- 1Epilepsy and Cognition Laboratory, Veterans Affairs, New York Harbor Healthcare System, Research Service, New York, NY, United States
- 2College of Arts and Science, New York University, New York, NY, United States
- 3Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Comprehensive Epilepsy Program, Stanford University School of Medicine, Palo Alto, CA, United States
- 4Department of Neurology, Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, New York University Langone Health, New York, NY, United States
- 5Department of Neurosurgery, New York University Langone Health, New York, NY, United States
- 6Department of Psychiatry, New York University Langone Health, New York, NY, United States
Studies of epilepsy patients provide insight into the neuroscience of human memory. Patients with remote memory deficits may learn new information but have difficulty recalling events from years past. The processes underlying remote memory impairment are unclear and likely result from the interaction of multiple factors, including hippocampal dysfunction. The hippocampus likely has a continued role in remote semantic and episodic memory storage over time, and patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) are at particular risk for deficits. Studies have focused on lateralization of remote memory, often with greater impairment in left TLE, which may relate to verbal task demands. Remote memory testing is restricted by methodological limitations. As a result, deficits have been difficult to measure. This review of remote memory focuses on evidence for its underlying neurobiology, theoretical implications for hippocampal function, and methodological difficulties that complicate testing in epilepsy patients.
Introduction
Anterograde memory deficits reflect the inability to form new memories, while retrograde amnesia refers to the loss of prior learned information. These forms of memory dysfunction provide insight into the processes of memory formation, consolidation, and retrieval. Nearly 50% of people with epilepsy have anterograde and retrograde memory impairments (1) which are often multifactorial in etiology, due to underlying hippocampal pathology (2, 3), frequent seizures (4, 5), early age of onset (6, 7), and long epilepsy duration (8). In 1953, patient H.M. developed severe anterograde and moderate, temporally graded retrograde amnesia following bilateral mesial temporal lobe resection for refractory epilepsy (9–11). H.M. revolutionized our understanding of the hippocampus' role in memory formation and paved the way for studies of anterograde memory in epilepsy. Remote retrograde memory deficits in epilepsy patients, however, are not well-understood. Patients with remote memory deficits may learn new information but have difficulty recalling events from long ago (12). The time point at which a memory is considered remote is ill-defined, but is typically ≥1 year (13). Patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) are at particular risk for remote memory dysfunction (14).
Research in remote memory has focused on autobiographical and general semantic memory. Episodic autobiographical memory is the recollection of personal events (i.e., celebrating your 10th birthday), while semantic autobiographical memory refers to factual knowledge of your past (i.e., the school you attended). In contrast, general semantic memory is minimally influenced by personal experience. General semantic memory pertains to common knowledge of public figures and events, such as recognition of famous faces and awareness of news items.
Anatomy of Remote Memory
The hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex are activated during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of remote memory. The anterior hippocampus is engaged during autobiographical recall and event construction, while the posterior hippocampus is implicated in event elaboration and spatial memory (15, 16). The hippocampus is comprised of multiple subregions that support establishment of new memories (“encoding”). Initial input from the entorhinal cortex (EC) projects via the perforant pathway to the Cornu Ammonis (CA), specifically area CA3, where intrinsic recurrent connections form a memory trace (17). The projection from the EC to CA3 may occur directly, or indirectly via the dentate gyrus, which reduces interference between similar information (17). The EC and CA3 activate CA1, which outputs information back to the EC and subiculum (17). Finally, the EC and subiculum relay information to neocortical locations (17). There is conflicting evidence, however, regarding the duration of the hippocampus' role in memory retrieval, resulting in opposing memory consolidation theories.
Across species, impairment of recent memory with relative preservation of remote memory is evident in the setting of hippocampal damage, a pattern termed “temporally graded retrograde amnesia” (18, 19). In contrast, patients with damage to lateral and anterior temporal neocortex can be amnestic for remote events (20). Autobiographical memory, for example, is intact in patients with damage restricted to medial temporal structures but is impaired when the injury extends to neocortex (21). These findings suggest that the hippocampus becomes less important for declarative memory storage and retrieval over time, the basic premise of the “standard model of systems consolidation” (SMSC) (22). This model proposes that new memories are encoded in both cortical and hippocampal regions, but are stored in progressively strengthened cortico-cortical synapses, while the hippocampal trace fades with time. The hippocampus is no longer required for retrieval once neocortical storage is sufficiently established (23). The hippocampal-dependent stage lasts up to 1 week after encoding, while the new information is gradually integrated into pre-existing cortical networks (24). Recall further strengthens cortico-cortical connections, resulting in the full transfer of the memory from the hippocampus to the neocortex (“consolidation”) (24). Retrieval of consolidated memories is independent of the hippocampus, predicting intact remote memory in patients with hippocampal lesions.
