Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 26 April 2023
Sec. Neuro-Otology
This article is part of the Research Topic Computational Neuroscience Approaches in Neuro-Otology View all 11 articles

Noise and vestibular perception of passive self-motion

  • 1Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Systems Medicine, Centre of Space Bio-medicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
  • 3Department of Civil Engineering and Computer Science Engineering, Centre of Space Bio-medicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy

Noise defined as random disturbances is ubiquitous in both the external environment and the nervous system. Depending on the context, noise can degrade or improve information processing and performance. In all cases, it contributes to neural systems dynamics. We review some effects of various sources of noise on the neural processing of self-motion signals at different stages of the vestibular pathways and the resulting perceptual responses. Hair cells in the inner ear reduce the impact of noise by means of mechanical and neural filtering. Hair cells synapse on regular and irregular afferents. Variability of discharge (noise) is low in regular afferents and high in irregular units. The high variability of irregular units provides information about the envelope of naturalistic head motion stimuli. A subset of neurons in the vestibular nuclei and thalamus are optimally tuned to noisy motion stimuli that reproduce the statistics of naturalistic head movements. In the thalamus, variability of neural discharge increases with increasing motion amplitude but saturates at high amplitudes, accounting for behavioral violation of Weber’s law. In general, the precision of individual vestibular neurons in encoding head motion is worse than the perceptual precision measured behaviorally. However, the global precision predicted by neural population codes matches the high behavioral precision. The latter is estimated by means of psychometric functions for detection or discrimination of whole-body displacements. Vestibular motion thresholds (inverse of precision) reflect the contribution of intrinsic and extrinsic noise to perception. Vestibular motion thresholds tend to deteriorate progressively after the age of 40 years, possibly due to oxidative stress resulting from high discharge rates and metabolic loads of vestibular afferents. In the elderly, vestibular thresholds correlate with postural stability: the higher the threshold, the greater is the postural imbalance and risk of falling. Experimental application of optimal levels of either galvanic noise or whole-body oscillations can ameliorate vestibular function with a mechanism reminiscent of stochastic resonance. Assessment of vestibular thresholds is diagnostic in several types of vestibulopathies, and vestibular stimulation might be useful in vestibular rehabilitation.

“It would be a dull, gray world without noise” (1).

1. Introduction

The vestibular system monitors all 6 directions of head motion, 3 translations and 3 rotations. Rotational movements are sensed by 3 roughly orthogonal semicircular canals, while gravito-inertial accelerations are sensed by two otolith organs (the utricle and the saccule). In concert with vision and proprioception, the vestibular system contributes to the perception of head position, orientation and displacement, in addition to controlling body posture, head and eye movements (2). When experimentally tested with repeated trials, vestibular perception of passive whole-body motion is quantified in terms of accuracy (systematic or average inter-trial error) and precision (trial-to-trial variability) of the subjective responses to the motion stimuli (3). Vestibular motion perception depends on both stimulus characteristics and noise (4).

In this review, we consider how various sources of noise impact on the neural processing of self-motion signals. But what is noise and how does it affect sensory processing? The answer is not as trivial as it would appear prima facie.

1.1. Definition of noise

In information theory, noise represents random, unpredictable perturbations of the transmitted signal (5). Specifically, noise is any form of interference that has the potential to alter a signal as it travels from a transmitter to a receiver, or within the receiver itself. The statistical properties of noise are defined by its power spectral density S(f). For instance, white noise has equal power in any band of a given bandwidth, i.e., S(f) = constant. The term of colored noise is used to refer to any non-white noise. Thus, pink noise has power spectral density that decays proportionally to frequency S(f) = 1/f, and Brownian noise has S(f) = 1/f2. The general power spectral density dependence on frequency is expressed as S(f) = constant/fα, with 0 < α < 2 (6).

We further distinguish intrinsic (or endogenous) from extrinsic (or exogenous) noise (7). The former refers to the stochastic fluctuations within the system under consideration, caused by the inherently probabilistic nature of the underlying processes. The latter refers to the stochastic processes outside the system.

1.1.1. Intrinsic noise

Intrinsic noise is ubiquitous at all levels of the vestibular system, from the peripheral apparatus in the inner ear to the neural networks in the brain. It can arise as a consequence of random fluctuations in thermodynamics, quantum mechanics, fluid mechanics, mechano-electrical transduction and amplification at the hair cells, membrane excitability, opening and closing of ion channels, synaptic transmission, spike timing, postspike recovery of excitability, or network connections at all stages of the vestibular pathways (8–11).

In particular, neuronal noise designates random influences on the transmembrane voltage of single neurons and, by extension, on the firing activity of neural networks (12–14). This noise can affect the transmission and integration of signals from other neurons, as well as modify the firing activity of neurons in isolation.

Several neural phenomena exhibit 1/fα power spectral density (see section 1.1). Examples are represented by the ion channel noise in neurons, the time density fluctuations of action potentials in the squid giant axon, the activity of ensembles of neurons in the human brain recorded from relaxed subjects by magnetoencephalography (6).

1.1.2. Extrinsic noise

Extrinsic noise is imposed by the fluctuating environment in which the body is immersed. For instance, it is represented by naturally occurring mechanical perturbations of the head and body, such as those occurring during a bus or metro ride. Extrinsic noise can also be applied during experimental or clinical manipulations to stimulate the vestibular system. This is the case of the random mechanical vibrations of the head and body (15, 16), air-conducted sound associated with loud clicks or tone bursts (17, 18), bone-conducted vibrational waves (17, 19, 20), percutaneous application of galvanic currents at the mastoids (21, 22), irrigation of cold or warm water or gas injection into the external auditory canal (23).

1.2. Noise or signal?

There is an important caveat in the discussion of noise versus signal processing. In contrast with electronics and artificial communication systems, in neurobiology noise is not always distinguishable from the signal, or it may even be part of the signal. Noise in one system may be considered the signal or dynamics in another system, or at a different spatiotemporal scale (13).

Variability of interspike timing (time intervals between action potentials) results from the inevitable effects of generating spikes by sensory or synaptic processes, and is a major source of intrinsic noise. However, neural discharge variability also represents an important part of the signal that is transmitted to other neurons. It becomes a critical component of the neural code by increasing information transmission (14, 24).

As for non-neural factors, head oscillations associated with daily activities, such as walking, running, going up/down the stairs, bus or metro rides, etc. result from the combined effects of environmental perturbations and neuromechanical responses (see section 7). They thus include both extrinsic noise and signals to the brain, and in particular the vestibular system (25–28).

1.3. Noise: A hindrance or a benefit?

The neural and behavioral consequences of noise can be opposite, resulting in a hindrance or an advantage depending on the context. Random fluctuations and disturbances limit the efficiency of biological networks across a wide spectrum of scales, by obscuring the signals or by interfering with the transfer of information (11; (29)). Irrespective of whether the source is intrinsic or extrinsic, the presence of noise can be disruptive by decreasing sensory precision and/or accuracy (30–33).

Although neural variability may augment from the sensory periphery to central neurons due to strong synaptic bombardment, there exist multiple mechanisms in the brain that tend to mitigate the impact of noise (8). For instance, both filtering and network topologies capable of suppressing colored fluctuations represent examples of noise-cancelling strategies (29).

Noise can also be a source of variability that cells exploit advantageously (13, 14, 34). First, variability of neural discharge (interspike time intervals) may depend on stimulus amplitude, increasing for low-amplitude stimuli but saturating at high-amplitude values (35). Second, the high variability of discharge displayed by central vestibular neurons may prevent phase-locking or entrainment (14, 36). Thus, under natural conditions, the vestibular system uses increased variability to promote fidelity of encoding by single neurons (37). Third, tuning properties of some vestibular neurons is optimally matched to the statistics of naturalistic noisy inputs from the environment (38). During brain development, internal models of physics encoded in spontaneous neural activity become gradually adapted to the statistical structure of the natural sensory environment (39). As a result, perceptual performance – which is the final output of sensory processing – can be exceedingly precise despite, or in some cases thanks to, the presence of some amount of noise.

One should further consider that variability of neural discharge contributes to noise in a very different way depending on whether noise is combined linearly (additive noise) or nonlinearly (multiplicative noise) with the signal. Thus, in a linear system, resting discharge variability contributes directly to trial-to-trial variability to repeated stimulus presentations. However, this is no longer true in a nonlinear system (40). This is why irregular vestibular afferents, which have higher resting discharge variability than their regular counterparts, display lower trial-to-trial variability during stimulation, leading to increased information transmission via spike timing than regular afferents ((41, 42); see section 4).

Not only can the nervous system limit the negative impact of noise on signal transmission, but it can actually draw direct benefits from the presence of noise. There is now ample evidence that noise can lead to a paradoxical enhancement of neural sensitivity under specific conditions (14). For instance, the addition of a given amount of noise to a subthreshold signal can jolt otherwise silent sensory neurons above their spiking threshold, a phenomenon known as subthreshold stochastic resonance [(43); see section 8]. Indeed, it has been argued that neural systems function reliably because they have evolved in the presence of noise (7, 11, 44, 45).

In this article, we provide a brief review of some effects of various sources of noise on the vestibular processing of passive self-motion cues, with a special emphasis on vestibular motion thresholds. Results from human and non-human primates will be mainly discussed. We will consider only marginally the effects of noise on the vestibular control of eye, head or limb movements. For these issues as well as for many others not covered here (e.g., active self-motion), the interested reader is referred to excellent reviews and books [(e.g., 2, 9, 46–48)].

2. Self-motion perception and noise

2.1. Vestibular thresholds

Behaviorally, vestibular motion perception is quantified by means of the psychometric functions for detection or discrimination of head-centred translations, rotations or tilts (3). The spread of the psychometric function corresponds to the vestibular threshold, that is, the minimum amount of motion necessary to reliably detect motion or recognize motion direction. Thresholds vary inversely to precision, and reflect the contribution of both intrinsic and extrinsic noise to vestibular perception (49). Here, intrinsic noise is contributed by all sources of noise from mechano-electrical transduction by hair cells in the inner ear to central processing of the stimuli in the brain. Extrinsic noise is contributed by the motion platforms used to deliver the stimuli, which typically add small random vibrations to the motion waveforms (50, 51).

In general, the idea that sensory systems have fixed thresholds - below which a stimulus does not generate any percept - has long been abandoned. In fact, limits to the detectability of small stimuli are set by noise. In the absence of noise, there would be no threshold: arbitrarily small stimuli would generate proportionately small but nonzero responses (31). In the presence of noise, the threshold for an ideal observer is related to the distance between signal distribution (centred at mean signal) and noise distribution (centred at zero signal). According to signal detection theory (52, 53), the stimulus is a random variable and the perceptual decision derives from a comparison between a sample from this random variable and a predefined criterion. The difference d′ between noisy representations normalized by their standard deviations generally increases with stimulus strength. The shape of the psychometric function depends on the distribution of the noisy representation (52). Gaussian noise results in a psychometric function that is a cumulative Gaussian (3).

2.2. Multisensory contributions

In addition to vestibular cues, other sources of sensory information potentially contribute to passive self-motion perception (visual, auditory, cutaneous, muscular, visceral) (54). However, in a typical experimental setting to test motion direction discrimination, appropriate measures are taken to minimize non-vestibular cues, for instance by masking visual and auditory cues related to motion stimuli. In such case, the predominant role of the vestibular organs has been demonstrated by showing that the corresponding thresholds in human patients (55) or monkeys (56) with vestibular ablation are considerably degraded.

