Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 08 March 2022
Sec. Pediatric Neurology
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Pediatric Neurology: 2021 View all 20 articles

Cross-Sectional Investigation of Brain Volume in Dyslexia

  • 1Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Jena University Hospital, Jena, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Jena University Hospital, Jena, Germany
  • 3Department of Neurology, Jena University Hospital, Jena, Germany

The goal of the study was to determine whether dyslexia is associated with differences in local brain volume, and whether these local brain volume differences show cross-sectional age-effects. We investigated the local volume of gray and white brain matter with voxel-based morphometry (VBM) as well as reading performance in three age groups of dyslexic and neurotypical normal reading subjects (children, teenagers and adults). Performance data demonstrate a steady improvement of reading skills in both neurotypical as well as dyslexic readers. However, the pattern of gray matter volumes tell a different story: the children are the only group with significant differences between neurotypical and dyslexic readers in local gray matter brain volume. These differences are localized in brain areas associated with the reading network (angular, middle temporal and inferior temporal gyrus as well as the cerebellum). Yet the comparison of neurotypical and normal readers over the age groups shows that the steady increase in performance in neurotypical readers is accompanied by a steady decrease of gray matter volume, whereas the brain volumes of dyslexic readers do not show this linear correlation between brain volume and performance. This is further evidence that dyslexia is a disorder with a neuroanatomical basis in the form of a lower volume of gray matter in parts of the reading network in early dyslexic readers. The present data point out that network shaping processes in gray matter volume in the reading network does take place over age in dyslexia. Yet this neural foundation does not seem to be sufficient to allow normal reading performances even in adults with dyslexia. Thus dyslexia is a disorder with lifelong consequences, which is why consistent support for affected individuals in their educational and professional careers is of great importance. Longitudinal studies are needed to verify whether this holds as a valid pattern or whether there is evidence of greater interindividual variance in the neuroanatomy of dyslexia.

Introduction

Developmental dyslexia affects about 5 percent of the population depending on diagnostic criteria (1). Even nowadays affected individuals still struggle to receive adequate support, thus dyslexia has far-reaching consequences for the suffering individual on scholastic, psychological and socio-economic levels (24). Individuals affected by dyslexia have problems with fluent and/or accurate reading, spelling and the proper acquisition of grapheme-phoneme correspondence. Dyslexia is not caused by a general cognitive impairment or a lack of an opportunity to learn (5). Several cognitive, sensoric and neurobiological deficits are suggested to cause dyslexia (6) which in turn are supposed to impact the acquisition and automation of the reading and spelling process (710). Reading processes take place in a large neural reading network comprised of broad areas in the dorsal superior temporal, ventral inferior temporal, and inferior frontal brain [e.g., (1113)]. The extent of the involvement of this particular reading network depends on cognitive demands of the reading task (14). Phonological processing strongly involves the dorsal reading system, whereas the ventral system is more involved with visual word form processing as well as the transfer of letter shape to phonological content (transfer of visual input to linguistic output units) (15). The extent to which either the dorsal or ventral system is most involved depends on the skill level involved in the reading process: beginning readers rely more on the phonological dorsal system and skilled readers rely more on the well-trained ventral visual reading system (1618). In those suffering from dyslexia, the dorsal and inferior frontal components of the neural reading network consistently display functional differences compared to the neural reading network of neurotypical readers (1921).

Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) is used in neuroimaging studies to determine whether dyslexia is associated with differences in local gray and white brain matter volumes in the reading network. VBM, without using a region of interest approach, is a fully automated method to identify regions of local volume differences in the whole brain.

Studies investigating the differences in local brain matter volume between neurotypical reading controls and readers with dyslexia identify various areas within the oral and written language networks [for an overview see (2228)]. Regions which repeatedly show differences in local brain volume between neurotypical reading and dyslexic subjects are in inferior parietal (29), temporo-parietal (29, 30) and superior temporal regions (31), the inferior frontal gyrus, the left and right fusiform gyrus (32) and the cerebellum (29, 32).

Recent studies using VBM demonstrate variations in brain matter volume in prereading children at risk for dyslexia (3335), across different language systems (36, 37) as well as differences in dyslexic children after a reading intervention (38). VBM studies thus indicate that dyslexia-specific morphometric differences can already be observed before the acquisition of written language and that these differences can be referred to as early neuroanatomical signatures for the subsequent reading problems. Finding no differences in dyslexia-specific VBM profiles across different language systems, Silani et al. (36) assume that there could be a common neuroanatomical basis irrespective of the language system. VBM studies also highlight the flexibility of the human brain since these studies demonstrate that these differences can be influenced by reading experience or training.

Several studies on the association between functional and volumetric differences in dyslexia detect that functional and volumetric differences between neurotypical and dyslexic readers are not only coexisting results, but can also serve as indicators of associated disorder characteristics. As these studies demonstrate (30, 39) there is evidence that areas which show dyslexia-specific hypo-activations in functional neuroimaging studies are related to differences in brain matter volumes between neurotypical and dyslexic readers.

