Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 01 September 2022
Sec. Neuro-Otology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuromuscular Control Modelling of Static and Dynamic Balance Maintenance View all 5 articles

Vertical ground reaction force oscillation during standing on hard and compliant surfaces: The “postural rhythm”

  • 1Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, Centro Studi Attività Motorie (CSAM), Pavia, Italy
  • 2Complexité, Innovation, Activités Motrices et Sportives (CIAMS), Université Paris-Saclay, Orsay, France
  • 3Complexité, Innovation, Activités Motrices et Sportives (CIAMS), Université d'Orléans, Orléans, France

When a person stands upright quietly, the position of the Centre of Mass (CoM), the vertical force acting on the ground and the geometrical configuration of body segments is accurately controlled around to the direction of gravity by multiple feedback mechanisms and by integrative brain centres that coordinate multi-joint movements. This is not always easy and the postural muscles continuously produce appropriate torques, recorded as ground reaction force by a force platform. We studied 23 young adults during a 90 s period, standing at ease on a hard (Solid) and on a compliant support (Foam) with eyes open (EO) and with eyes closed (EC), focusing on the vertical component of the ground reaction force (VGRF). Analysis of VGRF time series gave the amplitude of their rhythmic oscillations (the root mean square, RMS) and of their frequency spectrum. Sway Area and Path Length of the Centre of Pressure (CoP) were also calculated. VGRF RMS (as well as CoP sway measures) increased in the order EO Solid ≈ EC Solid < EO Foam < EC Foam. The VGRF frequency spectra featured prevailing frequencies around 4–5 Hz under all tested conditions, slightly higher on Solid than Foam support. Around that value, the VGRF frequencies varied in a larger range on hard than on compliant support. Sway Area and Path Length were inversely related to the prevailing VGRF frequency. Vision compared to no-vision decreased Sway Area and Path Length and VGRF RMS on Foam support. However, no significant effect of vision was found on VGRF mean frequency for either base of support condition. A description of the VGRF, at the interface between balance control mechanisms and sway of the CoP, can contribute information on how upright balance is maintained. Analysis of the frequency pattern of VGRF oscillations and its role in the maintenance of upright stance should complement the traditional measures of CoP excursions in the horizontal plane.

Introduction

During upright stance, postural muscle activations are distributed in time and space and produce the forces that move and brake the body segments, because our bipedal stance is unstable. The resultant dynamics of those local movements are recorded as ground reaction force by the platform upon which subjects stand (1, 2). The output of the platform sensors is often exploited to reconstruct the instantaneous positions of the Centre of Pressure (CoP), which is represented in the horizontal plane and is characterized by the length of its excursion (Path Length), the area of the surface covered (Sway Area) and the frequency content of its motion (36).

The body oscillates in an unpredictable way. During the natural standing posture, the Centre of Mass (CoM) is normally in front of the ankle joint and a gravity-induced torque caused by the imperfect correspondence of the CoP and CoM positions accelerates the body forward. This is opposed partly by the intrinsic ankle joint stiffness [(712), see (13, 14)] and partly by corrective actions of the postural muscles (1517). These act onto the upper and lower body masses and produce interacting torques about the hip, knee and ankle joints, thereby maintaining the body motion within a region much smaller than the boundary of the base of support (1823).

Standing upright implies control of both the vertical and the horizontal accelerations of the CoM. Any change in the CoP-CoM distance, which originates from muscle activities that make the joints/segments rotate, produces an imbalance broken down into horizontal and vertical torques. The CoP displacement relative to the CoM projection will create positive and negative horizontal torques allowing to maintain postural balance. The dynamic characteristics (frequencies and magnitudes) of the CoP displacements should be found in the horizontal forces. This is probably a reason why most studies on postural control were only interested in the CoP excursions. While the effective benefit of having exact knowledge about the values of the CoP displacement in the horizontal plane only is still being discussed [(4, 2427); see (28)], little attention has been devoted to the vertical component of the ground reaction force (VGRF). This is certainly not the same as the body weight, but can vary around that value depending on the action of the postural muscles counteracting gravity. Not all the effects of the production of torques by the postural muscles can give rise to changes in the CoP-CoM relationships in the horizontal plane. Conversely, these horizontal changes may even conceal some interesting features of the neural control that appear in the VGRF.

Therefore, in this preliminary study, we addressed the characteristics of the VGRF modulation, following prior work that considered VGRF oscillations along with the CoP displacement in the description of the mechanisms underpinning standing posture (2931). Being aware of some features of VGRF could contribute to enhance our insight on how balance is controlled and maintained, since the mechanical effect of the VGRF variations on the physics of the standing task have not been fully elucidated.

Here we compared the amplitude and frequency characteristics of the VGRF between vision and no-vision and standing on a hard floor vs. a foam pad, two conditions known to enhance body sway. We analyzed the VGRF in healthy young volunteers standing upright and leveraged the spectral analysis of the VGRF oscillations to help explain the effect upon postural stability of support surface features and vision. We also compared amplitude and frequency of the VGRF with the geometric measures of the CoP excursions. The effect of vision was analyzed in some detail, because vision selectively modulates the frequency of the CoP sway in certain frequency ranges (4, 32), but its effect on the VGRF features is still poorly understood (29, 33). On the other hand, the compliant support surface would magnify the VGRF and allow contrasting the VGRF under conditions requiring a different effort.

When healthy young subjects stand quietly on the bare force platform, the body sway is limited to within a small surface area and little muscular activity is present (34), while standing on a compliant surface, such as a foam pad, is obviously different (4, 3539) and highly effective in terms of CoP excursions. Subjects continuously control and correct their stance by exerting a greater effort compared to standing on hard ground (40, 41), where the low level, almost-tonic activity of the muscles about the ankle can normally restrain the CoM from falling forwards. On foam, the elastic properties of the compliant support would in some way modify the correcting actions by acting as a damped spring (42), and could produce slower oscillations. Hence, the amplitude of the VGRF oscillations is expected to vary more than on hard ground and their variability would be larger as a consequence of the effort of standing on the challenging support (37). Conversely, the frequency of the VGRF oscillation would be higher on hard than compliant support, because small-amplitude and frequent correcting reactions brought about by our multi-segmented body would suffice to counteract the CoM sway (20, 43). Different frequencies of VGRF oscillations with or without vision and on solid or compliant support would be signs of distinct balancing strategies employed to cope with modifications in sensory and support conditions (44).

Methods

Participants

Twenty-three healthy young adults (12 females and 11 males, age 28.8 ± 4.3 years old, height 171.6 ± 5.9 cm, and weight 65.6 ± 11.8 kg) participated in this study. They had no history of neurological or musculo-skeletal disorders and had no sight problems or their visual acuity was corrected. No subject reported injuries or occurrences of falls in the previous year. All participants gave written informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki principles, which was approved by the local review board (Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, approval number #2564-CE). The analysis was partly performed on previously recorded data (5) and partly on new data recorded in additional volunteers.

Procedures

Subjects were asked to maintain upright stance for 100 s on a force platform (Kistler 9286BA, Switzerland) with the outer profiles of the parallel bare feet at hip width. Subjects performed one trial in different visual and base of support conditions: eyes closed (EC) and eyes open (EO) with Solid (the force platform) or Foam support. Vision and base of support conditions were randomized across subjects and performed in different days (at least 1 day elapsed between conditions). In the Solid condition, the rigid platform was topped by a thin linoleum lamina. In the Foam condition, a standard pad (Airex Balance Pad, Switzerland; L 50 cm, W 41 cm, H 6 cm, density 55 g/dm3, Young's module 260 kPa) was placed onto the platform (45). The outline of the feet was marked on a paper sheet fixed on the top of the platform or on the foam pad. Subjects stood at ease (46, 47) and looked at the structured visual scene of the laboratory wall at 6 m distance (4, 48) with both Solid and Foam support. During the trials, subjects would avoid major movements of the upper body and head (in pitch, roll and yaw). All subjects were naive to foam-standing. The last 90 s-epoch of each 100 s stance trial was acquired (49), in order to exclude the adjusting phase occurring after stepping onto the platform or on the foam pad. None of the subjects lost balance while standing on Foam despite the increase in sway compared to Solid support (4). There was no obvious effort at maintaining balance, without displacing the feet, as by flexing the trunk (23) or by performing upper arm movements, as reported by the experimenters sitting at a short distance from the platform and observing the subjects during the acquisition. No subject ever lifted a foot or made a step, either.

Data acquisition and processing

The platform data, from which the ground reaction force was obtained and the CoP excursion computed, were acquired at the sampling frequency of 140 Hz by the SMART Capture software (BTS, Italy). Post-acquisition analysis was done using Excel (Microsoft, USA), LabVIEW (National Instrument, USA) and Origin (OriginLab Corporation, USA). The vertical component of the vertical ground reaction force (VGRF) and the CoP excursions along the anteroposterior (AP) and mediolateral (ML) directions were high-pass filtered at 0.01 Hz and low-pass filtered at 10 Hz with a fourth order Butterworth filter, after removing the respective mean values, with a software developed in LabVIEW. The length of the path (Path Length) was the total length of the time series (90 s) of the CoP displacement in the horizontal plane. Sway Area was the surface of the 95% ellipse fitted to the dispersion of the time-series data on the horizontal plane (50). The same LabVIEW software was used to calculate the Root Mean Square (RMS, the “effective value” of the waveform) of the VGRF for each subject, vision and support condition as a measure of the amplitude of the VGRF oscillations during the acquisition epoch.

The frequency analysis of the VGRF was performed by means of the Auto power spectrum Virtual Instrument (VI) algorithm of the LabVIEW functions. This VI calculated the fast Fourier transform of the VGRF time-series of each trial, subject, visual and support condition. The VI produced a single-sided power spectrum (the positive half of the frequency spectrum from 0.01 to 70 Hz). The resolution (sample frequency/sample number of the VGRF signal) was 0.011 Hz for the sampling frequency of 140 Hz (4) and a sample number of 12,600 (=90 * 140).

The power spectrum profile of the VGRF had the shape of a normal distribution function curve (see Results, Figure 3). Hence, for each participant and for each condition we fitted the VGRF spectrum profile with a Gaussian function, y = Ae-(f-μ)22σ2 by means of the "Curve Fitting” analysis tool of the software Origin, where A indicates the amplitude of the Gaussian function (i.e., the peak value of the curve corresponding to the mean value of the VGRF oscillation frequency), e is the Euler number, μ represents the frequency (f ) at which the peak in the power spectrum profile occurs (henceforth the Mean frequency) and σ is the Standard Deviation (SD) of the Gaussian function. For each experimental condition, the goodness of the fit was estimated by the Pearson's R coefficient. A Gaussian distribution curve was also fit to the mean spectrum profile obtained by averaging all individual subjects' spectra for each condition.