Motion perception typically involves more than one vestibular organ. Thus, head motion engages both otolith organs and semicircular canals, except for specific motion directions. In particular, rotations that do not change head orientation relative to gravity only engage semicircular canals, while interaural translations primarily engage the utricles, and craniocaudal translations engage the saccules. In the other cases, the brain integrates canal and otolith cues to determine self-motion more precisely than with either cue individually (57).

The otoliths sense gravito-inertial accelerations, and do not distinguish forces produced by changes in the orientation of the head relative to gravity from those produced during translational self-motion. In order to estimate head orientation, the brain integrates rotational head motion signals from the semicircular canals with gravito-inertial signals from the otoliths (2, 9). Integration of otolith and canal signals is based on an internal model of the effects of gravity and inertial motion, and involves the transformation from a head-centred to a gravity-centred reference frame (58–60).

In the light, vestibular cues are optimally combined with visual cues for self-motion discrimination (61–64), as well as for the perception of orientation relative to gravity (65, 66). Multisensory integration of self-motion cues can be explained based on probabilistic theories, such as Bayesian inference (46). The probability of a given heading direction can be derived by combining vestibular signals, optic flow, and prior knowledge, following Bayes’ rule. Each sensory source is weighted proportionally to its reliability (inverse of source variance or noise). By using prior knowledge about the expected structure of the signal and/or noise, sensory processing can compensate for the amount of noise (11, 67).

Bayesian models of self-motion perception address the problem of noise accumulation during inertial navigation. In theory, integration of vestibular signals at peripheral and central processing stages would result in the accumulation of errors over time (68). Since these errors do not typically occur, it has been posited that internal models of sensory dynamics and noise compute the probability distribution of motion variables by modeling velocity storage, for instance by using particle filtering (69, 70).

2.3. Vestibular pathways

Vestibular motion perception is the result of multistage processes (9). Sensory inputs are transduced by hair cells in the inner ear, which synapse on the vestibular afferents in cranial nerve VIII. The afferents project to the vestibular nuclei in the brainstem. From there, the central pathways involved in self-motion perception comprise the thalamus and vestibular cortical areas (47, 67). Noise enters at all stages of vestibular processing.

3. Noise at hair-cell transduction

Hair-cell mechanoceptors of different sensory organs (vestibular, auditory, vibratory, lateral line) operate over different frequency ranges, but they all use similar transduction mechanisms (9). In the absence of noise, hair-cell responses would be limited only by the physical constraints on the stimulus. The energy supplied to each hair cell at the human behavioral threshold is of the same order of magnitude as that of thermal motion (10). Sensitivity of hair cell bundles as mechanical detectors is very high (higher for auditory than vestibular cells), and the hair-cells responses are very fast (71). A hair cell is stimulated when the bundle of the cilia is deflected toward its tall edge (the kinocilium).

Since the cell makes instantaneous measurements of the position of the cilia, in theory, broadband voltage changes should be affected by at least the total displacement noise of the ciliary bundle. However, measurements show that the signals that are reliably detected from single hair cells can be considerably smaller than the broadband ciliary displacement noise, indicating that the hair cells filter the mechanical signal (31). Moreover, random noise due to the transduction of Brownian motion is reduced as each hair cell averages its input over an increasing period of time, since the cell typically receives many cycles of the stimulus (10). Averaging can mitigate the effects of noise also when several cells carry the same signal and each cell is affected by independent sources of noise (11).

There is also a mechanical substrate for response averaging and noise suppression, since the stereocilia of hair cells are constrained to move together. In fact, thermal movements of stereocilia located at opposite edges of a hair bundle exhibit high coherence and negligible phase lag (Figure 1) (72). Coupling tends to average random fluctuations in the movement of individual stereocilia. This assures the concerted gating of transduction channels, with the result of maximizing the sensitivity of mechanoelectrical transduction and amplifying the responses. In the vestibular organ, coupling is strengthened by the accessory structures (cupulae or otolithic membrane in canals and otolith organs, respectively) that constrain cilia movements.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Coherent motion of stereocilia of hair cells of bullfrog’s sacculus. (A) Left: scanning electron micrograph of a hair bundle. Right: schematic diagram of a slice along the hair bundle, with the two spots representing the laser beams employed in interferometric measurements positioned on the bundle’s opposite edges. (B) When the green (top trace) and red laser beams are focused on the opposite edges of a hair bundle, the two records of thermal motion are very similar, except for different amplitudes due to different stereociliary lengths. (C) Crosscorrelation averaged over 20 records (blue line) and superimposed on the corresponding autocorrelation (red dashed line). (D) Coherency spectrum from the opposite edges of 18 hair bundles shows high values and negligible phase lag at frequencies up to 10 kHz. The phase spectrum shown at a higher magnification (bottom trace) shows a systematic deviation of the mean phase lag from zero at frequencies close to the analog filters’ cutoff, due to anti-aliasing filters. (E) For comparison, the spectrum for the beams focused on the same edge of the hair bundle is highly similar to that in panel (D). The results in this Figure are for quiescent hair bundles, but the results for oscillating hair bundles are very similar. Reproduced with permission from Kozlov et al. (72).

Another mechanism that tends to reduce the impact of noise is represented by the band-limited frequency responsiveness of hair cells, allowing to reject noise components outside the preferred frequency range (10). For instance, viscous drag acting on the bundles of saccular hair cells limits Brownian motion to relatively low frequencies (73, 74).

Brownian motion of hair cell bundles might also facilitate mechano-electrical transduction of hair cells under specific conditions. Thus, by measuring the transduction current signal-to-noise ratio in isolated hair cells of the frog sacculus, Jaramillo and Wiesenfeld (75) found that very small (2–3 nm) random displacements of the bundles provide an optimal noise level that increases transduction sensitivity to weak signals. Bundles’ displacements that are smaller or larger of the critical level do not enhance the transduction sensitivity. They argued that Brownian motion can increase the transition rates between the open and closed states of the transduction channels through its influence on the hair bundles. Increases of signal-to-noise ratio were also observed in multiunit recordings from the afferent fibers of semicircular canals of chickens in response to endolymph movement and consequent cupula deflections (76). Both sets of results have been accounted for on the basis of stochastic resonance phenomena (see section 8).

4. Noise at vestibular afferents

For the vestibular afferents, noise shows up as variability of discharge. In mammals, hair cells synapse on two types of afferents from cranial nerve VIII, so called regular units and irregular units (9, 77). Regular units mainly provide bouton-endings to cylindrical type II hair cells in the peripheral (extrastriolar) zone of each vestibular organ. Irregular units have larger axons and provide calix-endings on flask-shaped type I hair cells, as well as bouton-endings to type II hair cells in the central (striolar) zone.

4.1. Variability of discharge rates

Both regular and irregular units possess a high resting discharge rate of action potentials (in the absence of any head motion), so that they can respond bidirectionally, increasing or decreasing the discharge rate as a function of motion direction. However, the variability (standard deviation) of interspike intervals is very small in regular units and very large in irregular units. Stochastic release of synaptic quanta results in synaptic noise that is responsible for the variability of interspike intervals. Simulations show that the spike timing precision depends on the small number of ion channels that are open at the action potential threshold (78). The resulting variability in spike timing is larger for weaker input signals, since the probability that the membrane potential crosses the spiking threshold depends more heavily on channel noise than for stronger inputs (11). The different regularity of discharge in vestibular afferents arises due to differences in synaptic noise and after-hyperpolarization (Figure 2) (79). In the regular units, after-hyperpolarization is deep and slow, and synaptic currents are sufficient for the average membrane voltage to cross the spiking threshold. In irregular units, quantal size and synaptic noise are larger and after-hyperpolarization is shallower and faster. These units require much less synaptic current to reach the same discharge rate as the regular units, but the voltage trajectory following the after-hyperpolarization does not reach the spiking threshold. As a result, their discharge is irregular because it is entirely governed by the random timing of synaptic quanta (79).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simulations of five consecutive interspike voltage trajectories for a regular neuron in panel (A), and for an irregular unit in panel (B). Horizontal dashed lines are the resting potential (0) and the critical firing level (Vr). The dotted curves on the third interspike interval for each unit are the mean voltage trajectories. Reproduced with permission from Smith and Goldberg (79).

4.2. Coding properties

Different discharge variabilities are associated with distinctly different coding properties of vestibular stimuli by the two types of afferents, which then form two distinct channels for information transmission to higher brain centers (47, 77). Regular afferents transmit more information via firing rate, they tend to follow end-organ (canals or otoliths) dynamics, and have better detection thresholds especially for small-amplitude stimuli. By contrast, irregular afferents transmit more information via spike timing, and they are better tuned to stochastic naturalistic vestibular stimuli even of large amplitude. This has been shown for rotations stimulating the semicircular canals (Figure 3) (42), as well as for translations stimulating the otoliths (41). In both cases, broadband filtered white noise (20 Hz cutoff) was used, roughly mimicking naturally occurring head motions (see section 7).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. In contrast with regular afferents, irregular afferents discriminate between different stimulus waveforms through precise spike timing. (A) Spiking responses from an irregular afferent to repeated presentations of three different stimulus waveforms (broadband noise angular velocity stimulus, 20 Hz cutoff) with identical statistics (mean 0°/s, standard deviation 20°/s). (B) Average responses (standard error bands as shaded gray) of the irregular afferent to multiple repeated presentations to three different stimulus waveforms (red, blue, green) at 1 ms (left), 6 ms and 100 ms (right) timescales. The responses were obtained by convolving the spike trains with exponential kernels (top insets) that decay with these time constants. (C) Same as in panel (A) but for a regular afferent. (D) Same as in panel (B) but for the regular afferent. Reproduced with permission from Jamali et al. (42).

At low frequencies of head motion stimulation (between about 0.1 and 10 Hz), both types of afferents respond roughly in phase with rotational velocity for canals and translational acceleration for otoliths (74, 80). At higher frequencies, both types of afferents have high-pass tuning such that neuronal response sensitivities increase with the frequency of head motion stimulation, but irregular afferents have considerably higher gains and phase leads than regular afferents (81).

4.3. Neurometric curves

In analogy to the psychometric curves determined with behavioral tests (see section 2.1), neurometric curves are constructed by fitting electrophysiological data as a function of the stimulus levels (82). Neural thresholds are computed as the standard deviation of the distribution of the neural responses. It is found that the precision of individual neurons in encoding head motion is considerably worse than the perceptual precision measured behaviourally [(80, 81); see however (83)]. Interestingly, neural thresholds of otolith afferents are independent of stimulus frequency and resting discharge regularity (80). This behavior originates from the parallel increment of trial-to-trial variability of discharge and neural sensitivity, such that their ratio remains constant (80), in contrast with what happens with the vestibular afferents from semicircular canals (81, 84).

By using motion stimuli resembling those encountered in naturalistic situations (25), it was found that, in contrast with single-afferent activity, the correlated activity between pairs of irregular afferents provides detailed information about the envelope of the head motion stimuli (85, 86). Modeling shows that such correlation-based coding of motion envelopes requires an optimal level of neural variability (86). This represents still another example of enhancement of information transmission due to noise inherent in neural responses (8).

5. Central processing in vestibular nuclei and ascending pathways

5.1. Vestibular nuclei

Most neurons of the vestibular nuclei integrate inputs from both otolith and canal afferents (9, 47). The main targets of projection in the vestibular nuclei are represented by position-vestibular-pause (PVP) neurons for the regular afferents and vestibular-only (VO) neurons for the irregular afferents (47). Detection thresholds for yaw rotation velocity of VO neurons are even higher than those of their afferent inputs, because VO neurons display greater variability (84). Therefore, ascending pathways from the vestibular nuclei must integrate information from populations of neurons to obtain perceptual performance levels comparable to those measured behaviourally (80, 84). Thus, estimated detection threshold at 1 Hz yaw rotation was about 4°/s for a pool of 12 VO neurons and about 2°/s for the same pool of regular afferents in macaque monkeys (84), compared to 0.5–1°/s threshold determined behaviourally in humans (87).

