 639798 Singapore e-mail: c.hughes@ntu.edu.sg

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.