In contrast, the multiple trace theory (MTT) posits that spatial and context-specific details of an episodic memory are encoded in the hippocampus and remain stored there indefinitely, although some information is transferred to the neocortex (25). The entire hippocampal-neocortical pathway forms the consolidated memory trace. During each subsequent retrieval, the trace is reactivated, and a new, slightly different trace is established, so that over time, multiple related traces exist to facilitate retrieval. The MTT also emphasizes a distinction between episodic and semantic memory, in that both rely on the hippocampus for encoding, but general semantic memory eventually becomes independent of the hippocampus. While hippocampal traces contain contextual, spatial, and temporal details, cortical traces are semantic and context-free. Personally significant semantic memories, however, may have continued mesial temporal involvement.
The MTT predicts temporally graded retrograde amnesia for general semantic memory following hippocampal injury (26). Hippocampal damage would preferentially affect recent general semantic memories, as remote memories would be well-established in the neocortex. Yet, numerous rodent studies provide opposing evidence, with flat retrograde amnesia gradients for semantic memory after complete hippocampal damage (26–29). The MTT also posits that episodic memories become more strongly established in the hippocampus over time as the number of traces pertaining to that memory increases. Recent episodic memories, which have fewer memory traces, should be more disrupted by partial hippocampal lesions than remote memories (30). While the MTT places importance on the amount of damaged tissue, the specific structures affected may be the relevant factor. Several rodent studies, however, showed that remote memories were more vulnerable to partial hippocampal damage than recent memories, even when the precise location of hippocampal damage varied (27–29). Further, electrophysiologic unit recordings (31, 32) and calcium imaging (33, 34) in rodents showed a constant or decreasing number of place cells representing spatial memories, which contrasts the MTT's prediction that the number of activated cells increases when re-experiencing a specific context. Although much of the rodent literature supports the MTT, conflicting evidence surrounding the memory consolidation process remains.
Derived from the MTT, the trace transformation theory (TTT) (35) accounts for changes in memories over time, transforming from highly detailed to coarse representations. The TTT emphasizes the role of the posterior hippocampus and its connections to perceptual posterior neocortical regions in supporting finely detailed memories and the anterior hippocampus for remembering the overall “gist.” The anterior hippocampus connects with anterior neocortical regions, notably the medial prefrontal cortex, where schemas represent common features across multiple events. These pathways are reactivated over the memory's lifetime, shifting to and from finely or coarsely detailed recollections. While detailed recollection will always require hippocampal activation, schematic memory may be supported by the neocortex alone.
The MTT and TTT are supported by neurophysiological data from patients with medically refractory epilepsy. Intracranial EEG recordings demonstrated sharp wave ripples, oscillatory patterns supporting memory reactivation, in the hippocampi during recall of autobiographical memories (36). Ripple rate was higher during retrieval of remote memories compared to more recent events, supporting a continued role of the hippocampus. Ripples preferentially occurred in the anterior hippocampus, which the authors considered “compatible with [a] gist-like recollection strategy.” With increasingly remote memories, ripple rate patterns associated with autobiographical events gradually became more similar to patterns of semantic retrieval, consistent with the theory that memory traces evolve over time to more general representations. Further, ripples correlated with cortical high frequency band activation, including posterior neocortex and medial prefrontal cortex, supporting the theory of recurrent hippocampal-neocortical interplay in these regions during autobiographical recall. Electrocorticographic data is consistent with functional MRI activation of hippocampal, medial frontal, and parietal regions during recall of remote autobiographical events (37).
Remote Memory Deficits in Epilepsy
Remote memory abilities in epilepsy patients could also help distinguish between these models. If the SMSC is true, remote memory should be relatively unaffected in patients with mesial TLE, although patients with neocortical epilepsy may be impaired. If the MTT/TTT is correct, patients with either mesial TLE or neocortical epilepsy may have impaired remote memory. The distinction may be blurred, however, in that focal epilepsy negatively impacts wide networks, such that mesial TLE may impair neocortical function. Further, the epileptogenic neocortical regions may lie outside of those supporting consolidation, such that remote memory could remain intact. Studies are limited by the inclusion of post-surgical epilepsy patients, in whom mesial and lateral structures are removed, and mixed TLE groups with either mesial or neocortical epileptogenic zones. Nevertheless, most studies demonstrated remote memory impairments in TLE (Table 1).