Methodological inconsistencies between existing VBM studies on dyslexia make it hard to compare results since they are strongly influenced by the language system (shallow vs. deep orthographic system), experimental designs (subject samples, diagnostic criteria for dyslexia) as well as methods for data acquisition and analysis, [i.e., modulation for absolute GMV, registration algorithms using either a group-specific or a priori template, kernel size used for smoothing as well as differences on the level of statistical analysis (i.e., level of statistical correction for multiple tests)]. These inconsistencies can lead to differences in the findings of the studies (22, 37). The investigation of age dependent differences in brain volume of dyslexic readers by comparing different study-results is thus difficult.

To our knowledge, no study has yet addressed characteristics in brain matter volume with VBM in dyslexic readers over a large age range.

The aim of the present study is therefore to investigate age-dependent differences in local brain matter volume in developmental dyslexia. We hypothesize that dyslexic and neurotypical readers show different local brain volumes in areas related to the neuronal reading network and that these local brain volume differences show cross-sectional age-effects when comparing three groups (children, teenagers and adults with dyslexia compared to neurotypical normal reading, age-matched controls). Since we apply the same experimental design as well as the same parameters for data acquisition and analysis to all groups, we should be able to overcome the methodological pitfalls mentioned above.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

The study comprises three groups of subjects with dyslexia (children, teenagers and adults) as well as three groups of neurotypical normal reading subjects matched a posteriori according to age and nonverbal IQ (children, teenagers and adults).

A total of 21 neurotypical children, 24 neurotypical teenagers, 27 neurotypical adults, 22 dyslexic children, 18 dyslexic teenagers as well as 22 dyslexic adults took part in the diagnostic session and MRI data acquisition. Due to inferior MRI Data quality of some participants, these individuals were excluded from the analysis. Thus, the analyses in the present study are based on a final sample of 20 neurotypical children, 21 neurotypical teenagers, 26 neurotypical adults, 21 dyslexic children, 17 dyslexic teenagers as well as 20 dyslexic adults.

The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee at the Jena University Hospital. Subjects and legal guardians were informed verbally and with written materials about the experimental procedure. All individual participants included in the study and their legal guardians gave written informed consent for their participation. They were informed that all published data are fully anonymized. All procedures performed in this study are in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Assessment of Sample Criteria

During a diagnostic session several standardized tests were applied. These data were used to assess the sample criteria. The IQ of all subjects had to be ≥85. Sample criteria for dyslexia was a double discrepancy: ≥1.5 standard deviations between nonverbal IQ and reading and spelling performance as well as reading and spelling performance < percentile rank 15. Sample criteria for neurotypical controls was discrepancy between nonverbal IQ and reading as well as spelling performance < 1 standard deviation.

Subjects with uncorrected impairments of sight or hearing, bilingual education, neurological or psychiatric disorders (especially ADHD) were excluded based on the information obtained in a detailed clinical screening interview as well as performance in an attention test. All subjects were right handed according to verbal assessment. For an overview of the results regarding study criteria please refer to Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample criteria.

As a measure of nonverbal intelligence, Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices was administered (40). The test measures the subjects' reasoning ability, the educative (“meaning-making”) component of Spearman's g. It is comprised of multiple-choice questions. For each test item, the subject is asked to identify the missing element that completes a pattern. Reading fluency and accuracy both in single word as well as in text reading was assessed by means of a standardized reading test [Zürcher Lesetest, (41)]. In this test subjects read different lists of single words as well as different texts respective to class level. Since the ZLT can be applied to a wide age range, the use of various test procedures was not necessary. The test acquires scores for the time needed as well as the errors made while reading the target words and texts. Spelling performance was assessed by dictation of a gap text by means of standardized spelling tests appropriate for the respective class or age level (4244). The tests acquire the number of misspelled words.

Attention was assessed via the so-called “cross-out-test.” In this test, the subject is confronted with visually similar items and is asked to cross out certain target items. Attention as well as the tendency toward impulsive behavior is assessed via the speed and accuracy with which differentiation between the visually similar items is achieved by the subject. This test was administered in order to ensure that deficits in attention span do not confound the results [d2, (45)].

Reading Experiment: Assessment of Reading and Phonological Skills as Dependent Variables

Since phonological processing plays a crucial role in the reading process, especially for beginning readers, we acquired additional neuropsychological data on reading skill and phonological decoding skills using single word reading (e.g., Baum Bein), reading of pseudowords (e.g., Bilza Bilaz) as well as the rhyming of pseudowords (e.g., Jurde Surde).

We used these tasks in order to trigger phonological reading processes at different levels of difficulty, requiring different levels of phonological skill: Reading of frequently used regular single words should require lowermost phonological skills. These words should engage highly automatized whole word reading strategies. Pseudoword reading should exercise an increased demand for phonological processing, since the pseudowords do not exist in the common vocabulary and have no entry in the mental lexicon. The unknown word material must be read by using phonological grapheme-phoneme correspondence skills. Rhyming of pseudowords is thought to require the most phonological processing skill as, in addition to the grapheme-phoneme correspondence skill, phonological short time storage is needed to keep up the phonological code of the pseudoword in order to make the rhyme judgment (10).

