Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 26 August 2021
Sec. Movement Disorders

The Thermodynamic Consequences of Parkinson's Disease

  • 1Neurosciences Department, Monash Medical Centre, Clayton, VIC, Australia
  • 2Department of Medicine, School of Clinical Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, Australia

Several lines of evidence point to a pervasive disturbance of energy balance in Parkinson's disease (PD). Weight loss, common and multifactorial, is the most observable sign of this. Bradykinesia may be best understood as an underinvestment of energy in voluntary movement. This accords with rodent experiments that emphasise the importance of dopamine in allocating motor energy expenditure. Oxygen consumption studies in PD suggest that, when activities are standardised for work performed, these inappropriate energy thrift settings are actually wasteful. That the dopaminergic deficit of PD creates a problem with energy efficiency highlights the role played by the basal ganglia, and by dopamine, in thermodynamic governance. This involves more than balancing energy, since living things maintain their internal order by controlling transformations of energy, resisting probabilistic trends to more random states. This review will also look at recent research in PD on the analysis of entropy—an information theory metric of predictability in a message—in recordings from the basal ganglia. Close relationships between energy and information converge around the concept of entropy. This is especially relevant to the motor system, which regulates energy exchange with the outside world through its flow of information. The malignant syndrome in PD, a counterpart of neuroleptic malignant syndrome, demonstrates how much thermodynamic disruption can result from breakdown of motor signalling in an extreme hypodopaminergic state. The macroenergetic disturbances of PD are consistent with a unifying hypothesis of dopamine's neurotransmitter actions—to adapt energy expenditure to prevailing economic circumstances.

Introduction

The prevalence of weight loss as a symptom suggests that many patients deviate from normal energy balance as Parkinson's disease (PD) progresses (1). Weight is often lost in phases during its course, followed by periods of stabilisation. Occasionally, weight change is rapid and severe enough to prompt investigations for malignancy. Many patients with advanced disease have low body weight and depleted fat stores. Alterations on the energy supply side of the equation are easier to identify. Depression, cognitive impairment, and olfactory deficits diminish appetite. Chewing and swallowing are impaired in advanced PD. Altered gut motility may affect absorption of nutrients.

PD patients have involuntary motor disorders that consume muscular energy—resting tremor, static muscle activation in rigidity, drug-induced dyskinesia and dystonia. Many PD patients are less physically active than healthy individuals. There is, however, evidence that the chief parkinsonian motor sign of bradykinesia involves defective energy of movement. That the dopaminergic deficit of PD creates a problem with energy efficiency highlights the role played by the basal ganglia, and by dopamine, in thermodynamic governance.

Thermodynamics is the study of energy in transformation. Its special term entropy is not easy to define—it means dispersed or unusable energy, randomness as opposed to order. The thermal energy of a cup of coffee becomes less concentrated as it spreads to its surrounds on cooling. The cup tips over, potential energy becomes kinetic energy as the liquid splashes on the floor, coffee molecules no longer confined to a vessel but distributed randomly all about. One does not observe such processes running in reverse. Energy is conserved when it transforms, but things adopt their most probable dispositions as it does so. There is, seemingly, an exception to these thermodynamic principles—living systems, which maintain wildly improbable degrees of complexity. Organisms achieve internal order through interactions with their environments. These are the iron laws of the physical world and even conditional suspensions turn out to be temporary. Entropy resumes its relentless march with death and decomposition.

While living systems are complex, they don't directly monitor or control their complexity. What they control is energy. Molecular mechanisms achieve this at a cellular level. For animals, macroenergetics rely heavily on the nervous system. Motor programming enables energy acquisition and manages the energy usage of skeletal muscle. This article will review the thermodynamic effects of PD. In addition to changes in energy balance, we will be looking at the role of signalling within the motor system. The flow of energy in the human body is regulated by the smaller amounts of energy that are used for the flow of information. Energy and information have a deep relationship, which converges around the concept of entropy (2).

Energy and Dopamine

Virtually all animals share certain attributes—ability to move, reliance on chemical energy ultimately sourced from plants, a central control network. While survival and reproductive fitness drive the Darwinian model of evolution, an animal's ability to capture energy is a vital pre-condition for success. Adaptive behaviours, whatever their goal, require energy efficiency in execution.

Dopamine's fundamental role may be as a token of energy. The idea of dopamine as the neurochemical of reward is well-accepted. Human impulse control disorders, resulting from direct or indirect overstimulation of dopamine receptors, emphasise hedonistic and addictive behaviours (3). Mesolimbic and mesocortical projections are the dopaminergic inputs to this “reward system,” which integrates cognitive, emotional and motor planning resources into complex behavioural responses.