In order to detect differences between base of support conditions, the values of the VGRF RMS, Mean frequency, SD of the Gaussian fit and Peak amplitude of each subject were plotted for Solid vs. Foam support. The relationship between Mean frequency and Peak amplitude of the VGRF oscillations was also studied by a linear regression model. All vision and support conditions collapsed, the relationship between Mean frequency and Peak amplitude of the VGRF oscillations showed a hyperbolic trend (see Results, Figure 6). These data were fitted with a homographic function: y = mxk + x by means of the iterative conjugate gradient method of the Excel Solver Utility.

The relationship between the VGRF oscillation frequency and the geometric measures of the CoP displacement was assessed by plotting the values of Sway Area and Path Length of each subject against the corresponding values of the Mean frequency calculated on the Gaussian fit of the VGRF spectrum. These relationships were studied by a linear regression model and the R2 was calculated. A linear regression model was also used to detect any relationships across subjects between the amplitude of the VGRF RMS and the geometric measures (Sway Area and Path Length).

Assumptions for parametric statistics, evaluated by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Levene's test, were met for all the variables of interest. The effects of the different visual and base of support conditions on VGRF RMS, VGRF Mean frequency, VGRF Peak amplitude, SD of the Gaussian fit, Path Length and Sway Area were compared by 2 (Solid or Foam) × 2 (vision conditions) repeated measure (rm) ANOVA. Where the differences were significant, the effect size ηp2 (partial eta squared) was reported. Post-hoc analysis was performed using the Fisher's LSD test. The significance level was set at 0.05. The minimum detectable effect size was computed using the G*Power sample size calculator (51). Given the number of our participants (n = 23), the study proved to have a sufficient power (>80%) to detect an effect size (Cohen's d) of 0.53. Statistical tests were performed using Statistica (Statsoft, USA).

Results

The amplitude of the VGRF oscillations during stance

The values of the time-series of the VGRF of a typical subject are depicted in Figure 1, together with the corresponding time series of the CoP excursions along the ML and AP directions. The four trial conditions are represented. An epoch of 30 s only (selected from the first part of the entire acquisition epoch to distinguish the signal features) has been shown for clarity of representation of the data time-series out of the entire trial duration (90 s). It appears from the upper panel of the left column of Figure 1 (EC Foam) that the VGRF values range between about −20 N and +20 N (changes with respect to the body weight) and the CoP excursions between −1.8 and 2 cm (ML) and −2.7 and 3 cm (AP). Moreover, the VGRF and the CoP signals have a remarkably different frequency content (compare in each panel the top with the middle and bottom traces). The EC conditions feature a larger amplitude of the noisy trace of the VGRF and larger excursions of the CoP oscillations.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. VGRF variations and CoP displacement in a representative subject. In the left column are reported the VGRF oscillations around the mean value (i.e., the subject's weight) and the CoP oscillations around the mean position for both ML and AP directions in the four different experimental conditions (eyes closed, EC or eyes open, EO) with Foam base of support and EC or EO with Solid base of support). Only the first 30s of the 90s acquisition epochs are shown. In the right side of the Figure, the VGRF oscillations are plotted against the CoP displacements on the horizontal plane in the four conditions of interest. VGRF oscillations and CoP displacements were the greatest in the EC Foam condition. With EO Foam, VGRF oscillations and CoP displacements diminished and became the smallest with Solid base of support where vision had little effect.

In the right column of Figure 1, the VGRF values are reported on the vertical axis, with their positive and negative values, their amplitude being referred to the color calibration bar on the right. On the horizontal plane, the corresponding point-to-point instantaneous CoP positions in the AP and ML axes are reported in black. The CoP excursions were small and occupied a surface area similar in the EO and EC conditions when standing on Solid support. Sway moderately increased in the EO Foam condition and was the largest in the EC Foam condition. During the 90 s acquisition epoch, the VGRF varied continuously around its mean value. VGRF oscillation amplitudes were much larger on Foam, less so with EO than EC, and similarly small on Solid support.

Figure 2A shows the mean values of the VGRF Root Mean Square (RMS) calculated across all subjects as a comprehensive measure of the amplitude of the VGRF oscillations over time for each visual and support condition. The VGRF RMS was the largest on the EC Foam condition and decreased in the order EC Foam > EO Foam > EC Solid ≥ EO Solid. There was a difference between Foam and Solid support [Foam > Solid, main effect, F(1, 22) = 114.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.84]. With EC, the RMS of the VGRF oscillations were greater than with EO [main effect, F(1, 22) = 37.16, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.63]. There was also an interaction between vision and support conditions [F(1, 22) = 43.86, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.66], because the difference between EC and EO was significant with Foam (post-hoc, p < 0.001) but not with Solid support (p = 0.79). In Figure 2B, the VGRF RMS data of all the subjects are plotted Solid vs. Foam. The graph shows in detail the much lower RMS of the oscillations on Solid (ranging from 1 to 2 N in the ordinate) than on Foam support (mainly ranging from 2 to 6 N in the abscissa). Almost all dots lie below the diagonal (where Solid = Foam). It also shows that removal of vision (pink dots) increased the RMS values compared to EO (gray dots) in most subjects on Foam but not on Solid support (the mean values of the gray and pink dots were different on the horizontal but not on the vertical axis). The regression lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam data were not different between visual conditions (F-test: slopes, p = 0.81; intercepts, p = 0.24).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The VGRF Root Mean Square (RMS). (A) The mean values of VGRF RMS are shown for the two vision conditions (eyes closed, EC; eyes open, EO) and for the two bases of support (Foam and Solid). (B) The values of VGRF RMS for Solid are plotted against those for Foam, for each subject and vision condition (EC, pink dots; EO, gray dots). The black dotted line is the identity line. The VGRF RMS were greater with Foam than Solid under both vision conditions. The equations of the regression lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam data were: EC, y = 0.14 x + 1.27 (R2 = 0.16, p = 0.06); EO, y = 0.17 x + 1.35 (R2 = 0.13, p = 0.09). Asterisks indicate significant differences (***p < 0.001).

The probabilistic distribution of the frequencies of the VGRF oscillations

The spectral profiles of the oscillations in the VGRF time series showed frequencies across a relatively ample range independently of vision and support conditions (Figure 3). The profiles had the shape of a normal distribution curve, where the frequencies at both the left and the right side of the peak had very low spectral amplitude. In all four conditions, the profiles featured a peak at an oscillation frequency around 4 Hz.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. VGRF frequency spectrum. The mean spectrum profiles across subjects are reported for EC Foam (A), EO Foam (B), EC Solid (C) and EO Solid (D) conditions. The VGRF spectrum profile shows a bell curve-like shape, fitted with a Gaussian (red) curve. The parameters of the curve fitting the spectrum are reported in each panel. The VGRF spectrum had a much greater amplitude with Foam than Solid support. Note the extended scale in the ordinates of (C,D), due to the peak amplitude of the spectrum with Solid support being less than one tenth of that with Foam (A,B). The VGRF spectrum had a much greater amplitude with Foam than Solid support. The effect of vision was unambiguous with Foam support only. The frequency at the peak of the Gaussian fit was around 4 Hz with Foam and around 5 Hz with Solid support. The Standard Deviation (SD) of the Gaussian fits were larger for Solid than Foam support.

The four panels of Figure 3 show the spectrum profiles, computed as the average of the spectra of all the subjects, with EC (left) and EO (right), on Foam (A and B) and on Solid ground (C and D). The more represented frequencies occurred in a relatively short range, between 3 and 6 Hz, regardless of vision and surface conditions. The overall amplitude of the spectra (note the different scale in the ordinate) was remarkably different in the order EC Foam > EO Foam > EC Solid ≥ EO Solid. The peak values of the Gaussian curves fitting the mean spectrum profile are reported in each panel. The shape of the curves reflected a strong tendency for the VGRF oscillation frequencies to gather around a common value, regardless of the characteristics of support and vision. The frequency of the spectrum at the peak of the Gaussian curve is referred to here as the Mean spectral frequency. There were relatively large standard deviations of the Gaussian curves, larger for the Solid (the curves were flat and broad) than the Foam support.

A Gaussian curve was then fit to the spectrum profile of each participant in the different vision and base of support conditions. For each power spectrum profile, the goodness of the fit was estimated by the Pearson's R coefficient. The mean value of R was about 0.6 for the Foam conditions (with both EC and EO) and about 0.3 for the Solid conditions (EC and EO). Figure 4A shows a summary of the Mean frequency values across subjects for each vision and support condition. There was a difference in Mean frequency between support conditions [main effect, Solid > Foam, F(1, 22) = 52.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.71], but not in Mean frequency between visual conditions [main effect, F(1, 22) = 2.74, p = 0.11]. The interaction between visual and support conditions was significant [F(1, 22) = 4.54, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.17], because there was a difference between EC and EO condition on Foam (post-hoc, p < 0.05) but not on Solid support (p = 0.78). Figure 4B shows the mean standard deviations (SD) of the Gaussian curves across subjects. The mean SDs were similar to those obtained by fitting the normal curve to the averaged spectra. The mean SD was greater for Solid than Foam support [main effect, F(1, 22) = 110.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.83] under both EC and EO conditions. There was no significant difference between visual conditions [main effect, F(1, 22) = 0.76, p = 0.39] and no significant interaction between visual and base of support conditions [F(1, 22) = 1.09, p = 0.31]. In Figure 4C, the individual values of the Mean frequency are plotted for Solid against Foam support condition, and the vision state is indicated by the colored dots (EC, pink; EO, gray). Across the subjects, for the Solid support (ordinate), frequencies ranged from about 3 to 6.5 Hz, whereas for Foam support the prevalent frequencies were comprised in a narrower range, from about 3.0 to 4.5 Hz, with a relatively large scatter across subjects. Reference to the identity line confirms that the prevailing frequencies of the VGRF oscillations had a higher value for Solid than Foam support in most subjects regardless of vision condition. Vision compared to no-vision produced a minor increase in the frequencies on Foam but not on Solid support. The regression lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam data were not different between visual conditions (F-test, slopes p = 0.8, intercepts p = 0.37). Figure 4D shows the values of the SDs of the Gaussian curves in each subject with Solid plotted against Foam condition. SDs were comprised in a larger range for Solid than Foam across subjects. The regression lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam SDs of the Gaussian fit were not different between visual conditions (slopes p = 0.8; intercepts p = 0.37).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The VGRF mean frequency and the SD of the Gaussian fit. The mean values of the VGRF Mean frequency (A) and of the SD of the Gaussian curve (B) calculated across subjects are reported for each visual and support condition. The relationship between the Solid and Foam support data for the Mean frequency values and SDs are shown in (C,D). Pink dots are referred to EC, gray dots to EO condition. The black dotted lines are the identity lines. The Mean frequency and the SD were greater for Solid than Foam support. The dot distribution in the abscissa shows that regardless of the support condition, there was no difference between EC and EO in VGRF Mean frequency and SD. The equations of the lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam Mean frequency values were: EC, y = 1.26 x + 0.06 (R2 = 0.32, p < 0.01); EO, y = 0.47 x + 2.84 (R2 = 0.05, p = 0.33). The regression lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam SDs of the Gaussian fit were y = 0.65 x + 2.12 (R2 = 0.03, p = 0.44) for EC, and y = 0.61 x + 1.86 (R2 = 0.02, p = 0.48) for EO. Asterisks indicate significant differences (*p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001).