For single word reading, frequently used German words were taken from third grade vocabulary (46, 47). Pseudowords were created on the basis of real words in which first the vowels were exchanged followed by stepwise exchange of consonants, until there was no longer an association for an existing German word. Subjects decided whether two items that were visually presented side by side on the computer screen (i.e., Baum Bein) were identical or not. Each decision required a key press, so that responses were registered through a key press of either index (stimuli are the same) or middle finger (stimuli are not the same) of the right hand. Reaction time and error rate were acquired via ERTS (48). The presentation rate of the stimuli was not fixed but subject-controlled with a maximal stimulus-presentation time of 5 s. If the subject pressed a key within these 5 s, the next pair of stimuli was presented after an inter-trial interval of 500 ms. The computer automatically switched to the next trial if the reaction time of 5 s was exceeded.

We hypothesized that improvement in reading and phonological skills (lower reaction times and error rates) in neurotypical and dyslexic readers should be observed from children to teenagers to adults and a potential phonological processing deficit should be reflected by increased reaction times and error rates over the three tasks in dyslexics compared to neurotypical readers. Performance in word reading, pseudoword reading and pseudoword rhyming is depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Word reading and phonological processing skills. NT-child, neurotypical reading children; D-child, children with dyslexia; NT-teen, neurotypical reading teenagers; D-teen, teenagers with dyslexia; NT-adult, neurotypical reading adults; D-adult, adults with dyslexia; ME, main effect; age, children, teenagers, adults; reading-level, neurotypical readers, readers with dyslexia. Only significant results are reported.

MRI Data Acquisition

MRI data were acquired using a Siemens Magnetom Vision 1.5 Tesla MRI Scanner (Erlangen, Germany). The head was fixated inside the head coil with extra padding in order to avoid movement artifacts. A high-resolution anatomical dataset of the whole brain (192 slices, T1-weighted, TR = 15 ms, TE = 5 ms, flip angle 30°, 1 mm slice thickness, magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence) was acquired.

Data Analysis / Diagnostic Data

Diagnostic data as well as the performance data on word reading and phonological processing were analyzed using 3 x 2 ANOVA with the factor “age group” (children, teenagers and adults) and the factor “reading-level” (neurotypical readers vs. readers with dyslexia).

Data Analysis / Voxel-Based Morphometry

Data Pre-processing and analysis were performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (Institute of Neurology, London, UK, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8, SPM, RRID:SCR_007037, version 8). For morphometric analysis of the data, we used voxel-based morphometry (VBM). This method involves the following steps: (1) spatial normalization of all images to a standardized anatomical space by removing differences in overall size, position, and global shape; (2) extracting gray and white matter from the normalized images; and (3) analyzing differences in local gray and white matter values across the whole brain (49). We applied an optimized method of VBM (50) using the VBM8 Toolbox (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm; VBM toolbox, RRID:SCR_014196, version 8) for both gray and white matter.

The segmentation procedure is further refined by accounting for partial volume effects (51), by applying adaptive maximum a posteriori estimations (52), and by applying a hidden Markov random field model (53). Because spatial normalization expands and contracts for some brain regions we scaled the segmented images by the amount of contraction, so that the total amount of gray or white matter in the images remained the same as it would be in the original images.

Due to the large differences in gray matter brain volume between the groups of children, teenagers, and adults we created a sample-specific template for spatial normalization. An iterative high-dimensional normalization approach provided by the DARTEL toolbox (54) was applied to the segmented tissue maps in order to normalize all images to a template. The DARTEL algorithm started with low-dimensional spatial normalization and calculated the average of all normalized segmentations. This averaged image was used in the next iteration and spatial resolution of the normalization was enhanced. This iteration scheme was repeated while the dimensionality of spatial normalization was increased. The result was a high-dimensionally warped brain with minimal bias, because a sample-specific template was used.

The resulting gray and white matter images were finally smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FWHM. We restricted the statistical analysis to areas with a minimum probability value of 0.1 to avoid possible edge-effects around tissue borders. Differences in local gray and white matter volume across the whole brain are analyzed with voxel-by-voxel t-test using the general linear model. We use a 3 × 2 ANOVA with factors age group (children, teenager and adults) and reading-level (neurotypical readers and readers with dyslexia) to test for differences between each group with dyslexia and their neurotypical control group. Since we use a modulation for Non-linear effects only that considers overall brain size, there is no need to correct for total intracranial volume (TIV) in the statistical model.

Furthermore, we tested for an interaction of the factors age group and reading-level. Results were considered significant for p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using FWE based on threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE), which avoids using an arbitrary threshold for the initial cluster-formation (55). Corresponding coordinates for each significant region are reported in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.

Additionally we performed correlation analyses between reading performance and gray matter volumes as well as age and gray matter volumes of those gray matter clusters which showed significant main effects for reading level in the comparison of neurotypical and dyslexic readers. In clusters with two cluster maxima, we chose the cluster with the larger TFCE values for the correlations (Cluster 1: MNI coordinates −51 −63 25; Cluster 3: MNI coordinates −42 −67 −29; Cluster 5: 47 −60 −32).

Results

Sample Criteria

For a summary of the descriptive statistics as well as the statistical results of the 3 x 2 ANOVAs of the sample criteria data please refer to Table 1. Reading and spelling performance of the neurotypical readers stays within the normal range for all three age groups, whereas reading performance shows better performance with rising age for the dyslexic readers, reaching normal reading performance levels in adulthood. However spelling performance remains below average for all three age groups of dyslexic readers.