Evidence from rodent experiments points to motor energy as the link between dopamine and food reward (4). Blocking or depleting dopamine in rats reduces the amount of physical but not cognitive effort that the animals will devote to obtaining food (5). The effects do not resemble those of appetite suppression by pre-feeding or by drugs, and hedonistic reactivity to sucrose is not sensitive to dopamine blockade (6, 7). Mice rendered hyperdopaminergic by knockdown of the dopamine transporter gene expend more energy to gain food (8). If availability is not constrained, this effort does not translate into an overall increase in consumption (they consume larger, less frequent meals). These experiments can be interpreted as showing that increased dopamine made the animals undervalue the energy outlaid on reward-seeking activities—the nexus between effort and reward gained was weaker, not stronger. But when food is harder to obtain, normal homeostatic control occurs, suppressing activities with unfavourable cost-benefit ratios (9). As will be discussed below, simple movements seem also to show an energy thrift effect, inappropriately prominent in PD and resulting in a loss of “vigor.”

Energy-marginal environments have pertained to much of human evolution (10). The progressive encephalization in humans and other primates involved energy trade-offs with gut size (11). Late life energy stresses associated with the human trait of longevity may predispose to neurodegeneration. Dopamine, in regulating movement and reward-seeking, has been integral across species to balancing energy outlay and input. Its functions are strongly conserved by evolution. Lamprey, jawless fish of ancient lineage, have, of all vertebrates, the most distant phylogenetic relationship with humans. The last common ancestor lived 560 million years ago. Regulating the restricted repertoire of lamprey motor activity, dopamine modifies striatal output with the same general pattern as mammals—a direct pathway with neurons expressing D1 receptors, and a D2 indirect pathway (12).

Energy Balance in PD

Some energy imbalance in PD can be traced to its motor deficits, which show a strong relationship with nigral cell loss and the resultant dopaminergic deficiency (13). The neuropathology of PD, though, affects other neuronal populations and other neurotransmitter systems. This contributes to the manifold reasons for reduced body mass in PD.

Weight Loss

Patients with PD have lower body weight in comparison with age-matched subjects, and are more frequently underweight (1, 14). A systematic review showed that between 0 and 24% of PD patients were malnourished, while a further 3–60% were at risk of malnutrition (15). Weight loss across the disease course is about 3–6 kg but can be as high as 12 kg (16). Malnutrition and unintended weight loss seem to correlate with disease progression (17), and are negatively associated with quality of life (18).

Low body mass index is correlated with lower dopamine transporter activity (19). There is an inverse relationship between body mass index changes and Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale motor score (20). Moderate or severe dyskinesia is also a risk factor for undernutrition (21).

Non-motor symptoms have been linked with weight loss in PD patients (22). Some studies show association of low protein intake with a lower olfactory score (23, 24). Impaired gastric emptying and bacterial overgrowth from small intestine dysmotility could affect nutrition, though correlations with body mass index were weak in a small number of studies (2527). PD patients with cognitive impairment or depression are at increased risk of weight loss and malnutrition, presumably because of effects on appetite and feeding (2830). Dysphagia may contribute to weight loss (31).

The mesolimbic dopamine system is modulated by nutritionally significant hormones (32). Leptin, which decreases food intake, can change food desire. Leptin attenuates the response of brain reward regions to food stimuli, and enhances activation of regions involved in cognitive inhibition. Ghrelin, on the other hand, is a hormone that stimulates food intake by enhancing the hedonic and incentive responses to food-related cues. Leptin and ghrelin levels were lower in PD patients who had lost weight (33).

In a prospective study, PD patients began to lose weight loss shortly before the diagnosis (17). They tended to increase their energy intake as their weight fell. Functional neurosurgery is associated with weight gain, and this effect is stronger for procedures that target the subthalamic nucleus (STN) (34). There may be a correlation with reduced dyskinetic movement (35, 36). After deep brain stimulation (DBS), weight gain appears to occur without significant increase in appetite or food intake (37).

Resting Energy Expenditure

Resting energy expenditure (REE), the energy output in the absence of physical activity, represents about 60% of daily energy output (38). While some of this is basal metabolic activity, contributions from skeletal muscle to total expenditure are considerable (Figure 1). Involuntary motor features of PD—tremor, rigidity, and drug-induced dyskinesia—could elevate energy consumption at rest. Evidence of abnormal REE in PD is, however, inconsistent. Indirect calorimetry estimates metabolic energy use from concentrations of oxygen and carbon dioxide in exhaled air. In studies that have shown increased REE, (41, 42) rigidity rather than tremor correlated better with REE measured in off states. Capecci et al. (43) found that REE fell by 8% after doses of dopaminergic medication, although subjects in another study who developed active on phase dyskinesia had a rise in their REE (41). Other researchers have found normal REE in PD, even in patients who had been losing weight (44, 45).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Components of energy expenditure in non-obese healthy adults (39, 40). EAT, exercise activity thermogenesis; NEAT, non-exercise activity thermogenesis (low-level physical activities of daily living, including some locomotion, upper limb movement, postural tone and maintenance); TEF—thermal effects of food (digestion and secondary metabolism). Skeletal muscle contributes roughly 20% of basal metabolic activity.

Reduction in tremor, rigidity and dyskinesia after DBS surgery could all have contributed to the reduced REE shown in several studies (35, 36, 46). Another, though, reported no change in REE after STN-DBS (47).

