Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 04 August 2014
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience

Functional MRI activation in white matter during the Symbol Digit Modalities Test

  • 1Department of Psychology/Neuroscience, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada
  • 2Faculty of Medicine, Department of Radiology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 3Biomedical Translational Imaging Centre, IWK Health Centre, Halifax, NS, Canada
  • 4Applied Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • 5Fraser Health Authority, Surrey Memorial Hospital, Surrey, BC, Canada

Background: Recent evidence shows that functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can detect activation in white matter (WM). Such advances have important implications for understanding WM dysfunction. A key step in linking neuroimaging advances to the evaluation of clinical disorders is to examine whether WM activation can be detected at the individual level during clinical tests associated with WM function. We used an adapted Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) in a 4T fMRI study of healthy adults.

Results: Results from 17 healthy individuals revealed WM activation in 88% of participants (15/17). The activation was in either the corpus callosum (anterior and/or posterior) or internal capsule (left and/or right).

Conclusions: The findings link advances in fMRI to an established clinical test of WM function. Future work should focus on evaluating patients with WM dysfunction.

Introduction

The ability to measure functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) activation in white matter (WM) using blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) contrast has potential to advance the clinical investigation of WM disorders (e.g., multiple sclerosis, diffuse axonal injury resulting from brain trauma). A key step in this respect is to examine whether neuropsychological tests that are associated with WM dysfunction can in fact elicit WM fMRI activation at the individual level.

To date, imaging methods have documented structural changes in WM and have attempted to link such changes to behavior (Anzola et al., 1990; Sperling et al., 2001; Charil et al., 2003; Ranjeva et al., 2006). Yet, in many cases, structural measures of WM integrity do not correlate with functional deficits experienced by the patient (i.e., the clinico-radiological paradox; e.g., Pelletier et al., 2009). Functional MRI can provide a more direct assessment, however, the concept of WM activation is controversial (Logothetis and Wandell, 2004). The prevailing assumptions that go against WM fMRI activation relate to two main issues: (1) fMRI signal in WM is thought to be near or below detection thresholds because the cerebral blood flow/volume are lower in WM than gray matter (Rostrup et al., 2000; Preibisch and Haase, 2001; Helenius et al., 2003; Wise et al., 2004; Van Der Zande et al., 2005); and (2) fMRI signal is thought to arise primarily from post-synaptic potentials in gray matter rather than action potentials in WM (Logothetis et al., 2001).

Indeed, there are a number of possible factors that may contribute to the detection of fMRI signal in WM. First, ion channels (e.g., Na+/K+) in unmyelinated axons and the nodes of Ranvier in myelinated axons have metabolic requirements that must be met (Tettamanti et al., 2002). Secondly, astrocytes, which are known to exist in WM (Orthmann-Murphy et al., 2008; Sun et al., 2010), have been proposed to be functionally entrained to metabolic requirements related to neurotransmitter reuptake/recycling and regulating “cerebral blood delivery” (Figley and Stroman, 2011). Specifically, it is possible that astrocytes, which have end feet on arterioles, mediate vasodilation in WM as a result of increased K+ uptake associated with spiking activity (Kalsi et al., 2004; Petzold and Murthy, 2011). Thirdly, it is also possible that NO producing neurons, which have been found in WM, yield a hemodynamic response (Barbaresi et al., 2013). Finally in terms of measurement sensitivity, BOLD signal has previously been detected in WM tissue during vascular challenges (e.g., breath-hold or hypercapnia) (Rostrup et al., 2000; Preibisch and Haase, 2001; Helenius et al., 2003; Macey et al., 2003; Van Der Zande et al., 2005; Mandell et al., 2008; Driver et al., 2010; Thomas et al., 2014).

Given the physiological viability of fMRI activation in WM, there are a rising number of studies from our group and others report WM activation (Maldjian et al., 1999; Tettamanti et al., 2002; Omura et al., 2004; Weber et al., 2005; D'Arcy et al., 2006; Zeffiro et al., 2007; Baudewig et al., 2008; Mazerolle et al., 2008, 2010; Gawryluk et al., 2009, 2011; Yarkoni et al., 2009; Newman et al., 2010; Fabri et al., 2011; Weis et al., 2011).

The ability to detect WM fMRI activation has clear implications for the evaluation of WM disease or damage. In order to prepare for clinical applications, research on WM fMRI must fulfill the following two criteria: (1) be linked to well-known clinical tests and (2) be demonstrable at the individual level.