On the other hand, VO neurons nonlinearly integrate convergent afferent inputs (88), and are ideally tuned to respond to the envelopes of natural vestibular stimuli (25). Not only do VO neurons respond to the stimuli envelopes, but they also encode naturalistic self-motion statistics through stimulus whitening, i.e., by rendering the spectral power of the resulting neuronal response about constant (40). Naturalistic self-motion can be simulated in the laboratory by applying rotations with a time-course similar to that recorded while the monkey performs normal behaviors, such as walking or jumping (40, 89). Stimulus whitening is not inherited from their afferent neurons (since the latter do not display it), but results from a match between the input distribution on the one hand, the frequency spectrum of neural tuning and neural variability on the other hand (40), consistent with the matched-filter principle (90). Further improvements of tuning to naturalistic stimuli occur at later processing stages of the vestibular pathways (see below).

PVP neurons have lower resting discharge variability than VO neurons, and do not display stimulus whitening (89). However, they faithfully encode the motion stimuli waveforms, a critical function to generate the compensatory VOR eye movements observed during naturalistic self-motion stimulation (89).

5.2. Thalamus

As we noticed above, neural variability increases from the vestibular periphery to vestibular nuclei, but it may decrease at higher processing stages. This was found to be the case for neurons of the ventral posterior lateral (VPL) thalamus (35). VPL receives direct inputs from the vestibular nuclei and projects to multiple regions of the vestibular cortex, such as the parieto-insular vestibular cortex, ventral intraparietal area, area 2v of the intraparietal sulcus, and area 3a in the sulcus centralis (91). Carriot et al. (35) quantified how neural gain and variability of VPL neurons vary as a function of stimulus amplitude, and computed the neural discrimination thresholds for yaw rotations. They found that neural gain decreased with increasing stimulus amplitude, as expected. Strikingly, however, neural variability initially increased at low amplitudes but saturated at high amplitudes. By pooling the activities of multiple thalamocortical neurons, they obtained neurometric discrimination thresholds that matched the psychometric perceptual thresholds reported in behavioral studies (92, 93), as well as the violations of Weber’s law reported in the same studies. Therefore, vestibular discrimination thresholds estimated for both neurons and behavior are affected by neural variability, in addition to neural gain (35).

VPL neurons projecting to cortex also demonstrate optimal encoding of naturalistic head motion stimuli (38). While these neurons ambiguously encode head velocity during artificial sinusoidal rotations, they faithfully encode head velocity during naturalistic noisy head rotations, with no significant phase lead across frequencies (Figure 4). In fact, during sinusoidal stimulation, a given firing rate (Figure 4E, left, horizontal line) was elicited by different instantaneous head velocity values (Figure 4E, left, vertical lines). By contrast, during noisy stimulation, for the same neuron a given firing rate (Figure 4C, left, horizontal line) was consistently elicited by similar instantaneous head velocity values (Figure 4C, left, vertical lines).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Vestibular thalamocortical neurons show reliable responses to naturalistic self-motion but not to artificial self-motion stimuli. (A) Schematic of peripheral and central vestibular pathways. Recordings are from the ventral posterior lateral (VPL) thalamus. (B) Left: Time series of a naturalistic self-motion stimulus. Right: Segment of this stimulus corresponding to the rectangle (top) and the firing rate response (bottom) of a VPL neuron. Inset: Stimulus (black) and spike train (red) power spectra from this neuron. Gray band: 95% confidence interval from a Poisson process whose power spectrum is independent of frequency. (C) Left: Firing rate versus head velocity for the neuron in panel (B). Right: Population-averaged (N = 28) firing rate as a function of head velocity. (D) Left: Artificial sinusoidal (8 Hz) self-motion stimulus. Right: Segment this stimulus corresponding to the rectangle (top) and the firing rate response (bottom) from a VPL neuron. (E) Left: Firing rate versus head velocity for the neuron in panel (D). The same firing rate (horizontal dashed line) can be elicited by multiple values of the head velocity (vertical dashed lines), which leads to ambiguity. Right: Population-averaged (N = 28) firing rate as a function of head velocity. (F) Population-averaged neural gain as a function of frequency for artificial (dashed) and naturalistic (solid) self-motion. (G) Population-averaged phase as a function of frequency for artificial (dashed) and naturalistic (solid) self-motion. Reproduced with permission from Carriot et al. (38).

On the other hand, VO neurons of vestibular nuclei projecting to VPL still exhibit significant phase leads relative to head velocity during both artificial and naturalistic head rotations (38). In the context of a different sensory system (the cortical visual system of ferrets), tuning of neural activity patterns to the statistics of the natural environment has been shown to be a product of brain development (39). Following a Bayesian approach, this process has been interpreted as the maturation of an internal probabilistic model, statistical regularization being achieved via environmental interactions shaping neural activity based on prior expectations. It is likely that a similar process occurs also in the developing vestibular system.

5.3. MSTd

Liu et al. (94) compared the neurometric thresholds for horizontal heading discrimination of vestibular nuclei, cerebellar nuclei and cortical area MSTd (dorsal part of Medial Superior Temporal area). Subcortical neurons showed robust choice probabilities that exceeded those seen in area MSTd. The differences in the strength of choice-related modulations across brain areas depended to a large extent on the structure of interneuronal noise correlations. Correlated noise was measured between pairs of neurons, and was used as a proxy of information capacity of neural population codes. Significant choice probabilities were observed almost exclusively for neurons that responded selectively to translation, whereas neurons that represented net gravito-inertial acceleration did not show choice probabilities. The study suggests that reliable choice probabilities emerge in subcortical vestibular pathways by means of a signal transformation that distinguishes translation from orientation relative to gravity. A subsequent study showed that responses of all MSTd neurons during heading discrimination are selectively decoded relative to their vestibular heading preference, and selective decoding plays at least an important role in determining choice probabilities as correlated noise (95).

In sum, the precision of motion discrimination of single neurons as derived from neurometric functions is generally mediocre, but the global precision predicted by neural population codes matches the high perceptual precision derived from behavioral psychometric functions (see section 6). The number of neurons required in neural population models to match perceptual precision is much lower than the actual number of vestibular neurons, suggesting redundancy of encoding (47). The redundancy in the system has been explained by making reference to estimation theory: central neural processing of vestibular inputs is equivalent to an estimator that attempts to determine the estimand (physical stimulus) using multiple observations (69, 80).

6. Vestibular motion thresholds in humans

Vestibular thresholds for motion direction discrimination depend on stimulus characteristics (e.g., motion direction, amplitude, frequency), body orientation in space, and individual factors (e.g., age, pathology, medication). Here we give a concise account, and we refer the interested reader to Diaz-Artiles and Karmali (4) and Kobel et al. (96) for thorough reviews.

6.1. Perceptual precision

Vestibular motion discrimination is often tested by applying single cycle sinusoidal accelerations along a specific head-centered direction [(e.g., 87, 97)], but other simple deterministic waveforms are also employed [(e.g., 62, 98)]. Motion direction discrimination in young healthy adults can be very precise, thresholds in the dark approaching about 1°/s for head rotations (87), 1 cm/s for linear translations (99), 2° for static tilts relative to gravity (100), and 0.5–1°/s for dynamic roll tilt motions (57, 101). The thresholds for motion detection are even lower than those for direction discrimination, since detection but not discrimination can generally take advantage of vibratory cues associated with the motion stimulus (50). This is because humans are extremely sensitive to minute vibrations (102). Hair cells and vestibular afferents respond not only to fluid-conducted stimuli, but also to bone-conducted vibrational waves (especially irregular afferents) (17, 19, 20). However, mechanical vibrations can engage multiple receptors (auditory, cutaneous, muscular, visceral) in addition to the vestibular ones.

6.2. Relation with motion direction

Direction recognition thresholds may depend on motion direction. Thus, a previous study found that thresholds are lower for yaw rotations than for pitch or roll rotations (103). However, recent studies using modern psychophysical methodology did not find any significant difference between yaw, pitch or roll rotations (104, 105). A more consistent report is that thresholds are lower for inter-aural translations than naso-occipital or cranio-caudal translations (106, 107). Rotation thresholds may further depend on the orientation of the rotation plane relative to the anatomical alignment of the vertical canals (105). Multisensory integration of otolithic gravity cues with semicircular canal rotation cues enhances perceptual precision for tilt motions at frequencies below 2 Hz (105). Heading direction discrimination is more precise in the horizontal than the vertical plane of the head, and along the naso-occipital and inter-aural axes than intermediate axes (108). Heading thresholds depend on motion direction in head coordinates (see above) and on body orientation (better performance in the upright orientation), but do not depend on motion direction in world coordinates, demonstrating that the nervous system compensates for gravity (108, 109), although incompletely (110).

Higher perceptual sensitivity along specific motion directions can be reconciled with the neurophysiology of the vestibular organs (106, 108, 110). Thus, higher sensitivity along the inter-aural than cranio-caudal axis depends, at least in part, on the higher sensitivity of utricular afferents (responding to forces in the horizontal plane of the head) than saccular afferents [responding to forces in the sagittal plane, (111)].

6.3. Dependency on stimulus amplitude

Vestibular perceptual precision generally decreases with increasing amplitude of the stimulus. For instance, just-noticeable-difference (JND) increases with increasing speed of yaw rotation (about the Earth vertical) or horizontal translation (49, 92, 93). Increments of JNDs with increasing stimulus intensity is due to several factors, such as non-linearities of neural processing along the vestibular pathways, increase of neural noise with increasing stimulus amplitude (49), and probabilistic decision-making based on noisy signals (112). However, Weber’s law is not strictly obeyed in vestibular perception, motion discrimination thresholds tending to saturate for large stimulus amplitudes (92, 93). Improved discrimination performance for large stimuli can help to sense motion and maintain balance, even for strong, challenging perturbations. As we remarked before (see section 5.2), violation of Weber’s law has been accounted for also on the basis of the tuning of VPL neurons as a function of stimulus amplitude (35).

6.4. Dependency on stimulus frequency

Velocity thresholds for translation and rotation are consistent with a high pass filter, being relatively constant at about 1 Hz and above, and increasing with decreasing frequency below 1 Hz (87, 98, 113). In particular, velocity tilt thresholds increase in threshold up to about 1 Hz, but then decrease at 2 Hz (105). High-pass filtering presumably affects perceptual decision making, but not perceptual magnitude estimates (114). On the other hand, roll tilt displacement thresholds are consistent with a predominant positional cue at low frequencies (provided by otoliths) and a predominant velocity cue at high frequencies (provided by semicircular canals). Position tilt thresholds show a plateau below about 0.1 Hz and decrease with increasing frequency (55, 57).

6.5. Response variability

The specific values of the vestibular motion discrimination thresholds exhibit small session-to-session, intra-subject variability (115–117), unless discrimination is experimentally trained (see below). However, thresholds vary considerably (up to two orders of magnitude) across different healthy individuals of the same age (4, 107, 118). Attention, motivation, fatigue, and brain functional organization are examples of individual features that may affect the specific value of motion threshold for the same input stimuli (119). Individual motor skills also may affect motion thresholds. For instance, competitive gymnasts, with a long-term training in the maintenance of exquisite postural control, show superior pitch detection thresholds compared to control participants (120).