Physical Activity and Energy

While physical activity is probably reduced overall in PD (44), comparisons of exercise at standard workloads point to an inefficiency of movement-related energy expenditure. Across various speeds of treadmill walking, the oxygen consumption rate of parkinsonian subjects exceeded that of normal controls by a 6–10% margin (48). Oxygen consumption at rest was no different in this study, suggesting that reduced walking economy in PD relates to physical activity and not to tremor or rigidity. Kalifa et al. (49) compared early PD with control subjects on matched cycling at moderate intensity and also showed increased oxygen consumption with exercise. In another cycling study, parkinsonian patients reached similar maximum oxygen consumption to controls, but at lower power outputs, again consistent with reduced economy of movement (50).

Bradykinesia and Energy of Movement

Bradykinesia is a shorthand for complex disturbances of initiation and execution of actions and the ability to sustain them (51). Akinesia (failure of initiation) and hypokinesia (underactive movement) both relate to bradykinesia, as does the sequence effect—repetitive movements becoming smaller or slower. Slowness itself is not always present, as is seen in the phenomenon of festination—a gait that hastens by rapid, small steps—or spoken words that run together at speed and are hard to understand. Loss of motor energy may be its defining character.

Fast goal-directed limb movements are executed by triphasic bursts of muscle activation in an agonist-antagonist-agonist sequence (52). In PD, this basic motor architecture is intact, but burst size is inadequate to impel the limb to its destination (53). Additional burst cycles need to be recruited, and the movement is deficient in acceleration and peak velocity. Movements are always underscaled in relation to their intended speed and range, though not necessarily in absolute terms (54). Thus, a large movement might comprise bursts that would have been adequate for a normal smaller one.

Parkinsonian movement occurs over an abnormally narrow dynamic range, consistent with reduced “motor motivation” (55). Rapid movements made by healthy subjects can be shown to be influenced by both speed-accuracy and energy cost trade-offs. PD patients balance speed and accuracy normally but assign a higher energetic cost to movement (56). The energy expense of a motor command corresponds to the force generated by the encoded action. In upper limb force-matching tasks, PD patients underestimate the force generated by the contralateral hand, experiencing the same subjective sense of effort at lower energy output than controls (57). Using a force-based definition of motor energy, Tinaz et al. (58) demonstrated that the sequence effect of bradykinesia can be explained as an inability to meet the cumulative energetic demand of a repetitive task. Simultaneous dual tasks seem to magnify the problem of deficient motor energy, with PD patients struggling sufficiently to energise a second task (59). Higher motor planning is affected—PD patients cannot augment motor effort to track unpredictable targets in a choice reaction time task (60). It has been hypothesised that changes in electroencephalographic beta oscillation power in relation to movement is a cerebral cortical manifestation of abnormal energy regulation in PD (61).

In PD, therefore, patterns of voluntary muscle activation are consistent with a scaling back of motor commands, as if to conserve energy based on inappropriately high estimations of energetic cost. More normal scaling can be temporarily restored by additional attentional resources mediated by sensory cues (62). Movement speed in PD is responsive to behavioural motivation, as it is in healthy subjects. In PD, reward had a weaker effect on energising movement than an aversive stimulus (63), recalling the kinesia paradoxica effect first reported in the pre-levodopa era (64). Avoidance is the opposite of reward-seeking, although both are subject to a cost-benefit calculation about how much energy should be invested in an action.

The usual movements of parkinsonian subjects look slow and under-powered. Yet recruitment of additional muscle contraction is required to complete a hypokinetic action (53), and underinvestment of energy may in the end make it more costly to move. Oxygen consumption research previously cited is consistent with a degree of inefficiency and wastefulness.

Cellular Energy Considerations

Abnormalities of energy metabolism, particularly mitochondrial function (65), have been theorised in the aetiology of PD. Certain mitochondrial defects could also increase the basal metabolic component of REE in PD by causing a degree of uncoupling of oxidative phosphorylation, and there is some evidence that brain heat production is increased in PD (66). Neuropathological research alludes to possible energy influences in selective neurodegeneration. As Braak pointed out, the neurons that are susceptible to α-synuclein deposition share certain characteristics (67). All are projection neurons that have axons disproportionately long and thin for their cell body size, and are unmyelinated or poorly myelinated (68, 69). A long, thinly myelinated nerve fibre requires more energy for impulse transmission than a shorter well-myelinated one (70, 71). For neurons of the substantia nigra, two additional factors are at play—an energy-expensive “pacemaker” function (72), and a high degree of axonal arborisation (73, 74). A broader discussion about oxidative stress (75) and other molecular mechanisms for nerve cell degeneration in PD that concern energy production (76) is, however, beyond the scope of this review.

Entropy and the Motor System

Living things exist because they can restrict themselves to a narrow range of improbable states. They do this by acquiring energy in an organised, usable form—free energy—then by controlling its transition to an unavailable form, the random molecular motion of heat. In animals, movement occupies a central place in this process.