With regards to the first criterion, neuropsychological testing has shown that patients with WM disease (e.g., multiple sclerosis) present with impairments on measures of attention, memory, and executive function (Wishart et al., 2001; Hoffmann et al., 2007; Rogers and Panegyres, 2007; Chiaravalloti and Deluca, 2008; Smith et al., 2011). The most common and profound cognitive deficits associated with WM disorder are evident on measures of information processing speed (Hoffmann et al., 2007; Rogers and Panegyres, 2007; Chiaravalloti and Deluca, 2008; Smith et al., 2011). Accordingly, one of the most common tests for assessing WM disorders is the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; Hoffmann et al., 2007). The SDMT is considered a robust diagnostic measure sensitive to cognitive impairment(s) across WM disorders (Felmingham et al., 2004; Hoffmann et al., 2007; Rogers and Panegyres, 2007; Chiaravalloti and Deluca, 2008). The SDMT has been previously modified for use with fMRI (e.g., Genova et al., 2009). In addition to the gray matter activation, at least one study has published figures depicting evidence of WM activation using the SDMT. While not reported in text, Genova et al. (2009) showed greater activation in the anterior corpus callosum and internal capsule for healthy controls relative to multiple sclerosis patients (see Figure 3, Genova et al., 2009). In fact, the corpus callosum and internal capsule are both regions that are functionally consistent with the task demands. Although the SDMT is not an interhemispheric transfer task per se, it is likely that information is transferred because both hemispheres are involved in the task [36]. Furthermore, previous DTI studies have shown that multiple sclerosis patients with low fractional anisotropy values in the corpus callosum have impaired performance on the SDMT (Yu et al., 2011). Activation in the posterior limb of the internal capsule is also likely to be task related given the involvement of the corticospinal tract in movement and the required motor response on the fMRI-adapted task. Even when present in the data, WM activation is often not reported. Rather, the result is often either ignored or dismissed as an artifact.

In terms of the second criterion, a growing number of studies have demonstrated white matter activation at the group level (e.g., Gawryluk et al., 2009, 2011; Yarkoni et al., 2009; Mazerolle et al., 2010). However, in clinical practice, it is essential to be able to interpret findings at an individual level. The current study used a clinical measure of information processing to study WM fMRI activation in key regions of healthy controls, with a focus on individual level results. Specifically, we hypothesized that the SDMT would elicit activation in the corpus callosum and internal capsule in the majority of individuals.

Methods

Participants

Seventeen healthy adults provided written informed consent for their participation. The participants (9 F) had a mean age of 27.23 years (SD = 3.36). Fifteen participants were right-handed and two were left-handed. Individuals with contraindications for MRI were excluded, as were individuals on psychotropic medications or with neurological damage. We also set a priori exclusion criteria for individuals who demonstrated head motion that exceeded one voxel and for individuals who were unable to complete the task. The study was approved by the National Research Council board of ethics.

Stimuli and Procedure

The main objective in modifying standardized clinical tests for research purposes is to keep the adapted version as close to the clinical administration as possible (Connolly and D'arcy, 2000). During the clinical written SDMT, the patient is asked to use a legend to fill in numbers that match with symbol/number pairs in a legend with a 90 s limit (Smith, 1982). The SDMT had recently been adapted for use with fMRI (e.g., Genova et al., 2009; Kohl et al., 2009). As in previous studies, the modified SDMT presented a legend involving the same symbol/number combinations as used in the clinical version. During active blocks, participants were shown a symbol/number combination below the legend and asked to respond whether the stimulus was a “match” or “not a match” with the legend using a hand held response pad. During rest blocks, participants fixated on the center of the screen.

The task and instructions were presented visually through back-projection to a screen mounted inside the bore (and viewed through a mirror mounted on the head coil) using E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc). The task was administered one time for each subject, and consisted of five active blocks (36 s) and five rest blocks (18 s), yielding a time of approximately 5 min. All subjects performed the clinical paper-and-pencil SDMT and a short practice of the adapted task prior to imaging. The SDMT was administered 15–20 min into the imaging session and fatigue was not shown to be an issue on a self-report exit questionnaire administered immediately following the session.

Data Acquisition

Data were acquired from a 4 T Varian INOVA whole body MRI system. Gradients were provided by a body coil (Tesla Engineering Ltd.) operating at a maximum of 35.5 mT/m at 120 T/m/s, and driven by 950 V amplifiers (PCI). The RF coil used was a TEM head coil (Bioengineering Inc.). All images were obtained within one 60-min session.

Functional MRI was conducted using an asymmetric spin-echo (ASE) spiral sequence that collects three images per slice per volume (Brewer et al., 2009). The three ASE spiral images have equal blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast, but increasing T2-weighting. Prior work has shown that increased T2-weighting improves sensitivity to WM fMRI activation (Brewer et al., 2009; Gawryluk et al., 2009). Accordingly, the three ASE images were combined using an inverted signal weighted averaging algorithm. A total of 26 slices were acquired, which allowed for whole brain coverage with the following parameters: 5 mm axial slices, 0.5 mm gap, 64 × 64 matrix (220 × 220 mm), 1 shot, TR = 3 s, TI = 1400 ms, TE = 68 ms, and TE* = 28 ms (where TE is the spin echo center and TE* is the asymmetric echo time).

