Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Neurol., 14 June 2022
Sec. Stroke

Left Hemisphere Bias of NIH Stroke Scale Is Most Severe for Middle Cerebral Artery Strokes

  • 1Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 2Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 3Department of Physical Medicine, Rehabilitation, and Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 4Department of Radiology, School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States

NIHSS score is higher for left vs. right hemisphere strokes of equal volumes. However, differences in each vascular territory have not been evaluated yet. We hypothesized that left vs. right differences are driven by the middle cerebral artery (MCA) territory, and there is no difference between hemispheres for other vascular territories. This study is based on data from 802 patients with evidence of acute ischemic stroke in one major arterial territory (MCA, n = 437; PCA, n = 209; ACA, n = 21; vertebrobasilar, n = 46). We examined differences in patients with left or right strokes regarding to lesion volume, NIHSS, and other covariates (age, sex, race). We used linear models to test the effects of these covariates on NIHSS. We looked at the whole sample as well as in the sample stratified by NIHSS (≤5 or >5) and by lesion location (MCA or PCA). Patients with left MCA strokes had significantly higher NIHSS than those with right strokes. Only patients with MCA strokes showed NIHSS score affected by the hemisphere when controlling for stroke volume and patient's age. This difference was driven by the more severe strokes (NIHSS>5). It is important to consider this systematic bias in the NIHSS when using the score for inclusion criteria for treatment or trials. Patients with right MCA stroke may be under-treated and left with disabling deficits that are not captured by the NIHSS.

1. Introduction

The National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS) is a valid and reliable tool most frequently used for clinically evaluating acute stroke (14). The NIHSS is associated with severity, long-term functional outcomes (57), infarct size, lesion location (1), and angiographic findings (810). Scoring the NIHSS consists of broad categories associated with stroke signs and symptoms (e.g., level of consciousness, motor performance, language, speech, neglect, etc.). Distinct clinical features or stroke syndromes can be appreciated depending on the specific vascular territory affected [i.e., anterior cerebral artery (ACA), posterior cerebral artery (PCA), middle cerebral artery (MCA)].

The NIHSS is designed to represent left and right cortical and motor function equally; however, there are more opportunities to award points for left hemisphere dysfunction than right (8, 11, 12). This is likely because up to 7 points are directly related to language deficits, and these deficits typically are associated with left MCA stroke only, particularly among right-handed people. Points attributable to left MCA cortical strokes are awarded across three categories: (1) orientation questions, (2) following commands, and (3) specific language tasks to determine signs of aphasia (e.g., picture description, confrontation naming, sentence reading), in addition to sensory and motor. In contrast, points attributable to right MCA cortical stroke are awarded in only one category: (1) neglect, (2) other than sensory and motor (Supplementary Table 1). Thus, even if the stroke volume is equal, NIHSS scores are often higher for left vs. right hemisphere stroke (8, 11). This bias may partially account for findings that stroke patients with right-hemisphere infarcts are 45% less likely to be treated with thrombolysis than patients with left-hemisphere infarcts (13).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and lesion characteristics.

Differences in NIHSS relative to specific vascular territories have not yet been evaluated. Deficits associated with bilateral PCA and ACA stroke are likely symmetrically represented in the NIHSS scale, because language is largely specific to the left MCA territory vs. right. However, left ACA and PCA stroke can cause aphasia [e.g., transcortical motor aphasia, optic aphasia; see (14)]. Hemispatial neglect can be caused by ACA, MCA, or PCA stroke. While neglect is more noticeable after right hemisphere stroke, right neglect after left hemisphere stroke may be almost as common (15). Thus, there are clear reasons to suspect that hemispheric bias in the NIHSS may be specific to MCA territory strokes. We hypothesized that right MCA strokes have larger infarct volumes than left MCA ischemic strokes, in groups with similar NIHSS, but there will be no difference between hemispheres for other vascular territories. Similarly, we hypothesized that after controlling for stroke volume and other covariates, the side of MCA infarcts, but not of infarcts in other territories, significantly affects the NIHSS.

2. Methods

This study included MRIs of patients admitted to the Comprehensive Stroke Center at Johns Hopkins Hospital with the clinical diagnosis of ischemic stroke, between 2009 and 2019 (Flowchart for data inclusion in Figure 1). It utilizes data from an anonymized dataset, created under waiver of informed consent (IRB00228775). We have complied with all relevant ethical regulations and the guidelines of the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board, that approved the present study (IRB00290649).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of data inclusion.