Reading and Phonological Skills

For performances in word and pseudoword reading and pseudoword rhyming there are highly significant main effects for factors age-group and reading-level. For reading of words and pseudowords there is a steady increase in performance from children to teenagers to adults in the neurotypical as well as in the dyslexic readers even though the latter are consistently slower over all three age groups compared to the neurotypical readers. For rhyming of pseudowords (the task with the highest demands on phonological processing skills), there is also an improved performance from children to teenagers in the neurotypical readers and the readers with dyslexia. However, adults with dyslexia do not show any further increase in performance. Their performance remains at the level of the teenagers with dyslexia.

Voxel-Based Morphometry

Gray Matter

Only children show significant main effects for the factor reading-level regarding local gray matter volume differences between neurotypical readers vs. readers with dyslexia (on a level corrected for multiple statistical comparisons, TFCE-statistic, FWE corrected, p = 0.05). Neurotypical children show significantly larger local volumes of gray matter than children with dyslexia in large left-sided temporo-parietal and frontal clusters [encompassing the inferior temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, V5, middle occipital gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, inferior parietal gyrus, angular gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, left and right superior temporal gyrus (encompassing the insulae), left and right cerebellum as well as left and right prefrontal clusters (encompassing the superior, middle and medial frontal gyrus)]. See Figure 2 for the differences in gray matter volume of the children and Table 2 for the coordinates of the local cluster maxima.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significant differences in local gray matter (TFCE-statistic, FWE corrected for multiple comparisons, p ≤ 0.05) for neurotypical reading children > children with dyslexia. (A) Render view of all clusters. (B) Depiction of axial and coronal slices in order to illustrate the displayed slices.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Coordinates and anatomical regions of main effect neurotypical reading children > children with dyslexia.

Three clusters, which are part of the left-sided reading network (angular gyrus and middle temporal gyrus), the left and right cerebellum, and the right fusiform gyrus show significant interaction effects of the factors age-group and reading-level regarding local gray matter brain volume.

The percent signal change in the cluster maxima as depicted in Figure 3 demonstrates, that the groups of readers with dyslexia (see Figure 3) mainly drive this interaction effect. Neurotypical readers present a steady decrease of percent signal change in all clusters from children to teenagers to adults. Whereas the comparison of the percent signal change in the readers with dyslexia displays an inverted U-shape: comparing the groups with dyslexia, children show smaller volumes than teenagers, and for teenagers larger volumes can be detected than for adults. Adults on the other hand show the smallest local volumes of gray matter compared to children and teenagers (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Significant differences in local gray matter (uncorrected, p ≤ 0.001, expected cluster level according to expected numbers of voxel per cluster: 75 voxel) as well as % signal changes in the cluster centers for interaction effect of factors age-group and reading-level. Coordinates of the clusters and percent signal change of the averaged clusters are depicted. NT, neurotypical readers; D, readers with dyslexia; Ch, children; T, teenagers; A, adults.

Correlation Analysis

As depicted in Tables 3 and 4 the correlation analysis between reading performance and gray matter volume shows two significant correlations for neurotypical readers in Cluster 1 and 5, there are no significant correlations between performance and gray matter volume for dyslexic readers. Looking at correlations between age and gray matter volume, there are significant negative correlations for neurotypical readers in Cluster 1 and 5 and significant negative correlations for dyslexic readers in Cluster 3 and 5. Scatterplots for these correlations are depicted in Figures 4, 5.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlational analysis reading performance and gray matter volume.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlational analysis age and gray matter volume.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Scatterplots for Gray Matter Volume vs. Reading Performance; Dys, Dyslexic Readers; NT, Neurotypical Readers; GM, Gray Matter Volume; reading time in milliseconds. (A) Cluster 1/MNI-coordinates: −51/−63/25, (B) Cluster 3/MNI-coordinates: −42/−67/−29, (C) Cluster 5/MNI-coordinates: 47/−60/−32.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Scatterplots for Gray Matter Volume vs. Age; Dys, Dyslexic Readers; NT, Neurotypical Readers; GM, Gray Matter Volume; age in years. (A) Cluster 1/MNI-coordinates: −51/−63/25, (B) Cluster 3/MNI-coordinates: −42/−67/−29, (C) Cluster 5/MNI-coordinates: 47/−60/−32.

White Matter

There are no significant differences in local white matter volume between the children, teenagers, and adults.

Discussion

Reading and spelling performance of the dyslexic readers shows a typical clinical pattern across the different age groups: as proven by significant main effects for age-group, reading-level and interaction effects the reading performance in the dyslexic readers improves significantly over age, reaching average performance in adulthood, whereas spelling performance in dyslexic readers stays below average in all three age groups. The fact that the three groups of dyslexic readers show slight differences in spelling performance can probably be explained by the fact that different tests appropriate to their respective ages had to be used.

There are three main study results for the VBM data: (1) Only children display a main effect for the factor “reading-level” regarding differences in local gray matter volume. (2) The significant interaction effect for the factors “age-group” and “reading-level” can be explained by a significant correlation of performance and gray matter volume in neurotypical readers, showing that performance increase is accompanied by gray matter volume decrease over age. Dyslexic readers do not show these correlations between performance and gray matter volume. (3) There are no significant differences in white matter.

