Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 27 March 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Interactions between emotions and social context: Basic, clinical and non-human evidence View all 22 articles

Beyond human intentions and emotions


Elsa Juan Elsa Juan1Chris Frum Chris Frum2Francesco Bianchi-Demicheli Francesco Bianchi-Demicheli3Yi-Wen Wang Yi-Wen Wang4James W. Lewis James W. Lewis5Stephanie Cacioppo* Stephanie Cacioppo6*
  • 1Psychology Department, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Center for Neuroscience, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA
  • 3Geneva University Hospital, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 4Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, China
  • 5Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, Center for Neuroscience, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA
  • 6High-Performance Electrical NeuroImaging Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Center for Cognitive and Social Neuroscience, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA

Although significant advances have been made in our understanding of the neural basis of action observation and intention understanding in the last few decades by studies demonstrating the involvement of a specific brain network (action observation network; AON), these have been largely based on experimental studies in which people have been considered as strictly isolated entities. However, we, as social species, spend much more of our time performing actions interacting with others. Research shows that a person's position along the continuum of perceived social isolation/bonding to others is associated with a variety of physical and mental health effects. Thus, there is a crucial need to better understand the neural basis of intention understanding performed in interpersonal and emotional contexts. To address this issue, we performed a meta-analysis using of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies over the past decade that examined brain and cortical network processing associated with understanding the intention of others actions vs. those associated with passionate love for others. Both overlapping and distinct cortical and subcortical regions were identified for intention and love, respectively. These findings provide scientists and clinicians with a set of brain regions that can be targeted for future neuroscientific studies on intention understanding, and help develop neurocognitive models of pair-bonding.

Introduction

Throughout the past three decades, a growing number of studies have shown that one understands actions and intentions of other people by shaping one's understanding and anticipation of the environment based on one's own motor system (Jeannerod, 2001; Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009; Becchio et al., 2012). Theories on embodied cognition and simulation extend these findings by suggesting that reading the intention of others occurs through a direct and automatic matching process between observed and performed actions, and via the re-activation of the bodily states that were originally active during past self-related sensori-motor experiences—as if the observers were “reliving” the observed motor experiences (Rizzolatti et al., 2001; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Niedenthal et al., 2005; Lewis et al., 2006; Niedenthal, 2007; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2008; Grafton, 2009). The recent development in neuroimaging sheds lights on the neural activations mediating this mechanism (Grafton, 2009; Becchio et al., 2012). For instance, neuroimaging studies show that reading intentions of others recruit brain areas that are also activated when someone performs the same action (Grafton, 2009, for review; Ortigue et al., 2009; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). Interestingly, these brain areas can be divided into two functionally separable brain networks (Grafton, 2009; Ortigue et al., 2009). The first, referred to as the “action observation network” (AON), involves an inferior fronto-parietal network (FPN) and includes a subset of areas that are associated within the putative human Mirror Neuron System (hMNS; Grafton et al., 1996; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Desmurget et al., 2009; Grafton, 2009). This AON system is thought to be particularly important for integrating sensori-motor information during perceptual judgments about actions (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004), and also for understanding hand-object interactions and intentions on the basis of embodied cognitive mechanisms (Iacoboni et al., 2005; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2007; Grafton, 2009). The second brain network, referred to as the “social-network” (SN), recruits brain areas involved in social interaction (Grafton, 2009; Sugiura et al., 2009; Wakusawa et al., 2009; Canessa et al., 2012). This SN includes the medial prefrontal cortex, precuneate cortex, insula, and amygdala (Wheatley et al., 2007; Grafton, 2009). Both networks include the posterior part of the superior temporal sulcus (pSTS); the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and the part of the angular gyrus that is near the ascending limb of STS (Allison et al., 2000; Pelphrey et al., 2004; Thompson et al., 2005, 2007; Pelphrey and Morris, 2006; Materna et al., 2008). Interestingly, the STS region, notably its posterior part (pSTS), is also recruited by relatively low level processes such as observation of visual biological motion (Jellema et al., 2000), auditory biological actions (Bidet-Caulet et al., 2005; Gazzola et al., 2006; Lewis et al., 2011), and other operations such as social inferential processing in tasks requiring mentalizing, and theory of mind (Grezes et al., 2004; Saxe et al., 2004; Schultz et al., 2004; Grossman et al., 2005; Frith and Frith, 2006; Brass et al., 2007; Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009).