Amplitudes of the mean values of the VGRF oscillation spectra across vision and support surface conditions

The Peak amplitude calculated on the Gaussian fit is a measure of the amplitude of the VGRF oscillation at its Mean frequency of oscillations. The peak of the curves indicates a strong prevalence of the spectral amplitudes around 4 Hz. We analyzed these amplitudes in Figure 5. The mean peak values of the VGRF spectrum (N2) are reported for each vision and support surface condition. The Peak amplitude of the VGRF spectrum was small on Solid support (both EC and EO), increased in the EO Foam condition, and was the largest in the EC Foam condition (Figure 5A).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. VGRF Peak amplitude. The mean VGRF Peak amplitude (A) are reported for each vision and support condition. Amplitude was the greatest in EC Foam condition and decreased with EO Foam. It was the smallest with Solid support, where vision had no effect. (B) The VGRF amplitudes for Solid are plotted against those for Foam, for each subject and vision condition (EC, pink dots; EO, gray dots). The equations of the lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam data points were: EC, y = 0.024 x- 0.0003 (R2 = 0.23, p < 0.05); EO, y = 0.039 x + 0.001 (R2 = 0.26, p < 0.05). Asterisks indicate significant differences (***p < 0.001).

Differences were significant between support conditions [Foam > Solid: main effect, F(1, 22) = 87.36, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.79] and between visual conditions [EC > EO, main effect F(1, 22) = 79.16, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.78]. There was a significant interaction between visual and support conditions [F(1, 22) = 84.34, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.79]. Post-hoc analysis showed a difference between EC and EO with Foam (p < 0.001) but not with Solid support, where both Peak amplitudes were quite low (EC = EO, p = 0.97). In Figure 5B the Peak amplitude of the power spectrum on Solid base of support was plotted against the amplitude of power spectrum on Foam for EC (pink dots) and EO conditions (gray dots) for all subjects. The regression lines fitting the Solid vs. Foam data had a similar slope (p = 0.62), but different intercepts (p < 0.05).

In Figure 6, the Mean frequencies of the VGRF spectrum (i.e., those at the peak of the Gaussian curves) were plotted for each subject against the corresponding Peak amplitudes. Across the subjects, large peak spectral amplitudes corresponded to relatively lower spectral Mean frequencies. This was true when standing on Solid support as well, even if the amplitudes of the VGRF oscillations were small. The equations of the lines fitting the values of the Mean frequencies plotted against the Peak amplitudes are reported in Table 1 for each vision and support condition.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Relationship between Mean frequency and Peak amplitude of the VGRF spectrum. For each subject, the values of the Mean frequency are plotted against those of the Peak amplitude of the Gaussian curve in the different visual and base of support conditions (EC Foam, red; EC Solid, yellow; EO Foam, green; EO Solid, blue). The data distribution showed a hyperbolic trend. The black dotted line corresponds to the hyperbola fitting the data (equation in the inset).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Equations of the lines fitting the values of VGRF Mean frequency versus Peak amplitude in the four standing conditions.

Considering all vision and support conditions together, the scatter of the data suggested the possibility that a hyperbolic function could fit the relationship between the two variables, in the assumption that a common behavior underpins the VGRF oscillations across all subjects and all conditions. The hyperbolic fit through all data points shows that large peak amplitudes of the VGRF spectra were associated with low frequencies and vice versa. The equation of the function is reported in Figure 6.

Relationship between VGRF spectrum and geometric variables (sway area and path length)

The mean values of Sway Area and Path Length are reported in Figure 7 for each vision and support conditions. Sway Area (Figure 7A) was greater with Foam than Solid support [main effect, F(1, 22) = 190.67, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.89] and with EC than EO [main effect, F(1, 22) = 76.71, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.78]. A significant interaction between vision and support conditions was found [F(1, 22) = 64.41, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.74]. There was a difference in Sway Area between EO and EC with Foam (post-hoc, p < 0.001) but not with Solid support (post-hoc, p = 0.38). Also for Path Length (Figure 7B) there was a significant difference between support conditions [Foam > Solid: main effect, F(1, 22) = 204.54, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.90] and between vision conditions [EC > EO: main effect, F(1, 22) = 106.96, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.83]. The interaction between vision and support conditions was significant [F(1, 22) = 103.25, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.82]. There was a difference between EC and EO in Path Length with Foam (post-hoc, p < 0.001) but not with Solid support (post-hoc, p = 0.48).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Geometric measures. Mean values of Sway Area (A) and Path Length (B) for each visual and base of support conditions. Both Sway Area and Path Length were greater with Foam than Solid support. The effect of vision was significant only with Foam. Asterisks indicate significant differences (***p < 0.001).

There was a noticeable similarity between Figure 2A (showing VGRF RMS in the tested conditions) and Figure 7B. Therefore, in Figure 8, the values of Sway Area (A) and Path Length (B) have been plotted against the corresponding values of the VGRF RMS. In both cases, there was an obvious proportionality between the two variables, Sway Area and Path Length generally increasing across vision and support conditions with the increase in VGRF RMS. Sway Area and Path Length have also been plotted against the corresponding values of the prevalent VGRF frequency at the peak of the Gaussian curve (Figures 8C,D). Across the subjects, the larger Sway Area or Path Length, the lower the prevailing frequencies of the VGRF oscillations. All visual and support conditions contributed to these relationships. However, within each condition, the slope of the linear relationships was not equally strong, either with or without vision. The equations of the lines fitting the values of Sway Area or Path Length against both the VGRF RMS and the VGRF Mean frequency are reported in Tables 2, 3, respectively. Of note, even in the EC Foam conditions, the excursions of the CoP were limited, attesting a good postural control. The mean value across subjects of the maximal excursions was 2.6 ± 0.8 cm in the ML direction and 3.0 ± 0.7 cm in the AP direction.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Relationship between geometric measures and VGRF RMS and Mean frequency. Sway Area of each subject was plotted against the corresponding value of VGRF RMS (A) and of VGRF Mean frequency (C). On the right, Path Length was plotted against the corresponding value of VGRF RMS (B) and VGRF Mean frequency (D) (EC Foam, red; EC Solid, yellow; EO Foam, green; EO Solid, blue). The equations of the regression lines for each variable and condition are in Tables 2, 3.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Equations of the lines fitting the values of Sway Area and Path Length versus VGRF RMS in the four standing conditions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Equations of the lines fitting the values of Sway Area and Path Length versus VGRF Mean frequency in the four standing conditions.

Discussion

Several past and recent studies have secured crucial information on the human gait thanks to the analysis of the vertical ground reaction force (VGRF) recorded during the stance phase of walking (5255), where the GRF shows a range of orientations of major components, including horizontal (frictional) forces with the ground. Characteristics of the human upright stance have been addressed based on the analysis of the VGRF as well. However, the vertical force active on force sensors of the platform upon which subjects stand has been exploited almost exclusively in order to describe the excursions of the Centre of Pressure (CoP) on the horizontal plane (7, 56, 57). VGRFs have been recorded and analyzed sparingly, even if they are at the interface between the “postural control strategy” and the body sway measures deduced by the CoP excursions (58, 59). Looking at the VGRF with new eyes, however, has given direct, intriguing perspective on the control of body balance by standing subjects and on the plan of action to maintain the upright posture under different sensory and support surface conditions (60).

Here, we have found that: (a) the amplitude (RMS) of the VGRF oscillations is larger on the compliant that hard support surface; (b) there is a modest correspondence between VGRF RMS and amplitude of Sway Area and Path Length of the CoP; (c) the oscillations of the VGRF have a dominant frequency around 4–5 Hz in both support conditions, but the frequency range is larger on hard than compliant support; (d) vision compared to no-vision reduces the amplitude of the VGRF oscillation on the compliant but not hard surface; (e) vision does not affect the frequency of oscillation (on either supports); (f) in general, the higher the oscillation frequency, the lower the amplitude of the VGRF oscillations. We have also confirmed that the VGRF prevalent frequencies are well beyond the oscillation frequencies of the CoP excursion in either the anteroposterior or mediolateral direction.

Amplitudes and frequencies of the VGRF: The effect of the support base

The oscillations of the VGRF around the body weight are of moderate amplitude having a RMS value of about 5 N (or 0.5 kg) on compliant support, and even smaller on the hard platform, about 2 N (or about 0.2 kg). Then, since subjects were standing on both feet and actively controlling the postural muscles, particularly on the compliant support, a reasonable, supposition is that the vertical force repeatedly increased underneath one and decreased underneath the other foot in the mediolateral direction, dictated by the “hip strategy” implying a non-negligible CoP SD in the ML direction as shown by Sozzi et al. (28). Conversely, in the anteroposterior direction, when sway is under the control of the muscles around the ankle, the vertical force would decrease below the toes and increase at the heels or vice versa during the ankle dorsiflexions and plantarflexions. This is in keeping with the larger CoP SD in the frontal than in the sagittal plane (28). In this light, the frequency of the oscillations of the VGRF would depend on the rate of change in the decreasing and increasing phases of the rhythmic motion of the body and its segments in the vertical direction. These would produce the resulting vertical force changes due to the torques exerted by the activated muscles on the support surface (38).

The largely superimposable VGRF oscillation frequency values at the peak of the spectrum profile (as identified by the Gaussian curve fitted to it) when standing on both the hard and the compliant support bases was an unexpected finding. This occurred in spite of much larger force oscillations (more than ten times) on the compliant than hard support, quantified by the root mean square of the time series (RMS, or “signal strength,” in N). There was a significant difference in the frequencies of the VGRF between Foam and Solid support indicating a consistent behavior among the subjects in each of these two conditions. But the difference was moderate, because the mean frequency of VGRF oscillations at the peak of the Gaussian curve was only about 13% larger on hard than compliant surface. Instead, a consistent difference in the power spectrum profile was shown by the frequency range at both sides of the peak of the spectrum, whereby the Gaussian curve had a low peak and broad slope (a larger SD) with hard support, indicating that VGRF frequencies were represented in a larger range of values. These data are in keeping with a previous report by Oggero et al. (33). The highest VGRF amplitudes were found in a range between 2 and 8 Hz, with pronounced peak around 4 Hz, also by Günther and Wagner (43) in subjects standing quietly on a solid base of support (see their Figure 7).