Autobiographical Memory in Epilepsy
Autobiographical memory was impaired in TLE patients compared to healthy controls, although it did not correlate with subjective memory complaints (55), which deserves further study. The Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI) and Modified Crovitz Test were intact for personal semantics in pre-surgical TLE patients, the majority with mesial lesions, but memory for autobiographical episodes was impaired relative to controls (49). Similarly, subjects with mesial TLE, a subset post-surgical excision with lateral resections of unclear extent, had intact semantic autobiographical memory, but poorer episodic memory on the AMI than controls across all time periods (47). Pre-surgical TLE patients, the majority with hippocampal pathology, were impaired when generating friends' names (semantic autobiographical memory) on the Autobiographical Fluency Test, with the right TLE patients performing worse than controls when generating autobiographical events (46). Pre-surgical data largely support the MTT, in that hippocampal lesions were associated with impaired episodic remote memory. Data regarding semantic autobiographical memory, however, lend inconsistent support for the hypothesis that semantic memory is consolidated strictly in neocortex (46).
Variable autobiographical memory performance is evident post-anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL). Patients post-ATL had normal autobiographical event recall on the Autobiographical Fluency Test (43). In contrast, a small sample of post-left temporal lobectomy patients had impaired autobiographical memory for pre-surgical events (38). Pre- and post-surgical patients also had impaired memory for autobiographical events, in that memories contained impoverished perceptual detail (56). Reasons for variable results are unclear, but may relate to the laterality or extent of resection. One study included patients post-ATL, which removes neocortical and mesial structures, and patients post-selective amygdalohippocampectomy (SAH), which spares neocortex (56), but did not directly compare the two groups.
General Semantic Memory in Epilepsy
General semantic memory is consistently impaired in TLE, pre- and post-temporal resection. Like studies of autobiographical memory, data support the MTT/TTT, demonstrating a continual role for the hippocampus in retrieval of remote memory, but do not suggest that consolidated semantic memories are restricted to neocortex. Hippocampal ripples, for example, were of similar magnitude during general semantic and autobiographical episodic remote memory retrieval (36). Patients with TLE with either mesial or neocortical lesions had poorer recall and recognition for general semantic memory of public news items compared to healthy controls, extratemporal epilepsy patients, and primary generalized epilepsy patients (12). Pre-surgical left TLE patients, most with hippocampal pathology, had impaired recall and recognition of public knowledge, assessed by the Famous Faces, Famous Events, and Public Fluency Tests (46). Pre-surgical patients with TLE, most with mesial lesions, had poorer performance than healthy controls when naming famous faces and scenes, answering questions about news events, and indicating whether famous figures were dead or alive (49). Post-left temporal lobectomy, general semantic memory deficits were also evident when naming famous faces, providing knowledge about famous events and people, and recognizing names of short-lived television shows (38).
Lateralization of Remote Memory in Epilepsy
Evidence for lateralization of remote memory in epilepsy patients is mixed. Some data indicate no relationship between laterality of the seizure onset zone and memory for public news items (12) or autobiographical memory (47). Both left and right TLE groups were impaired, for example, when generating friends' names (46). Other data suggest lateralization of certain types of remote memory, with impaired autobiographical event fluency in right TLE and deficits regarding public knowledge in left TLE (46). Reduced right hippocampal activation was evident in patients with right mesial TLE compared to controls during remote autobiographical event recall, not explained by hippocampal volume (37). In contrast, hippocampal ripples occurred preferentially in the left anterior hippocampus during autobiographical event recall, in patients with either left, right, or bilateral seizure onset zones (36). Overall, when an asymmetry was present, patients with left TLE were typically more impaired, as seen with tests of public knowledge (38, 46, 49) and autobiographical memory (38, 49). Whether these findings relate to the verbal nature of tasks is unknown. Object naming and semantic and phonological fluency correlated with general semantic and autobiographical memory, which could explain group differences (46), although not seen in all studies (12).