Although these findings provide valuable information about the brain mechanisms involved in the understanding of actions performed by strangers, they do not tell much about the brain mechanisms involved in the understanding of a significant other [a person with whom the participant intends to be with (i.e., a participant's partner in an intimate relationship or a best friend in an companionate relationship)]. To date, studies on intention understanding have been largely based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in which participants have been considered as strictly isolated entities i.e., focusing mostly on the action type rather than on the relationship with the agent and the observer. However, people typically spend most of their time in social settings interacting with significant others. Research shows that a person's position along the continuum of perceived social isolation/bonding to others is associated with a variety of physical and mental health effects (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2012). For instance, people who subjectively feel isolated live shorter lives than those who feel they have strong, dependable, meaningful social bonds (Cacioppo and Patrick, 2008; Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2012 for review).

As a consequence, there is a health-related need to better understand the functional dynamic of our brain during actions performed in an interpersonal context. This is critical as we spend much of our lifetime interacting with significant others, acquaintances, as well as strangers.

A growing body of research in psychology highlights the importance of studying the processing of significant others by demonstrating the influence of implicit processing of significant others (compared to strangers) on the individual's perception and cognitive processes. For instance, evidence suggests that the emotional bond between an actor and a perceiver may facilitate mutual intention perception, with a stronger bond associated with faster intention understanding (Cutting and Kozlowski, 1977; Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008; Ortigue et al., 2010a).

Inspired by the theories on embodied cognition and simulation theories, one explanation for this facilitation effect is that intention understanding may be based, in part, upon mechanisms of self-expansion among significant others. Through self-expansion mechanisms, a collective unconscious mental representation may be formed among individuals who share self-characteristics, values, and actions in a common environment (Agnew and Etcheverry, 2006). In line with evolutionary theory's claim that intense emotional experiences during a lifetime (e.g., passionate love) may be a central human motivation to expand one's self (Aron and Aron, 1996; Barkow et al., 1992), the self-expansion theory of pair-bonding is a hallmark in dyadic relationships (Aron and Aron, 1996). As an illustration, couples in love often refer to one another as the “better half” or the “completion of oneself,” and they refer to “We” rather than “I” (Hatfield and Sprecher, 1986; Hatfield and Rapson, 1993; Aron and Aron, 1996), therefore suggesting that there is a cognitive expansion of their self in a beloved, and vice versa an integration of the beloved's values and characteristics in their self (Hatfield and Walster, 1978; Aron and Aron, 1996; Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008). From a cognitive and social viewpoint, self-expansion means that each partner makes a decision (conscious or not) to include the significant other in their own mental self-representation (Aron and Aron, 1996), allowing the formation of a shared mental representation of the self and partner. Recent neuroimaging studies of love (Cacioppo et al., 2012, for review) provide further support in favor of this self-expansion model of love by demonstrating a recruitment of self-related brain network in people who are in love (see Cacioppo et al., 2012, for review). Based on these recent findings in social neuroscience and relationship science (Ortigue et al., 2009, 2010a,b; Cacioppo et al., 2012; Canessa et al., 2012) and based on the self-expansion theory of pair-bonding (Aron and Aron, 1996; Bianchi-Demicheli et al., 2006; Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008; Ortigue et al., 2010a,b), we hypothesized a common pattern (notably within SN) of activation between love and intention tasks (Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008). The rationale for identifying areas of overlap between tasks that involve love and intention is that love varies as a function of the extent to which an individual prefers or desires interaction with another person. This preference or desire, in turn, may activate networks associated more with behavioral intentions in everyday life.

To test this hypothesis, we statistically explored the neural similarities and differences of the neural bases between intention and passionate love for a partner by performing a meta-analysis of fMRI studies involving intention understanding and love, respectively.