Thermodynamic Entropy

Entropy means disorder, medical students are taught in their foundation scientific studies. Thermodynamics started with the Industrial Revolution and enquiries into the conversion of the energy of steam into mechanical work by cyclical engines. Ludwig Boltzmann (1844–1906), the Austrian physicist who gave entropy its commonly used mathematical expression, considered the degree of statistical randomness of molecules in an enclosed gas. This notional quantity defines the entropy of the system. If all particles are moving with complete unpredictability, the entropy is maximal. It is much more likely for gas molecules to behave this way than, say, all line up on one side of the container. The second law of thermodynamics states that entropy does not decrease in a closed system. The universe tends to an ever-greater state of disorder, the probability of this process corresponding to the arrow of time. Biological systems maintain their order while increasing entropy in their surroundings by a greater degree.

Boltzmann's idea of a list of “information” about the particles of a gas (Figure 2), with the longest list corresponding to the most probable, most random, highest entropy macrostate, leads on to the use of the term in information theory.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Boltzmann imagined a container of gas as a set of “microstates,” based on the position and momentum of its individual molecules. In his formula for entropy S, W represents the number of microstates, were it possible to count them.

Information Entropy

At the end of World War 2, the mathematician Claude Shannon (1916–2001), who had worked on cryptography during the conflict, turned his attention to peacetime communication applications. His 1948 publication “A Mathematical Theory of Communication” (77), concerning coding and compression in the transmission of data, was a starting point of the discipline of information theory. Recognising the essentially probabilistic character of “entropy,” he fashioned a usage that disconnected the term from the physical world—the degree of randomness or unpredictability in a stream of symbols that compose a message. Shannon's entropy determines the flow of information, his term henceforth italicised, for it carries another shade of meaning from its “facts provided or learned” dictionary definition.

As illustrated in Figure 3, information is the reduction of uncertainty by transmission of data that could not easily be known or predicted. Synonymous with information in information theory is surprise—that which is not expected. A message is informative by the degree to which its content is surprising. The expression for information entropy has a resemblance to Boltzmann's equation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Three coins have known but different probability profiles. Each is to be tossed 10 times. A sender wants to transmit the results in a binary stream, 1 = heads, 0 = tails. Bottom right: a general expression for information entropy H takes the probabilities P(x) of the outcomes of a variable X.

Shannon's principles were developed for the intentional communication of meaningful messages. Entropy quantifies unpredictability and describes the performance of a channel for any and all messages that might be sent over it. Information refers to how much (not what) has been understood from an actual message. A truly random distribution of symbols, could, with appropriate coding, transmit the maximum amount of information, and could not losslessly be compressed. Entropy, though, is the theoretical capacity to send information; meaningless random noise has high entropy but does not inform because uncertainty about the vast number of possible messages it might contain cannot be reduced. Alphabetic English has lower entropy (E and T are common, X and Z rare, Q is nearly always followed by U, etc.); this degree of predictability places a limit on information transfer though it allows some compression (as in Morse code—E and T are sent by single keystrokes while Z needs 4).

Energy and Information

Information theory is a branch of mathematics and thermodynamics describes properties of the physical world. Yet information and energy have links that go well beyond some parallels in their mathematical formulae. Information needs to be registered, processed or erased by a physical system. This requires energy, usually electromagnetic, to be dispersed. It is possible to calculate rough comparisons between information gained (in bits) and energy transformed (in joules) for the transmission of digital message between two smartphones. It costs about 10,000 ATP molecules to send one bit of information across a synapse in the retina of a fly (79).

Communication of information requires energy and transformations of energy involve information. What distinguishes usable, or free, energy from the randomness of thermodynamic equilibrium is information—reduced uncertainty. Carbohydrate molecules possess information because it is known that their atoms are bound together in a certain way and not all moving randomly about. In this sense also, information is physical (80).

As will be discussed, a “free energy” characterisation of information underpins an ambitious theory about self-organisation in the nervous system.

Entropy Measurements of Biological Data

Biological data in a time series have dynamic irregularities that are not captured by “linear” time and frequency analysis. By considering the degree of randomness vs. predictability in a signal, Shannon's information entropy concepts can be used to infer additional layers of temporal organisation in nervous system activity. There are a number of entropy metrics, beginning with Pincus's 1991 work on approximate entropy (81). They quantify repetition as a marker of predictability by comparing short segments of data in a time series (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Approximate Entropy algorithm applied to an annual rainfall (mm) series for Melbourne. Statistical properties of marginal probabilities are a shortcut to the probability character of a whole sample. A block size m and scaling or filter parameter r are selected. Here, m = 2, and r = 25% of the standard deviation. Blocks are assessed in turn for matches—BOTH corresponding numbers must lie within the ± 36 filter. In the top row, Block 1 matches with Block 17, and Block 2 with Block 15. In the bottom row, m has been raised by one—all 3 numbers must correspond to within ± 36. Block 2 with Block 15 is a match. All m = 2 and 3 matches are then tallied to calculate Approximate Entropy. Interested readers can find theoretical and practical treatments of information entropy measurement elsewhere (78).

The question then arises as to what biological quality is being captured by entropy analysis. Entropy, because it gives additional capacity for coded information, is sometimes considered a measurement of complexity (82). The words, though, are not synonymous. A system that is simple and uniform is likely to have a regular, relatively predictable output of low information entropy. But a completely random system, with no attributes of biological complexity, would transmit a structureless, high entropy signal. True biological complexity sits at an intermediate point on this spectrum. Diseases of the motor system could cause alterations in both directions—loss of processing capacity resulting in simplified output (of low entropy) that imposes rigid, less adaptable control; or more random, higher entropy signals that reflect network disarray.