Contributing Factors to Remote Memory Deficits
Several studies examined the effects of seizure-related variables on remote memory. Epilepsy duration and history of generalized seizures did not impact memory for autobiographical or public facts and events (46). Nor was autobiographical memory associated with hippocampal sclerosis or temporal lobe resection (47). Further, a history of status epilepticus did not predict remote memory for public events (12). Data regarding the impact of antiseizure medications (ASMs), however, are conflicting. Post-temporal lobectomy patients taking ASMs had poorer autobiographical semantic fluency than patients not taking ASMs (43), and pre-surgical TLE patients taking ASM polytherapy had impaired autobiographical event fluency compared to patients taking monotherapy (46). The number of ASMs also negatively correlated with recognition of famous events (46). No correlation was seen, however, between the number of ASMs and recall of news events (12), although studies differed in testing procedures, types of epilepsy included, and degree of seizure control. Data regarding age of onset are also variable. Poorer memory for famous events correlated with earlier age of onset (46), but this was not seen in other studies (12, 47, 49). Effects of seizure frequency are mixed, as well. Patients with less frequent seizures had better memory for personal (5) and public events (5, 12) than patients with more frequent seizures. Seizure-free patients also had greater autobiographical semantic fluency (for the previous 5-year period) than patients with active epilepsy (43). Other data demonstrated no significant correlation between seizure frequency and memory for personal events, public events, or famous faces (49). Results may be limited, however, by difficulty in measuring seizure frequency. Future studies should consider the potential impact of interictal epileptiform discharges and psychiatric comorbidities.
While similar deficits in autobiographical memory were seen in subjects with or without surgical resection (47, 56), the impact of surgery has not been studied within individual subjects or in general remote episodic or semantic memory. Further, current studies may be limited by a lack of detail regarding the extent of neocortical resection (47). “Standard” ATLs are not standardized, in that variable amounts of neocortical and mesial tissue may be resected. The impacts of surgical approach and size of the resection are unknown. Comparison of performance post-ATL with more selective surgeries may help clarify the role of temporal neocortex and duration of hippocampal involvement in remote memory. SAH, however, can disrupt cortical and subcortical fibers in the approach to mesial structures. Depending upon the surgical approach, SAH could be expected to impair remote memory function in a similar manner as ATL (57, 58). Future studies may consider the effects of laser induced thermal therapy (LITT) on remote memory, as this technique can create more selective lesions within the mesial temporal lobe. Stereotactic laser amygdalohippocampectomy can preserve anterograde verbal memory, highlighting the importance of extra-hippocampal structures in memory performance (59). Postoperative studies may also be enhanced by including MRI quantification of the resected structures as a covariate.
Methodological Issues in Assessing Remote Memory
Studies of temporal lobe damage provide conflicting support for the SMSC, MTT, and TTT. To understand variability across studies, we must consider the tasks used (Table 2) (39, 40, 44, 45, 48, 50, 51, 54, 60, 61). Several methodological factors limit remote memory testing, including a lack of control over the time and circumstances of encoding. Long-standing anterograde memory deficits may result in poor initial memory acquisition, which may falsely appear as remote, retrograde deficits. Further, the date of the event may not correspond to the time of acquisition, such that the “age” of a memory may be uncertain. It is also difficult to control the frequency of memory rehearsal or re-exposure, which may allow easier recall or recognition. Tests of autobiographical fluency (45), for example, can include recurrent events.
Autobiographical memory is often complicated by emotional and contextual factors (62). Emotional events create vivid autobiographical episodic memories, often resulting in highly detailed “flashbulb” memories of the moment an event occurred. These emotionally-laden memories are more often rehearsed and more easily recalled, which could lead to an overestimation of remote memory abilities. Emotional memories also involve extratemporal regions, including frontal cortex, such that effects of TLE localization and lateralization are not straightforward. Third-party validation of self-reported memories may present challenges, as it may be difficult to find someone to corroborate the subject's responses, and the third party may not accurately remember events. It is difficult to establish response accuracy, and the detail required for a “correct” response is unclear.
Assessments of general semantic knowledge also pose several methodological issues. People who lived outside of the country in which the event occurred may not have been exposed to the material being tested. Exposure to news items may also vary based on interest (63). Further, questions regarding “recent” events will become outdated over time, requiring questionnaires to be updated frequently. Many tests assess material from the past several decades, of which younger subjects may be unaware. Related events or events of a similar nature may be easily confused. Lastly, some news items may be more prominent than others, appear in the news for greater lengths of time, or become popular again after several years, such that saliency may vary.