For structural registration purposes, a 3D T1-weighted FLASH whole brain anatomical image was collected. The parameters were as follows: TR = 10 ms, TI = 700 ms, TE = 5 ms, flip 11°, and 256 × 224 × 80 matrix (220 × 192 × 160 mm).

Data Analyses

Motion correction was carried out using SPM software (Friston et al., 1995; Oakes et al., 2005). Motion parameters were examined carefully for each subject to ensure that motion was not correlated with the task. Other pre-statistics processing steps were performed in FMRIB Software Library (FSL) using fMRI expert analysis tool (FEAT) version 5.3 (Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009). These steps included non-brain removal using BET (Smith, 2002), spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 5 mm (analyses were also performed without smoothing), mean-based intensity normalization of all volumes by the same factor, and highpass temporal filtering (100 s cutoff). Statistical analyses were performed using a model-based approach (General Linear Model). Time-series statistical analysis was carried out using FILM with local autocorrelation correction (Woolrich et al., 2001). Statistical thresholding was performed in FEAT using a cluster-based approach that is corrected for multiple comparisons. Z statistic images were reported using a corrected threshold for clusters determined by Z > 2.3 and a cluster significance threshold of P = 0.05 (Worsley et al., 1992). FLIRT was used to register functional data to anatomical images (DOF = 7) and to register anatomic images to the Montreal Neurological Institute template [12 DOF (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002)]. Subsequently, in a similar approach to Mazerolle et al. (2013), FNIRT was used to refine the registration to standard space (Andersson et al., 2007a,b). Activation maps were displayed in FSLView (Z > 2.5).

Analyses were performed at the individual and group levels, although individual analyses were focused upon, in order to capture variability that is relevant to future patient studies/applications. To verify WM fMRI activation, individual data were examined against both the anatomic underlay and the raw spiral images (task vs. rest). The local maxima of clusters in the corpus callosum and internal capsule were also determined (using an increased threshold approach) to ensure that the cluster was centered in white matter. Subsequently, masks of the corpus callosum and internal capsule (based off of the JHU WM labels atlas) were tailored to each individual (i.e., the masks were examined for each subject and if areas outside of the regions of interest were captured, the masks were manually trimmed according to the subject's anatomy) and applied using pre-threshold masking to examine these regions of interest (ROIs).

In order to examine the relationship between groups with different levels of WM activation and behavioral data, split halves t-tests were performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS).

Results

Functional MRI Results

WM activation was present in 88% of participants (15/17). The activation was in either the corpus callosum (anterior and/or posterior) or internal capsule (left and/or right). Fifteen participants showed activation in the corpus callosum (7 anterior, 5 posterior, 3 both anterior, and posterior). Eight of these participants also showed activation in the internal capsule.

Table 1 details the extent and maximum intensity of activation in the corpus callosum and internal capsule for each subject. Figure 1 shows the results of the ROI analyses for an illustrative subject overlaid on the subject's anatomical.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The extent and maximum intensity of activation in the corpus callosum (CC) and internal capsule (IC) using a cluster-based threshold (z > 2.3, p < 0.05), behavioral scores and demographic data for 15/17 subjects with white matter activation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Corpus callosum (top) and internal capsule (bottom) ROI results overlaid on anatomical data for a single subject (S9) during the SDMT. The ROI mask is shown in blue. Images are in radiological view. Activation related to the task is displayed in red-yellow with a Z threshold of 2.5 to more clearly depict the activation.

Gray matter activation was observed at the individual level in occipital, parietal, temporal and frontal regions (including regions associated with visual stimulation and motor activation), as well as in the cerebellum. Figure 2 shows whole brain activation results for a representative individual (overlaid on the subject's anatomical).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Activation in white and gray matter during the adapted SDMT overlaid on anatomical data for a representative individual (S3). Activation clusters in the corpus callosum and internal capsule are pointed out in green. Images are in radiological view. Activation related to the task is displayed in red-yellow with a Z threshold of 2.5 to more clearly depict the activation.

The SDMT task revealed white matter activation at the group level in both the corpus callosum and internal capsule (Figure 3). The gray matter results at the group level mirror those depicted at the individual level (Figure 2).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Corpus callosum activation (circled in yellow) and (B) internal capsule activation (circled in yellow) at the group level (N = 17) during the SDMT. Images are in radiological view. Activation related to the task is displayed in red-yellow with a Z threshold of 3.0 to more clearly depict the activation.