The most represented VGRF frequencies would speak in favor of a basic mode of muscle activation underpinning standing (that we have named here the “postural rhythm”), common to hard or compliant bases of support (61). The small differences in the prevailing oscillation frequencies between compliant and hard support can be reasonably attributed to the mechanical properties of the foam pad (featuring a visco-elastic structure), which introduces a time delay between the development of the muscle torques and the recorded force, allowing a more extended joint excursion than on hard support and leading to lower frequency of the continuous rhythmic muscle action (37). On the hard support, the VGRF oscillations would be much smaller in amplitude because weak muscle activity (in time and space) would be amply sufficient for body balancing. Hence, the control of upright posture would result primarily in minimal ankle motion with stiff knee at a somewhat higher prevailing VGRF frequency (62), while the larger instability on foam would produce more hip and knee than ankle motion and slightly lower peak frequencies of force oscillation (63). We should add that standing on either support did never produce instability or kind of stumbling, forcing the subjects to lift a foot or move an arm.

The effect of vision

The comparable VGRF frequencies when standing with and without vision were unanticipated. It is known that the amplitude of the sway of the CoP excursions is definitely larger with EC than EO (64) and larger on compliant than solid support [see e.g., (4)]. The median frequency of the CoP excursions on the horizontal plane (both AP and ML) is higher with EC than EO as well (4, 65, 66). However, the mean frequencies of the VGRF oscillations showed no major effect of vision. If anything, frequencies were seemingly slightly higher with EO than EC. The lower frequency at the peak of the Gaussian curve on compliant than hard support was also independent of vision. It seems that access to vision does not modulate the frequency of the muscle actions producing the VGRF oscillations. While this comes as no surprise when standing on hard support, where the CoP excursions and sway area are often superimposed with or without vision (65, 67) and where the effect of vision is larger for narrow feet positions (68), the larger body sway on Foam EC than EO is independent of the VGRF frequency. Rather, vision largely modulates the amplitude of the VGRF oscillations (the RMS) concurrently with the effects on body sway (Figures 1, 8A,B). This is also suggested by the shape of the relationship between VGRF frequency and VGRF amplitude, featuring a hyperbolic association, where the amplitude of the oscillations ranged from near zero N2 (on hard support, regardless of vision) to 0.16 N2 (without vision), while the frequency of the oscillations ranged from about 4 to about 6 Hz (Figure 6).

Hence, vision seems to hardly affect the basic rhythm of the muscle actions but to limit their vigor. Under different conditions (standing on a translating platform), the activity of many postural muscles rapidly diminished when vision was allowed, leading to reduction of body segments' swinging back and forth, without effect on the latency of the responses (69, 70). Similarly, standing on a very mobile support was not possible without vision (71). No direct question has been posed here or methods employed in order to assess the consistency of the frequency of the descending command when standing on hard or compliant support, with or without vision. However, we would remind here the conclusions by Jacobs et al. (72), who found no significant differences in cortico-muscular coherence, as a sign of cortical involvement in human standing balance, when comparing standing with eyes open to standing with eyes closed and standing on hard vs. compliant surface. We would speculate that the increased amplitude of the VGRF oscillations with EC on compliant support would be a sign that the vestibular reflexes take the lead under this condition (7375), without affecting the basic, rhythmic pattern set by the cortical control of stance. As a side note, we would remind that the dichotomy vision/no-vision in postural control has to be still convincingly demonstrated. Schmid et al. (76) cast doubt on the simple, conservative hypothesis of two fundamentally different balancing strategies. Rather, the body seemed to exhibit a continuous mode of balancing patterns as a function of visual acuity.

The postural rhythm

It is remarkable that the muscle actuators of balance control and corrections are substantially rhythmic within a restricted cadence range, and the rhythm is little affected by support compliance. Here, the frequency range of the VGRF is compatible with values found by others (33, 43, 7779). Around these prevailing frequencies, a wider range of lower and higher oscillations is shown by the tails of the Gaussian curve, broader on hard than compliant support.

Interestingly, the prevailing frequency values are close to the human body resonant frequency, assumed to be in the range of 3 to 7 Hz (78, 8083). The continuous variations in the VGRF oscillation frequency would be basically tuned to the body resonant frequency and to the capacity of the muscles to produce the corrective balance reactions. These would be in turn associated in some respect to their twitch time course. The torque exerted by the muscles around the ankles, in standing subjects voluntarily contracting their leg and foot muscles in a reaction time mode, has a duration of around 250 ms (84) similar for both toe-down and toe-up actions. Others have found that a figure of 3–4 bursts per second is compatible with an “intermittent” control (85, 86). In principle, then, the lowermost frequency would be set by the time duration of the mechanical action of the muscle twitch time course (15, 16).

For example, when standing on the compliant support, the toe-up action leading to a forward body displacement would be promptly counterbalanced by the toe-down action, thereby producing a peak in the VGRF at a frequency around 4 Hz. If the two legs would operate (at least partly) independently, the slope of the Gaussian curve with a peak around 4 Hz would easily materialize. The same basic pattern of control would be exploited for standing on hard support. As said, however, on hard support the borderline frequencies are relatively more represented, subjects showing smaller spectral amplitude at (relatively) lower and higher oscillation frequencies. When the support is hard, the postural rhythm is tuned to frequencies continuously variable in a wider range than on compliant support, including frequencies from about 3 Hz to about 8 Hz. It is expected that low frequencies occur on hard support (15), because longer “stable” periods would occur on a safe and easy stance. On the other hand, notably so, frequencies around 7–8 Hz have been recorded on the bare platform on vestibular galvanic stimulation (87). The higher small-amplitude frequencies on hard support would speak for small-amplitude sub-tetanic contractions, with production of force ripples at shorter intervals. These frequencies are still below the production of fused muscle contractions but would tend to favor some tonically maintained levels of force in the slow-twitch postural muscles [having long twitch time course, see e.g., (88)]. In all cases, the higher VGRF oscillation frequencies appear to find their upper limit in the body resonant frequency, hence they are seemingly appropriate to produce the necessary sway corrections with the minimal possible effort. The significantly lower prevailing frequency and the more limited frequency range with compliant than hard support (33) would be explained by the minor fluctuating changes in the resonant frequency observed under relaxed than stiff posture, likely due to variable bending of lower limb joints (78, 89).

Interestingly, continuous antagonist muscles co-activation (likely featuring small, high frequency VGRF oscillations) is linked to poor postural steadiness in the elderly, and is accompanied by enhanced stiffness and larger body sway (9092). Co-contraction has been associated with increased CoP excursion frequency and shown to be not necessarily helpful for stabilization of upright posture (93, 94).

The oscillation frequency of the VGRF are not represented in the frequencies of CoP oscillations

One wonders why the prevailing frequency of VGRF variation is so different from those of the excursion of the CoP, where the 98% of the power spectrum is normally comprised below 2 Hz (4, 5, 95), and where the median frequency attests itself at about 0.3 Hz with EC and 0.15 Hz with EO (4, 77, 96, 97). This comes as no surprise, though, because the CoP location is the resultant of the torques acting onto various positions of the CoP on the platform and can theoretically have infinite solutions. Suffice it to say that, if the upper segments of the body (e.g., trunk and head) move with their own speed (probably low) back and forth or sidewise or in any directions of the space, their displacement would affect the horizontal position of the Centre of Mass (CoM) and the CoP excursion but not necessarily the VGRF frequency. These motions would have an effect on the CoP oscillation frequency according to their own displacement speed, which is in turn dependent on the force of the muscles acting on the mass of those segments (60, 98).

While the twitch time course of a postural muscle can be of the order of several tenths of ms, the elicited movement of the body can be fast (but definitely not as fast as the twitch time-course) or extremely slow as when the postural conditions impose a quasi-isometric muscle contraction. If anything, our findings support the view that modeling body sway with an inverted pendulum hardly provides for a useful generalization of the balance strategies (5, 22, 99). Fluctuations in ankle, knee and hip joints participate in upright stance adjustments (100). These fluctuations may not be obvious in the frequency spectrum of the CoP displacement, but frequencies around 4–8 Hz have been recently attributed to angular fluctuations in the leg joints (20, 21). The rhythmic contributions of the knee joint to the mechanical dynamics of the standing body would mostly appear as vertical motions of the CoM with corresponding frequency content, in keeping with our findings obtained by spectral analysis of the VGRF. It has been recently shown that continuous motions of ankle, knee and hip joints induce coordinated shifts of the line of action of the ground reaction force above or below the CoM, producing changes in both the VGRF and CoP oscillations (92, 98, 101). Under different conditions (locomotion), very small displacements of the CoP can produce very large, slow displacements of the CoM by minimally changing the CoP instantaneous position and the direction of the VGRF vector, as occurs during walking and gentle turning (102).

Interestingly, a minor increase in the CoP frequency spectrum in both the frontal and the sagittal plane between 3.5 and 6 Hz frequency had been described by Krafczyk et al. (103) in people with psychogenic vertigo standing on a compliant surface. This had been attributed to co-activation of anti-gravity muscles, and interpreted as a shift from automatic to conscious control. An effect of surface compliance was noted at high frequencies (4 to 7 Hz) in the CoP spectrum in medio-lateral direction by Singh et al. (32) and attributed to high frequency activity of the muscles controlling the lateral body sway. This possibility merits further investigation, and would require analysis of the VGRF oscillations together with the analysis of the CoP frequencies in a larger range beyond the usual upper limit of 2–3 Hz [see (4)] that is often chosen because most of the CoP spectrum is normally comprised below 2 Hz.

Relationship of VGRF oscillation amplitudes with the common geometric markers of stance

It is remarkable that the spectral amplitudes measured by the RMS or the spectral amplitudes at the prevailing frequency (as seen in Figures 2, 6, respectively) almost mirror the pattern of mean Sway Area and Path Length in the corresponding vision and support conditions (Figures 7A,B). It would not be wise to assume a strict cause-effect association between these variables, because there is an ample variation across subjects (Figure 8). A significant association is present only on the compliant support (Figures 8A,B), where both VGRF spectral amplitudes and amplitude of body sway, independently, would express the difficulty of standing on the compliant surface. Conversely, on the hard support, the association disappears. Here, the coefficients of determination are weak and not significant, indicating that important confounding factors interfere. Therefore, the amplitude of the spectrum of the VGRF at its maximally represented frequency of oscillation cannot be considered a proxy for body oscillation, particularly when standing on hard support (59).