The Transient News Events Test (TNET) accounts for several of these methodological issues. It controls for saliency by using news items with a similar frequency of reporting, measured by the number of mentions in the New York Times Article Archive. Frequency of rehearsal is largely controlled by including events that had similar declines in the number of mentions over a 5-year period, allowing assessment of memories from restricted time periods. The variation in what can be considered a “correct” response is minimized, as the amount of detail required is clearly defined for each question. Finally, subjects are asked about events occurring when they were 10 years of age onwards, accounting for a possible lack of media exposure in early childhood.
Epilepsy poses additional challenges to remote memory testing. Early onset of epilepsy and resultant functional reorganization may have two consequences: anterograde and retrograde memory impairments may be difficult to differentiate, and different cognitive processes for encoding and retrieval may be used when compared to healthy subjects (64). Further, interrupted schooling may affect exposure to test items.
Discussion
The literature lends insight into the brain regions involved in remote memory, though our current understanding of remote memory storage and retrieval remains limited. The exact role of the hippocampus is unclear, resulting in opposing theories of memory consolidation: the SMSC and MTT/TTT. The study of remote memory deficits in epilepsy can help to distinguish between these competing theories, as epilepsy may be associated with lesions of the underlying pathways. Data suggest a continued role for the hippocampus in remote general and autobiographical semantic and episodic memory and argue against the hypothesis that general semantic memory storage is restricted to neocortex. Studies are constrained, however, by methodological limitations of remote memory tests. We need improved, standardized testing for remote memory loss in epilepsy patients, and its inclusion in clinical neuropsychological batteries should be considered, as remote memory loss may be a source of disability not detected by current clinical testing. Further study of epilepsy-related factors and mechanisms of decline would help elucidate how seizures impair remote memory.
Author Contributions
SR conducted the literature review and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. KM contributed to the literature review and critical revision of the manuscript. WB and OD contributed to critical discussions regarding the topic. BL-M conceived of the manuscript and contributed to the literature review and drafts of the manuscript. All authors agree to be accountable for the content of the work.
Funding
This work was supported by a New York University Dean's Undergraduate Research Fund (DURF) award (SR) and a United States (U.S.) Department of Veterans Affairs Clinical Sciences R&D (CSRD) Service Career Development Award (IK2 CX-001255) (BL-M).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Witt JA, Helmstaedter C. Should cognition be screened in new-onset epilepsies? A study in 247 untreated patients. J Neurol. (2012) 259:1727–31. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6526-2
2. Addis DR, Moscovitch M, McAndrews MP. Consequences of hippocampal damage across the autobiographical memory network in left temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain. (2007) 130(Pt 9):2327–42. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm166
3. Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1957) 20:11–21. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
4. Dikmen S, Matthews CG. Effect of major motor seizure frequency upon cognitive-intellectual functions in adults. Epilepsia. (1977) 18:21–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1977.tb05583.x
5. Voltzenlogel V, Vignal JP, Hirsch E, Manning L. The influence of seizure frequency on anterograde and remote memory in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Seizure. (2014) 23:792–8. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2014.06.013
6. Dikmen S, Matthews CG, Harley JP. The effect of early versus late onset of major motor epilepsy upon cognitive-intellectual performance. Epilepsia. (1975) 16:73–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1975.tb04723.x
7. Lespinet V, Bresson C, N'Kaoua B, Rougier A, Claverie B. Effect of age of onset of temporal lobe epilepsy on the severity and the nature of preoperative memory deficits. Neuropsychologia. (2002) 40:1591–600. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00012-X
8. Elger CE, Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M. Chronic epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurol. (2004) 3:663–72. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00906-8
9. Corkin S. Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: clinical course and experimental findings in HM. Semin Neurol. (1984) 4:249–59. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1041556
10. Corkin S. What's new with the amnesic patient H.M.? Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:153–60. doi: 10.1038/nrn726
11. Sagar HJ, Cohen NJ, Corkin S, Growdon JH. Dissociations among processes in remote memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1985) 444:533–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37637.x
12. Bergin PS, Thompson PJ, Baxendale SA, Fish DR, Shorvon SD. Remote memory in epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2000) 41:231–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb00145.x