Materials and Methods

Literature Search

We performed a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies of intention understanding and passionate love, respectively. All papers and books in the literature published up to May 2011 (inclusive) were considered for this review, subject to two general limitations: the publication had to be a manuscript, chapter or book, and the title and abstract had to be available in English. Materials were identified through computer-based search, as described below.

Selection Criteria for Intention Understanding Literature

Our systematic computer-based search was based on the published literature of functional neuroimaging studies on intention understanding using MEDLINE library through PubMed database. Key words used for this search were “intention understanding,” “action understanding,” “fMRI,” and “neuroimaging.” Publications were selected on the basis of the following criteria: (1) fMRI neuroimaging studies; (2) with healthy adult participants, and (3) paradigms included stories (text or cartoons) or video-clips on intention understanding only. In all selected studies, participants' instruction was either to observe intentions, to infer the intentions of the actions performed by others, or to answer questions about the intention of the actions (i.e., “why”). In all the studies we classified as “intention understanding” agents were strangers (unfamiliar people) only. A list of the studies and contrast conditions for intention understanding are shown in Table A1.

Selection Criteria for Passionate Love Literature

We similarly performed a computer-based search of functional neuroimaging studies on passionate love using MEDLINE library through PubMed database. Expanding on our earlier study (Ortigue et al., 2010b), we used the key words “love,” “couple,” “fMRI,” and “neuroimaging.” Publications were selected on the basis of the following criteria: (1) fMRI neuroimaging studies; (2) with healthy adult participants in love with a partner, (3) paradigms included viewing partner's face or partner's name, i.e., tasks related to their beloved partner. A list of the studies and contrast conditions for passionate love are shown in Table A2.

Meta-Analysis Methods

To provide readers with a synthesized and statistical view of the common and different brain networks mediating intention understanding and passionate love, we analyzed the distribution of peak coordinates related to intention understanding (Figure 1, blue), passionate love (red), and regions common to both (purple). Using techniques reported previously by our group (Lewis, 2006; Ortigue et al., 2010b; Cacioppo et al., 2012), we adopted a Multi-level Kernel Density Analysis (MKDA) approach (Wager et al., 2009). This approach quantitatively tested for consistency and specificity of regional activation across the two sets of studies: it minimizes biases such as having one study that reports many activation foci from dominating the meta-analysis, and it accounts for the smoothness of reported data, false-positive rates, and statistical power. Thus, the reported peak coordinates within a study's contrast maps are weighted in the meta-analysis by study quality and sample size. We calculated the number of statistical contrast maps that activated each voxel in the brain using 10 mm kernel (roughly matching the three dimensional spatial resolution of the reported data). Monte Carlo simulations (10,000 iterations) were used to obtain a threshold and establish statistical significance against a null hypothesis that activated regions in the resulting pair-wise contrast maps are not spatially consistent (i.e., that they are randomly distributed throughout the brain). The use of the distribution of maximum values provides a strong control of family wise error rate and is an established method for multiple comparisons correction (Nichols and Holmes, 2002). All voxels (which constitute the various brain region volumes) whose density exceeded the 99.9th percentile value under the null hypothesis were considered significant (i.e., Family Wise Error Rate corrected for spatial extent at p < 0.001). Brain coordinates obtained using the MKDA method above were entered into the SPM8 software program (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) using Anatomy Toolbox version 18 (Eickhoff et al., 2005) in order to facilitate identification and labeling of each activation peak. The MNI coordinates and volumes of significantly overlapping clusters (brain foci) were extracted using the AFNI software plug-in 3dcluster (Cox, 1996). Displayed localizations were further validated by visual inspection relative to the Duvernoy and Bourgoin brain atlas (Duvernoy and Bourgoin, 1999). To visualize the meta-analysis results (Figure 1), the significantly overlapping contrast indicator maps for the intention studies and the love studies, and the intention-overlap-love studies were projected (using MNI-Talairach coordinate space) onto the left and right hemisphere Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based atlas (PALS atlas), which is an atlas of cortical surfaces that represent the averaged cortical surfaces of 12 individuals (http://brainmap.wustl.edu; Van Essen, 2005). The left and right cortical surfaces were inflated to reveal major sulci of the brain to facilitate viewing of the data.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Meta-analysis results revealing brain regions and networks unique to either Intention Understanding (blue) and Passionate Love (red) and statistically significant overlap between the two (purple). Upper panel illustrates a typical brain surface model. Lower panel illustrate a slightly inflated rendering of the PALS atlas cortical surface to facilitate visualization of the resulting activation foci. Brain activations significant at p < 0.001, FWER corrected. Variations from transparent to solid colors indicate the following terminology: Transparent red, all passionate love study only foci; Transparent blue, all intention study only foci; Purple, overlap of above conditions; Solid red, unique to passionate love only studies; Solid blue, unique to intention only studies.