Of note, the stability boundary (where the subjects lean as far as possible in all directions of the horizontal plane without losing stability) has been shown to be just smaller on foam than on hard surface, and the velocity of the CoP excursion to be higher (104). We would also add that the largest excursions of the CoP in the horizontal plane in our subjects were largely within the stability boundaries, regardless of vision condition and support feature. These have been found to be about 10 and 7 cm in ML and AP, respectively, slightly greater with than without vision and on hard than solid support (104). In our hands, CoP never really came close to the boundaries, because its maximal excursions hardly ever exceeded 2–3 cm in the most critical stance condition (Foam, EC) (as in Figure 1).

Proprioception subserves postural adjustments on foam

The use of a compliant standing surface has been often motivated by the attempt to disturb, attenuate or nullify the proprioceptive information (105110). Such disturbing effect would be stronger in older persons (35) even if foam was not discriminant in all studies (111). Teasdale et al. (112) showed that removal of one sensory input in isolation (e.g., standing on foam) was not sufficient to differentiate between elderly and young adults, and explained this as the result of compensation by the remaining sensory sources. We do not believe that a compliant surface excludes or disrupts proprioception. There is no reason why proprioception should be attenuated or muddled when standing on foam. Muscle spindles are very sensitive to rapid muscle stretches [see (113)], particularly so for the primary terminations of the large Ia fibers (114). Conversely, the secondary terminations of the group II afferent fibers are sensitive to elongation (less so to its velocity). Both receptors produce clear cut and powerful homonymous and heteronymous motor reflexes (115117) and afferent volleys reaching distant spinal centres (118) and supra-spinal centres as well (119122). The findings by Chiang and Wu (123) support a similar role of proprioceptive information when standing on hard or compliant surface. In both circumstances, when the platform upon which subjects stood was rotated, the angular velocity of the ankle joint was similar, and the latencies of the evoked reflex responses were just minimally increased by standing on foam. If anything, it might be crudely schematised that standing on the complaint surface exploits a powerful input along the Ia fibers, while standing on the hard surface favors group II fiber activity (124127). Hence, both spindle afferent inputs from the muscles of the foot (116) and of those around the ankle would contribute their information to a common feedback control of stance on hard and compliant surface (17, 128, 129). Of course, these inputs would not be the same under both support surface conditions (130), and the reasonable conclusion is that standing on a compliant or on a hard surface are two different tasks with a common task goal. But this is kind of a truism. We would rather suggest that one of the reasons for the broader VGRF frequency spectrum profile on hard support might depend on the prevalent input of the group II spindle fibers, which are sensitive to position more than velocity of body displacement and able to elicit reflex responses on both the ipsi- and contralateral leg muscles (117).

The interaction between sensory inputs and motor commands

The modest (but significant) difference in the frequency range of the oscillations in the VGRF on hard and compliant support would speak for a common sensory input to the brain and a common feedback operation. Whereas, the much larger peak amplitude of the spectrum for compliant than hard support surface would be explained by the strong and rhythmic activity of the muscles involved in the balance correcting reactions (131), in turn connected with the need for more vigorous contractions. On compliant surface, the feedback would recruit several body muscles (20, 75, 118, 132134) and operate together with anticipatory activations (135, 136). This interaction would be modulated by the sensory inputs and efference copies connected with the continuous body sway and muscle actions, respectively (137).

The information from the cutaneous receptors of the foot sole may fine-tune the VGRF frequencies. Recently, these receptors, with focus on their role in posture, have received attention by various groups [(138141); see (142)]. Their contribution is different between hard and compliant surface, not least because in the former case a continuous stimulation of these receptors occurs over the foot sole, while in latter case sway could help periodically release skin compression to reactivate mechanoreceptors (143). The larger contact with the compliant than hard support surface might enhance the feedback control from the feet [see (144)].

The control mode would possibly imply both a temporal check during the major reactions to destabilization and an amplitude check during the unavoidable counterbalancing phase (145). Balancing and counterbalancing would require alternate actions transmitted to the support surface by reciprocal activation of, say, the triceps of one body side and the pretibial muscles on the other side when the destabilization occurs in the roll plane and activates muscles in addition to those about the hip (146). In the case of the pitch plane, this alternation would occur in rapid succession and simultaneously on both sides (146). The frequency of the descending commands might be similar in both circumstances. Conversely, the “safer” condition of standing on a hard support with feet apart allows an easier selection of an optimal time- and space-dependent strategy aimed at minimizing muscle activation (147, 148), under conditions where minor but precise activation of the intrinsic foot muscles can be very effective (18, 149).

In general, we would admit that different subjects across the various standing conditions choose slightly different standing modes, by modulating both the amplitude of sway and the frequency of VGRF. The hyperbola fit to all subjects' VGRF mean frequency against the VGRF peak amplitude (Figure 6) would suggest a common pattern, whereby oscillations at predominantly high frequency values (on hard support) are associated to small-amplitude VGRF values, while lower frequencies (particularly on compliant support) are associated to large-amplitude force oscillations. A similar pattern is shown by the plot of Sway Area vs. VGRF frequency across subjects, where the higher VGRF frequencies are associated with small CoP displacement (be it Sway Area or Path Length) and the opposite is true for low frequencies. Of course, the data points corresponding to different vision and support conditions cluster around different regions of the hyperbola, but we would be surprised of an unlike distribution. In a sense, this hyperbolic pattern indicates that the intensity of the effort implicit in standing is broadly constant in a population of young healthy subjects. This intensity is either allocated to the rhythmicity of the production of the active muscle forces or to the control of the amplitude of the contraction, therefore of the body movements allowed. In this context, vision and support compliance stipulate their constraints in compliance with the properties of the neural control (41). The hyperbola also shows that VGRF oscillation frequencies have a limit below 3 Hz in all subjects on both compliant and hard support. Thus, it would be safe to state that subjects are never quasi-static, suggesting that a control programme is always running.

Limitations

The number of participants was limited and the study was restricted to young healthy adults. In addition, we considered only one test per condition (vision and support) per subject. Even so, where vision and support conditions produced significant differences in VGRF (for VGRF RMS, Peak amplitude and Mean frequency, SD of the Gaussian fit) and in the geometric measures, the effect size was greater than the minimum effect size set by our sample of 23 participants. Absence of trial reiteration was justified by the great adaptation that we recently described in the frequency values of the CoP excursions in repeated successive trials (28). However, the long acquisition epoch (90 s) would permit to be confident on the values of the recorded patterns. As a further limitation, the participants to these experiments were not instructed to stand “as still as possible” (150) under all test conditions. We were concerned that that instruction would have favored a stiff attitude and a possible stumbling reaction when subjects felt unstable on foam. The instruction was the same on hard support. We cannot deny the possibility that this loose specification enhanced the inter-individual variability and the idiosyncratic characteristics of postural sway (151). We did not manipulate the between-feet distance, either (152), or the characteristics of the foam pad (38, 153), because many volunteers were not ready to repeat the trials in different days. Further, our platform did not allow separate recording of the VGRF of the right and left foot [see (26)] that would have helped identify the role of the inter-leg coordination in setting the postural rhythm. Moreover, there has been no optokinetic (or otherwise) direct recording of the body segments' motion. This would be necessary because most recent models of human stance are based on motion in the sagittal plane, but standing on a compliant surface is more complex. Moreover, ECG/EMG recording that might have helped disclose the contribution of the heart beat to the VGRF oscillations (21, 154, 155). This would be relevant in people with some forms of a central nervous system pathology (156). These experiments are needed, and will likely offer independent ways to confirm the suggestions put forward so far.

Conclusions and perspectives

Together with imposed perturbations, support surface manipulations provide an additional tool to investigate how sensory and motor signals are integrated to control balance during standing. The present findings have addressed the features of the VGRF under hard and compliant surface in young healthy subjects, and are preliminary to more extended research. The foam support produces mechanical perturbations and sensory inflows that elicit active compensatory responses achieving body stabilization with the least effort. These mechanical responses are quite rhythmic on foam, and still rhythmic on hard support, even if less ample and regular. Under both circumstances, though, a prevalent frequency in the VGRF emerges, close to the natural body resonant frequency. Vision does not affect the peak VGRF frequency. Anyhow, vision helps strongly reduce the amplitude of the oscillation frequencies and probably restrains sway by focussing on the anticipatory control of upright body orientation [(157, 158); see (159)], without necessarily “recruiting” new, special frequencies of postural muscle activation as appreciable in the VGRF oscillation pattern.

At variance with the traditional measures of CoP excursions in the horizontal plane, the VGRF is directly affected by the postural muscle action. The information provided by the analysis of the VGRF is roughly superimposed to that of the geometric measures (length and surface covered by the CoP trajectory), if the amplitude (the RMS) of the VGRF oscillations is compared to the traditional geometric measures. But, VGRF oscillations frequencies are instead only loosely related to the geometric measures. It seems that the VGRF oscillations represent a basic rhythm that creates the conditions for the successful action of superimposed focused muscle activations, responsible for the emergence of stabilizing torques (98). Since the CoM would not always oscillate in precisely the vertical direction, the horizontal component of the GRF would create the horizontal translations of the CoM (101).

We suggest that future analysis of the frequency pattern of VGRF oscillation and of the EMG bursts of the postural muscles would help address the origin and the modulation of supra spinal control, the reflex and intrinsic joint stiffness components participating in the control of posture [see (160)], and the physiological mechanisms responsible for balance modifications (161). The effect of clinically relevant sensory and motor deficits [e.g., otolith dysfunction, (162, 163)] might be usefully addressed by adding analysis of the prevailing VGRF oscillation frequencies to those of the geometric measures. Further, since adaptation over successive standing trials has been recently described (5), it will be relevant to investigate whether and how VGRF can be independently modulated by the adaptation process to enhance postural stability. We hope that the present structural approach can have diverse applications in the field of balance control. It might help classify and understand postural states in various populations with balance problems, as has been done already with older persons and Parkinson's disease patients (29, 31, 77, 164). We would speculate that reduction in the nerve conduction velocity in neuropathies or muscle weakness of various origin might find convenient markers in the VGRF frequency or RMS, respectively [see (165), for a discourse on this possibility]. Further research is needed to connect the present findings to possible sources of potential disorders in the postural control system.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, upon reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri IRCCS, Approval Number #2564-CE. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MS conceived the idea for the study. SS performed the recruitment of participants and the collection of data. SS and MS performed the data analysis and drafted the article. M-CD revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