Behavioral Results

Analyses of behavioral data demonstrated that participants performed the fMRI adapted SDMT with a mean accuracy of 91.71% (SD = 5.59%). The mean reaction time was 1432.00 ms (SD = 175.55 ms). The clinical written SDMT revealed that all participants' scores were in the normal range (mean = 62.88, SD = 12.33). A split halves t-test revealed a significant difference in fMRI accuracy scores between the group with activation in only the corpus callosum (mean = 87.57, SD = 5.83) and in both the corpus callosum and internal capsule (mean = 94.75, SD = 3.62, p = 0.01).

Discussion

The current study evaluated whether the adapted SDMT elicited WM fMRI activation in healthy controls. As predicted, activation was observed in the corpus callosum and internal capsule for 88% of individuals and at the group level (N = 17).

These findings are consistent with previous fMRI results for the SDMT that were shown but not reported [36]. Importantly, the gray matter activation observed in the current study is also consistent with previous studies, which revealed activation in occipital, parietal, temporal, and frontal regions and the cerebellum (Forn et al., 2009; Genova et al., 2009).

The WM activation clusters were also generally consistent with the SDMT task requirements. For instance, the anterior corpus callosum is known to connect to pre-frontal and motor cortical areas, which fit with both the decision making component and motor response required (Stančák et al., 2000; Iacoboni and Zaidel, 2004; Zarei et al., 2006). The posterior corpus callosum is thought to link parietal areas involved in sensory integration related to the visual-perceptual task demands (Zarei et al., 2006; Mazerolle et al., 2008). Activation in the internal capsule, which contains corticospinal fibers and is thought to directly connect to the primary motor cortex (Nolte, 2002), which is also consistent with the motor component of the task. Importantly, variability between subjects has previously been demonstrated in gray matter (Miller et al., 2009). Such variability might be based on differences in underlying vasculature, regional/individual differences in hemodynamic response functions, or differences in strategy or effort during the task. This type of variability may be difficult to detect when testing is limited to traditional behaviorally-based measures and will be a key consideration in future patient studies/applications. In order to examine the relationship between behavioral scores and fMRI results, the individuals with activation in the CC and IC and those with activation in the CC alone were compared. The results revealed lower accuracy scores in the group with less white matter activation, which could potentially reflect the strategy or level of effort put forth/required during the adapted SDMT task.

Interpretation of white matter fMRI activation might be further improved by evaluating its relationship to gray matter. In a previous study, we used an interhemispheric transfer task that elicited activation in both hemispheres as well as the corpus callosum. We also collected DTI data and showed that tracts connecting the activation in each hemisphere were co-localized to the white matter activation (Mazerolle et al., 2010). Thus, there is an anatomical substrate by which white and gray matter activation may be related. However, it will be important for future studies on WM fMRI to collect DTI data as well in order to allow for examination of the structural connections between different areas of activation (e.g., internal capsule). Furthermore, DTI data would allow for an examination of the relation between microstructural properties (e.g., fractional anisotropy) and white matter activation.

The functional connectivity among regions of white and gray matter activation area is also of interest. Recent work evaluated resting state functional connectivity within white matter tracts, showing temporal intervoxel correlations that demonstrated anisotropy (i.e., voxels were more correlated within a tract than with random voxels matched for distance; Ding et al., 2013). This approach could potentially be extended to study the functional connections between white and gray matter at rest. Understanding the relations between task-based white and gray matter activation is also an interesting avenue for future research. For example, whether the gray matter activation is predictive of signal changes in white matter could be explored.

Although WM represents approximately 50% of the tissue in the brain (Black, 2007), fMRI has rarely been investigated in this tissue. As mentioned, the idea of WM fMRI activation is controversial and such results are often ignored (Logothetis and Wandell, 2004). While we have shown that WM fMRI activation can be detected, characterized, and linked to a neuropsychological test, the current techniques may benefit from optimization.

In particular, it remains possible that data acquisition methods can be optimized for detection of WM fMRI activation. For example, we employed an ASE spiral sequence [which can provide increased sensitivity to WM activation; (Gawryluk et al., 2009)] and used 4T MRI, which is more sensitive than 1.5T MRI (Mazerolle et al., 2013). However, there are other studies that have used standard imaging sequences and reported white matter activation at 1.5T (e.g., Fabri et al., 2011). It may also be possible to increase sensitivity to white matter activation by using a white matter specific hemodynamic response function. Although there is some evidence that the hemodynamic response function in the corpus callosum resembles the canonical hemodynamic response function (Fraser et al., 2012), other work has demonstrated a slower response function in white matter (Yarkoni et al., 2009). Part of the difficulty in interpreting how and when white matter activation is detected is that some groups report these findings and others do not (e.g., Genova et al., 2009). This variability makes it difficult to assess when and with what types of parameters investigators are detecting white matter activation.