Results

Based on our search criteria, we found a total of 25 fMRI studies. This included 17 studies (21 experimental paradigms) for intention understanding (Table A1; Pelphrey et al., 2004; Walter et al., 2004; den Ouden et al., 2005; Iacoboni et al., 2005; Hamilton and Grafton, 2006, 2008; Wang et al., 2006; Brass et al., 2007; Buccino et al., 2007; Ciaramidaro et al., 2007; de Lange et al., 2008; Ortigue et al., 2009; Liew et al., 2010; Newman-Norlund et al., 2010; Ramsey and Hamilton, 2010; Carter et al., 2011; Jastorff et al., 2011) and eight studies (10 experimental paradigms) for passionate love (Table A2; Bartels and Zeki, 2000; Aron et al., 2005; Ortigue et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2009; Zeki and Romaya, 2010; Stoessel et al., 2011; Xu et al., 2011; Acevedo et al., 2012), involving a total of 457 participants. The number of participants included in each study ranged from 10 to 36 (for further details see Tables A1, A2).

Results confirm previous studies by demonstrating that understanding intentions of strangers involved the brain areas involved in both SN and AON, including areas sustaining embodied cognition, simulation, and self-other perception (such as vMPFC, BA6, MTG/STG, Angular gyrus, see Table A3; Figure 1, blue) as well as in the precuneus and the left anterior cingulate cortex, and the right middle and posterior cingulate cortex. In addition, activations in subcortical areas, such as putamen activity were also observed (see Table A3 for further details). Similarly, results on passionate love activations reinforced previous studies (e.g., Acevedo et al., 2012 and Cacioppo et al., 2012) by demonstrating a distributed cortical and subcortical network of SN as well as brain areas known to be involved in partner preference, rewarding experiences, goal-directed actions, habit formation, and decision-making (for further details see Table A4).

Common Brain Network between Intention Understanding and Passionate Love

Our fMRI meta-analysis revealed a shared brain network between intention understanding and passionate love (Figure 1; purple; Table A5) that includes brain regions sustaining social cognition, embodied cognition, mentalizing about self-other, such as bilateral pSTS/IPL, bilateral inferior frontal cortex (IFC), ventro-medial prefrontal cortex (vMPFC), anterior insula as well as brain regions involved in the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopaminergic pathways (caudate nucleus, thalamus, putamen, and parahippocampal area; Table A5).

Discussion

The present research highlights a shared network between love and intention, which includes (1) areas that overlap with areas related to dopamine circuits; and (2) several regions implicated in social cognition, embodied cognition, attachment, mental state representation, and self-representation. These results are consistent with previous studies indicating that both love and intention involve goal-directed and rewarding behaviors towards a specific partner. The recruitment of dorsal parts of the striatum, such as the caudate and putamen, which are innervated by dopamine coming from both the VTA and substantia nigra, is in line with recent work in animals showing that these brain areas are critical in the development of a pair bond and conditioned partner preference (Pfaus, 1999, 2009; Young et al., 2005, for review), as well as in the activation of stereotyped motor patterns (habits) related to conditioned incentive cues (Everitt and Robbins, 2005). Although dopamine transmission in the ventral striatum has been shown to be stimulated in the presence of both unconditioned and conditioned rewarding incentive cues stimuli (Pfaus et al., 2001; Aragona et al., 2003; Postuma and Dagher, 2006; Pfaus, 2009, for review), responses made to conditioned rewards appear to involve more dorsal striatal networks, among which are outputs of the dorsal striatum to cortical regions such as the insula (Postuma and Dagher, 2006)—a brain region which binds integrated visceral feedback with emotional and cognitive responses (Craig, 2002; Ibanez et al., 2010; Berntson et al., 2011; Cacioppo et al., 2012).