The funding for this study was provided by the ICS Maugeri SB, through the Ricerca Corrente program of the Italian Ministry of Health.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the helpful comments of the reviewers. MS would like to thank A Nardone for facilitating the measurements at the laboratory CSAM, ICS Maugeri, Pavia.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Kapteyn TS, Bles W, Njiokiktjien CJ, Kodde L, Massen CH, Mol JM, et al. Standardization in platform stabilometry being a part of posturography. Agressologie. (1983) 24:321–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. Eng JJ, Winter DA. Estimations of the horizontal displacement of the total body centre of mass: considerations during standing activities. Gait Posture. (1993) 1:141–4. doi: 10.1016/0966-6362(93)90055-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Panjan A, Šarabon N. Review of methods for the evaluation of human body balance. Medicine. (2010) 10:178294276. doi: 10.2478/v10237-011-0036-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sozzi S, Nardone A, Schieppati M. Specific posture-stabilizing effects of vision and touch are revealed by distinct changes of body oscillation frequencies. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:756984. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.756984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Sozzi S, Schieppati M. Balance adaptation while standing on a compliant base depends on the current sensory condition in healthy young adults. Front Hum Neurosci. (2022) 16:839799. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.839799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gatev P, Koleva V, Petkova G, Dimitrova D, Ilieva L. Changes in plantar pressures during bipedal stance with different stance width. Acta Physiol Pharmacol Bulg. (2001) 26:137–42.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

7. Winter DA, Prince F, Frank JS, Powell C, Zabjek KF. Unified theory regarding A/P and M/L balance in quiet stance. J Neurophysiol. (1996) 75:2334–43. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.6.2334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Caron O, Faure B, Brenière Y. Estimating the centre of gravity of the body on the basis of the centre of pressure in standing posture. J Biomech. (1997) 30:1169–71. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(97)00094-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Morasso PG, Schieppati M. Can muscle stiffness alone stabilize upright standing? J Neurophysiol. (1999) 82:1622–6. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bouisset S, Do MC. Posture, dynamic stability, and voluntary movement. Neurophysiol Clin. (2008) 38:345–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2008.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sakanaka TE, Lakie M, Reynolds RF. Sway-dependent changes in standing ankle stiffness caused by muscle thixotropy. J Physiol. (2016) 594:781–93. doi: 10.1113/JP271137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Amiri P, Kearney RE. Ankle intrinsic stiffness changes with postural sway. J Biomech. (2019) 85:50–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2019.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ivanenko Y, Gurfinkel vs. human postural control. Front Neurosci. (2018) 12:171. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Morasso P. Centre of pressure vs. centre of mass stabilization strategies: the tightrope balancing case. R Soc Open Sci. (2020) 7:200111. doi: 10.1098/rsos.200111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Loram ID, Maganaris CN, Lakie M. Human postural sway results from frequent, ballistic bias impulses by soleus and gastrocnemius. J Physiol. (2005) 564:295–311. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.076307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Masani K, Vette AH, Kawashima N, Popovic MR. Neuromusculoskeletal torque-generation process has a large destabilizing effect on the control mechanism of quiet standing. J Neurophysiol. (2008) 100:1465–75. doi: 10.1152/jn.00801.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Amiri P, Kearney RE. Identification of central and stretch reflex contributions to human postural control. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. (2021) 29:497–507. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2021.3057785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Schieppati M, Hugon M, Grasso M, Nardone A, Galante M. The limits of equilibrium in young and elderly normal subjects and in parkinsonians. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1994) 93:286–98. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(94)90031-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Simoneau M, Corbeil P. The effect of time to peak ankle torque on balance stability boundary: experimental validation of a biomechanical model. Exp Brain Res. (2005) 165:217–28. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2290-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Günther M, Grimmer S, Siebert T, Blickhan R. All leg joints contribute to quiet human stance: a mechanical analysis. J Biomech. (2009) 42:2739–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2009.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Koltermann JJ, Beck H, Beck M. Investigation of the correlation between factors influencing the spectrum of center of pressure measurements using dynamic controlled models of the upright stand and subject measurements. Appl Sci. (2020) 10:3741. doi: 10.3390/app10113741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Shiozawa K, Lee J, Russo M, Sternad D, Hogan N. Frequency-dependent force direction elucidates neural control of balance. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2021) 18:145. doi: 10.1186/s12984-021-00907-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Oddsson LI. Control of voluntary trunk movements in man. mechanisms for postural equilibrium during standing. Acta Physiol Scand Suppl. (1990) 595:1–60.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. Raymakers JA, Samson MM, Verhaar HJ. The assessment of body sway and the choice of the stability parameter(s). Gait Posture. (2005) 21:48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2003.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Visser JE, Carpenter MG, van der Kooij H, Bloem BR. The clinical utility of posturography. Clin Neurophysiol. (2008) 119:2424–36. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Wang Z, Newell KM. Inter-foot coordination dynamics of quiet standing postures. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 47:194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schut IM, Pasma JH, Roelofs JMB, Weerdesteyn V, van der Kooij H, Schouten AC, et al. Estimating ankle torque and dynamics of the stabilizing mechanism: No need for horizontal ground reaction forces. J Biomech. (2020) 106:109813. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2020.109813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sozzi S, Ghai S, Schieppati M. Incongruity of Geometric and Spectral Markers in the Assessment of Body Sway. Front Neurol. (2022) 13:929132. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.929132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Berme N, Oggero E, Pagnacco G. Power spectrum characteristics of physiologic and pathologic tremor. Acta Bioeng Biomech. (1999) 1:71–88.

Google Scholar

30. Pagnacco G, Heiss DG, Oggero E. Muscular contractions and their effect on the vertical ground reaction force during quiet stance—part II: mathematical model. Biomed Sci Instrum. (2001) 37:233–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

31. Minamisawa T, Sawahata H, Takakura K, Yamaguchi T. Characteristics of temporal fluctuation of the vertical ground reaction force during quiet stance in Parkinson's disease. Gait Posture. (2012) 35:308–11. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.09.106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Singh NB, Taylor WR, Madigan ML, Nussbaum MA. The spectral content of postural sway during quiet stance: influences of age, vision and somatosensory inputs. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. (2012) 22:131–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2011.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Oggero E, Carrick FR, Pagnacco G. Frequency content of standard posturographic measures—biomed 2013. Biomed Sci Instrum. (2013) 49:48–53.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Panzer VP, Bandinelli S, Hallett M. Biomechanical assessment of quiet standing and changes associated with aging. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (1995) 76:151–7. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(95)80024-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Redfern MS, Moore PL, Yarsky CM. The influence of flooring on standing balance among older persons. Hum Factors. (1997) 39:445–55. doi: 10.1518/001872097778827043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Tanaka H, Uetake T. Characteristics of postural sway in older adults standing on a soft surface. J Hum Ergol (Tokyo). (2005) 34:35–40. doi: 10.11183/jhe1972.34.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Fransson PA, Gomez S, Patel M, Johansson L. Changes in multi-segmented body movements and EMG activity while standing on hard and foam support surfaces. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2007) 101:81–9. doi: 10.1007/s00421-007-0476-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Patel M, Fransson PA, Lush D, Gomez S. The effect of foam surface properties on postural stability assessment while standing. Gait Posture. (2008) 28:649–56. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Pagnacco G, Carrick FR, Pascolo PB, Rossi R, Oggero E. Learning effect of standing on foam during posturographic testing preliminary findings. Biomed Sci Instrum. (2012) 48:332–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

40. Strang AJ, Haworth J, Hieronymus M, Walsh M, Smart LJ. Structural changes in postural sway lend insight into effects of balance training, vision, and support surface on postural control in a healthy population. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2011) 111:1485–95. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1770-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Houdijk H, Brown SE, van Dieën JH. Relation between postural sway magnitude and metabolic energy cost during upright standing on a compliant surface. J Appl Physiol. (2015) 119:696–703. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00907.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zhao Y, Kearney RE. Closed-loop system identification of ankle dynamics with compliant loads. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. (2007) 2007:4919–22. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2007.4353443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Günther M, Wagner H. Dynamics of quiet human stance: computer simulations of a triple inverted pendulum model. Comput Methods Biomech Biomed Eng. (2016) 19:819–34. doi: 10.1080/10255842.2015.1067306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Schut IM, Engelhart D, Pasma JH, Aarts RGKM, Schouten AC. Compliant support surfaces affect sensory reweighting during balance control. Gait Posture. (2017) 53:241–7. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lin CC, Roche JL, Steed DP, Musolino MC, Marchetti GF, Furman GR, et al. Test-retest reliability of postural stability on two different foam pads. J Nat Sci. (2015) 1:e43.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

46. Hufschmidt A, Dichgans J, Mauritz KH, Hufschmidt M. Some methods and parameters of body sway quantification and their neurological applications. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr. (1980) 228:135–50. doi: 10.1007/BF00365601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bonnet CT. Advantages and disadvantages of stiffness instructions when studying postural control. Gait Posture. (2016) 46:208–10. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lee IC, Pacheco MM, Newell KM. The precision demands of viewing distance modulate postural coordination and control. Hum Mov Sci. (2019) 66:425–39. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2019.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ruhe A, Fejer R, Walker B. Center of pressure excursion as a measure of balance performance in patients with non-specific low back pain compared to healthy controls: a systematic review of the literature. Eur Spine J. (2011) 20:358–68. doi: 10.1007/s00586-010-1543-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Schubert P, Kirchner M. Ellipse area calculations and their applicability in posturography. Gait Posture. (2014) 39:518–22. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cappozzo A, Leo T, Pedotti AA. general computing method for the analysis of human locomotion. J Biomech. (1975) 8:307–20. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(75)90083-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Pedotti A. Simple equipment used in clinical practice for evaluation of locomotion. IEEE Transact Biomed Eng BME. (1977) 24:456–61. doi: 10.1109/TBME.1977.326186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gruben KG, Boehm WL. Ankle torque control that shifts the center of pressure from heel to toe contributes non-zero sagittal plane angular momentum during human walking. J Biomech. (2014) 47:1389–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2014.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Crenna P, Frigo C. Monitoring gait by a vector diagram technique in spastic patients. In: Delwaide PJ, Young RR, editors Clinical Neurophysiology in Spasticity. (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers) (1985), p. 109–24.