6.6. Conditioning and learning

Experimental exposure to conditioning passive motions can modify perceptual thresholds in a subsequent session. Thus, 10-min of conditioning stimuli consisting of high-amplitude stochastic yaw rotations (up to 300 deg/s2) increased yaw perceptual thresholds substantially (121). By contrast, 20-min of conditioning stimuli consisting of small-amplitude, subliminal interaural sinusoidal translations (2 cm/s2) significantly reduced the thresholds for motion discrimination along the same axis (122), as well as along the naso-occipital axis (123). Also, 5-days of intensive roll-tilt training ameliorated the corresponding discrimination thresholds as well as postural sway, if performance feedback was given after each trial (124). Presumably, both motor skills practice in experts (120) and perceptual training in non-experts (124) increase the signal-to-noise ratio of vestibular processing. However, the neural mechanisms underlying these perceptual improvements remain unknown. One possibility is that improvements result from an enhanced ability to integrate canal and otolith signals. Specifically, perceptual thresholds might be lowered by progressively reweighting different vestibular neural signals via Hebbian synaptic plasticity (124). The weight given to each source would progressively become proportional to its reliability (the inverse of the variance of the source).

6.7. Age dependency

On average, velocity motion thresholds do not depend on sex, but they depend on age (99, 107). Average thresholds do not change significantly with age between 18 and 40 years. However, after the age of 40, performance deteriorates systematically by about 15–83% per decade, the specific extent of decrement depending on motion direction (107) (see section 9.1 for potential substrates of these age effects).

6.8. Relation with VOR and posture

Since the JND for yaw rotations is not significantly different from the trial-to-trial variability of vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), it has been argued that noise sources inherent to the nervous system are shared by perceptual and VOR imprecision ((49); see however (125) for a different conclusion). A similar argument applies to the observed correlation between vestibular thresholds and postural stability. Higher thresholds tend to be associated with greater postural sway in young healthy people (101, 126). In elderly people, higher vestibular thresholds correlate with balance test failures (107). It has been hypothesized that postural sway arises from sensory noise, including vestibular, proprioceptive, and visual sources, in addition to motor noise (127).

7. Perceptual effects of externally applied noisy stimuli

As noticed above (see section 6.1), vestibular perceptual thresholds are typically tested with simple deterministic motion stimuli, such as sinusoidal stimuli. In this case, extrinsic noise is contributed by the random vibrations that motion platforms typically add to the motion stimuli [(50, 51), see section 2.1]. This kind of noise obviously does not represent the physical stimulus, and must be disentangled from the signal by the brain for the discrimination task.

7.1. Head oscillations under natural conditions

However, extrinsic noise does represent part of the physical stimulus processed by the vestibular system under most circumstances of daily life. Thus, the head routinely undergoes random displacements in all directions, for instance during standing posture, walking, running, going up/down the stairs, bus or metro rides, etc. These head displacements result from the combined effects of environmental perturbations (e.g., contact forces) and neuromechanical responses, the latter including voluntary movements, stabilizing reflexes, and body (head, neck, torso, limbs) biomechanics (25–28, 128). Interestingly, these head displacements provide both extrinsic noise and signals to the brain, since they contain information as to the specific behavioral context (i.e., they are different when walking vs. playing soccer).

Head oscillations have been quantified in different animal species (rodents, monkeys and humans) during normal activities typical of the animal species (25, 85, 129). These oscillations reach high intensities and contain a substantial power at high frequencies (up to 20 Hz). Moreover, they deviate from scale invariance (i.e., the power law dependency on frequency, S(f) = 1/fα) for both raw data (25, 129) and envelopes (85). Importantly, as we previously remarked (see sections 4 and 5), several vestibular neurons are less well tuned to deterministic sinusoidal stimuli than to broadband stochastic stimuli that reproduce natural head oscillations recorded during normal behaviours (40, 89). This has been shown to be the case for the irregular afferents of the vestibular cranial nerve (42), VO neurons in the vestibular nuclei (40), and neurons of the ventral posterior lateral (VPL) thalamus projecting to the vestibular cortex (38). The idea is that vestibular responses are molded on the natural biomechanical behavior of the head. Variability and tuning of specific populations of vestibular neurons complement the statistics of natural head motion stimuli (40). These neurons encode head velocity at all useful frequencies of stochastic motion stimuli much more faithfully than they do for sinusoidal stimuli [Figure 4; (38)].

7.2. Perceptual responses to mechanical perturbations

In view of the above background, it is interesting to consider also the perceptual responses to noisy external perturbations that roughly mimic natural conditions. The hypothesis is that some optimal level of random perturbations, entraining specific populations of central vestibular neurons, can enhance vestibular motion discrimination.

In humans, mechanical perturbations applied during vestibular motion discrimination tasks have been shown to increase, decrease or leave unaffected the thresholds, depending on the type and intensity of noise. Thus, wide-spectrum vibrations (1–500 Hz colored noise) applied directly to the mastoid did not affect the threshold for yaw rotation discrimination (130). Strong (about 140 cm/s2 peak acceleration) vertical whole-body oscillations (6 Hz) significantly increased (almost doubled) horizontal heading-direction thresholds (16). On the other hand, weak (between about 3 and 6 cm/s2 RMS) whole-body oscillations in the antero-posterior direction significantly decreased the thresholds of motion discrimination in the same direction as the perturbations (15).

In the latter study, the mechanical perturbations were added on top of single cycles of sinusoidal acceleration (Figures 5AE). Perturbations consisted of bandpass (1.8–30 Hz) white noise ε ( t ) , scaled in amplitude as a function of the variance δ2 of the acceleration stimulus corresponding to the individual threshold determined for each participant during the baseline condition, given the large intersubject variability of the baseline values (see section 6.5):

ε ( t ) = ε ( t ) k δ 2     (1)
FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Enhancement of vestibular motion discrimination by stochastic whole-body perturbations. Sitting participants reported the perceived direction of antero-posterior 1 Hz sinusoidal motions. The amplitude of the stimulus was adjusted based on an adaptive staircase. (A). Unperturbed stimuli in the forward (thick lines) and backward (thin lines) directions at baseline threshold (blue horizontal line) for a typical subject. (B–E) Perturbed stimuli with increasing noise amplitude [k = 0.5–2 in Equation 2, from panels (B–E)]. (F) Box-and-whisker plots of motion discrimination thresholds at population level (N = 30 participants). Overlaid line is the best fit of the population data with Equation 9 of stochastic resonance. Data from La Scaleia et al. (15).

The proportionality constant k was 0, 0.5, 1, 1.5, or 2 in different blocks of trials (Figures 5AE), presented in randomized order. The overall acceleration stimuli applied in the trials with noise were:

a ( t ) = A s i n ( 2 π f t ) + ε ( t )     (2)

It was found that the threshold was non-linearly related to noise level (Figure 5F). The lowest thresholds (best performance enhancement) were obtained at low-to-intermediate noise levels, while thresholds with higher noise did not differ from baseline values without noise. However, there was considerable inter-subject variability of the optimal noise level and the extent of performance improvement. Participants with higher thresholds in the unperturbed conditions benefitted more (i.e., showed greater threshold improvements) from added noise than participants with lower unperturbed thresholds.

7.3. Perceptual responses to GVS

Galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) is frequently used to probe the vestibular system. GVS consists in electrical stimulation of the peripheral vestibular system obtained by placing an anodal electrode on the mastoid process behind one ear and a cathodal electrode behind the other ear (21, 22). The vestibular stimulation occurs without head motion. Noisy broadband GVS has been shown to ameliorate vestibular perception (131–135), as well as other vestibular-mediated responses such as ocular counter-rolling (136), balance (133, 137–139), vestibulo-spinal reflexes (135), mobility (140), locomotion (134, 141), and cross-modal visual perception (142). The effects on performance generally follow the nonlinear trend observed by (15) using mechanical noise, i.e., optimal GVS amplitude coincides with low-to-intermediate noise levels. As in the case of mechanical perturbations, also for GVS the optimal stimulus amplitude and the extent of performance improvement are quite variable across individuals [(e.g., 137–139, 143)].

Optimal GVS amplitude has been shown to depend also on the individual level of vestibular function. Thus, greater noise amplitudes are generally required for individuals with reduced vestibular function due to aging (136) or bilateral vestibulopathy (143). However, persons with vestibular hypofunction improve more in response to GVS than normovestibular persons (143, 144).

As mentioned above, adding GVS on top of head motion stimuli can improve the thresholds of motion direction discrimination. Thus, GVS significantly decreased the thresholds for roll-tilt (131, 132) and inter-aural translation (145), but not the thresholds for yaw-rotation with the head pitched forward 71° (primarily resulting in stimulation of the semicircular canals), suggesting that GVS mainly affects otolith-mediated perception (146).

7.4. Neural substrates of GVS responses

In contrast with the hypothesis that GVS mainly affects the otolith system, an electrophysiological study in macaques showed that GVS generates robust activation of both canal and otolith primary afferents, with the result that neuronal detection thresholds in response to GVS are about constant across all afferents (147). This study also showed that the neural tuning of vestibular afferents differs drastically for GVS versus natural motion stimulation, due to the fact that GVS bypasses both the body biomechanics and the mechano-electrical transduction of the vestibular organs (147). This is because GVS directly activates the hair cells and vestibular afferents via electrical transmission (21). Since GVS determines simultaneous activation of primary afferents from all vestibular end organs on one side with concomitant inhibition of those on the contralateral side, the resulting stimulation pattern has no physiological motion equivalent (147). Moreover, GVS produces asymmetric activation of both semicircular canal and otolith afferents to the onset versus offset and cathode versus anode of applied current (148). In this respect, therefore, using mechanical noise instead of electrical noise to probe vestibular function may yield results more directly comparable with the natural behavior of the system.

8. Stochastic resonance and related phenomena

We have previously remarked that, depending on the context, noise can degrade or improve information processing and performance. In particular, there is now considerable evidence that a given amount of noise can lead to a paradoxical enhancement of neural sensitivity under specific conditions (see sections 1.2 and 3). In particular, the non-linear responses elicited by the application of either mechanical or galvanic noise (see section 7) are reminiscent of the phenomenon of subthreshold stochastic resonance, whereby an optimum level of random noise enhances the detection of weak signals (34, 43, 149, 150).

8.1. Classical stochastic resonance for periodic stimuli

Stochastic resonance was initially proposed in geophysics to account for the 100,000-year periodicity of the Earth ice ages (151, 152). According to this hypothesis, a weak periodicity in the insolation due to changes of Earth orbital parameters would determine regular transitions in the bistable energy potential describing long-term changes in global climate (151, 152). The concept and underlying formalism of stochastic resonance have subsequently found wide application to several neurophysiological phenomena subject to noise. We now summarize the key elements (34, 153).