The overlap between passionate love and intention understanding in brain areas, such as the vMPFC, is consistent with a growing body of studies unraveling the recruitment of this brain area during tasks that require introspections about self and by tasks that require inferences about the minds of others perceived to be similar to self (Jenkins et al., 2008). The activation of the anterior insula is also consistent with earlier studies from our laboratory and others implicating a role in meta-representations of self (for further details see, Cacioppo et al., 2012). Other overlapping areas were also activated, such as left and right IFC and MTG/pSTS with a slight left hemisphere lateralization. These areas are in close proximity to classical Broca's and Wernicke's language regions, which entail frontal and temporo-parietal regions that are more highly interconnected via the arcuate fasciculus white matter fiber tracts in humans relative to chimpanzees and to monkeys (Rilling, 2008). One intriguing prospect is that the evolution of these interconnected inferior fronto-parietal regions may relate to pair-bonding and degree of social attachments, as well as other more uniquely human qualities such as language (Kim et al., 2010).

By identifying specific functional brain regions and networks in a large sample of healthy subjects, the present analysis reinforces the consistency and specificity of the brain regions that are being reported in the burgeoning body of studies on love and intention understanding. Interestingly, by revealing an additive brain network for both intention understanding and passionate love, the present findings offer a new way to look at the neurobiology of the loving mind during embodied cognition through the lens of a specific subset of AON and SN regions of interest. This provides scientists and clinicians with a unique and strong rationale to further investigate neurocognitive models that may explain the modulations of these common regions and brain networks in future studies on intention understanding in dyads.

One limitation of the present meta-analysis study was some of the fMRI studies of intention contrasted the condition of interest either to resting state(s) (e.g., Iacoboni et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2006; Newman-Norlund et al., 2010), while others used a variety of different control conditions. Consequently, the resulting activation patterns (Figure 1) must be interpreted with this caveat in mind. However, the goal of the present study was to provide a first pass glimpse at determining (and identifying) candidate brain regions or networks that may show preferential or selective activation to scenarios related to embodied cognition, pair-bonding, and self-expansion mechanisms. The present results do provide novel support for an overlapping brain network between these mechanisms. Future fMRI studies investigating the processing of significant others (compared to strangers) during intention understanding in the same paradigm is thus warranted/needed. The systematic neuroscientific study of the modulations of the neural network for understanding the intentions of significant others in healthy subjects, neurological patients after brain damage, and patients suffering from chronic social and affective disorders (such as autism), will provide critical insights on the spatio-temporal dynamics of self-expansion and embodied mechanisms that may mediate dyadic interactions, notably among couples.

Financial Disclosure/Funding

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article: Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant #PP00_1_128599/1 to Stephanie Cacioppo), the Mind Science Foundation (Grant #TSA2010-2 to Stephanie Cacioppo, Francesco Bianchi-Demicheli), NCRR NIH COBRE Grant E15524 (Grant #E15524 to the Sensory Neuroscience Research Center of West Virginia University to James W. Lewis), the MOE Project of Key Research Institute of Humanities and Social Sciences (Grant #12JJD190004 to Yi-Wen Wang), and the Program for New Century Excellent Talents in Universities (Grant #NCET-11-1065 to Yi-Wen Wang).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Fisher, H. E., and Brown, L. L. (2012). Neural correlates of long-term intense romantic love. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 145–159.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Agnew, C. R., and Etcheverry, P. E. (2006). “Cognitive interdependence: considering self-in-relationship,” in Self and Relationships: Connecting Intrapersonal and Interpersonal Processes, eds K. D. Vohs and E. J. Finkel (New York, NY: Guilford), 274–293.