From the 2,888 DWIs quality-controlled for clinical analysis, 1,849 DWIs showed lesions classified by a neuroradiologist as result of acute or early subacute ischemic stroke, with no evidence of hemorrhage. From those, we included 802 individuals who had NIHSS recorded at admission, in the same occasion as the MRI, and at least 90% of the infarct lesion constrained to a single vascular territory (MCA, PCA, ACA, vertebrobasilar). Note that the stringent inclusion criteria might lead to unascertained (although inevitable) bias in the analysis. The present study focuses on the largest groups of lesions, affecting the MCA (n = 498) and PCA (n = 237) territories. The summary of demographics and lesion profiles is in Table 1. The lesion core was defined in DWI, in combination with the Apparent Diffusion Coefficient maps (ADC) by two experienced evaluators and was revised by a neuroradiologist until reaching a final decision by consensus. Further details are in our previous publication (16).

In an initial exploratory analysis, we examined differences in groups of patients with left or right stroke regarding to lesion volume, NIHSS and other covariates (age, sex, race). We used t-test for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables. We then used generalized linear models to test the effects of lesion side, volume, patient's age, sex, and race on NIHSS. We used Akaike information criterion (AIC) to evaluate the impact of covariates or of their interactions in the models. We looked at the whole sample as well as in the sample stratified by lesion location (MCA or PCA) and by NIHSS [≤5 or >5; as NIHSS ≤ 5, considered “mild stroke”, are most strongly associated with good prognosis and short hospital discharge to home (17, 18)]. We additionally stratified the MCA strokes by vascular “subdivision” (superior: lesions with more than 75% of volume in the frontal areas vs. inferior: lesions with more than 75% of volume in the temporo-parietal areas), by possible etiology (atherosclerotic or cardiogenic thrombotic), and tested the effect of ASPECTS (19) in the general linear models, in place of the volumes. The statistical analysis was performed with R.

3. Results

As shown in Table 1, there were no significant differences between left and right strokes in terms of patient's age, sex, and race. The distribution of these variables was similar in left and right strokes. Patients with left MCA strokes had significantly higher NIHSS than those with right MCA strokes. This difference was not significant in patients with PCA strokes. This left hemisphere bias is illustrated in Figure 2. In addition, right MCA strokes tended to be larger than the right counterparts (although not significantly at p < 0.05). This tendency persisted after stratification for NIHSS (≤5 or >5) as shown in Supplementary Table 2, stroke etiology (atherosclerotic or cardioembolic), and in strokes affecting the inferior MCA area, but not the superior (Supplementary Table 4). Note however, that the sample size of superior MCA strokes and cardioembolic strokes is small, and these results must be interpreted with caution.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustrative cases of patients with right (A,C) and left (B,D) strokes, with similar NIHSS and very different infarct volumes (A,B); or similar infarct volumes and very different NIHSS (C,D). (A) 76 year-old man, NIHSS = 4, infarct volume of 11.9 cc; (B) 84 year-old man, NIHSS = 5, infarct volume of 2.2 cc; (C) 34 year-old man, NIHSS = 6, infarct volume of 39 cc; (D) 35 year-old man, NIHSS=10, infarct volume of 38 cc.

The initial linear models to assess the effects of covariates in NIHSS included stroke side, volume, patient's age, sex, race, and interactions between stroke side and volume. As the effect of race was not significant in any model, race was further excluded from the analysis. Patient's sex and interactions between stroke side and volume were marginally associated to NIHSS (p-value for sex = 0.034; p-value for the interaction between side and volume = 0.03), only when considering the whole sample. The models with or without these covariates were equivalent (both showed AIC = 4,753). Therefore, sex and interaction between stroke volume and side were also excluded from further models. In summary, the final models used age, lesion side, and volume as predictors. They revealed that in patients with MCA strokes, and not in those with PCA strokes, NIHSS score is affected by the infarct side (p-value for infarct side = 0.00491) even after controlling for stroke volume and patient's age, as shown Table 2 and Supplementary Table 3. This effect was driven by the more severe strokes (NIHSS>5). In addition, stroke volume and patient's age significantly correlated with NIHSS.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. P-values for the generalized linear models and covariates of the models to predict the NIHSS.