The overdamped motion x(t) of a Brownian particle with a bistable potential in the presence of noise and periodic forcing is described by:

d x ( t ) / d t = d U ( x ) / d x + A 0 cos ( ω 0 t ) + ε ( t )     (3)

where the trigonometric function represents a weak periodic forcing and ε(t) is random noise (intrinsic or extrinsic as in Equations 1, 2). ε(t) is classically Gaussian white noise (zero mean in the time-domain) and strength equal to q2 ( k δ 2 in Equation 1). U(x) is the reflection-symmetric quartic potential driving the dynamics of the system:

U ( x ) = x 4 4 λ x 2 2     (4)

This double-well potential has two minima located at x ± = ± λ , separated by a potential barrier with height equal to:

Δ U ( x ) = λ 2 4     (5)

In the absence of periodic forcing, x(t) fluctuates around the local stable states with a statistical variance proportional to noise intensity. Noise-induced transitions are described by the Kramers rate equation of chemical kinetics (154), with transition time given by:

τ ( q 2 ) = 1 2 π λ e λ 2 2 q 2     (6)

In the presence of periodic forcing, the reflection symmetry of the system is broken. The new potential W(x,t) becomes:

W ( x , t ) = U ( x ) A 0 x ( t ) cos ( ω 0 t )     (7)

The periodic signal now biases the particle toward one or the other potential well, so that the particle jumps to the more globally stable well. For small amplitudes, the mean response of the system to the periodic signal is given by:

x ( t ) = A ( q 2 ) cos ( ω 0 t φ ( q 2 ) )     (8)

where A(q2) and ϕ (q2) represent, respectively, the noise dependent amplitude and phase lag:

A ( q 2 ) = A 0 λ q 2 τ ( q 2 ) τ ( q 2 ) 2 + ω 0 2 4     (9)
φ ( q 2 ) = arctan ( ω 0 2 τ ( q 2 ) )     (10)

It has subsequently been shown that bell-shaped relationships captured by Equation 9 between the detection performance and noise can appear also with threshold-like systems (155) and in response to aperiodic signals (156).

8.2. Stochastic resonance in neurons

Stochastic resonance can arise in low-dimensional excitable systems, as shown for the Fitzhugh–Nagumo (FHN) neuron model. An optimal nonzero noise intensity maximizes the signal transmission in bistable FHN in response to either periodic or aperiodic inputs superimposed on a noisy background (8, 155). Stochastic resonance has also been found in higher-dimensional neuronal processes, due to the resemblance between interspike interval histograms and residence-time distributions of noise-driven bistable systems (34). Thus, stochastic resonance has been advocated for the observations that noise externally added to muscle spindles (157), cutaneous receptors (158) or vestibular hair cells (75, 76) enhances their responses to weak stimuli, which normally would be subliminal. Also, noise added to visual (159), auditory (160, 161), tactile (162) stimuli, or added directly to cortical networks (163) can improve the corresponding sensory thresholds.

Endogenous noise can also generate stochastic resonance (164). This was suggested to be the case for the Brownian motion of hair cell bundles and the enhancement of mechano-electrical transduction (see section 3) (75).

8.3. Perceptual effects of stochastic resonance

The effects of extrinsic noise on vestibular function have been reviewed above (see section 7). As a visualization aid to compare the results with stochastic resonance behavior, Equation 9 prediction is overlaid on the population results of vestibular motion discrimination in the presence of mechanical noise in Figure 5F (15). It has been argued that, at threshold, the perceptual discrimination of motion direction in a two-alternative forced-choice task [such as that of La Scaleia et al. (15)] involves a bistable potential well (see section 8.1): one well corresponds to perception of motion in one direction, and the other well perception of motion in the opposite direction (165). If the perceptual decision behaves like a Brownian particle, an optimal level of noise added to the system breaks the potential symmetry, and shifts the responses toward the correct direction with small physical stimuli. This would result in a stochastic resonance effect (165).

8.4. Stochastic facilitation

It has been proposed to use the more general term of stochastic facilitation to encompass biologically relevant noise benefits (24). This is because these benefits are not necessarily associated with frequency resonance in the system, and the responses may not strictly obey Equation 9 characteristic of classical stochastic resonance.

The presence or absence of stochastic resonance (or facilitation) in response to extrinsic noise depends not only on the amplitude of the extrinsic noise but also on that of the intrinsic one. Thus, stochastic resonance does not occur when the endogenous noise level is already equal to or higher than the optimal noise level for stochastic resonance (150). In this case, adding extrinsic noise would only degrade the detection performance. The influence of endogenous noise also explains why the optimal level of extrinsic noise and the extent of performance improvement are so variable across individuals [(e.g., 15, 137–139, 143, 162, 163)]. As we previously remarked for the variability of vestibular motion thresholds (see section 6.5), neural noise can vary substantially as a function of individual factors, such as attention, motivation, fatigue, brain functional organization (11, 119).

In this regards, a long-known psychological phenomenon - the Yerkes-Dodson law - is closely reminiscent of stochastic resonance: a cognitive performance increases with physiological arousal, but only up to a point (166). When arousal becomes too high, performance decreases. However, both the cognitive performance and the arousal are quite variable across individuals.

Parallel summing arrays of noisy threshold elements can display another form of stochastic resonance, which occurs when the signal is predominantly suprathreshold (167). It has been shown that, in theory, this so-called suprathreshold stochastic resonance can lead to significantly greater signal enhancements than can be obtained using subthreshold signal levels (167).

Stochastic resonance is but one of the phenomena exhibited by neural systems driven by noise (8, 168). For instance, if a neuron receives a subthreshold periodic input, noise can elicit spike firings that tend to be locked to the input (13). This is because a small amount of noise will determine an output with the strongest signature of the periodic input. This effect depends on a linearization of the threshold by noise, and on disruption of phase locking.

Another related phenomenon is represented by the coherence resonance, which consists in noise-sustained oscillations at an optimal noise intensity. Moreover, noise can enhance the synchronization and pattern formation in excitable systems (8, 168).

9. Some clinical considerations

9.1. Aging

As we remarked above (see section 6.7), perceptual thresholds for motion discrimination tend to deteriorate in asymptomatic older persons. Vestibular function is affected by aging after the age of 40, about two decades earlier than visual acuity, odor discrimination, or speech intelligibility (107). It has been argued that this early functional decline may depend on the specific vulnerability of the vestibular apparatus to the action of free radicals (107). Vestibular afferents may be damaged by free radicals earlier than other sensory afferents because of their higher metabolic demands. Primary vestibular afferents and central vestibular neurons in primates have high resting discharge rates (9). High resting activity involves heavy metabolic loads, with extensive oxidative ATP production by the mitochondria. Mitochondria can contribute oxidative stress and free radicals, which may cause local damage to the vestibular system (107).

9.1.1. Effects on postural stability

The decline of vestibular function with aging affects both motion perception and postural stability. In fact, multivariate analyses showed that roll tilt perceptual thresholds correlate robustly with age and performance in static balance tasks (169). For example, at 60 years of age, the chance of failing the most demanding postural condition (i.e., standing with eyes closed on foam support, which requires increased vestibular reliance) was about 5% for the participants with the lowest roll tilt thresholds, but it was about 80% for those with the highest roll tilt thresholds (169). The relevance of this finding is that it is known that failure in this demanding postural task correlates with a high risk of falls in daily life (170).

9.2. Vestibulopathies

Vestibular motion thresholds are also elevated in the peripheral and central vestibulopathies that determine hypofunction [(see 4, 96)]. Vestibular hypofunction can be idiopathic, postsurgical, neoplastic, autoimmune, or it can result from Meniere’s disease or medication side effects. For instance, earth-vertical translation thresholds in patients with total bilateral labyrinthectomy (vestibular ablation) were up to 57 times greater than normal (55). In these patients, performance decrements were smaller for discrimination of motion directions (e.g., roll tilt) where other sensory cues (e.g., somatosensory) can contribute appreciably. Idiopathic bilateral vestibulopathy mainly affects lateral canal and utricular thresholds, while it may spare vertical canal and saccular function (171). In patients with unilateral vestibular nerve section, detection thresholds for yaw rotation were 2 higher than for healthy persons, as expected by assuming that the lesion halved the variance of both the signal and noise (172). In vestibular migraine (173) and persistent perceptual-postural dizziness (174) motion thresholds can be lower than in age-matched controls, indicating enhanced sensitivity of the vestibular apparatus.

As we remarked before (see sections 4, 5, and 7), healthy vestibular neurons at different stages of central pathways are tuned to natural head motion stimuli. Head motion is first sensed and then processed by central vestibulo-motor pathways. This results in a feedback loop that influences subsequent behavior, including head motion itself. Accordingly, loss of peripheral vestibular inputs in patients with chronic unilateral vestibular hypofunction alters the statistics of head movements during natural self-motion, especially for tasks that depend on rapid vestibular feedback, such as repetitive jumping or walking on foam (175).

As in the case of aging, altered motion thresholds in vestibular patients often lead to postural disturbances. It has been argued that increased noise in vestibular feedback caused by aging or pathology reduces the signal-to-noise ratio in vestibulo-spinal pathways, leading to increased sway, imbalance, and higher risk of falls (176).

9.3. Future promises from vestibular stimulation

In line of principle, vestibular stimulation interventions such as those reviewed above (see section 7) might be helpful to ameliorate either motion perception or postural instability in patients. However, current results are still insufficient to draw definitive conclusions. Thus, noisy GVS alone improved balance in bilateral vestibulopathies (137, 177). However, when it was combined with vestibular rehabilitation training in the same category of patients, it did not lead to any significant improvement relative to that obtained with vestibular rehabilitation alone (178).

9.4. Strong acoustic noise and vibrations

In this article, we mainly considered the effects of low to medium-intensity extrinsic noise. A separate issue is represented by the pathological effects of strong acoustic noise and vibrations, such as those present in heavy industry or military environment. Here, we only mention a few pertinent findings. Noise exposure higher than 90 dB for 8 h/day for prolonged periods of time can cause hearing loss as well as vestibular dysfunction (179–181). Moreover, canal deficits diagnosed with video head impulse test were found in workers exposed to daily, intense whole-body vibrations (182).

10. Conclusion

We considered some effects of both extrinsic (exogenous) and intrinsic (endogenous) noise on the perceptual responses and neural processing of self-motion signals at different stages of the vestibular pathways. Although noise can interfere with neural processing and performance, all vestibular neurons, starting from the mechanoceptors of the inner ear up to cortical neurons, exhibit built-in mechanisms that tend to limit the impact of noise, and in several cases to exploit noise so as to enhance sensitivity to naturalistic motion stimuli. We also showed that stochastic facilitation of neural processing and behavioral performance can be obtained experimentally by applying optimal levels of noisy external perturbations on top of subliminal motion stimuli. This is a potentially promising approach to be further explored for vestibular rehabilitation in case of vestibular hypofunction (e.g., in elderly people) or vestibulopathies.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

The experimental work performed in the authors’ laboratories was supported by the Italian Ministry of Health (Ricerca corrente, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Ricerca Finalizzata RF-2018-12365985), Italian Space Agency (grant I/006/06/0), INAIL (BRIC 2019 BRISK and BRIC 2022 LABORIUS), and Italian University Ministry (PRIN grant 2020EM9A8X_003 and NRRP project MNESYS PE00000006). #NEXTGENERATIONEU (NGEU) National Recovery and Resilience Plan project A Multiscale integrated approach to the study of the nervous system in health and disease, DN. 1553 11.10.2022.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Cohen, L. The history of noise. IEEE Signal Process Mag. (2005) 22:20–45. doi: 10.1109/MSP.2005.1550188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Angelaki, DE, and Cullen, KE. Vestibular system: the many facets of a multimodal sense. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2008) 31:125–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Merfeld, DM. Signal detection theory and vestibular thresholds: I basic theory and practical considerations. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 210:389–405. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2557-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Diaz-Artiles, A, and Karmali, F. Vestibular precision at the level of perception, eye movements, posture, and neurons. Neuroscience. (2021) 468:282–320. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Shannon, CE. Communication in the presence of noise. Proc IRE. (1949) 37:10–21. doi: 10.1109/JRPROC.1949.232969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Ward, LM, and Greenwood, PE. 1/f noise. Scholarpedia. (2007) 2:1537. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.1537

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Tsimring, LS. Noise in biology. Rep Prog Phys. (2014) 77:026601. doi: 10.1088/0034-4885/77/2/026601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Destexhe, A, and Rudolph-Lilith, M. Neuronal noise. New York: Springer (2012). 458 p.