Allison, T., Puce, A., and McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 267–278.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aragona, B. J., Liu, Y., Curtis, J. T., Stephan, F. K., and Wang, Z. (2003). A critical role for nucleus accumbens dopamine in partner-preference formation in male prairie voles. J. Neurosci. 23, 3483–3490.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., and Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 327–337.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aron, E. N., and Aron, A. (1996). Love and expansion of the self: the state of the model. Pers. Relat. 3, 45–58.

Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport 11, 3829–3834.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Becchio, C., Manera, V., Sartori, L., Cavallo, A., and Castiello, U. (2012). Grasping intentions: from thought experiments to empirical evidence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:117. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00117

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berntson, G. G., Norman, G. J., Bechara, A., Bruss, J., Tranel, D., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). The insula and evaluative processes. Psychol. Sci. 22, 80–86.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Grafton, S. T., and Ortigue, S. (2006). The power of love on the human brain. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 90–103.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bidet-Caulet, A., Voisin, J., Bertrand, O., and Fonlupt, P. (2005). Listening to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area. Neuroimage 28, 132–139.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., and Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: inferential processes versus action simulation. Curr. Biol. 17, 2117–2121.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buccino, G., Baumgaertner, A., Colle, L., Buechel, C., Rizzolatti, G., and Binkofski, F. (2007). The neural basis for understanding non-intended actions. Neuroimage 36, 119–127.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cacioppo, J. T., and Patrick, B. (2008). Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection. New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company.

Cacioppo, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Frum, C., Pfaus, J. G., and Lewis, J. W. (2012). The common neural bases between sexual desire and love: a multilevel kernel density fMRI analysis. J. Sex. Med. 9, 1048–1054.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cacioppo, S., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Decoding the invisible forces of social connections. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:51. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Canessa, N., Alemanno, F., Riva, F., Zani, A., Proverbio, A. M., Mannara, N., et al. (2012). The neural bases of social intention understanding: the role of interaction goals. PLoS ONE 7:e42347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042347

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carter, E. J., Hodgins, J. K., and Rakison, D. H. (2011). Exploring the neural correlates of goal-directed action and intention understanding. Neuroimage 54, 1634–1642.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Choi, J. C., Park, S. K., Kim, Y.-H., Shin, Y. W., Kwon, J. S., Kim, J. S., et al. (2006). Different brain activation patterns to pain and pain-related unpleasantness during the menstrual cycle. Anesthesiology 105, 120–127.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Enrici, I., Erk, S., Pia, L., Bara, B. G., et al. (2007). The intentional network: how the brain reads varieties of intentions. Neuropsychologia 45, 3105–3113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cutting, J. E., and Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: gait perception without familiarity cues. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 9, 353–356.

de Lange, F. P., Spronk, M., Willems, R. M., Toni, I., and Bekkering, H. (2008). Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Curr. Biol. 18, 454–457.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

den Ouden, H. E., Frith, U., Frith, C., and Blakemore, S. J. (2005). Thinking about intentions. Neuroimage 28, 787–796.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Desmurget, M., Reilly, K. T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese, C., and Sirigu, A. (2009). Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 324, 811–813.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duvernoy, H. M., and Bourgoin, P. (1999). The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy with MRI, and Blood Supply. Wien: Springer.

Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25, 1325–1335.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eickhoff, S. B., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., and Amunts, K. (2006). The human parietal operculum. I. Cytoarchitectonic mapping of subdivisions. Cereb. Cortex 16, 254–267.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1481–1489.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., and Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr. Biol. 16, 1824–1829.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grafton, S. T. (2009). Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 97–117.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography. Exp. Brain Res. 112, 103–111.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Grezes, J., Frith, C., and Passingham, R. E. (2004). Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. J. Neurosci. 24, 5500–5505.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grossman, E. D., Battelli, L., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Res. 45, 2847–2853.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamilton, A. C., and Grafton, S. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 26, 1133–1137.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamilton, A. F., and Grafton, S. T. (2008). Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1160–1168.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hatfield, E., and Rapson, R. L. (1993). Historical and cross-cultural perspectives on passionate love and sexual desire. Annu. Rev. Sex Res. 4, 67–97.

Hatfield, E., and Sprecher, S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in intimate relationships. J. Adolesc. 9, 383–410.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hatfield, E., and Walster, G. W. (1978). A New Look at Love. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.

Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol. 3:e79. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ibanez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., and Manes, F. (2010). Clinical effects of insular damage in humans. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 397–410.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jastorff, J., Clavagnier, S., Gergely, G., and Orban, G. A. (2011). Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. Cereb. Cortex 21, 318–329.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14, S103–S109.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jellema, T., Baker, C. I., Wicker, B., and Perrett, D. I. (2000). Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. Brain Cogn. 44, 280–302.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jenkins, A. C., Macrae, C. N., and Mitchel, J. P. (2008). Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgements of self and others. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 4507–4512.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, P., Leckman, J. F., Mayes, L. C., Newman, M. A., Feldman, R., and Swain, J. E. (2010). Perceived quality of maternal care in childhood and structure and function of mothers' brain. Dev. Sci. 13, 662–673.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, W., Kim, S., Jeong, J., Lee, K.-U., Ahn, K.-J., Chung, Y.-A., et al. (2009). Temporal changes in functional magnetic resonance imaging activation of heterosexual couples for visual stimuli of loved partners. Psychiatry Investig. 6, 19–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kurth, F., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 519–534.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lewis, J. W. (2006). Cortical networks related to human use of tools. Neuroscientist 12, 211–231.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lewis, J. W., Phinney, R. E., Brefczynski, J. A., and DeYoe, E. A. (2006). Lefties get it “right” when hearing tool sounds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1314–1330.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lewis, J. W., Talkington, W. J., Puce, A., Engel, L. R., and Frum, C. (2011). Cortical networks representing object categories and high-level attributes of familiar real-world action sounds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2079–2101.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liew, S. L., Han, S., and Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2010). Familiarity modulates mirror neuron and mentalizing regions during intention understanding. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1986–1997.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., and Thier, P. (2008). The posterior superior temporal sulcus is involved in social communication not specific for the eyes. Neuropsychologia 46, 2759–2765.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morosan, P., Rademacher, J., Schleicher, A., Amunts, K., Schormann, T., and Zilles, K. (2001). Human primary auditory cortex: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference system. Neuroimage 13, 684–701.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newman-Norlund, R., van Schie, H. T., van Hoek, M. E. C., Cuijpers, R. H., and Bekkering, H. (2010). The role of inferior frontal and parietal areas in differentiating meaningful and meaningless object-directed actions. Brain Res. 1315, 63–74.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nichols, T. E., and Holmes, A. P. (2002). Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science 316, 1002–1005.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., and Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 184–211.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ortigue, S., and Bianchi-Demicheli, F. (2008). Why is your spouse so predictable? Connecting mirror neuron system and self-expansion model of love. Med. Hypotheses 71, 941–944.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Hamilton, A. F., and Grafton, S. T. (2007). The neural basis of love as a subliminal prime: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1218–1230.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ortigue, S., Patel, N., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., and Grafton, S. T. (2010a). Implicit priming of embodied cognition on human motor intention understanding in dyads in love. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 27, 1001–1015.

Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Patel, N., Frum, C., and Lewis, J. W. (2010b). Neuroimaging of love: fMRI meta-analysis evidence toward new perspectives in sexual medicine. J. Sex. Med. 7, 3541–3552.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ortigue, S., Thompson, J. C., Parasuraman, R., and Grafton, S. T. (2009). Spatio-temporal dynamics of human intention understanding in temporo-parietal cortex: a combined EEG/fMRI repetition suppression paradigm. PLoS ONE 4:e6962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006962