Caveats

One limitation of the current study relates to the investigation of the relationship between the clinical and adapted SDMT. There are differences that exist between the tasks (e.g., with the legend replaced on each trial, the working memory component has been removed from the adapted version). However, it remains difficult to compare the two versions of the task given that the scoring of the tasks is inherently different. The current study used an adapted version of the SDMT that has been used in the literature (to confirm an unreported finding). Given the potential for this task to be of clinical use, future efforts may focus on validation or standardization.

Given that the detection of fMRI activation in white matter remains controversial, there is skepticism and concern regarding the nature of such findings. From an analysis perspective, registration can be particularly challenging when the focus is on examining small white matter and subcortical structures, such as the internal capsule. The current study used a non-linear approach to refine registration to standard space. Non-linear registration techniques have been used in diffusion tensor imaging studies, which also focus on white matter (Smith et al., 2007) and have been shown to improve subcortical registration (Chakravarty et al., 2009; Klein et al., 2009). Nevertheless, registration is a common concern, and there is still potential for misregistration caused by susceptibility-induced distortions in the functional images. Registration for a representative subject from the current study is shown in Supplemental Figure 1.

From a physiological perspective, a caveat of the current study relates to partial volume effects, which cannot be completely ruled out at the current spatial resolution. Future studies should use higher resolution data acquisition to minimize these effects. In terms of data analyses, the current data were also examined without smoothing applied with no changes to the results.

Conclusions

The current study is the first to investigate WM fMRI activation associated with a clinical measure. The SDMT was implemented because it is commonly used in clinical settings to detect WM dysfunction (Hoffmann et al., 2007). Given that the fMRI adapted SDMT demonstrated WM activation in predicted regions, it shows potential as a clinical assessment tool. Given the individual variability in activation with this task, we speculate that the SDMT fMRI task may be best suited to tracking progression/changes within individuals in WM function over the course of diseases (i.e., longitudinal evaluations of patients). This idea is supported by previous studies that have demonstrated that the SDMT can be used to predict “clinically meaningful cognitive decline” (Morrow et al., 2010) and that it is the “most sensitive” test to measure cognitive decline longitudinally in patients with multiple sclerosis (Amato et al., 2010). The next step in this line of research is to use the fMRI adapted SDMT to test patients with WM disorder to further explore the clinical value of this technique.

Author Contributions

Jodie R. Gawryluk was involved in the conceptualization and design of the study, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data and drafting the manuscript. Erin L. Mazerolle was involved in the acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data and revising the manuscript. Steven D. Beyea and Ryan C. N. D'Arcy were involved in the acquisition of data and critically revised the manuscript for intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank C. Liu, D. McAllindon, and J. Quenneville, who assisted with data collection. This work was funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the National Research Council, the Nova Scotia Health Research Foundation, and the Killam Trusts.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00589/abstract

Supplemental Figure 1. An example of the registration from functional to standard space (as depicted by FSL) for a representative subject.

References

Amato, M. P., Portaccio, E., Goretti, B., Zipoli, V., Iudice, A., Pina, D. D., et al. (2010). Relevance of cognitive deterioration in early relapsing-remitting MS: a 3-year follow-up study. Mult. Scler. 16, 1474–1482. doi: 10.1177/1352458510380089

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andersson, J. L. R., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. M. (2007a). Non-Linear Optimization. FMRIB Technical Report, TR07JA1.

Andersson, J. L. R., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. M. (2007b). Non-Linear Registration, aka Spatial Normalisation. FMRIB Technical Report, TR07JA2.

Anzola, G. P., Bevilacqua, L., Cappa, S. F., Capra, R., Faglia, L., Farina, E., et al. (1990). Neuropsychological assessment in patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis and mild functional impairment: correlation with magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 53:142. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.53.2.142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbaresi, P., Fabri, M., and Mensa, E. (2013). Characterization of NO-producing neurons in the rat corpus callosum. Brain Behav. 4, 317–336. doi: 10.1002/brb3.218

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baudewig, J., Bohm, J., Dechent, P., Rothenberger, A., and Roessner, V. (2008). “Interhemispheric transfer visualized by fMRI: are there BOLD signal changes in white matter?,” in Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (Melbourne, VIC), 618.