Google Scholar

56. Panzer VP, Hallett M. Biomechanical assessment of upright stance in Parkinson's disease: a single-subject study. Clin Biomech. (1990) 5:73–80. doi: 10.1016/0268-0033(90)90041-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Prieto TE, Myklebust JB, Hoffmann RG, Lovett EG, Myklebust BM. Measures of postural steadiness: differences between healthy young and elderly adults. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. (1996) 43:956–66. doi: 10.1109/10.532130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Karlsson A, Frykberg G. Correlations between force plate measures for assessment of balance. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). (2000) 15:365–9. doi: 10.1016/S0268-0033(99)00096-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Önell A. The vertical ground reaction force for analysis of balance? Gait Posture. (2000) 12:7–13. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00053-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bleuse S, Cassim F, Blatt JL, Defebvre L, Derambure P, Guieu JD, et al. Vertical torque allows recording of anticipatory postural adjustments associated with slow, arm-raising movements. Clin Biomech. (2005) 20:693–99. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2005.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Creath R, Kiemel T, Horak F, Peterka R, Jeka JA. unified view of quiet and perturbed stance: simultaneous co-existing excitable modes. Neurosci Lett. (2005) 377:75–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.11.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Di Giulio I, Baltzopoulos V, Maganaris CN, Loram ID. Human standing: does the control strategy preprogram a rigid knee? J Appl Physiol. (2013) 114:1717–29. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01299.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Tanabe H, Fujii K, Kouzaki M. Intermittent muscle activity in the feedback loop of postural control system during natural quiet standing. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:10631. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-10015-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Paulus WM, Straube A, Brandt T. Visual stabilization of posture. physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. Brain. (1984) 107:1143–63. doi: 10.1093/brain/107.4.1143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Kollegger H, Wöber C, Baumgartner C, Deecke L. Stabilizing and destabilizing effects of vision and foot position on body sway of healthy young subjects: a posturographic study. Eur Neurol. (1989) 29:241–5. doi: 10.1159/000116420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Schieppati M, Tacchini E, Nardone A, Tarantola J, Corna S. Subjective perception of body sway. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1999) 66:313–22. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.66.3.313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lord SR, Menz HB. Visual contributions to postural stability in older adults. Gerontology. (2000) 46:306–10. doi: 10.1159/000022182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Šarabon N, Rosker J, Loefler S, Kern H. The effect of vision elimination during quiet stance tasks with different feet positions. Gait Posture. (2013) 38:708–11. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. De Nunzio AM, Schieppati M. Time to reconfigure balancing behavior in man: changing visual condition while riding a continuously moving platform. Exp Brain Res. (2007) 178:18–36. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0708-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Sozzi S, Nardone A, Schieppati M. Calibration of the Leg Muscle Responses Elicited by Predictable Perturbations of Stance and the Effect of Vision. Front Hum Neurosci. (2016) 10:419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ivanenko YP, Levik YS, Talis VL, Gurfinkel VS. Human equilibrium on unstable support: the importance of feet-support interaction. Neurosci Lett. (1997) 235:109–12. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00721-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Jacobs JV, Wu G, Kelly KM. Evidence for beta corticomuscular coherence during human standing balance: effects of stance width, vision, and support surface. Neuroscience. (2015) 298:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Welgampola MS, Colebatch JG. Vestibulospinal reflexes: quantitative effects of sensory feedback and postural task. Exp Brain Res. (2001) 39:345–53. doi: 10.1007/s002210100754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Day BL, Guerraz M. Feedforward vs. feedback modulation of human vestibular-evoked balance responses by visual self-motion information. J Physiol. (2007) 582:153–61. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.132092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Wallace JW, Rasman BG, Dalton BH. Vestibular-evoked responses indicate a functional role for intrinsic foot muscles during standing balance. Neuroscience. (2018) 377:150–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.02.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Schmid M, Casabianca L, Bottaro A, Schieppati M. Graded changes in balancing behavior as a function of visual acuity. Neuroscience. (2008) 153:1079–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Minamisawa T, Chiba N, Inoue K, Nakanowatari T, Suzuki E. Analysis of vertical micro acceleration while standing reveals age-related changes. Geriatrics (Basel). (2020) 5:105. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics5040105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Matsumoto Y, Griffin MJ. Dynamic response of the standing human body exposed to vertical vibration: influence of posture and vibration magnitude. J Sound Vibrat. (1998) 212:85–107. doi: 10.1006/jsvi.1997.1376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Kasagi Y, Shimoyama I, Yoshida S, Nakazawa K, Fukutake T. Effect of aging on the vibration of human body in the vertical direction during quiet stance. Changes. (2009) 3:10–2.

Google Scholar

80. Brenière Y. Why we walk the way we do. J Mot Behav. (1996) 28:291–8. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1996.10544598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Brownjohn JMW, Zheng X. Discussion of human resonant frequency. Proc SPIE - Int Soc Opt Eng. (2001) 4317:469–74. doi: 10.1117/12.429621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Mandal BB, Srivastava AK. Risk from vibration in Indian mines. Indian J Occupation Environ Med. (2006) 10:2. doi: 10.4103/0019-5278.27460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Matsumoto Y, Griffin MJ. Comparison of biodynamic responses in standing and seated human bodies. J Sound Vibrat. (2000) 238:691–704. doi: 10.1006/jsvi.2000.3133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Nardone A, Schieppati M. Postural adjustments associated with voluntary contraction of leg muscles in standing man. Exp Brain Res. (1988) 69:469–80. doi: 10.1007/BF00247301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Bottaro A, Casadio M, Morasso PG, Sanguineti V. Body sway during quiet standing: is it the residual chattering of an intermittent stabilization process? Hum Mov Sci. (2005) 24:588–615. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Bottaro A, Yasutake Y, Nomura T, Casadio M, Morasso P. Bounded stability of the quiet standing posture: an intermittent control model. Hum Mov Sci. (2008) 27:473–95. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Reynolds RE. Vertical torque responses to vestibular stimulation in standing humans. J Physiol. (2011) 589:3943–53. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.209163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Enoka RM, Duchateau J. Rate coding and the control of muscle force. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. (2017) 7:a029702. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a029702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Nawayseh N, Hamdan S. Apparent mass of the standing human body when using a whole-body vibration training machine: effect of knee angle and input frequency. J Biomech. (2019) 82:291–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Donath L, Kurz E, Roth R, Zahner L, Faude O. Leg and trunk muscle coordination and postural sway during increasingly difficult standing balance tasks in young and older adults. Maturitas. (2016) 91:60–8. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2016.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Vette AH, Sayenko DG, Jones M, Abe MO, Nakazawa K, Masani K, et al. Ankle muscle co- contractions during quiet standing are associated with decreased postural steadiness in the elderly. Gait Posture. (2017) 55:31–6. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Yamagata M, Gruben K, Falaki A, Ochs WL, Latash ML. Biomechanics of vertical posture and control with referent joint configurations. J Mot Behav. (2021) 53:72–82. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2020.1723483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Warnica MJ, Weaver TB, Prentice SD, Laing AC. The influence of ankle muscle activation on postural sway during quiet stance. Gait Posture. (2014) 39:1115–21. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Yamagata M, Falaki A, Latash ML. Effects of voluntary agonist-antagonist coactivation on stability of vertical posture. Motor Control. (2019) 23:304–26. doi: 10.1123/mc.2018-0038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kanekar N, Lee YJ, Aruin AS. Frequency analysis approach to study balance control in individuals with multiple sclerosis. J Neurosci Methods. (2014) 222:91–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Winter DA, Patla AE, Prince F, Ishac M, Gielo-Perczak K. Stiffness control of balance in quiet standing. J Neurophysiol. (1998) 80:1211–21. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Salsabili H, Bahrpeyma F, Esteki A, Karimzadeh M, Ghomashchi H. Spectral characteristics of postural sway in diabetic neuropathy patients participating in balance training. J Diabetes Metab Disord. (2013) 12:29. doi: 10.1186/2251-6581-12-29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Boehm WL, Nichols KM, Gruben KG. Frequency-dependent contributions of sagittal-plane foot force to upright human standing. J Biomech. (2019) 83:305–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Schweigart G, Mergner T. Human stance control beyond steady state response and inverted pendulum simplification. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 185:635–53. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1189-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Hsu WL, Scholz JP, Schöner G, Jeka JJ, Kiemel T. Control and estimation of posture during quiet stance depends on multijoint coordination. J Neurophysiol. (2007) 97:3024–35. doi: 10.1152/jn.01142.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Dutt-Mazumder A, Gruben KG. Modulation of sagittal-plane center of pressure and force vector direction in human standing on sloped surfaces. J Biomech. (2021) 119:110288. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2021.110288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Turcato AM, Godi M, Giordano A, Schieppati M, Nardone A. The generation of centripetal force when walking in a circle: insight from the distribution of ground reaction forces recorded by plantar insoles. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2015) 12:4. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-12-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Krafczyk S, Schlamp V, Dieterich M, Haberhauer P, Brandt T. Increased body sway at 3.5–8 Hz in patients with phobic postural vertigo. Neurosci Lett. (1999) 259:149–52. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00917-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Haibach PS, Slobounov SM, Slobounova ES, Newell KM. Virtual time-to-contact of postural stability boundaries as a function of support surface compliance. Exp Brain Res. (2007) 177:471–82. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0703-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Shepard NT. The clinical use of dynamic posturography in the elderly. Ear Nose Throat J. (1989) 68:940–3.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

106. Di Fabio RP, Badke MB, McEvoy A, Breunig A. Influence of local sensory afference in the calibration of human balance responses. Exp Brain Res. (1990) 80:591–9. doi: 10.1007/BF00227998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Yardley L, Lerwill H, Hall M, Gresty M. Visual destabilization of posture in normal subjects. Acta Otolaryngol. (1992) 112:14–21. doi: 10.3109/00016489209100777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Krizková M, Hlavačka F. Binaural monopolar galvanic vestibular stimulation reduces body sway during human stance. Physiol Res. (1994) 43:187–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

109. Lord SR, Ward JA. Age-associated differences in sensori-motor function and balance in community dwelling women. Age Aging. (1994) 23:452–60. doi: 10.1093/ageing/23.6.452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Gerber ED, Nichols P, Giraldo C, Sidener L, Huang CK, Luchies CW, et al. Rambling-trembling center-of-pressure decomposition reveals distinct sway responses to simulated somatosensory deficit. Gait Posture. (2022) 91:276–83. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2021.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Baloh RW, Jacobson KM, Beykirch K, Honrubia V. Static and dynamic posturography in patients with vestibular and cerebellar lesions. Arch Neurol. (1998) 55:649–54. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.5.649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Teasdale N, Stelmach GE, Breunig A. Postural sway characteristics of the elderly under normal and altered visual and support surface conditions. J Gerontol. (1991) 46:B238–244. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.6.B238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Matthews PBC. Mammalian Muscle Receptors and Their Central Actions. London: Edward Arnold (1972).