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pelphrey, K., Morris, J. P., and McCarthy, G. (2004). Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1706–1716.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pelphrey, K. A., and Morris, J. P. (2006). Brain mechanisms for interpreting the actions of others from biological-motion cues. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 136–140.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pfaus, J. G. (1999). Neurobiology of sexual behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 751–758.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pfaus, J. G. (2009). Pathways of sexual desire. J. Sex. Med. 6, 1506–1533.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., and Centeno, S. (2001). Conditioning and sexual behavior: a review. Horm. Behav. 40, 291–321.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Postuma, R. B., and Dagher, A. (2006). Basal ganglia functional connectivity based on a meta-analysis of 126 positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging publications. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1508–1521.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ramsey, R., and Hamilton, A. F. (2010). Understanding actors and object-goals in the human brain. Neuroimage 50, 1142–1147.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rilling, J. K. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 426–428.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2007). Mirror neurons and motor intentionality. Funct. Neurol. 22, 205–210.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Further reflections on how we interpret the actions of others. Nature 455, 589.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 264–274.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saxe, R., Xiao, D. K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D. I., and Kanwisher, N. (2004). A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42, 1435–1446.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schultz, J., Imamizu, H., Kawato, M., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Activation of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attribution by intentional objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1695–1705.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stoessel, C., Stiller, J., Bleich, S., Boensch, D., Doerfler, A., Garcia, M., et al. (2011). Differences and similarities on neuronal activities of people being happily and unhappily in love: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuropsychobiology 64, 52–60.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sugiura, M., Wakusawa, K., Sekiguchi, A., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., et al. (2009). Extraction of situational meaning by integrating. multiple meanings in a complex environment: a functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2676–2688.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, J. C., Clarke, M., Stewart, T., and Puce, A. (2005). Configural processing of biological motion in human superior temporal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 25, 9059–9066.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, J. C., Hardee, J. E., Panayiotou, A., Crewther, D., and Puce, A. (2007). Common and distinct brain activation to viewing dynamic sequences of face and hand movements. Neuroimage 37, 966–973.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Essen, D. C. (2005). A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28, 635–662.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Overwalle, F., and Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 48, 564–584.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wager, T. D., Lindquist, M. A., Nichols, T. E., Kober, H., and Van Snellenberg, J. X. (2009). Evaluating the consistency and specificity of neuroimaging data using meta-analysis. Neuroimage 45(1 Suppl.), S210–S221.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wakusawa, K., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of processing situational relationships between a part and the whole: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 48, 486–496.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Walter, H., Adenzato, M., Ciaramidaro, A., Enrici, I., Pia, L., and Bara, B. G. (2004). Understanding intentions in social interaction: the role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1854–1863.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., and Dapretto, M. (2006). Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 107–121.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wheatley, T., Milleville, S. C., and Martin, A. (2007). Understanding animate agents: distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychol. Sci. 18, 469–474.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, X., Aron, A., Brown, L., Cao, G., Feng, T., and Weng, X. (2011). Reward and motivation systems: a brain mapping study of early-stage intense romantic love in Chinese participants. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 249–257.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Young, L. J., Murphy Young, A. Z., and Hammock, E. A. D. (2005). Anatomy and neurochemistry of the pair bond. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 51–57.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeki, S., and Romaya, J. P. (2010). The brain reaction to viewing faces of opposite- and same-sex romantic partners. PLoS ONE 5:e15802. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015802

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Table A1. List of fMRI studies on intention understanding.

TABLE A2
www.frontiersin.org

Table A2. List of fMRI studies on passionate love.

TABLE A3
www.frontiersin.org

Table A3. Intention understanding activation peaks.

TABLE A4
www.frontiersin.org

Table A4. Passionate love activations peaks.

TABLE A5
www.frontiersin.org

Table A5. Common activation peaks between intention understanding and passionate love studies.

Keywords: social neuroscience, embodied cognition, intention understanding, love, neuroimaging, fMRI meta-analysis

Citation: Juan E, Frum C, Bianchi-Demicheli F, Wang Y-W, Lewis JW and Cacioppo S (2013) Beyond human intentions and emotions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:99. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00099

Received: 15 December 2012; Accepted: 08 March 2013;
Published online: 27 March 2013.

Edited by:

Agustin Ibanez, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Argentina

Reviewed by:

Aldo Ragazzoni, University of Florence, Italy
Blas Couto, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Argentina

Copyright © 2013 Juan, Frum, Bianchi-Demicheli, Wang, Lewis and Cacioppo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Stephanie Cacioppo, High-Performance Electrical NeuroImaging Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Center for Cognitive and Social Neuroscience, The University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637, USA. e-mail: cacioppos@uchicago.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.