Black, S. E. (2007). Imaging white matter and the burden of small vessel disease. Brain Cogn. 63, 191–196. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.08.010

CrossRef Full Text

Brewer, K. D., Rioux, J. A., D'arcy, R. C. N., Bowen, C. V., and Beyea, S. D. (2009). Asymmetric spin-echo (ASE) spiral improves BOLD fMRI in inhomogeneous regions. NMR Biomed. 22, 654–662. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1380

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chakravarty, M. M., Sadikot, A. F., Germann, J., Hellier, P., and Bertrand, G. (2009). Comparison of piece-wise linear, linear, and nonlinear atlas-to-patient warping techniques: analysis of the labeling of subcortical nuclei for functional neurosurgical applications. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3574–3595. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20780

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Charil, A., Zijdenbos, A. P., Taylor, J., Boelman, C., Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., et al. (2003). Statistical mapping analysis of lesion location and neurological disability in multiple sclerosis: application to 452 patient data sets. Neuroimage 19, 532–544. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00117-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chiaravalloti, N. D., and Deluca, J. (2008). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 7, 1139–1151. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Connolly, J. F., and D'arcy, R. C. N. (2000). Innovations in neuropsychological assessment using event-related brain potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 37, 31–47. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00093-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

D'Arcy, R. C. N., Hamilton, A., Jarmasz, M., Sullivan, S., and Stroink, G. (2006). Exploratory data analysis reveals visuovisual interhemispheric transfer in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. 55, 952–958. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20839

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ding, Z., Newton, A. T., Xu, R., Anderson, A. W., Morgan, V. L., and Gore, J. C. (2013). Spatio-temporal correlation tensors reveal functional structure in human brain. PLoS ONE 8:e82107. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Driver, I., Blockley, N., Fisher, J., Francis, S., and Gowland, P. (2010). The change incerebrovascular reactivity between 3 T and 7 T measured using graded hypercapnia. Neuroimage 51, 274–279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fabri, M., Polonara, G., Mascioli, G., Salvolini, U., and Manzoni, T. (2011). Topographical organization of human corpus callosum: an fMRI mapping study. Brain Res. 1370, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Felmingham, K. L., Baguley, I. J., and Green, A. M. (2004). Effects of diffuse axonal injury on speed of information processing following severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 18, 564–571. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.564

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Figley, C. R., and Stroman, P. W. (2011). The role(s) of astrocytes and astrocyte activity in neurometabolism, neurovascular coupling, and the production of functional neuroimaging signals. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 577–588. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07584.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Forn, C., Belloch, V., Bustamante, J.-C., Garbin, G., Parcet-Ibars, M. À., Sanjuan, A., et al. (2009). A Symbol Digit Modalities Test version suitable for functional MRI studies. Neurosci. Lett. 456, 11–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.081

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fraser, L. M., Stevens, M. T., Beyea, S. D., and D'Arcy, R. C. N. (2012). White versus gray matter:fMRI hemodynamic responses show similar characteristics, but differ in peak amplitude. BMC Neurosci. 1, 13–91. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-91

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J., Frith, C. D., Poline, J.-B., Heather, J. D., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995). Spatial registration and normalization of images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 3, 165–189. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460030303

CrossRef Full Text

Gawryluk, J. R., Brewer, K. D., Beyea, S. D., and D'arcy, R. C. N. (2009). Optimizing the detection of white matter fMRI using asymmetric spin echo spiral. Neuroimage 45, 83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gawryluk, J. R., D'arcy, R. C. N., Mazerolle, E. L., Brewer, K. D., and Beyea, S. D. (2011). Functional mapping in the corpus callosum: a 4T fMRI study of white matter. Neuroimage 54, 10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Genova, H. M., Hillary, F. G., Wylie, G., Rypma, B., and Deluca, J. (2009). Examination of processing speed deficits in multiple sclerosis using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 383–393. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090535

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helenius, J., Perkiö, J., Soinne, L., Østergaard, L., Carano, R. A., Salonen, O., et al. (2003). Cerebral hemodynamics in a healthy population measured by dynamic susceptibility contrast MR imaging. Acta Radiol. 44, 538–546. doi: 10.1080/j.1600-0455.2003.00104.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoffmann, S., Tittgemeyer, M., and Von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 20, 275–280. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32810c8e87

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iacoboni, M., and Zaidel, E. (2004). Interhemispheric visuo-motor integration in humans: the role of the superior parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia 42, 419–425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maldjian, J. A., Gottschalk, A., Detre, J. A., and Alsop, D. (1999). “Basal Ganglia and white matter activation using functional MRI at 4 Tesla,” in Proceedings of the 7th Annual Meeting of the International Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine (Philadelphia).