Main Effect for Factor “Reading-Level” in the Children

Children show differences in local gray matter brain volume in temporo-parietal areas [fusiform gyrus, angular gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus (insulae)]. These clusters correspond to findings from other VBM studies in which readers with dyslexia display anatomical differences in these particular brain areas (2932). These areas are critical parts of the reading network since they are related to phonological processing [superior temporal and temporo-parietal brain areas, (56)], skilled and automated reading, visual-auditive integration and memory for word images [inferior temporal brain areas (15, 18)].

Could This Main Effect Be Due to a Lack of Reading Experience?

Krafnick and colleagues (57) argue that differences in brain volume between neurotypical reading children and children with dyslexia are most likely due to a lack of reading experience, since the comparison to reading-level matched neurotypical children does not display a significant difference in volume. It is likely that the reading experience plays an important role in the shaping of the reading network, however differences in brain volume in prereading children with or without risk for dyslexia can also be observed (33, 35). Thus, the effects of the present study do not seem to be only due to a lack of reading experience.

In addition to the differences in gray matter volume observed in the temporo-parietal cluster, the present study also demonstrates differences in bilateral prefrontal regions. Black and colleagues (34) report reduced gray matter brain volume in the prefrontal brain areas of children at risk for dyslexia. They associate these reduced volumes with differences in functions such as naming, verbal fluency (58, 59), executive processing (2) and working memory. The working memory is an important factor in the development of efficient reading skills as this is the means by which speech sounds are sustained during reading. These anatomical differences in brain areas important for the development of automated reading skills, efficient phonological processing skills and verbal working memory could be the neuroanatomical correlate for a markedly poorer baseline in the development of reading and spelling skills.

Association Between Performance and Brain Volume in Neurotypical Readers

The regular pattern of brain matter volume development over the whole life span is that of an inverted U-shape (60). Normally there is an increase of brain volume with a peak of brain volume around the age of 6–8 years, followed by a steady decrease of brain volume as an ongoing lifelong process.

The mechanism behind this phenomenon is called synaptic pruning (13, 6163). In general it is assumed, that the key to better cognitive performance lies in neural processes like pruning and cortical thinning in order to create a well-functioning cortical network. We know from the neurotypical developing brain, that overproduction of neurons and connections are the starting point in brain development. The initial network is unorganized with many superfluous connections making communication disorganized and inefficient. It is suggested that the subsequent decrease in gray matter brain volume over age in the neurotypical brain reflects the sculpting process for a well-functioning mature brain with efficient neuronal networks (60).

Recent research confirms that experience-dependent changes in brain structures extends throughout the lifespan (13, 26, 64), and that reading experience in the neurotypical reading brain successively leads to a reduction of brain volume. There are also studies on reading development which demonstrate that lower brain volumes correspond to better reading skills (13, 64).

Performance data of the present study indicate that there is a steady increase in reading skill for all reading tasks in neurotypical readers from children, to the teenage years, to adulthood. This improvement in performance is accompanied by a steady decrease of gray matter volume. Thus, findings from our neurotypical reading control group agree with the literature insofar that better reading and spelling skills are related to lower volumes of gray matter brain volume in the reading network.

Dyslexia Specific Differences in Brain Matter Volume

Compared to the brain volumes of the neurotypical readers in our sample, the dyslexic readers do not show these linear effects when looking at correlations of performance and brain volumes. The behavioral data indicate that neurotypical readers as well as dyslexic subjects show progress in reading and spelling performance with age. However, as correlational analyses results demonstrate, this steady increase in performance is not paralleled by a linear decrease of brain volume in dyslexic readers. Children with dyslexia demonstrate smaller gray matter brain volume and lower reading performance compared to the teenagers with dyslexia as can be seen in Figure 3. The improvement in reading skills in the teenagers with dyslexia is combined with larger brain volume in this group. This is not in line with the amount of brain volumes of the neurotypical reading control group in which the progress in performance of the teenagers is accompanied by lower brain volume.

Yet that progress in performance can lead to volume increase is in line with various studies, demonstrating that acquisition of a skill is accompanied by increase in gray brain matter (26, 65). Krafnick and colleagues (38) described that behavioral training effects in children with dyslexia are accompanied by rising brain volumes in the left anterior fusiform gyrus/hippocampus, left precuneus, right hippocampus and right anterior cerebellum. After this volume increase, a developmental effect similar to the neurotypical readers is occurring in the form of a decrease of local brain volume accompanied by an increase in the performance level. As these developments can be observed in regions associated to the reading network, this strongly suggests that preexisting structural differences in children are varied by experience-dependent structural change involving dendritic and synaptic pruning (13). The interaction effect can therefore be described as neuronal developmental delay, leading to partial compensation by a subsequent volume increase and then decrease with age and rising performance skills.

That volume changes do not proceed in a straightforward single direction (only lower brain volumes accompany better performance) is demonstrated by Linkersdörfer and colleagues (13): they describe that both, volume in- as well as decrease is associated with progress in performance. The results of the present study are thus further evidence for the flexibility of the human brain.