13. Rich JB. Remote memory. In: Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer New York LLC (2011). p. 2154–5.
15. Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn Sci. (2013) 17:230–40. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.005
16. Zeidman P, Maguire EA. Anterior hippocampus: the anatomy of perception, imagination and episodic memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2016) 17:173–82. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.24
17. Barry DN, Maguire EA. Remote memory and the hippocampus: a constructive critique. Trends Cogn Sci. (2019) 23:128–42. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.005
18. Zola-Morgan SM, Squire LR. The primate hippocampal formation: evidence for a time-limited role in memory storage. Science. (1990) 250:288–90. doi: 10.1126/science.2218534
19. Bayley PJ, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. The fate of old memories after medial temporal lobe damage. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:13311–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4262-06.2006
20. Squire LR, Clark RE, Knowlton BJ. Retrograde amnesia. Hippocampus. (2001) 11:50–5. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2001)11:1 <50::AID-HIPO1019>3.0.CO;2-G
21. Bayley PJ, Gold JJ, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. The neuroanatomy of remote memory. Neuron. (2005) 46:799–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.034
22. Squire LR, Alvarez P. Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1995) 5:169–77. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80023-9
23. Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:119–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607
24. Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. (2004) 55:51–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050
25. Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1997) 7:217–27. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80010-4
26. Sparks FT, Spanswick SC, Lehmann H, Sutherland RJ. Neither time nor number of context-shock pairings affect long-term dependence of memory on hippocampus. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2013) 106:316–23. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.05.008
27. Lehmann H, Lacanilao S, Sutherland RJ. Complete or partial hippocampal damage produces equivalent retrograde amnesia for remote contextual fear memories. Eur J Neurosci. (2007) 25:1278–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05374.x
28. Lehmann H, Sparks FT, O'Brien J, McDonald RJ, Sutherland RJ. Retrograde amnesia for fear-potentiated startle in rats after complete, but not partial, hippocampal damage. Neuroscience. (2010) 167:974–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.005
29. Sutherland RJ, O'Brien J, Lehmann H. Absence of systems consolidation of fear memories after dorsal, ventral, or complete hippocampal damage. Hippocampus. (2008) 18:710–8. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20431
30. Sutherland RJ, Lee JQ, McDonald RJ, Lehmann H. Has multiple trace theory been refuted? Hippocampus. (2020) 30:842–50. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23162
31. Karlsson MP, Frank LM. Network dynamics underlying the formation of sparse, informative representations in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:14271–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4261-08.2008
32. Lever C, Wills T, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O'Keefe J. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature. (2002) 416:90–4. doi: 10.1038/416090a
33. Rubin A, Geva N, Sheintuch L, Ziv Y. Hippocampal ensemble dynamics timestamp events in long-term memory. Elife. (2015) 4:e12247. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12247
34. Ziv Y, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Hamel EO, Ghosh KK, Kitch LJ, et al. Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nat Neurosci. (2013) 16:264–6. doi: 10.1038/nn.3329
35. Sekeres MJ, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. The hippocampus and related neocortical structures in memory transformation. Neurosci Lett. (2018) 680:39–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.05.006
36. Norman Y, Raccah O, Liu S, Parvizi J, Malach R. Hippocampal ripples and their coordinated dialogue with the default mode network during recent and remote recollection. Neuron. (2021) 109:2767–80.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.06.020
37. St-Laurent M, Moscovitch M, McAndrews MP. The retrieval of perceptual memory details depends on right hippocampal integrity and activation. Cortex. (2016) 84:15–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.08.010
38. Barr WB, Goldberg E, Wasserstein J, Novelly RA. Retrograde amnesia following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia. (1990) 28:243–55. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90018-J
39. Albert MS, Butters N, Levin J. Temporal gradients in the retrograde amnesia of patients with alcoholic Korsakoff's disease. Arch Neurol. (1979) 36:211–6. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1979.00500400065010
40. Barr WB. Retrograde Amnesia and Unilateral Temporal Lobectomy [Dissertation/PhD thesis]. New York, NY: New School for Social Research (2008).