Google Scholar

9. Goldberg, JM, Wilson, VJ, Cullen, KE, Angelaki, DE, Broussard, DM, Buttner-Ennever, J, et al. The vestibular system: A sixth sense. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2012). 541 p.

Google Scholar

10. Hudspeth, AJ. The cellular basis of hearing: the biophysics of hair cells. Science. (1985) 230:745–52.

Google Scholar

11. Faisal, AA, Selen, LP, and Wolpert, DM. Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gerstner, W, and Kistler, WM. Spiking neuron models: Single neurons, populations, plasticity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2002). 480 p.

Google Scholar

13. Longtin, A. Neuronal noise. Scholarpedia. (2013) 8:1618. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.1618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stein, RB, Gossen, ER, and Jones, KE. Neuronal variability: noise or part of the signal? Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:389–97. doi: 10.1038/nrn1668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. La Scaleia, B, Lacquaniti, F, and Zago, M. Enhancement of vestibular motion discrimination by small stochastic whole-body perturbations in young healthy humans. Neuroscience. (2023) 510:32–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Rodriguez, R, and Crane, BT. Effect of vibration during visual-inertial integration on human heading perception during eccentric gaze. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0199097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Curthoys, IS, MacDougall, HG, Vidal, P-P, and de Waele, C. Sustained and transient vestibular systems: a physiological basis for interpreting vestibular function. Front Neurol. (2017) 8:117. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Indovina, I, Mazzarella, E, Maffei, V, Cesqui, B, Passamonti, L, and Lacquaniti, F. Sound-evoked vestibular stimulation affects the anticipation of gravity effects during visual self-motion. Exp Brain Res. (2015) 233:2365–71. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4306-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Todd, NP, Rosengren, SM, and Colebatch, JG. A utricular origin of frequency tuning to low-frequency vibration in the human vestibular system? Neurosci Lett. (2009) 451:175–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.12.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Young, ED, Fernandez, C, and Goldberg, JM. Responses of squirrel monkey vestibular neurons to audio-frequency sound and head vibration. Acta Otolaryngol. (1977) 84:352–60. doi: 10.3109/00016487709123977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dlugaiczyk, J, Gensberger, KD, and Straka, H. Galvanic vestibular stimulation: from basic concepts to clinical applications. J Neurophysiol. (2019) 121:2237–55. doi: 10.1152/jn.00035.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Fitzpatrick, RC, and Day, BL. Probing the human vestibular system with galvanic stimulation. J Appl Physiol. (2004) 96:2301–16. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00008.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lopez, C, Blanke, O, and Mast, FW. The human vestibular cortex revealed by coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Neuroscience. (2012) 212:159–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. McDonnell, MD, and Ward, LM. The benefits of noise in neural systems: bridging theory and experiment. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:415–25. doi: 10.1038/nrn3061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Carriot, J, Jamali, M, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. Statistics of the vestibular input experienced during natural self-motion: implications for neural processing. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:8347–57. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0692-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Fino, PC, Raffegeau, TE, Parrington, L, Peterka, RJ, and King, LA. Head stabilization during standing in people with persisting symptoms after mild traumatic brain injury. J Biomech. (2020) 112:110045. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2020.110045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hirasaki, E, Moore, ST, Raphan, T, and Cohen, B. Effects of walking velocity on vertical head and body movements during locomotion. Exp Brain Res. (1999) 127:117–30. doi: 10.1007/s002210050781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wang, L, Zobeiri, OA, Millar, JL, Souza Silva, W, Schubert, MC, and Cullen, KE. Continuous head motion is a greater motor control challenge than transient head motion in patients with loss of vestibular function. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2021) 35:890–902. doi: 10.1177/15459683211034758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ronellenfitsch, H, Dunkel, J, and Wilczek, M. Optimal noise-canceling networks. Phys Rev Lett. (2018) 121:208301. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.121.208301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Barlow, HB, Levick, WR, and Yoon, M. Responses to single quanta of light in retinal ganglion cells of the cat. Vis Res. (1971) S3:87–101.

Google Scholar

31. Bialek, W. Physical limits to sensation and perception. Annu Rev Biophys Biophys Chem. (1987) 16:455–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bb.16.060187.002323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Lillywhite, PG, and Laughlin, SB. Transducer noise in a photoreceptor. Nature. (1979) 277:569–72. doi: 10.1038/277569a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Heekeren, HR, Marrett, S, Bandettini, PA, and Ungerleider, LG. A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature. (2004) 431:859–62. doi: 10.1038/nature02966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gammaitoni, L, Hanggi, P, Jung, P, and Marchesoni, F. Stochastic resonance. Rev Mod Phys. (1998) 70:223–87. doi: 10.1103/RevModPhys.70.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Carriot, J, Cullen, KE, and Chacron, MJ. The neural basis for violations of Weber’s law in self-motion perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2021) 118:e2025061118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2025061118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cullen, KE. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control. Trends Neurosci. (2012) 35:185–96. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Schneider, AD, Cullen, KE, and Chacron, MJ. In vivo conditions induce faithful encoding of stimuli by reducing nonlinear synchronization in vestibular sensory neurons. PLoS Comput Biol. (2011) 7:e1002120. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Carriot, J, McAllister, G, Hooshangnejad, H, Mackrous, I, Cullen, KE, and Chacron, MJ. Sensory adaptation mediates efficient and unambiguous encoding of natural stimuli by vestibular thalamocortical pathways. Nat Commun. (2022) 13:2612. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30348-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Berkes, P, Orbán, G, Lengyel, M, and Fiser, J. Spontaneous cortical activity reveals hallmarks of an optimal internal model of the environment. Science. (2011) 331:83–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1195870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Mitchell, DE, Kwan, A, Carriot, J, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. Neuronal variability and tuning are balanced to optimize naturalistic self-motion coding in primate vestibular pathways. elife. (2018) 7:e43019. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Jamali, M, Carriot, J, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. Coding strategies in the otolith system differ for translational head motion vs. static orientation relative to gravity. elife. (2019) 8:e45573. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jamali, M, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. Self-motion evokes precise spike timing in the primate vestibular system. Nat Commun. (2016) 7:13229. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. McDonnell, MD, and Abbott, D. What is stochastic resonance? Definitions, misconceptions, debates, and its relevance to biology. PLoS Comput Biol. (2009) 5:e1000348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Buzsaki, G. Rhythms of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press (2006). 448 p.

Google Scholar

45. Gordus, A, Pokala, N, Levy, S, Flavell, SW, and Bargmann, CI. Feedback from network states generates variability in a probabilistic olfactory circuit. Cells. (2015) 161:215–27. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Angelaki, DE, Klier, EM, and Snyder, LH. A vestibular sensation: probabilistic approaches to spatial perception. Neuron. (2009) 64:448–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cullen, KE. Vestibular processing during natural self-motion: implications for perception and action. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2019) 20:346–63. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0153-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Merfeld, DM. Spatial orientation and the vestibular system In: S Carroll, editor. Sensation and perception. 3rd ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates (2012). 328–61.

Google Scholar

49. Nouri, S, and Karmali, F. Variability in the vestibulo-ocular reflex and vestibular perception. Neuroscience. (2018) 393:350–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Chaudhuri, SE, Karmali, F, and Merfeld, DM. Whole body motion-detection tasks can yield much lower thresholds than direction-recognition tasks: implications for the role of vibration. J Neurophysiol. (2013) 110:2764–72. doi: 10.1152/jn.00091.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. MacNeilage, PR, Turner, AH, and Angelaki, DE. Canal– otolith interactions and detection thresholds of linear and angular components during curved-path self-motion. J Neurophysiol. (2010) 104:765–73. doi: 10.1152/jn.01067.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Green, DM, and Swets, JA. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley (1966). 455 p.

Google Scholar

53. Macmillan, NA, and Creelman, CD. Detection theory: A user's guide. 2nd ed. Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum (2005). 492 p.

Google Scholar

54. Mittelstaedt, H. Somatic graviception. Biol Psychol. (1996) 42:53–74. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05146-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Valko, Y, Lewis, RF, Priesol, AJ, and Merfeld, DM. Vestibular labyrinth contributions to human whole-body motion discrimination. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:13537–42. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2157-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gu, Y, DeAngelis, GC, and Angelaki, DE. A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals. Nat Neurosci. (2007) 10:1038–47. doi: 10.1038/nn1935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lim, K, Karmali, F, Nicoucar, K, and Merfeld, DM. Perceptual precision of passive body tilt is consistent with statistically optimal cue integration. J Neurophysiol. (2017) 117:2037–52. doi: 10.1152/jn.00073.2016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Glasauer, S. Interaction of semicircular canals and otoliths in the processing structure of the subjective zenith. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1992) 656:847–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25272.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Laurens, J, and Angelaki, DE. The functional significance of velocity storage and its dependence on gravity. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 210:407–22. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2568-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Merfeld, DM, Park, S, Gianna-Poulin, C, Black, FO, and Wood, S. Vestibular perception and action employ qualitatively different mechanisms. I frequency response of VOR and perceptual responses during translation and tilt. J Neurophysiol. (2005) 94:186–98. doi: 10.1152/jn.00904.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Butler, JS, Smith, ST, Campos, JL, and Bülthoff, HH. Bayesian integration of visual and vestibular signals for heading. J Vis. (2010) 10:23. doi: 10.1167/101123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Fetsch, CR, Turner, AH, DeAngelis, GC, and Angelaki, DE. Dynamic reweighting of visual and vestibular cues during self-motion perception. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:15601–12. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2574-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Fetsch, CR, and DeAngelis, GC. Angelaki DE visual–vestibular cue integration for heading perception: applications of optimal cue integration theory. Eur J Neurosci. (2010) 31:1721–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07207.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Karmali, F, Lim, K, and Merfeld, DM. Visual and vestibular perceptual thresholds each demonstrate better precision at specific frequencies and also exhibit optimal integration. J Neurophysiol. (2014) 111:2393–403. doi: 10.1152/jn.00332.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. La Scaleia, B, Lacquaniti, F, and Zago, M. Body orientation contributes to modelling the effects of gravity for target interception in humans. J Physiol. (2019) 597:2021–43. doi: 10.1113/JP277469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Miller, WL, Maffei, V, Bosco, G, Iosa, M, Zago, M, Macaluso, E, et al. Vestibular nuclei and cerebellum put visual gravitational motion in context. J Neurophysiol. (2008) 99:1969–82. doi: 10.1152/jn.00889.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Noel, JP, and Angelaki, DE. Cognitive, systems, and computational neurosciences of the self in motion. Annu Rev Psychol. (2022) 73:103–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-021021-103038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Mayne, R. A system concept of the vestibular organs In: HH Kornhuber, editor. Handbook of sensory physiology, Vestibular system Part 2: psychophysics, applied aspects and general interpretations, vol. VI. Berlin: Springer (1974). 493–580.