Within the MCA strokes, the models controlled for age and stroke volumes showed that NIHSS score is affected by the infarct side in inferior MCA infarcts and in atherosclerotic strokes (Supplementary Table 4). However, we note again that sample size of superior MCA infarcts and cardioembolic strokes is small, limiting the power of this analysis. NIHSS score was still affected by the infarct side when ASPECTS—a metric of direct clinical relevance—was considered in the place of infarct volume (Supplementary Table 5). This is not surprising as ASPECTS and infarct volumes are highly correlate between themselves (r = 0.72, in our sample) and with NIHSS score (r = 0.59 and 0.55, respectively). As happened when we categorized by NHISS scores, this effect was driven by the most severe strokes (ASPECTS < 8).

4. Discussion

This study evaluated the association between NIHSS score and lesion volume by vascular territory, specifically the MCA and PCA territories. We confirmed our hypothesis that right MCA ischemic strokes are larger than left MCA ischemic strokes, especially for higher NIHSS scores. We also confirmed that the NIHSS significantly depends on the infarct side of MCA strokes, after correcting for lesion volume and other covariates. Although generally in agreement with previous studies (8, 11, 12), this study was substantially larger than previous studies (n = 153–312), and evaluated the bias for separate vascular territories. We also controlled for age and sex, as both variables may be differential associated with infarct volume.

Previous authors have explained the hemispheric bias as a reflection of greater points given for language deficits (typically left hemisphere symptom) than hemispatial neglect (typically thought of as right hemisphere symptom). Gottesman et al. found supplementing the NIHSS with more points for neglect (assessed with line cancellation and visual extinction) could correct the bias. This additive approach would require 2–3 min but could potentially correct the left hemisphere bias (12). This bias could be corrected in other ways (e.g., eliminating orientation and commands), but this approach would yield a less complete neurological exam. It is important to recognize that the NIHSS does not capture right cortical dysfunction. Other right, mostly MCA, cortical functions include empathy (20, 21), recognition and expression of affective prosody [tone of voice to convey emotions (22, 23), recognition of facial expression (24), awareness of deficits (25), integration of information (getting the “big picture”) (26, 27), understanding humor, and metaphor (28, 29), etc.]. However, these cortical functions are more difficult to assess reliably at bedside.

Limitations of the study include the fact that we do not have reliable information about handedness of the patients. NIHSS bias could potentially be modified in non-right-handed people, but probably does not systematically modify scores more in left hemisphere or right hemisphere stroke [as there are cases of both right stroke aphasia and left stroke severe neglect in left-handed patients (30)]. We also do not have reliable information about the time of symptoms onset for many patients, but most of the scans were 6 h after symptoms (in the patients with time of onset recorded) and therefore the likelihood of significant changes in the stroke volumes based on timing is small (31).

Despite the study's limitations, the findings may have implications for future research protocols and clinical practices that utilize the NIHSS. Some treatment protocols (e.g., involving endovascular therapy) have excluded patients with low NIHSS scores. As such, patients with large volume right hemisphere stroke and low NIHSS scores (e.g., right temporal strokes that spare motor functions) may be under-treated. These patients may be left with disabling deficits that substantially impede social function and human relationships, such as failure to empathize, understand emotional tone of voice or facial expression or humor (32). Likewise, use of a “diffusion-clinical mismatch” (3335) that uses an NIHSS score in comparison the volume of ischemia on DWI, has been advocated for thrombectomy up to 24 h post-onset of stroke. Because clinical deficits reflect volume of hypoperfusion more than volume of DWI infarct (3638), patients with right inferior division MCA strokes are likely not to meet the clinical criteria for this important intervention but are likely to have disabling sequelae. Thus, the NIHSS alone may not be optimal for determining the lower limits of stroke treatment eligibility, specifically for right MCA stroke patients.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are publicly available. This data can be found here: Zenodo, https://zenodo.org/, 10.5281/zenodo.5722286 (39).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Johns Hopkins Internal Review Board. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