41. Squire LR, Slater PC, Chace PM. Retrograde amnesia: temporal gradient in very long term memory following electroconvulsive therapy. Science. (1975) 187:77–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1109228
42. Goldberg E, Barnett J. Remote Memory Battery. New York, NY: Albert Einstein Medical College (1985).
43. Lah S, Grayson S, Lee T, Miller L. Memory for the past after temporal lobectomy: impact of epilepsy and cognitive variables. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:1666–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.008
44. Shum DHK, O'Gorman JG. A test of remote memory for use in Australia. Aust J Psychol. (2001) 53:36–44. doi: 10.1080/00049530108255120
45. Dritschel BH, Williams JMG, Baddeley AD, Nimmo-Smith I. Autobiographical fluency: a method for the study of personal memory. Mem Cogn. (1992) 20:133–40. doi: 10.3758/BF03197162
46. Lah S, Lee T, Grayson S, Miller L. Effects of temporal lobe epilepsy on retrograde memory. Epilepsia. (2006) 47:615–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00476.x
47. Viskontas I V, McAndrews MP, Moscovitch M. Remote episodic memory deficits in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. J Neurosci. (2000) 20:5853–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-15-05853.2000
48. Kopelman MD, Baddeley AD, Wilson BA. AMI: The Autobiographical Memory Interview: Manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment (1990).
49. Voltzenlogel V, Després O, Vignal JP, Steinhoff BJ, Kehrli P, Manning L. Remote memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2006) 47:1329–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00555.x
50. Crovitz HF, Schiffman H. Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bull Psychon Soc. (1974) 4:517–8. doi: 10.3758/BF03334277
51. Graham KS, Hodges JR. Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex in long-term memory storage: evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. (1997) 11:77–89. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.77
52. Thomas-Antérion C, Laurent B, Lemesle B, Laporte S, Michel D. Etude de la mémoire rétrograde événementielle chez les traumatisés crâniens (à partir de 36 cas). Ann Réadaptation Médecine Phys. (1994) 37:381–8.
53. Thomas-Antérion C, Guinvarch S, Rode G. Mémoires antérogrades et rétrogrades après un traumatisme crânien: recherches de corrélations chez neuf patients. Ann Réadaptation Médecine Phys. (1997) 40:519–6. doi: 10.1016/S0168-6054(97)84828-3
54. Kapur N, Ellison D, Smith MP, Mclellan DL, Burrows EH. Focal retrograde amnesia following bilateral temporal lobe pathology. A neuropsychological and magnetic resonance study. Brain. (1992) 115:73–85. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.73
56. St-Laurent M, Moscovitch M, Jadd R, Mcandrews MP. The perceptual richness of complex memory episodes is compromised by medial temporal lobe damage. Hippocampus. (2014) 24:560–76. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22249
57. Von Rhein B, Nelles M, Urbach H, Von Lehe M, Schramm J, Helmstaedter C. Neuropsychological outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy: subtemporal versus transsylvian approach. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2012) 83:887–93. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-302025
58. Gleissner U, Helmstaedter C, Schramm J, Elger CE. Memory outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy: a study in 140 patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2002) 43:87–95. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.24101.x
59. Drane DL, Willie JT, Pedersen NP, Qiu D, Voets NL, Millis SR, et al. Superior verbal memory outcome after stereotactic laser amygdalohippocampotomy. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:779495. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.779495
60. O'Connor MG, Sieggreen MA, Bachna K, Kaplan B, Cermak LS, Ransil BJ. Long-term retention of transient news events. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2000) 6:44–51. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700611050
61. Leeman BA, Macklin EA, Schomer DL, O'Connor MG. Transient News Events Test: feasibility in assessment of post-temporal lobectomy remote memory deficits. Epilepsy Behav. (2009) 16:113–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.06.012
62. Holland AC, Kensinger EA. Emotion and autobiographical memory. Phys Life Rev. (2010) 7:88–131. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.01.006
63. Kapur N, Thompson P, Kartsounis LD, Abbott P. Retrograde amnesia: clinical and methodological caveats. Neuropsychologia. (1999) 37:27–30. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00065-7
Keywords: remote memory, long-term memory, consolidation, autobiographical memory, episodic memory, epilepsy, seizures, hippocampus
Citation: Rastogi S, Meador KJ, Barr WB, Devinsky O and Leeman-Markowski BA (2022) Remote Memory in Epilepsy: Assessment, Impairment, and Implications Regarding Hippocampal Function. Front. Neurol. 13:855332. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.855332
Received: 15 January 2022; Accepted: 10 February 2022;
Published: 08 April 2022.
Edited by:
Yvonne Höller, University of Akureyri, IcelandReviewed by:
Fernando Cendes, State University of Campinas, BrazilCopyright © 2022 Rastogi, Meador, Barr, Devinsky and Leeman-Markowski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Beth A. Leeman-Markowski, beth.leeman-markowski@nyulangone.org