Google Scholar

69. Karmali, F, and Merfeld, DM. A distributed, dynamic, parallel computational model: the role of noise in velocity storage. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 108:390–405. doi: 10.1152/jn.00883.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Laurens, J, and Droulez, J. Bayesian processing of vestibular information. Biol Cybern. (2007) 96:389–404. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0133-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Holt, JR, Corey, DP, and Eatock, RA. Mechanoelectrical transduction and adaptation in hair cells of the mouse utricle, a low-frequency vestibular organ. J Neurosci. (1997) 17:8739–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-22-08739.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kozlov, AS, Risler, T, and Hudspeth, AJ. Coherent motion of stereocilia assures the concerted gating of hair-cell transduction channels. Nat Neurosci. (2007) 10:87–92. doi: 10.1038/nn1818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Denk, W, Webb, WW, and Hudspeth, AJ. Mechanical properties of sensory hair bundles are reflected in their Brownian motion measured with a laser differential interferometer. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (1989) 86:5371–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.14.5371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Fernandez, C, and Goldberg, JM. Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III Response dynamics J Neurophysiol. (1976) 39:996–1008. doi: 10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Jaramillo, F, and Wiesenfeld, K. Mechanoelectrical transduction assisted by Brownian motion: a role for noise in the auditory system. Nat Neurosci. (1998) 1:384–8. doi: 10.1038/1597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Flores, A, Manilla, S, Huidobro, N, De la Torre-Valdovinos, B, Kristeva, R, Mendez-Balbuena, I, et al. Stochastic resonance in the synaptic transmission between hair cells and vestibular primary afferents in development. Neuroscience. (2016) 322:416–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Eatock, RA, and Songer, JE. Vestibular hair cells and afferents: two channels for head motion signals. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2011) 34:501–34. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Schneidman, E, Freedman, B, and Segev, I. Ion channel stochasticity may be critical in determining the reliability and precision of spike timing. Neural Comput. (1998) 10:1679–703. doi: 10.1162/089976698300017089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Smith, CE, and Goldberg, JM. A stochastic after hyperpolarization model of repetitive activity in vestibular afferents. Biol Cybern. (1986) 54:41–51. doi: 10.1007/BF00337114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Jamali, M, Carriot, J, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. Strong correlations between sensitivity and variability give rise to constant discrimination thresholds across the otolith afferent population. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:11302–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0459-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Sadeghi, SG, Chacron, MJ, Taylor, MC, and Cullen, KE. Neural variability, detection thresholds, and information transmission in the vestibular system. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:771–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4690-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Yu, X-j, Dickman, JD, DeAngelis, GC, and Angelaki, DE. Neuronal thresholds and choice-related activity of otolith afferent fibers during heading perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2015) 112:6467–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507402112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Yu, X-j, Dickman, JD, and Angelaki, DE. Detection thresholds of macaque otolith afferents. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:8306–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1067-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Massot, C, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. Information transmission and detection thresholds in the vestibular nuclei: single neurons vs population encoding. J Neurophysiol. (2011) 105:1798–814. doi: 10.1152/jn.00910.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Carriot, J, Jamali, M, Cullen, KE, and Chacron, MJ. Envelope statistics of self-motion signals experienced by human subjects during everyday activities: implications for vestibular processing. PLoS One. (2017) 12:e0178664. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Metzen, MG, Jamali, M, Carriot, J, Ávila-Ǻkerberg, O, Cullen, KE, and Chacron, MJ. Coding of envelopes by correlated but not single-neuron activity requires neural variability. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2015) 112:4791–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418224112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Grabherr, L, Nicoucar, K, Mast, FW, and Merfeld, DM. Vestibular thresholds for yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis as a function of frequency. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 186:677–81. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1350-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Massot, C, Schneider, AD, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. The vestibular system implements a linear-nonlinear transformation in order to encode self-motion. PLoS Biol. (2012) 10:e1001365. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Mackrous, I, Carriot, J, Cullen, KE, and Chacron, MJ. Neural variability determines coding strategies for natural self-motion in macaque monkeys. elife. (2020) 9:e57484. doi: 10.7554/eLife.57484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Barlow, H. The exploitation of regularities in the environment by the brain. Behav Brain Sci. (2001) 24:602–7. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Lopez, C, and Blanke, O. The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. (2011) 67:119–46. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Mallery, RM, Olomu, OU, Uchanski, RM, MilitchinV, A, and Hullar, TE. Human discrimination of rotational velocities. Exp Brain Res. (2010) 204:11–20. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2288-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Naseri, AR, and Grant, PR. Human discrimination of translational accelerations. Exp Brain Res. (2012) 218:455–64. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3035-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Liu, S, Gu, Y, DeAngelis, GC, and Angelaki, DE. Choice-related activity and correlated noise in subcortical vestibular neurons. Nat Neurosci. (2013) 16:89–97. doi: 10.1038/nn.3267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Gu, Y, Angelaki, DE, and DeAngelis, GC. Contribution of correlated noise and selective decoding to choice probability measurements in extrastriate visual cortex. elife. (2014) 3:e02670. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Kobel, MJ, Wagner, AR, Merfeld, DM, and Mattingly, JK. Vestibular thresholds: a review of advances and challenges in clinical applications. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:643634. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.643634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Suri, K, and Clark, TK. Human vestibular perceptual thresholds for pitch tilt are slightly worse than for roll tilt across a range of frequencies. Exp Brain Res. (2020) 238:1499–509. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-05830-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Soyka, F, Robuffo Giordano, P, Beykirch, K, and Bulthoff, HH. Predicting direction detection thresholds for arbitrary translational acceleration profiles in the horizontal plane. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 209:95–107. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2523-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Roditi, RE, and Crane, BT. Directional asymmetries and age effects in human self-motion perception. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. (2012) 13:381–401. doi: 10.1007/s10162-012-0318-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. De Vrijer, M, Medendorp, WP, and Van Gisbergen, JA. Accuracy-precision trade-off in visual orientation constancy. J Vis. (2009) 9:9–15. doi: 10.1167/9.2.9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Wagner, AR, Kobel, MJ, and Merfeld, DM. Impact of canal-otolith integration on postural control. Front Integr Neurosci. (2021) 15:773008. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2021.773008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Morioka, M, and Griffin, MJ. Absolute thresholds for the perception of fore-and-aft, lateral, and vertical vibration at the hand, the seat, and the foot. J Sound Vib. (2008) 314:357–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jsv.2007.12.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Benson, AJ, Hutt, EC, and Brown, SF. Thresholds for the perception of whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. Aviat Space Environ Med. (1989) 60:205–13.

Google Scholar

104. Allred, AR, and Clark, TK. Vestibular perceptual thresholds for rotation about the yaw, roll, and pitch axes. Exp Brain Res. (2023):1–15. doi: 10.1007/s00221-023-06570-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Wagner, AR, Kobel, MJ, and Merfeld, DM. Impacts of rotation axis and frequency on vestibular perceptual thresholds. Multisens Res. (2022) 35:259–87. doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Benson, AJ, Spencer, MB, and Stott, JRR. Thresholds for the detection of the direction of whole-body, linear movement in the horizontal plane. Aviat Space Environ Med. (1986) 57:1088–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

107. Bermúdez Rey, MC, Clark, TK, Wang, W, Leeder, T, Bian, Y, and Merfeld, DM. Vestibular perceptual thresholds increase above the age of 40. Front Neurol. (2016) 7:162. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. MacNeilage, PR, Banks, MS, DeAngelis, GC, and Angelaki, DE. Vestibular heading discrimination and sensitivity to linear acceleration in head and world coordinates. J Neurosci. (2010) 30:9084–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1304-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. La Scaleia, B, Ceccarelli, F, Lacquaniti, F, and Zago, M. Visuomotor interactions and perceptual judgments in virtual reality simulating different levels of gravity. Front Bioeng Biotechnol. (2020) 8:76. doi: 10.3389/fbioe.2020.00076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Kobel, MJ, Wagner, AR, and Merfeld, DM. Impact of gravity on the perception of linear motion. J Neurophysiol. (2021) 126:875–87. doi: 10.1152/jn.00274.2021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Fernandez, C, and Goldberg, JM. Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. J Neurophysiol. (1976) 39:970–84. doi: 10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Deco, G, and Rolls, ET. Decision-making and Weber's law: a neurophysiological model. Eur J Neurosci. (2006) 24:901–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04940.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Soyka, F, Giordano, PR, Barnett-Cowan, M, and Bulthoff, HH. Modeling direction discrimination thresholds for yaw rotations around an earth-vertical axis for arbitrary motion profiles. Exp Brain Res. (2012) 220:89–99. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3120-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Merfeld, DM, Clark, TK, Lu, YM, and Karmali, F. Dynamics of individual perceptual decisions. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 115:39–59. doi: 10.1152/jn.00225.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Clark, TK, Yi, Y, Galvan-Garza, RC, Bermudez Rey, MC, and Merfeld, DM. When uncertain, does human self-motion decision-making fully utilize complete information? J Neurophysiol. (2018) 119:1485–96. doi: 10.1152/jn.00680.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Hartmann, M, Furrer, S, Herzog, MH, Merfeld, DM, and Mast, FW. Self-motion perception training: thresholds improve in the light but not in the dark. Exp Brain Res. (2013) 226:231–40. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3428-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Lee, TL, Shayman, CS, Oh, Y, Peterka, RJ, and Hullar, TE. Reliability of vestibular perceptual threshold testing about the yaw axis. Ear Hear. (2020) 41:1772–4. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000859

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Guedry, FE. Psychophysics of vestibular sensation In: HH Kornhuber, editor. Vestibular system part 2: Psychophysics, applied aspects and general interpretations. Berlin: Springer-Verlag (1974). 3–154.

Google Scholar

119. Vidal, PP, and Lacquaniti, F. Perceptual-motor styles. Exp Brain Res. (2021) 239:1359–80. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06049-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Bringoux, L, Marin, L, Nougier, V, Barraud, PA, and Raphel, C. Effects of gymnastics expertise on the perception of body orientation in the pitch dimension. J Vestib Res. (2000) 10:251–8. doi: 10.3233/VES-2000-10602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Fitzpatrick, RC, and Watson, SR. Passive motion reduces vestibular balance and perceptual responses. J Physiol. (2015) 593:2389–98. doi: 10.1113/JP270334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Keywan, A, Dietrich, H, and Wuehr, M. Subliminal passive motion stimulation improves vestibular perception. Neuroscience. (2020) 441:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.05.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Keywan, A, Yassin, G, Jahn, K, and Wuehr, M. Subliminal conditioning of vestibular perception generalizes within otolith organs. J Neurol. (2022) 269:5258–61. doi: 10.1007/s00415-022-10982-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Wagner, AR, Kobel, MJ, Tajino, J, and Merfeld, DM. Improving self-motion perception and balance through roll tilt perceptual training. J Neurophysiol. (2022) 128:619–33. doi: 10.1152/jn.00092.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Seemungal, BM, Gunaratne, IA, Fleming, IO, Gresty, MA, and Bronstein, AM. Perceptual and nystagmic thresholds of vestibular function in yaw. J Vestib Res. (2004) 14:461–6. doi: 10.3233/VES-2004-14604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Karmali, F, Goodworth, AD, Valko, Y, Leeder, T, Peterka, RJ, and Merfeld, DM. The role of vestibular cues in postural sway. J Neurophysiol. (2021) 125:672–86. doi: 10.1152/jn.00168.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Kuo, AD. An optimal state estimation model of sensory integration in human postural balance. J Neural Eng. (2005) 2:S235–49. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/2/3/S07

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Pozzo, T, Berthoz, A, and Lefort, L. Head stabilization during various locomotor tasks in humans: I. Normal subjects. Exp Brain Res. (1990) 82:97–106.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