Entropy Measurements in PD

Analysis of gait from wearable accelerometers during normal living shows increased entropy in PD (83). Accelerometer recordings of standing postural stability show the same trend—sample entropy in all three axes is significantly greater than for healthy older subjects (84).

Tremor, a predictably regular phenomenon in its own right, shows different entropy tendencies to gait and balance. Kinematic and electromyographic recordings of parkinsonian tremor have lower approximate entropy compared with physiological tremor in healthy controls (85, 86). Both STN-DBS and dopaminergic medication increase entropy of parkinsonian tremor (87, 88).

Recordings of neuronal activity in the basal ganglia show a fairly consistent pattern—the parkinsonian state has increased entropy. This is seen in interspike intervals, which presumably transmit coded data. Intra-operative recordings from the globus pallidus interna during DBS procedures show higher neuronal approximate entropy in PD compared with dystonic patients (89). In the MPTP primate model of PD, DBS of the STN reduces entropy in globus pallidus interna neuronal activity (90). During intra-operative recordings in PD patients undergoing DBS procedures, entropy in STN firing pattern went down when doses of subcutaneous apomorphine that ameliorated parkinsonism without inducing dyskinesia were administered (91). Recordings from freely ambulant parkinsonian patients implanted with STN-DBS showed, under medication-off and DBS-off conditions, greater entropy in those subject to gait freezing, especially when freezing was actually occurring (92). Electroencephalographic activity in PD shows increased entropy in comparison to healthy controls (93).

The concept of information entropy is independent of coding, content, and type of channel. What can be surmised in PD is that the configuration of the flow of information about energy of movement is altered.

Darbin et al. (94) have proposed an entropy hypothesis for neuronal firing in basal ganglia disorders. Low entropy neuronal activity could result in hyperlegible messaging and hyperkinetic motor output. On the other hand, high entropy signals, as observed in PD, give rise to hypokinesia. A high entropy channel could carry more coded information, or more random noise. But less predictable signals would likely require greater processing to decipher them. They would also be less compressible without loss of meaning, interfering with the way that motor transmissions must be funnelled from larger to smaller neuronal populations when descending the motor control hierarchy (95). High entropy in the parkinsonian basal ganglia appears to result in “pathological” information transfer, partly rectified by dopaminergic drugs or partly blocked by DBS (96).

Predictive Coding of Movement and Free-Energy

The brain is a device that tests hypotheses about the external world. According to this theory, neural circuits employ a version of Bayes' rule for the conditional probability of events, whereby expectations—predictive beliefs, or prior probabilities—are updated in response to new evidence. This has become an important principle in understanding human perception, and in machine learning programmes that try to mimic it. Bayesian perceptual inference may approximate a neural coding principle underlying a continuous process of prediction error minimisation between forecast and actual sensory input that operates across all levels of the hierarchy of the central nervous system (97, 98).

Predictive processing encompasses the control of movement. Prediction error minimisation can also be achieved by changing sensation through action to make it fit with expectations—active inference. That is, the brain anticipates the proprioceptive outcome of a motor command. The theory posits a simple algorithm enabling the brain's complex hierarchical balance between “top down” projections of expectation and “bottom up” sensory traffic. While it is possible to show that perception and movement function as if Bayesian hypothesis-testing is embedded in the neural circuitry, it is not clear how such processing might have become established and evolved to its present level of sophistication.

Karl Friston, one of the originators of the predictive coding account of brain activity, has appealed to thermodynamics for an answer to this question (99). Living systems exist by restricting themselves to a small range of improbable states. By creating boundaries with the disordered outside world, and by exchanging energy and matter with it, they cause these to be the most probable internal arrangements. As Claude Shannon used “entropy” as a term for randomness in a data stream, so Friston links the prediction error of predictive coding with the thermodynamic variable of free energy. Free-energy (italicised to distinguish from its thermodynamic counterpart), he writes, “is an information theory quantity that bounds the evidence for a model of data” (99).

In thermodynamics, free energy is the energy available to do work. The relevant equation can be paraphrased;

Free Energy Equals Total Energy Minus Entropy

In performing mechanical or chemical work, some free energy is dissipated into entropy—the random molecular motion of heat—which, in a closed system, is unavailable to do work. In an inefficient engine, or an explosive chemical reaction, nearly all free energy might be so dispersed. Biochemical free energy, which drives the direction of reactions, is minimised when a protein folds to its functional 3-dimensional conformation. The probability of finding a molecular system in one state as opposed to another is determined by the difference in their free energies (100). It is therefore more probable that proteins occupy desirably folded steady states than undergo further thermodynamic transition. Living systems minimise free energy to maximise the chance that their complex, improbable elements will retain their stability.