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2001). A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 5, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalsi, A. S., Greenwood, K., Wilkin, G., and Butt, A. M. (2004). Kir4.1 expression by astrocytes andoligodendrocytes in CNS white matter: a developmental study in the rat optic nerve. J. Anat. 204, 475–485. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-8782.2004.00288.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klein, A., Andersson, J., and Ardekani, B. (2009) Evaluation of 14 nonlinear deformation algorithmsapplied to human brain MRI registration. Neuroimage 46, 786–802. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kohl, A. D., Wylie, G. R., Genova, H. M., Hillary, F. G., and Deluca, J. (2009). The neuralcorrelates of cognitive fatigue in traumatic brain injury using functional MRI. Brain Inj. 23, 420–432. doi: 10.1080/02699050902788519

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., and Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157. doi: 10.1038/35084005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Logothetis, N. K., and Wandell, B. A. (2004). Interpreting the BOLD Signal. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 66, 735–769. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.66.082602.092845

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Macey, P. M., Alger, J. R., Kumar, R., Macey, K. E., Woo, M. A., and Harper, R. M. (2003). GlobalBOLD MRI changes to ventilatory challenges in congenital central hypoventilationsyndrome. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 139, 41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2003.09.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mandell, D. M., Han, J. S., Poublanc, J., Crawley, A. P., Kassner, A., Fisher, J. A., et al. (2008). Selective reduction of blood flow to white matter during hypercapnia corresponds with leukoaraiosis. Stroke 39, 1993–1998. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.501692

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mazerolle, E. L., Beyea, S. D., Gawryluk, J. R., Brewer, K. D., Bowen, C. V., and D'arcy, R. C. N. (2010). Confirming white matter fMRI activation in the corpus callosum: co-localization with DTI tractography. Neuroimage 50, 616–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mazerolle, E. L., D'arcy, R. C. N., and Beyea, S. D. (2008). Detecting functional magnetic resonance imaging activation in white matter: interhemispheric transfer across the corpus callosum. BMC Neurosci. 9:84. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-84

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mazerolle, E. L., Gawryluk, J. R., Dillen, K. N. H., Patterson, S. A., Feindel, K. W., Beyea, S. D., et al. (2013). Sensitivity to white matter fMRI activation Increases with field strength. PLoS ONE 8:e58130. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058130

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, M. B., Donovan, C.-L., Van Horn, J. D., German, E., Sokol-Hessner, P., and Wolford, G.L. (2009). Unique and persistent individual patterns of brain activity across different memory retrieval tasks. Neuroimage 48, 625–635. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.033

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morrow, S. A., Drake, A., Zivadinov, R., Munschauer, F., Weinstock-Guttman, B., and Benedict, R. H. B. (2010). Predicting loss of employment over three years in multiple sclerosis: clinically meaningful cognitive decline. Clin. Neuropsychol. 24, 1131–1145. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2010.511272

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Hauser, P., Newport, E. L., and Bavelier, D. (2010). Dissociating neural subsystems for grammar by contrasting word order and inflection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 7539–7544. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003174107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nolte, J. (2002). The Human Brain: An Introduction to Its Functional Anatomy. Philadelphia, PA: Mosby Year Book Inc.

Oakes, T. R., Johnstone, T., Ores Walsh, K. S., Greischar, L. L., Alexander, A. L., Fox, A. S., et al. (2005). Comparison of fMRI motion correction software tools. Neuroimage 28, 529–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.058

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Omura, K., Tsukamoto, T., Kotani, Y., Ohgami, Y., Minami, M., and Inoue, Y. (2004). Different mechanisms involved in interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information. Neuroreport 15, 2707–2711.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Orthmann-Murphy, J. L., Abrams, C. K., and Scherer, S. S. (2008). Gap junctions couple astrocytes and oligodendrocytes. J. Mol. Neurosci. 35, 101–116. doi: 10.1007/s12031-007-9027-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pelletier, J., Audoin, B., Reuter, F., and Ranjeva, J. (2009). Plasticity in MS: from Functional Imaging to Rehabilitation. Int. MS J. 16, 26–31.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Petzold, G. C., and Murthy, V. N. (2011). Role of astrocytes in neurovascular coupling. Neuron. 71, 782–797. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preibisch, C., and Haase, A. (2001). Perfusion imaging using spin labeling methods: contrast-to-noise comparison in functional MRI applications. Magn. Res. Med. 46, 172–182. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1173

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ranjeva, J. P., Audoin, B., Au Duong, M. V., Confort-Gouny, S., Malikova, I., Viout, P., et al. (2006). Structural and functional surrogates of cognitive impairment at the very early stage of multiple sclerosis. J. Neurol. Sci. 245, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2005.09.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rogers, J. M., and Panegyres, P. K. (2007). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: evidence-based analysis and recommendations. J. Clin. Neurosci. 14, 919–927. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2007.02.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rostrup, E., Law, I., Blinkenberg, M., Larsson, H. B. W., Born, A. P., Holm, S., et al. (2000). Regional differences in the CBF and BOLD responses to hypercapnia: a combined PET and fMRI study. Neuroimage 11, 87–97. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0526

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Smith, A. M., Walker, L. A., Freedman, M. S., Berrigan, L. I., St, Pierre, J., Hogan, M. J., et al. (2011). Activation patterns in multiple sclerosis on the computerized tests of information processing. J. Neurol. Sci. 312, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.08.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, S. M., Johansen-Berg, H., Jenkinson, M., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Miller, K. L., et al. (2007). Acquisition and voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data with tract-based spatial statistics. Nat. Protoc. 2, 499–503. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.45