In sum, these morphometric and behavioral data combined appear to indicate that the differences in brain matter volume in the children are an early neuroanatomical signature of the dyslexia-specific reading problems. Additionally, even though the patients with dyslexia show progress in performance, it appears that the neuronal processes leading to this increase in performance are different from those in the neurotypical readers. Whereas neurotypical readers display a steady fine-tuning of the neural reading network and a decrease of gray matter, in readers with dyslexia progress in performance does not show these linear correlations.

Methodological Issues

VBM is primarily a tool to investigate effects in gray matter, whereas its sensitivity in detecting effects in white matter is inferior because in T1-weighted MR images, this tissue type is characterized by the presence of large homogeneous regions with only small changes in signal. Changes in white matter fiber tracts can be better detected using Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI). However, this does not mean that white matter analysis is less reliable, but it is more difficult to detect effects compared to gray matter. Sophisticated methods such as DTI are simply more powerful at detecting effects in white matter, but when effects are found with VBM, they are no less reliable.

Because our findings are restricted to effects in the children only, one might ask whether our results are exaggerated due to normalizing them to a template for an older age group. Therefore, we additionally preprocessed and reanalyzed our children's data by creating a children-specific template. By using this alternative approach, we observe an almost identical pattern in the differences between the neurotypical reading children and children with dyslexia. Thus, it is very unlikely that the effects in children are driven by the use of a template, which is created from the whole sample of children, teenagers, and adults.

With respect to the bilateral findings across the brain, one might speculate that these findings could be caused by the fact that dyslexic children have smaller brains. However, we account for individual differences in brain size in our approach. Although segmented images are scaled by the extent of contraction or expansion due to spatial normalization, we do not scale by the linear effects of spatial normalization. Thus, overall brain size differences are corrected for, while local differences in the brains are preserved.

Limitations of the Study

The drawback of our design is that it cannot uncover whether differences in brain volume are preexisting before reading acquisition and thus cause differences in reading skill development, or whether these anatomical differences result from the failure of learning to read. Additionally the cross-sectional design does not allow answering developmental questions in a straightforward manner. However as Casey and colleagues point out (60), longitudinal MRI studies investigating the structural brain development in children and teenagers (66, 67) observe similar patterns as cross-sectional studies (6870). Thus, also cross-sectional investigations provide valuable information.

Conclusion

Brain development is a lifelong process with regressive as well as progressive learning and experience based neuronal changes (26, 60). Longitudinal studies provide reliable evidence for a causal relationship between the learning experience and subsequent changes in brain volume. Looking at the current state of the literature, a straightforward conclusion for neuronal differences in brain volume between neurotypical normal and dyslexic readers is compounded due to the heterogeneity of VBM study results. This study attempts to shed light on these questions by maintaining the constancy of certain crucial experimental settings such as the diagnostic criteria, the methods of data measurement and data analysis while comparing different age groups of neurotypical and dyslexic readers. The VBM and behavioral data point out that reading deficits in individuals with dyslexia are associated with gray matter volume differences in the reading network compared to neurotypical readers and that in those two groups behavioral improvement in reading skills is reflected in different neuroanatomical patterns. Even though there is some compensation by an increase in the brain volume and subsequent network shaping, this neural baseline is not sufficient to allow for the development of neurotypical reading skills even in dyslexic adults.

Thus, this study, in its investigation of neuroanatomical and behavioral data over a wide age range is further evidence that dyslexia is a disorder with lifelong consequences, which is why consistent support for affected individuals in their educational and professional careers is of great importance. Longitudinal studies, which include investigations of the individually developing dyslexic brain as well as the question whether longitudinally (until well into adulthood) remediation after reading intervention leads to anatomical profiles similar to those of neurotypical readers are needed in order to come to understand whether these findings hold as a valid pattern or whether there is evidence of greater interindividual variance in the neuroanatomy of dyslexia.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Local Ethics Committee at the Jena University Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

CL and ML contributed to patients' recruitment and data collection. All authors contributed to data processing, participated in drawing up the manuscript, involved in the intellectual workup for the article, read, and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the [Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft] under Grant [LI 2659/2-1], the [Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft] under Grant [BL 435/3-1 and BL 435/3-2].

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. McCandliss BD, Noble KG. The development of reading impairment: a cognitive neuroscience model. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2003) 9:196–204. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Shaywitz SE, Morris R, Shaywitz BA. The education of dyslexic children from childhood to young adulthood. Annual Rev Psychol. (2008) 59:451–75. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Strehlow U, Haffner J. Definitionsmöglichkeiten und sich daraus ergebende Häufigkeit der umschriebenen Lese- bzw. Rechtschreibstörung - theoretische Überlegungen und empirische Befunde an einer repräsentativen Stichprobe junger Erwachsener. Z fur Kinder- und Jugendpsy Psychother. (2002) 30:113–26. doi: 10.1024//1422-4917.30.2.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Esser G, Wyschkon A, Schmidt MH. Long-term outcome in 8-year-old children with specific reading retardation: results at age 25 years. Z Klin Psychol Psychother. (2002) 31:235–42. doi: 10.1026/0084-5345.31.4.235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Dilling H, Mombour W, Schmidt MH, Schulte-Markwort E. Zweite Achse - Umschriebene Entwicklungsrückstände. In: Dilling H, Mombour W, Schmidt MH, Schulte-Markwort E, editors. Weltgesundheitsorganisation: Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Störungen (ICD-10 Kapitel V (F), Diagnostische Kriterien für Forschung und Praxis). Bern: Verlag Hans Huber (2000). p. 281–96.