An accurate model of an action yields predictable incoming sensory signals of low “surprise” content. Yet the brain has no innate yardstick for entropy, or surprise. Rather, it controls the “free energy” character of information encoded in probability distributions of expectation, which is minimised by reducing prediction error. If returning signals are unsurprising, probability distributions don't require updating to new data. Using complex mathematics, Friston shows that calculations based on free-energy, which places an upper limit on surprise, approximate Bayes' rule. Free-energy, abstract informational quantity, exists through actual neuronal energy, which is needed to code its probability estimates (80). Accurate predictions are frugal with thermodynamic free energy, since little additional neuronal activity is needed to modify these distributions to unsurprising feedback. Friston says that any self-organising system that is at nonequilibrium steady state with its environment must, behind physical and statistical boundaries, minimise its free-energy (101). By doing so, it will continue to occupy its small set of preferred states.

The Free-energy principle has been described as a piece of philosophical-mathematical reasoning that seeks to explain nervous and mental phenomena (102). At another level, it says that life depends on exploiting relationships between energy and information to achieve thermodynamic outcomes. This cursory and unmathematical synopsis of Free-energy shows what might be happening in sensori-motor neurotransmission, though the theory can only be applied conceptually to PD. Underscaled bradykinetic movements that have to be augmented by secondary muscle activation, or that decrement on repetition, imply that free-energy is not being minimised in PD. “Surprising” sensory feedback delivers continual prediction errors on actions that fall short of expectation.

Malignant Syndromes of PD as Thermodynamic Crises

In its fully developed form, neuroleptic malignant syndrome is a life-threatening combination of pyrexia, rigidity with rhabdomyolysis, autonomic instability and altered mental state (103). There are milder versions, with a continuum that extends from severe drug-induced parkinsonism. Dopaminergic D2 receptor blockade by antipsychotic drugs is likely to be the primary cause, and peripheral explanations around skeletal muscle fibre toxicity are less satisfactory. This conclusion is reinforced by the occurrence of neuroleptic malignant-like states in PD (104108). Sudden withdrawal of dopaminergic therapy, intercurrent infection and dehydration are the most common triggers. It can emerge from profound off phases in patients with motor fluctuations (109). Virtually all of the features of a neuroleptic drug-induced malignant state are reproduced in its PD counterpart (110).

Energy dysregulation is central to the malignant syndrome in PD. The metaphor of “meltdown” captures the runaway thermal effects. In a state of extreme parkinsonism, there little capacity for muscles to perform voluntary mechanical work. Instead, abnormal muscle contraction liberates large amounts of heat energy. Thermoregulation is overwhelmed. Some inbuilt load-limiting governor is overridden, muscles exceed their metabolic safety points, and start to break down.

Malignant syndromes underline the extent of the energy resources normally held in harness by the motor system. They bring into relief thermodynamic roles of dopamine in the basal ganglia by showing the consequences of these extreme hypodopaminergic states. Chemical energy stores in skeletal muscle are dissipated as entropy, while the complexity of muscle cells themselves is degraded by rhabdomyolysis. Information entropy of motor signals in malignant syndromes have not been studied, but some inferences can be made. Continuous involuntary skeletal muscle activation implies motor messages dominated by random output, far from the minimal signal-dependent noise of optimum control (111). A crisis of thermodynamic entropy is brought about by the breakdown of information flow within the motor system.

Conclusions

PD predisposes to negative energy balance across its course. While impaired nutritional intake contributes, there are abnormalities on the energy output side. These relate to core motor aspects of the disease, for which the dopaminergic deficit is responsible. The elusive character of bradykinesia is best explained as an under-investment of energy of movement. But when activities are standardised for work performed, PD patients paradoxically expend more energy, suggesting that these inappropriate energy thrift settings are ultimately wasteful.

To subtract energy output from input is to consider energy balance in PD as one might for an engine. This highlights important differences from living things, with their improbable defiance of the second law of thermodynamics. They resist its dispersive effects by managing transformations of their energy. Entropy measurements disclose nothing about the coding scheme or content of basal ganglia neural signals. These information flows are controlling energy exchange through muscular work. Increased neuronal entropy in PD, which coincides with hypokinesia, can be seen as a marker of defective energy transfer.

These enquiries into thermodynamic aspects of PD align with a unifying hypothesis of dopamine's neurotransmitter actions (8)—to adapt energy expenditure to prevailing economic circumstances by influencing the traffic of information about energy.