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sperling, R. A., Guttmann, C. R. G., Hohol, M. J., Warfield, S. K., Jakab, M., Parente, M., et al. (2001). Regional magnetic resonance imaging lesion burden and cognitive function in multiple sclerosis: a longitudinal study. Arch. Neurol. 58, 115. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.1.115

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stančák, A. Jr., Lücking, C. H., and Kristeva-Feige, R. (2000). Lateralization of movement-related potentials and the size of corpus callosum. Neuroreport 11, 329–332. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200002070-00021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sun, D., Lye-Barthel, M., Masland, R. H., and Jakobs, T. C. (2010). Structural remodeling of fibrous astrocytes after axonal injury. J. Neurosci. 30, 14008–14019. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3605-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tettamanti, M., Paulesu, E., Scifo, P., Maravita, A., Fazio, F., Perani, D., et al. (2002). Interhemispheric transmission of visuomotor information in humans: fMRI evidence. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1051–1058.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thomas, B. P., Liu, P., Park, D. C., van Osch, M. J. P., and Lu, H. (2014). Cerebrovascular reactivity in the brain white matter: magnitude, temporal characteristics, and age effects. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 34, 242–247. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Der Zande, F. H. R., Hofman, P. A., and Backes, W. H. (2005). Mapping hypercapnia-induced cerebrovascular reactivity using BOLD fMRI. Neuroradiology 47, 114–120. doi: 10.1007/s00234-004-1274-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weber, B., Treyer, V., Oberholzer, N., Jaermann, T., Boesiger, P., Brugger, P., et al. (2005). Attention and interhemispheric transfer: a behavioral and fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 113–123. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weis, S., Leube, D., Erb, M., Heun, R., Grodd, W., and Kircher, T. (2011). Functional neuroanatomy of sustained memory encoding performance in healthy aging and in Alzheimer's disease. Int. J. Neurosci. 121, 384–392. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2011.565892

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wise, R. G., Ide, K., Poulin, M. J., and Tracey, I. (2004). Resting fluctuations in arterial carbon dioxide induce significant low frequency variations in BOLD signal. Neuroimage 21, 1652–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wishart, H. A., Flashman, L., and Saykin, A. J. (2001). The neuropsychology of multiple sclerosis: contributions of neuroimaging research. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 3, 373–378. doi: 10.1007/s11920-996-0029-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Woolrich, M. W., Jbabdi, S., Patenaude, B., Chappell, M., Makni, S., Behrens, T., et al. (2009). Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45, S173–S186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Woolrich, M. W., Ripley, B. D., Brady, J. M., and Smith, S. M. (2001). Temporal Autocorrelation in univariate linear modelling of FMRI data. Neuroimage 14, 1370–1386. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Worsley, K. J., Evans, A. C., Marrett, S., and Neelin, P. (1992). A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 12, 900–918. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1992.127

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yarkoni, T., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Conturo, T. E., and Braver, T. S. (2009). BOLD correlates of trial-by-trial reaction time variability in gray and white matter a multi-study fMRI analysis. PLoS ONE 4:e4257. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004257

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yu, H. J., Christodoulou, C., Bhise, V., Greenblatt, D., Patel, Y., Serafin, D., et al. (2011). Multiple white matter tract abnormalities underlie cognitive impairment in RRMS. Neuroimage 59, 3713–3722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zarei, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Smith, S., Ciccarelli, O., Thompson, A. J., and Matthews, P. M. (2006). Functional anatomy of interhemispheric cortical connections in the human brain. J. Anat. 209, 311–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2006.00615.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeffiro, T., Vasios, C., and Belliveau, J. (2007). “Neural mechanisms of visuomotorinterhemispheric transfer: the Poffenberger paradigm,” in Proceedings of the 13th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (Chicago).

Keywords: neuroimaging, functional magnetic resonance imaging, white matter, Symbol Digit Modalities Test, neuropsychology

Citation: Gawryluk JR, Mazerolle EL, Beyea SD and D'Arcy RCN (2014) Functional MRI activation in white matter during the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:589. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00589

Received: 11 April 2014; Accepted: 15 July 2014;
Published online: 04 August 2014.

Edited by:

John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Arun Bokde, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Chase R. Figley, University of Manitoba, Canada

Copyright © 2014 Gawryluk, Mazerolle, Beyea and D'Arcy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ryan C. N. D'Arcy, Fraser Health Authority, Surrey Memorial Hospital, Suite 400, Central City Tower 13450 - 102nd Avenue, Surrey, BC V3T 0H1, USA e-mail: rdarcy@sfu.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.