6. Ramus F, Ahissar M. Developmental dyslexia: the difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cogn Neuropsychol. (2012). 29:104–22. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.677420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Ramus F. Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2003) 13:212–8. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bradley L, Bryant PE. Categorizing sounds and learning to read–a causal connection. Nature. (1983) 301:419–21. doi: 10.1038/301419a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bruck M. Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficit. Dev Psychol. (1992) 28:874–86. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Georgiewa P, Grunling C, Ligges M, Filz C, Moller U, Blanz B. Age dependent changes in phonological reading in dyslexia. Z Klin Psychol Psychother. (2004) 33:281–9. doi: 10.1026/1616-3443.33.4.281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sandak R, Mencl WE, Frost SJ, Pugh KR. The neurobiological basis of skilled and impaired reading: recent findings and new directions. Sci Stud Read. (2004) 8:273–92. doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0803_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ligges C, Blanz B. Survey of fMRI results regarding a phonological deficit in children and adults with dyslexia: fundamental deficit or indication of compensation? Z fur Kinder- Jugendpsychiatrie Psychother. (2007). 35:107–17. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917.35.2.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Linkersdorfer J, Jurcoane A, Lindberg S, Kaiser J, Hasselhorn M, Fiebach CJ, et al. The association between gray matter volume and reading proficiency: a longitudinal study of beginning readers. J Cogn Neurosci. (2015) 27:308–18. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Price CJ. A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage. (2012) 62:816–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Jenner AR, Katz L, Frost SJ, Lee JR, et al. Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. J Commun Disord. (2001) 34:479–92. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9924(01)00060-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Turkeltaub PE, Gareau L, Flowers DL, Zeffiro TA, Eden GF. Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nat Neurosci. (2003) 6:767–73. doi: 10.1038/nn1065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Schlaggar BL, McCandliss BD. Development of neural systems for reading. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2007) 30:475–503. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Booth J, Mehdiratta N, Burman D, Bitan T. Developmental increases in effective connectivity to brain regions involved in phonological processing during tasks with orthographic demands. Brain Res. (2008) 189:78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1301–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Maisog JM, Einbinder ER, Flowers DL, Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF. A Metaanalysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. (2008) 1145:237–59. doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Richlan F. Developmental dyslexia: dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Front Hum Neurosc. (2012) 6:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ramus F, Altarelli I, Jednorog K, Zhao J, di Covella LS. Neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: pitfalls and promise. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 84:434–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Eckert M. Neuroanatomical markers for dyslexia: a review of dyslexia structural imaging studies. Neuroscientist. (2004) 10:362–71. doi: 10.1177/1073858404263596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Eckert MA, Berninger VW, Vaden KI, Gebregziabher M, Tsu L. Gray matter features of reading disability: a combined meta-analytic and direct analysis approach. eNeuro. (2015). 3: ENEURO.0103-15.2015. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0103-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Eckert MA, Vaden KI, Maxwell AB, Cute SL, Gebregziabher M, Berninger VW, et al. Common brain structure findings across children with varied reading disability profiles. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-05691-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Richardson FM, Price CJ. Structural MRI studies of language function in the undamaged brain. Brain Struct Funct. (2009) 213:511–23. doi: 10.1007/s00429-009-0211-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Elnakib A, Soliman A, Nitzken M, Casanova MF, Gimel'farb G, El-Baz A. Magnetic resonance imaging findings for dyslexia: a review. J Biomed Nanotechnol. (2014) 10:2778–805. doi: 10.1166/jbn.2014.1895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Hum Brain Mapp. (2013) 34:3055–65. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Eckert M, Leonard CM, Wilke M, Eckert M, Richards T, Richards A, et al. Anatomical signatures of dyslexia in children: unique information from manual and voxel based morphometry brain measures. Cortex. (2005) 41:304–15. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70268-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hoeft F, Meyler A, Hernandez A, Juel C, Taylor-Hill H, Martindale JL, et al. Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2007) 104:4234–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609399104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Steinbrink C, Vogt K, Kastrup A, Muller HP, Juengling FD, Kassubeek J, et al. The contribution of white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: insights from DTI and VBM at 3.0 T. Neuropsychologia. (2008) 46:3170–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kronbichler M, Wimmer H, Staffen W, Hutzler F, Mair A, Ladurner G. Developmental dyslexia: gray matter abnormalities in the occipitotemporal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. (2008) 29:613–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Raschle NM, Chang M, Gaab N. Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. Neuroimage. (2011) 57:742–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Black JM, Tanaka H, Stanley L, Nagamine M, Zakerani N, Thurston A, et al. Maternal history of reading difficulty is associated with reduced language-related gray matter in beginning readers. Neuroimage. (2012) 59:3021–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Saygin ZM, Norton ES, Osher DE, Beach SD, Cyr AB, Ozernov-Palchik O, et al. Tracking the roots of reading ability: white matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:13251–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Silani G, Frith U, Demonet JF, Fazio F, Perani D, Price C, et al. Brain abnormalities underlying altered activation in dyslexia: a voxel based morphometry study. Brain. (2005). 128:2453–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jednoróg K, Marchewka A, Altarelli I, Monzalvo Lopez AK, van Ermingen-Marbach M, Grande M, et al. How reliable are gray matter disruptions in specific reading disability across multiple countries and languages? Insights from a large-scale voxel-based morphometry study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2015) 36:1741–54. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Krafnick AJ, Flowers DL, Napoliello EM, Eden GF. Gray matter volume changes following reading intervention in dyslexic children. Neuroimage. (2011) 57:733–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Linkersdoerfer J, Lonnemann J, Lindberg S, Hasselhorn M, Fiebach CJ. Grey matter alterations co-localize with functional abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: an ALE meta-analysis. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e43122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Raven J. Standard Progressive Matrices. Göttingen: Beltz-Test GmbH (1996).