Google Scholar

114. Banks RW. The innervation of the muscle spindle: a personal history. J Anat. (2015) 227:115–35. doi: 10.1111/joa.12297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Burke D, Gandevia SC, McKeon B. The afferent volleys responsible for spinal proprioceptive reflexes in man. J Physiol. (1983) 339:535–52. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Schieppati M, Nardone A, Siliotto R, Grasso M. Early and late stretch responses of human foot muscles induced by perturbation of stance. Exp Brain Res. (1995) 105:411–22. doi: 10.1007/BF00233041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Corna S, Galante M, Grasso M, Nardone A, Schieppati M. Unilateral displacement of lower limb evokes bilateral EMG responses in leg and foot muscles in standing humans. Exp Brain Res. (1996) 109:83–91. doi: 10.1007/BF00228629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Clair JM, Okuma Y, Misiaszek JE, Collins DF. Reflex pathways connect receptors in the human lower leg to the erector spinae muscles of the lower back. Exp Brain Res. (2009) 196:217–27. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1849-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Ganchev GN, Yankov E. Somatosensory evoked potential of the stretch-reflex (T-reflex). Acta Physiol Pharmacol Bulg. (1979) 5:63–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

120. Cohen L, Starr A. About the origin of cerebral somatosensory potentials evoked by Achilles tendon taps in humans. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1985) 62:108–16. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(85)90022-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Mildren RL, Peters RM, Hill AJ, Blouin JS, Carpenter MG, Inglis JT, et al. Frequency characteristics of human muscle and cortical responses evoked by noisy Achilles tendon vibration. J Appl Physiol. (2017) 122:1134–44. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00908.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Banks RW, Ellaway PH, Prochazka A, Proske U. Secondary endings of muscle spindles: Structure, reflex action, role in motor control and proprioception. Exp Physiol. (2021) 106:2339–66. doi: 10.1113/EP089826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Chiang JH, Wu G. The influence of foam surfaces on biomechanical variables contributing to postural control. Gait Post. (1997) 5:239–45. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(96)01091-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Schieppati M, Nardone A. Group II spindle afferent fibers in humans: their possible role in the reflex control of stance. Prog Brain Res. (1999) 123:461–72. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62882-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Marchand-Pauvert V, Nicolas G, Marque P, Iglesias C, Pierrot-Deseilligny E. Increase in group II excitation from ankle muscles to thigh motoneurones during human standing. J Physiol. (2005) 566:257–71. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.087817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Windhorst U. Muscle proprioceptive feedback and spinal networks. Brain Res Bull. (2007) 73:155–202. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Windhorst U. Muscle spindles are multi-functional. Brain Res Bull. (2008) 75:507–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Di Giulio I, Maganaris CN, Baltzopoulos V, Loram ID. The proprioceptive and agonist roles of gastrocnemius, soleus and tibialis anterior muscles in maintaining human upright posture. J Physiol. (2009) 587:2399–416. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.168690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Mademli L, Mavridi D, Bohm S, Patikas DA, Santuz A, Arampatzis A, et al. Standing on unstable surface challenges postural control of tracking tasks and modulates neuromuscular adjustments specific to task complexity. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:6122. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84899-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Forestier N, Terrier R, Teasdale N. Ankle muscular proprioceptive signals' relevance for balance control on various support surfaces: an exploratory study. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. (2015) 94:20–7. doi: 10.1097/PHM.0000000000000137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Huang M, Yick KL, Ng SP, Yip J, Cheung RT. The effect of support surface and footwear condition on postural sway and lower limb muscle action of the older women. PLoS One. (2020) 15:e0234140. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0234140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Danna-Dos-Santos A, Degani AM, Boonstra TW, Mochizuki L, Harney AM, Schmeckpeper MM, et al. The influence of visual information on multi-muscle control during quiet stance: a spectral analysis approach. Exp Brain Res. (2015) 233:657–69. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4145-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Ridge ST, Rowley KM, Kurihara T, McClung M, Tang J, Reischl S, et al. Contributions of intrinsic and extrinsic foot muscles during functional standing postures. Biomed Res Int. (2022) 5:7708077. doi: 10.1155/2022/7708077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Allum JH, Pfaltz CR. Visual and vestibular contributions to pitch sway stabilization in the ankle muscles of normals and patients with bilateral peripheral vestibular deficits. Exp Brain Res. (1985) 58:82–94. doi: 10.1007/BF00238956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Masani K, Popovic MR, Nakazawa K, Kouzaki M, Nozaki D. Importance of body sway velocity information in controlling ankle extensor activities during quiet stance. J Neurophysiol. (2003) 90:3774–82. doi: 10.1152/jn.00730.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Varghese JP, Beyer KB, Williams L. Miyasike-daSilva V, McIlroy WE. Standing still: is there a role for the cortex? Neurosci Lett. (2015) 590:18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.01.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Teixeira LA, Maia Azzi N, de Oliveira JÁ, Ribeiro D. Automatic postural responses are scaled from the association between online feedback and feedforward control. Eur J Neurosci. (2020) 51:2023–32. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Patel M, Fransson PA, Johansson R, Magnusson M. Foam posturography: standing on foam is not equivalent to standing with decreased rapidly adapting mechanoreceptive sensation. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 208:519–27. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2498-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Thompson C, Bélanger M, Fung J. Effects of plantar cutaneo-muscular and tendon vibration on posture and balance during quiet and perturbed stance. Hum Mov Sci. (2011) 30:153–71. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2010.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Preszner-Domjan A, Nagy E, Szíver E, Feher-Kiss A, Horvath G, Kranicz J, et al. When does mechanical plantar stimulation promote sensory re-weighing: standing on a firm or compliant surface? Eur J Appl Physiol. (2012) 112:2979–87. doi: 10.1007/s00421-011-2277-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Peters RM, McKeown MD, Carpenter MG, Inglis JT. Losing touch: age-related changes in plantar skin sensitivity, lower limb cutaneous reflex strength, and postural stability in older adults. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 116:1848–58. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Felicetti G, Thoumie P, Do MC, Schieppati M. Cutaneous and muscular afferents from the foot and sensory fusion processing: Physiology and pathology in neuropathies. J Peripher Nerv Syst. (2021) 26:17–34. doi: 10.1111/jns.12429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Fabre M, Antoine M, Robitaille MG, Ribot-Ciscar E, Ackerley R, Aimonetti JM, et al. Large postural sways prevent foot tactile information from fading: neurophysiological evidence. Cereb Cortex Commun. (2020) 2:94. doi: 10.1093/texcom/tgaa094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Viseux FJF. The sensory role of the sole of the foot: Review and update on clinical perspectives. Neurophysiol Clin. (2020) 50:55–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2019.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Mengarelli A, Cardarelli S, Fioretti S, Burattini L, Nardo FD, Verdini F, et al. Center of pressure based assessment of balance responses to repeated perturbations of upright stance. In: EMBEC NBC (Singapore: Springer), p. 262–5.

Google Scholar

146. Horlings CG, Küng UM, Honegger F, Van Engelen BG, Van Alfen N, Bloem BR, et al. Vestibular and proprioceptive influences on trunk movements during quiet standing. Neuroscience. (2009) 161:904–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Kiemel T, Zhang Y, Jeka JJ. Identification of neural feedback for upright stance in humans: stabilization rather than sway minimization. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:15144–53. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1013-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Sozzi S, Honeine JL, Do MC, Schieppati M. Leg muscle activity during tandem stance and the control of body balance in the frontal plane. Clin Neurophysiol. (2013) 124:1175–86. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Kelly LA, Kuitunen S, Racinais S, Cresswell AG. Recruitment of the plantar intrinsic foot muscles with increasing postural demand. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). (2012) 27:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2011.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Nishiwaki Y, Takebayashi T, Imai A, Yamamoto M, Omae K. Difference by instructional set in stabilometry. J Vestibul Res. (2000) 10:157–61. doi: 10.3233/VES-2000-10305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Sakanaka TE, Lakie M, Reynolds RF. Individual differences in intrinsic ankle stiffness and their relationship to body sway and ankle torque. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0244993. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Goodworth AD, Mellodge P, Peterka RJ. Stance width changes how sensory feedback is used for multisegmental balance control. J Neurophysiol. (2014) 112:525–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.00490.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Gosselin G, Fagan M. Foam pads properties and their effects on posturography in participants of different weight. Chiropr Man Therap. (2015) 23:2. doi: 10.1186/s12998-014-0045-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Stürm R, Nigg B, Koller EA. The impact of cardiac activity on triaxially recorded endogenous microvibrations of the body. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. (1980) 44:83–96. doi: 10.1007/BF00421767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Pagnacco G, Heiss DG, Oggero E. Muscular contractions and their effect on the vertical ground reaction force during quiet stance–Part I: hypothesis and experimental investigation. Biomed Sci Instrum. (2001) 37:227–32.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

156. Gurfinkel VS, Kots YM, Paltsev EI, Feldman AG. The com- pensation of respiratory disturbances of erect posture of man as an example of the organization of interarticular interaction. In: Gelfand IM, Gurfinkel VS, Formin SV, Tsetlin ML, editors, Models of the Structural Functional Organization of Certain Biological Systems (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press) (1971), p. 382–395.

Google Scholar

157. Gatev P, Thomas S, Kepple T, Hallett M. Feedforward ankle strategy of balance during quiet stance in adults. J Physiol. (1999) 514:915–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.915ad.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Santos MJ, Kanekar N, Aruin AS. The role of anticipatory postural adjustments in compensatory control of posture: Biomechanical analysis. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. (2010) 20:398–405. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Peterka RJ. Sensory integration for human balance control. Handb Clin Neurol. (2018) 159:27–42. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63916-5.00002-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Papegaaij S, Taube W, Baudry S, Otten E, Hortobágyi T. Aging causes a reorganization of cortical and spinal control of posture. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:28. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Zemková E, Hamar D. Physiological mechanisms of post-exercise balance impairment. Sports Med. (2014) 44:437–48. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0129-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Fujimoto C, Suzuki S, Kinoshita M, Egami N, Sugasawa K, Iwasaki S, et al. Clinical features of otolith organ-specific vestibular dysfunction. Clin Neurophysiol. (2018) 29:238–45. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Strupp M, Feil K, Dieterich M, Brandt T. Bilateral vestibulopathy. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 137:235–40. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63437-5.00017-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Kerr G, Morrison S, Silburn P. Coupling between limb tremor and postural sway in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:386–94. doi: 10.1002/mds.21851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Kunkel M, Freudenthaler N, Steinhoff BJ, Baudewig J, Paulus W. Spatial-frequency-related efficacy of visual stabilization of posture. Exp Brain Res. (1998) 121:471–7. doi: 10.1007/s002210050483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: vertical ground reaction force (VGRF), frequency spectra analysis, vision, support surface, body sway

Citation: Sozzi S, Do M-C and Schieppati M (2022) Vertical ground reaction force oscillation during standing on hard and compliant surfaces: The “postural rhythm”. Front. Neurol. 13:975752. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.975752

Received: 23 June 2022; Accepted: 10 August 2022;
Published: 01 September 2022.

Edited by:

Ramona Ritzmann, Clinic Rennbahn AG, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Kreg Gruben, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
Elizabeth Diane Thompson, University of Delaware, United States

Copyright © 2022 Sozzi, Do and Schieppati. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marco Schieppati, marco.schieppati@icsmaugeri.it; marco.schieppati@icloud.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.