EV collected the data and drafted the work. GK and AF collected and analyzed the data. MS significantly reviewed the draft. AH and AF conceived and designed the study, interpreted the data, and drafted the work. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported in part by the National Institute of Deaf and Communication Disorders, NIDCD, through R01 DC05375, R01 DC015466, and P50 DC014664 (AH, EV, MS, AF).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2022.912782/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Brott T, Adams HP Jr, Olinger CP, Marler JR, Barsan WG, Biller J, et al. Measurements of acute cerebral infarction: a clinical examination scale. Stroke. (1989) 20:864–70. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.20.7.864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Goldstein LB, Bertels C, Davis JN. Interrater reliability of the NIH stroke scale. Arch Neurol. (1989) 46:660–2. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1989.00520420080026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lyden P, Brott T, Tilley B, Welch KM, Mascha EJ, Levine S, et al. Improved reliability of the NIH stroke scale using video training. NINDS TPA stroke study group. Stroke. (1994) 25:2220–6. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.25.11.2220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kasner SE. Clinical interpretation and use of stroke scales. Lancet Neurol. (2006) 5:603–12. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70495-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Adams HP, Davis PH, Leira EC, Chang KC, Bendixen BH, Clarke WR, et al. Baseline NIH stroke scale score strongly predicts outcome after stroke: a report of the trial of org 10172 in acute stroke treatment (toast). Neurology. (1999) 53:126. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.1.126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Frankel MR, Morgenstern LB, Kwiatkowski T, Lu M, Tilley BC, Broderick JP, et al. Predicting prognosis after stroke: a placebo group analysis from the national institute of neurological disorders and stroke RT-PA stroke trial. Neurology. (2000) 55:952–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.7.952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Weimar C, Konig I, Kraywinkel K, Ziegler A, Diener H. Age and national institutes of health stroke scale score within 6 hours after onset are accurate predictors of outcome after cerebral ischemia: development and external validation of prognostic models. Stroke. (2004) 35:158–62. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000106761.94985.8B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Fink JN, Selim MH, Kumar S, Silver B, Linfante I, Caplan LR, et al. Is the association of national institutes of health stroke scale scores and acute magnetic resonance imaging stroke volume equal for patients with right-and left-hemisphere ischemic stroke? Stroke. (2002) 33:954–8. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000013069.24300.1D

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Nakajima M, Kimura K, Ogata T, Takada T, Uchino U, Minematsu K, et al. Relationships between angiographic findings and national institutes of health stroke scale score in cases of hyperacute carotid ischemic stroke. Am J Neuroradiol. (2004) 25:238–41. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.555664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fischer U, Arnold M, Nedeltchev K, Brekenfeld C, Ballinari P, Remonda L, et al. NIHSS score and arteriographic findings in acute ischemic stroke. Stroke. (2005) 36:2121–5. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000182099.04994.fc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Woo D, Broderick JP, Kothari RU, Lu M, Brott T, Lyden PD, et al. Does the national institutes of health stroke scale favor left hemisphere strokes? Stroke. (1999) 30:2355–9. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.30.11.2355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gottesman RF, Kleinman JT, Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Newhart M, et al. The NIHSS-plus: improving cognitive assessment with the NIHSS. Behav Neurol. (2009) 22:11–5. doi: 10.1155/2010/123903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Di Legge S, Fang J, Saposnik G, Hachinski V. The impact of lesion side on acute stroke treatment. Neurology. (2005) 65:81–6. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000167608.94237.aa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Tippett DC, Hillis AE. Vascular aphasia syndromes. In: Hickok G, Small S, editors. Neurobiology of Language. San Diego, CA: Elsevier (2016). p. 913–22. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00073-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kleinman JT, Newhart M, Davis C, Heidler-Garya J, Gottesman RF, Hillisabc AE, et al. Right hemispatial neglect: frequency and characterization following acute left hemisphere stroke. Brain Cogn. (2007) 64:50–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Liu C-F, Hsu J, Xu X, Ramachandran S, Wang V, Miller MI, et al. Deep learning-based detection and segmentation of diffusion abnormalities in acute ischemic stroke. Commun Med. (2021) 1:1–18. doi: 10.1101/2021.10.19.21257543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Schlegel D, Kolb SJ, Luciano JM, Tovar JM, Cucchiara BL, Liebeskind DS, et al. Utility of the NIH stroke scale as a predictor of hospital disposition. Stroke. (2003) 34:134–7. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000048217.44714.02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Rundek T, Mast H, Hartmann A, Boden-Albala B, Lennihan L, Lin I-F, et al. Predictors of resource use after acute hospitalization: the northern Manhattan stroke study. Neurology. (2000) 55:1180–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.8.1180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Barber PA, Hill MD, Eliasziw M, Demchuk AM, Pexman JHW, Hudon ME, et al. Imaging of the brain in acute ischaemic stroke: comparison of computed tomography magnetic resonance diffusion-weighted imaging. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2005) 76:1528–33. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.059261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hillis AE. Inability to empathize: brain lesions that disrupt sharing and understanding another's emotions. Brain. (2014) 137:981–97. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Leigh R, Oishi K, Hsu J, Lindquist M, Gottesman RJ, Jarso S, et al. Acute lesions that impair affective empathy. Brain. (2013) 136:2539–49. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ross ED, Monnot M. Neurology of affective prosody and its functional-anatomic organization in right hemisphere. Brain Lang. (2008) 104:51–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Durfee AZ, Sheppard SM, Blake ML, Hillis AE. Lesion loci of impaired affective prosody: a systematic review of evidence from stroke. Brain Cogn. (2021) 152:105759. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Blonder LX, Heilman KM, Ketterson T, Rosenbek J, Raymer A, Crosson B, et al. Affective facial lexical expression in aprosodic versus aphasic stroke patients. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2005) 11:677–85. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Moroa V, Pernigoa S, sakiris MT, Avesani R, Edelstyn NMJ, Jenkinson PM, et al. Motor versus body awareness: voxel-based lesion analysis in anosognosia for hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia following right hemisphere stroke. Cortex. (2016) 83:62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Minga J, Johnson M, Blake ML, Fromm D, MacWhinney B. Making sense of right hemisphere discourse using RHDbank. Top Lang Disord. (2021) 41:99–122. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hillis Trupe E, Hillis A. Paucity vs. verbosity: Another analysis of right hemisphere communication deficits. Clin Aphasiol. (1985) 15:83–96.

28. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. (1998) 62:89–106. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Brownell H, Martino G. Deficits in inference and social cognition: the effects of right hemisphere brain damage on discourse. In: Beeman M, Chiarello C, editors. Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. Lincoln: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers (1998). p. 309–28.

30. Caramazza A, Hillis AE. Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient. Nature. (1990) 346:267–9. doi: 10.1038/346267a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wheeler HM, Mlynash M, Inoue M, Tipirnini A, Liggins J, Bammer R, et al. The growth rate of early DWI lesions is highly variable associated with penumbral salvage clinical outcomes following endovascular reperfusion. Int J Stroke. (2015) 10:723–9. doi: 10.1111/ijs.12436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hillis AE, Tippett DC. Stroke recovery: surprising influences and residual consequences. Adv Med. (2014) 2014:378263. doi: 10.1155/2014/378263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Nogueira RG, et al. Thrombectomy 6 to 24 hours after stroke with a mismatch between deficit infarct. N Engl J Med. (2018) 378:11–21. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc1801530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Prosser J, Butcher K, Allport L, Parsons M, MacGregor L, Desmond P, et al. Clinical-diffusion mismatch predicts the putative penumbra with high specificity. Stroke. (2005) 36:1700–4. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000173407.40773.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Reineck LA, Agarwal S, Hillis AE. “Diffusion-clinical mismatch” is associated with potential for early recovery of aphasia. Neurology. (2005) 64:828–33. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152983.52869.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hillis AE, Barker P, Beauchamp N, Gordon B, Wityk R. MR perfusion imaging reveals regions of hypoperfusion associated with aphasia and neglect. Neurology. (2000) 55:782–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.6.782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hillis AE, Wityk RJ, Barker PB, Beauchamp NJ, Gailloud P, Murphy K, et al. Subcortical aphasia and neglect in acute stroke: the role of cortical hypoperfusion. Brain. (2002) 125:1094–104. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hillis AE, Wityk RJ, Barker PB, Ulatowski JA, Jacobs MA. Change in perfusion in acute nondominant hemisphere stroke may be better estimated by tests of hemispatial neglect than by the national institutes of health stroke scale. Stroke. (2003) 34:2392–6. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000089681.84041.69

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Faria AV. NIHSS_802_stroke [Data set]. Zenodo. (2021). doi: 10.5281/zenodo.5722286 (2021).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stroke, NIHSS, MCA, bias, left hemisphere

Citation: Vitti E, Kim G, Stockbridge MD, Hillis AE and Faria AV (2022) Left Hemisphere Bias of NIH Stroke Scale Is Most Severe for Middle Cerebral Artery Strokes. Front. Neurol. 13:912782. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.912782

Received: 04 April 2022; Accepted: 06 May 2022;
Published: 14 June 2022.

Edited by:

Cheng-Yang Hsieh, Sin-Lau Christian Hospital, Taiwan

Reviewed by:

Daisuke Wajima, Okinawa Prefectural Nanbu Medical Center, Japan
Kang-Po Lee, E-Da Hospital, Taiwan

Copyright © 2022 Vitti, Kim, Stockbridge, Hillis and Faria. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andreia V. Faria, YWZhcmlhMSYjeDAwMDQwO2pobWkuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.