Google Scholar

41. Grissemann H. Zürcher Lesetest (ZLT). Göttingen: Hans Huber (2000).

42. Jäger A. Rechtschreibtest R - T. Göttingen: Hogrefe (1974).

43. Rathenow P. Westermann Rechtschreibtest 4/5. Braunschweig: Georg Westermann (1980).

44. Rathenow P, Laupenmühle D, Vöge J. Westermann Rechtschreibtest 6+ (WRT6+). Braunschweig: Georg Westermann (1981).

45. Brickenkamp R. Test d2: Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test. Göttingen: Hogrefe (1994).

Google Scholar

46. Ortmann WD. Hochfrequente deutsche Wortformen II. München: Druckerei Kemmler (1976).

47. Pregel DR G. Der Wortschatz im Grundschulalter: Häufigkeitswörterbuch zum verbalen, substantivistischen und adjektivistischen Wortgebrauch. Hildesheim: Georg Olms AG (1987).

48. BeriSoft Cooperation. Experimental Run Time System (ERTS). BeriSoft Cooperation (1987-1999).

49. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry–the methods. Neuroimage. (2000) 11:805–21. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RNA, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tohka J, Zijdenbos A, Evans A. Fast and robust parameter estimation for statistical partial volume models in brain MRI. Neuroimage. (2004) 23:84–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rajapakse JC, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. Statistical approach to segmentation of single-channel cerebral MR images. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (1997) 16:176–86. doi: 10.1109/42.563663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Cuadra MB, Cammoun L, Butz T, Cuisenaire O, Thiran JP. Comparison and validation of tissue modelization and statistical classification methods in T1-weighted MR brain images. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (2005) 24:1548–65. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2005.857652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. (2007) 38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Smith SM, Nichols TE. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage. (2009) 44:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bitan T, Cheon J, Lu D, Burman DD, Gitelman D, Mesulam M-M, et al. Developmental changes in activation and effective connectivity in phonological processing. Neuroimage. (2007) 38:564–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Krafnick AJ, Flowers DL, Luetje MM, Napoliello EM, Eden GF. An Investigation into the origin of anatomical differences in dyslexia. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:901–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2092-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Fiez JA, Petersen SE. Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (1998) 95:914–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Siddiqui SV, Chatterjee U, Kumar D, Siddiqui A, Goyal N. Neuropsychology of prefrontal cortex. Indian J Psychiatry. (2008) 50:202–8. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.43634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci. (2005) 9:104–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Chechik G, Meilijson I, Ruppin E. Neuronal regulation: a mechanism for synaptic pruning during brain maturation. Neural Comput. (1999) 11:2061–80. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kharitonova M, Martin RE, Gabrieli JDE, Sheridan MA. Cortical gray-matter thinning is associated with age-related improvements on executive function tasks. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2013) 6:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Wandell BA, Yeatman JD. Biological development of reading circuits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:261–8. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Houston SM, Lebel C, Katzir T, Manis FR, Kan E, Rodriguez GG, et al. Reading skill and structural brain development. Neuroreport. (2014) 25:347–52. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Lu LH, Leonard CM, Thompson PM, Kan E, Jolley J, Welcome SE, et al. Normal developmental changes in inferior frontal gray matter are associated with improvement in phonological processing: a longitudinal MRI analysis. Cerebral Cortex. (2007) 17:1092–99. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2004) 101:8174–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, Toga AW. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci. (2004) 24:8223–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Jernigan TL, Hesselink JR, Sowell E, Tallal PA. Cerebral structure on magnetic-resonance-imaging in language-impaired and learning-impaired children. Arch Neurol. (1991) 48:539–45. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1991.00530170103028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipursky RB, Lim KO. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol. (1994) 51:874–87. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1994.00540210046012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Casey BJ, Kozuch PL, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb Cortex. (1996) 6:551–60. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.4.551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reading, developmental dyslexia, local brain volume, gray matter, VBM, structural MRI

Citation: Ligges C, Ligges M and Gaser C (2022) Cross-Sectional Investigation of Brain Volume in Dyslexia. Front. Neurol. 13:847919. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.847919

Received: 17 January 2022; Accepted: 04 February 2022;
Published: 08 March 2022.

Edited by:

Maria Pia Bucci, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Reviewed by:

Angela Jocelyn Fawcett, Swansea University, United Kingdom
Todd L. Richards, University of Washington, United States

Copyright © 2022 Ligges, Ligges and Gaser. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carolin Ligges, Carolin.Ligges@med.uni-jena.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.