129. Carriot, J, Jamali, M, Chacron, MJ, and Cullen, KE. The statistics of the vestibular input experienced during natural self-motion differ between rodents and primates. J Physiol. (2017) 595:2751–66. doi: 10.1113/JP273734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Kabbaligere, R, Layne, CS, and Karmali, F. Perception of threshold-level whole-body motion during mechanical mastoid vibration. J Vestib Res. (2018) 28:283–94. doi: 10.3233/VES-180636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Galvan-Garza, RC, Clark, TK, Mulavara, AP, and Oman, CM. Exhibition of stochastic resonance in vestibular tilt motion perception. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:716–22. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Keywan, A, Wuehr, M, Pradhan, C, and Jahn, K. Noisy galvanic stimulation improves roll-tilt vestibular perception in healthy subjects. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:83. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Keywan, A, Badarna, H, Jahn, K, and Wuehr, M. No evidence for after-effects of noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation on motion perception. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:1–7. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59374-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Mulavara, AP, Kofman, IS, De Dios, YE, Miller, C, Peters, BT, Goel, R, et al. Using low levels of stochastic vestibular stimulation to improve locomotor stability. Front Syst Neurosci. (2015) 9:117. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Wuehr, M, Boerner, JC, Pradhan, C, Decker, J, Jahn, K, Brandt, T, et al. Stochastic resonance in the human vestibular system – noise-induced facilitation of vestibulospinal reflexes. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:261–3. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Serrador, JM, Deegan, BM, Geraghty, MC, and Wood, SJ. Enhancing vestibular function in the elderly with imperceptible electrical stimulation. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:336. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18653-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Fujimoto, C, Yamamoto, Y, Kamogashira, T, Kinoshita, M, Egami, N, Uemura, Y, et al. Noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation induces a sustained improvement in body balance in elderly adults. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:1–8. doi: 10.1038/srep37575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Goel, R, Kofman, I, Jeevarajan, J, De Dios, Y, Cohen, HS, Bloomberg, JJ, et al. Using low levels of stochastic vestibular stimulation to improve balance function. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0136335. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Mulavara, AP, Fiedler, MJ, Kofman, IS, Wood, SJ, Serrador, JM, Peters, B, et al. Improving balance function using vestibular stochastic resonance: optimizing stimulus characteristics. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 210:303–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2633-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Putman, EJ, Galvan-Garza, RC, and Clark, TK. The effect of Noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation on learning of functional mobility and manual control nulling sensorimotor tasks. Front Hum Neurosci. (2021) 15:756674. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.756674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Iwasaki, S, Fujimoto, C, Egami, N, Kinoshita, M, Togo, F, Yamamoto, Y, et al. Noisy vestibular stimulation increases gait speed in normals and in bilateral vestibulopathy. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:709–15. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Voros, JL, Sherman, SO, Rise, R, Kryuchkov, A, Stine, P, Anderson, AP, et al. Galvanic vestibular stimulation produces cross-modal improvements in visual thresholds. Front Neurosci. (2021) 15:640984. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.640984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Iwasaki, S, Yamamoto, Y, Togo, F, Kinoshita, M, Yoshifuji, Y, Fujimoto, C, et al. Noisy vestibular stimulation improves body balance in bilateral vestibulopathy. Neurology. (2014) 82:969–75. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Nooristani, M, Bigras, C, Lafontaine, L, Bacon, BA, Maheu, M, and Champoux, F. Vestibular function modulates the impact of nGVS on postural control in older adults. J Neurophysiol. (2021) 125:489–95. doi: 10.1152/jn.00512.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Keywan, A, and Jahn, K. Wuehr M Noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation primarily affects otolith-mediated motion perception. Neuroscience. (2019) 399:161–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.12.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Zink, R, Bucher, SF, Weiss, A, Brandt, T, and Dieterich, M. Effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation on otolithic and semicircular canal eye movements and perceived vertical. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1998) 107:200–5. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00056-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Kwan, A, Forbes, PA, Mitchell, DE, Blouin, JS, and Cullen, KE. Neural substrates, dynamics and thresholds of galvanic vestibular stimulation in the behaving primate. Nat Commun. (2019) 10:1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09738-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Forbes, PA, Kwan, A, Mitchell, DE, Blouin, JS, and Cullen, KE. The neural basis for biased behavioral responses evoked by galvanic vestibular stimulation in primates. J Neurosci. (2023) 43:1905–19. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0987-22.2023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Moss, F, Ward, LM, and Sannita, WG. Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin Neurophysiol. (2004) 115:267–81. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Aihara, T, Kitajo, K, Nozaki, D, and Yamamoto, Y. How does stochastic resonance work within the human brain?–Psychophysics of internal and external noise. Chem Phys. (2010) 375:616–24.

Google Scholar

151. Benzi, R, Sutera, A, and Vulpiani, A. The mechanism of stochastic resonance. J Phys A Math Gen. (1981) 14:L453–7. doi: 10.1088/0305-4470/14/11/006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Nicolis, G, and Nicolis, C. Stochastic aspects of climatic transitions-additive fluctuations. Tellus. (1981) 33:225–34.

Google Scholar

153. Rouvas-Nicolis, C, and Nicolis, G. Stochastic resonance. Scholarpedia. (2007) 2:1474. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.1474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Kramers, HA. Brownian motion in a field of force and the diffusion model of chemical reactions. Physica. (1940) 7:284–304. doi: 10.1016/S0031-8914(40)90098-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Wiesenfeld, K, Pierson, D, Pantazelou, E, Dames, C, and Moss, F. Stochastic resonance on a circle. Phys Rev Lett. (1994) 72:2125–9. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.72.2125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Collins, JJ, Chow, CC, and Imhoff, TT. Aperiodic stochastic resonance in excitable systems. Phys Rev. (1995) 52:R3321.

Google Scholar

157. Cordo, P, Inglis, JT, Verschueren, S, Collins, JJ, Merfeld, DM, Rosenblum, S, et al. Noise in human muscle spindles. Nature. (1996) 383:769–70. doi: 10.1038/383769a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Collins, JJ, Imhoff, TT, and Grigg, P. Noise-enhanced information transmission in rat SA1 cutaneous mechanoreceptors via aperiodic stochastic resonance. J Neurophysiol. (1996) 76:642–5.

Google Scholar

159. Simonotto, E, Riani, M, Seife, C, Roberts, M, Twitty, J, and Moss, F. Visual perception of stochastic resonance. Phys Rev Lett. (1997) 78:1186.

Google Scholar

160. Zeng, FG, Fu, QJ, and Morse, R. Human hearing enhanced by noise. Brain Res. (2000) 869:251–55. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02475-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Long, Z-C, Shao, F, Zhang, Y-P, and Qin, Y-G. Noise-enhanced hearing sensitivity. Phys Lett A. (2004) 323:434–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physleta.2004.02.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Collins, JJ, Imhoff, TT, and Grigg, P. Noise-enhanced tactile sensation. Nature. (1996) 383:770. doi: 10.1038/383770a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. van der Groen, O, and Wenderoth, N. Transcranial random noise stimulation of visual cortex: stochastic resonance enhances central mechanisms of perception. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:5289–98. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4519-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Douglass, JK, Wilkens, L, Pantazelou, E, and Moss, F. Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance. Nature. (1993) 365:337–40. doi: 10.1038/365337a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Galvan-Garza, R. Enhancement of perception with the application of stochastic vestibular stimulation. PhD Thesis. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology (2016).

Google Scholar

166. Yerkes, RM, and Dodson, JD. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol. (1908) 18:459–82. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Stocks, NG. Suprathreshold stochastic resonance in multilevel threshold systems. Phys Rev Lett. (2000) 84:2310–3. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.84.2310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Lindner, B, Garcıa-Ojalvo, J, Neiman, A, and Schimansky-Geier, L. Effects of noise in excitable systems. Phys Rep. (2004) 392:321–424. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2003.10.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Karmali, F, Rey, BMC, Clark, TK, Wang, W, and Merfeld, DM. Multivariate analyses of balance test performance, vestibular thresholds, and age. Front Neurol. (2017) 8:578. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Agrawal, Y, Carey, JP, Della Santina, CC, Schubert, MC, and Minor, LB. Disorders of balance and vestibular function in US adults: data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2001–2004. Arch Intern Med. (2009) 169:938–44. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.66

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Priesol, AJ, Valko, Y, Merfeld, DM, and Lewis, RF. Motion perception in patients with idiopathic bilateral vestibular hypofunction. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2014) 150:1040–2. doi: 10.1177/0194599814526557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Cousins, S, Kaski, D, Cutfield, N, Seemungal, B, Golding, JF, Gresty, M, et al. Vestibular perception following acute unilateral vestibular lesions. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e61862. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. King, S, Priesol, AJ, Davidi, SE, Merfeld, DM, Ehtemam, F, and Lewis, RF. Self-motion perception is sensitized in vestibular migraine: pathophysiologic and clinical implications. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50803-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Wurthmann, S, Holle, D, Obermann, M, Roesner, M, Nsaka, M, Scheffler, A, et al. Reduced vestibular perception thresholds in persistent postural-perceptual dizziness-a cross-sectional study. BMC Neurol. (2021) 21:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12883-021-02417-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Zobeiri, OA, Ostrander, B, Roat, J, Agrawal, Y, and Cullen, KE. Loss of peripheral vestibular input alters the statistics of head movement experienced during natural self-motion. J Physiol. (2021) 599:2239–54. doi: 10.1113/JP281183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Wagner, AR, Chaudhari, AM, and Merfeld, DM. Might vestibular “noise” cause subclinical balance impairment and falls? Int J Phys Med Rehabil. (2021) 9:001.

Google Scholar

177. Wuehr, M, Nusser, E, Decker, J, Krafczyk, S, Straube, A, Brandt, T, et al. Noisy vestibular stimulation improves dynamic walking stability in bilateral vestibulopathy. Neurology. (2016) 86:2196–202. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Eder, J, Kellerer, S, Amberger, T, Keywan, A, Dlugaiczyk, J, Wuehr, M, et al. Combining vestibular rehabilitation with noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation for treatment of bilateral vestibulopathy. J Neurol. (2022) 269:5731–7. doi: 10.1007/s00415-022-11033-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Golz, A, Westerman, ST, Westerman, LM, Goldenberg, D, Netzer, A, Wiedmyer, T, et al. The effects of noise on the vestibular system. Am J Otolaryngol. (2001) 22:190–6. doi: 10.1053/ajot.2001.23428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Stewart, CE, Holt, AG, Altschuler, RA, Cacace, AT, Hall, CD, Murnane, OD, et al. Effects of noise exposure on the vestibular system: a systematic review. Front Neurol. (2020) 11:593919. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.593919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Wang, Y-P, and Young, Y-H. Vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials in chronic noise-induced hearing loss. Otolaryng Head Neck Surg. (2007) 137:607–11. doi: 10.1016/j.otohns.2007.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Yilmaz, N, and Ila, K. Effect of vibration on the vestibular system in noisy and noise-free environments in heavy industry. Acta Otolaryngol. (2019) 139:1014–8. doi: 10.1080/00016489.2019.1666213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: hair cells, vestibular afferents, central vestibular pathways, vestibular thresholds, galvanic stimulation, stochastic resonance, vestibulopathies, vestibular rehabilitation

Citation: Lacquaniti F, La Scaleia B and Zago M (2023) Noise and vestibular perception of passive self-motion. Front. Neurol. 14:1159242. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1159242

Received: 05 February 2023; Accepted: 29 March 2023;
Published: 26 April 2023.

Edited by:

Faisal Karmali, Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Andrew R. Wagner, The Ohio State University, United States
Maurice Chacron, McGill University, Canada
Jerome Carriot, McGill University, Canada

Copyright © 2023 Lacquaniti, La Scaleia and Zago. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francesco Lacquaniti, bGFjcXVhbml0aUBtZWQudW5pcm9tYTIuaXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.