Author Contributions

PK and LP-D contributed to the conception, writing and editing of this manuscript, and do agree to be accountable for the content of the work. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Bachmann CG, Trenkwalder C. Body weight in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2006) 21:1824–30. doi: 10.1002/mds.21068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Tribus M, McIrvine EC. Energy and information. Sci Am. (1971) 225:179–90. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0971-179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lopez AM, Weintraub D, Claassen DO. Impulse control disorders and related complications of Parkinson's disease therapy. Semin Neurol. (2017) 37:186–92. doi: 10.1055/s-0037-1601887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Salamone JD, Yohn SE, López-Cruz L, San Miguel N, Correa M. Activational and effort-related aspects of motivation: neural mechanisms and implications for psychopathology. Brain. (2016) 139:1325–47. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hosking JG, Floresco SB, Winstanley CA. Dopamine antagonism decreases willingness to expend physical, but not cognitive, effort: a comparison of two rodent cost/benefit decision-making tasks. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2015) 40:1005–15. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Pecina S, Cagniard B, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW, Zhuang X. P54 Hyperdopaminergic mutant mice have higher 'wanting' but not 'liking' for sweet rewards. J Neurosci. (2003) 23:9395–402. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-28-09395.2003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron. (2015) 86:646–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Beeler JA, Frazier CRM, Zhuang X. Putting desire on a budget: dopamine and energy expenditure, reconciling reward and resources. Front Integr Neurosci. (2012) 6:49. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Beeler JA, Frazier CRM, Zhuang X. Dopaminergic enhancement of local food-seeking is under global homeostatic control. Eur J Neurosci. (2012) 35:146–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07916.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Leonard WR, Ulijaszek SJ. Energetics and evolution: an emerging research domain. Am J Hum Biol. (2002) 14:547–50. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.10068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Aiello LC, Wheeler P. The expensive-tissue hypothesis. Curr Anthropol. (1995) 36:199–221. doi: 10.1086/204350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Grillner S, Robertson B, Stephenson-Jones M. The evolutionary origin of the vertebrate basal ganglia and its role in action selection. J Physiol. (2013) 591:5425–31. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.246660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Greffard S, Verny M, Bonnet A-M, Beinis Y-B, Gallinari C, Meaume S, et al. Motor score of the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale as a good predictor of Lewy body-associated neuronal loss in the substantia nigra. Arch Neurol. (2006) 63:584–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.4.584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kashihara K. Weight loss in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. (2006) 253:38–41. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-7009-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Sheard JM, Ash S, Silburn PA, Kerr GK. Prevalence of malnutrition in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review. Nutr Rev. (2011) 69:520–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2011.00413.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Beyer PL, Palarino MY, Michalek D, Busenbark K, Koller WC. Weight change and body composition in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Am Diet Assoc. (1995) 95:979–83. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(95)00269-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Chen H, Zhang SM, Hernán MA, Willett WC, Ascherio A. Weight loss in Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. (2003) 53:676–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.10577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Akbar U, He Y, Dai Y, Hack N, Malaty I, McFarland NR, et al. Weight loss and impact on quality of life in Parkinson's disease. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0124541. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lee YF. Antipsychotic-induced Pisa syndrome: a 2-year follow-up study. Clin Neuropharmacol. (2018) 41:60–3. doi: 10.1097/WNF.0000000000000274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wills AMA, Pérez A, Wang J, Mang J, Su X, Morgan J, et al. Association between change in body mass index, Unified Parkinson's disease rating scale scores, and survival among persons with Parkinson disease. JAMA Neurol. (2016) 73:321–8. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.4265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Markus HS, Tomkins AM, Stern GM. Increased prevalence of undernutrition in Parkinson's disease and its relationship to clinical disease parameters. J Neural Transm (P-D Sect). (1993) 5:117–25. doi: 10.1007/BF02251202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Aiello M, Eleopra R, Rumiati RI. Body weight and food intake in Parkinson's disease. A review of the association to non-motor symptoms. Appetite. (2015) 84:204–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ådén E, Carlsson M, Poortvliet E, Stenlund H, Linder J, Edström M, et al. Dietary intake and olfactory function in patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson's disease: a case-control study. Nutr Neurosci. (2011) 14:25–31. doi: 10.1179/174313211X12966635733312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sharma JC, Turton J. Olfaction, dyskinesia and profile of weight change in Parkinson's disease: identifying neurodegenerative phenotypes. Park Relat Disord. (2012) 18:964–70. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gabrielli M, Bonazzi P, Scarpellini E, Bendia E, Lauritano EC, Fasano A, et al. Prevalence of small intestinal bacterial overgrowth in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2011) 26:889–92. doi: 10.1002/mds.23566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Tan AH, Mahadeva S, Thalha AM, Gibson PR, Kiew CK, Yeat CM, et al. Small intestinal bacterial overgrowth in Parkinson's disease. Park Relat Disord. (2014) 20:535–40. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fasano A, Visanji NP, Liu LWC, Lang AE, Pfeiffer RF. Gastrointestinal dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. (2015) 14:625–39. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00007-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lorefält B, Ganowiak W, Pålhagen S, Toss G, Unosson M, Granérus A-K. Factors of importance for weight loss in elderly patients with Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Scand. (2004) 110:180–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2004.00307.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Sheard JM, Ash S, Mellick GD, Silburn PA, Kerr GK. Malnutrition in a sample of community-dwelling people with Parkinson's disease. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e53290. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pilhatsch M, Kroemer NB, Schneider C, Ebersbach G, Jost WH, Fuchs G, et al. Reduced body mass index in Parkinson's disease: contribution of comorbid depression. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2013) 201:76–9. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31827ab2cc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lorefält B, Ganowiak W, Wissing U, Granérus A-K, Unosson M. Food habits and intake of nutrients in elderly patients with Parkinson's disease. Gerontology. (2006) 52:160–8. doi: 10.1159/000091825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: implications for obesity. Trends Cogn. Sci. (2011) 15:37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fiszer U, Michałowska M, Baranowska B, Wolińska-Witort E, Jeske W, Jethon M, et al. Leptin and ghrelin concentrations and weight loss in Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Scand. (2010) 121:230–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2009.01185.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Locke MC, Wu SS, Foote KD, Sassi M, Jacobson CE, Rodriguez RL, et al. Weight changes in subthalamic nucleus vs globus pallidus internus deep brain stimulation: results from the COMPARE Parkinson's disease DBS cohort. Neurosurgery. (2011) 68:1233–7. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0b013e31820b52c5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Macia F, Perlemoine C, Coman I, Guehl D, Burbaud P, Cuny E, et al. Parkinson's disease patients with bilateral subthalamic deep brain stimulation gain weight. Mov Disord. (2004) 9:206–12. doi: 10.1002/mds.10630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Montaurier C, Morio B, Bannier S, Derost P, Arnaud P, Brandolini-Bunlon M, et al. Mechanisms of body weight gain in patients with Parkinson's disease after subthalamic stimulation. Brain. (2007) 130:1808–18. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kistner A, Lhommée E, Krack P. Mechanisms of body weight fluctuations in Parkinson's disease. Front Neurol. (2014) 5:84. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hills AP, Mokhtar N, Byrne NM. Assessment of physical activity and energy expenditure: an overview of objective measures. Front Nutr. (2014) 1:5. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2014.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Levine JA. Non-exercise activity thermogenesis (NEAT). Nutr Rev. (2004) 62:S82–S97. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2004.tb00094.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Soares MJ, Müller MJ. Resting energy expenditure and body composition: critical aspects for clinical nutrition. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2018) 72:1208–14. doi: 10.1038/s41430-018-0220-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Levi S, Cox M, Lugon M, Hodkinson M, Tomkins A. Increased energy expenditure in Parkinson's disease. Br Med J. (1990) 301:1256–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.301.6763.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Markus HS, Cox M, Tomkins AM. Raised resting energy expenditure in Parkinson's disease and its relationship to muscle rigidity. Clin Sci. (1992) 83:199–204. doi: 10.1042/cs0830199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Capecci M, Petrelli M, Emanuelli B, Millevolte M, Nicolai A, Provinciali L, et al. Rest energy expenditure in Parkinson's disease: role of disease progression and dopaminergic therapy. Park Relat Disord. (2013) 19:238–41. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Toth MJ, Fishman PS, Poehlman ET. Free-living daily energy expenditure in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neurology. (1997) 48:88–91. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.1.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Delikanaki-Skaribas E, Trail M, Wong WWL, Lai EC. Daily energy expenditure, physical activity, and weight loss in Parkinson's disease patients. Mov Disord. (2009) 24:667–71. doi: 10.1002/mds.22372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Perlemoine C, Macia F, Tison F, Corman I, Ghuel D, Burbaud P, et al. Effects of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation and levodopa on energy production rate and substrate oxidation in Parkinson's disease. Br J Nutr. (2005) 93:191–8. doi: 10.1079/BJN20041297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lammers NM, Sondermeijer BM, Twickler TB, de Bie RM, Ackermans MT, Fliers E, et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation does not influence basal glucose metabolism or insulin sensitivity in patients with Parkinson's disease. Front Neurosci. (2014) 8:95. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Christiansen CL, Schenkman ML, McFann K, Wolfe P, Kohrt WM. Walking economy in people with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2009) 24:1481–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.22621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kalifa TM, Ziv N, Bergman H, Nusair S, Arkadi D. Increased energy expenditure during posture maintenance and exercise in early Parkinson disease. Health Sci Rep. (2018) 1:e14. doi: 10.1002/hsr2.14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Stanley RK, Protas EJ, Jankovic J. Exercise performance in those having Parkinson's disease and healthy normals. Med Sci Sport Exerc. (1999) 31:761–6. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199906000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hallett M. Bradykinesia: Why do Parkinson's patients have it and what trouble does it cause? Mov Disord. (2011) 26:1579–81. doi: 10.1002/mds.23730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hallett M, Shahani BT, Young RR. EMG analysis of stereotyped voluntary movements in man. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1975) 38:1154–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.38.12.1154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Hallett M, Khoshbin S. A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain. (1980) 103:301–14. doi: 10.1093/brain/103.2.301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Beraidelli A, Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD. Scaling of the size of the first agonist EMG burst during rapid wrist movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1986) 49:1273–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.49.11.1273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Baraduc P, Thobois S, Gan J, Broussolle E, Desmurget M. A common optimization principle for motor execution in healthy subjects and parkinsonian patients. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:665–77. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1482-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Mazzoni P, Hristova A, Krakauer JW. Why don't we move faster? Parkinson's disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:7105–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lafargue G, D'Amico A, Thobois S, Broussolle E, Sirigu A. The ability to assess muscular force in asymmetrical Parkinson's disease. Cortex. (2008) 44:82–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2005.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Tinaz S, Pillai AS, Hallett M. Sequence effect in Parkinson's disease is related to motor energetic cost. Front Neurol. (2016) 7:83. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Wu T, Hallett M. Neural correlates of dual task performance in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2008) 79:760–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.126599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Moisello C, Perfetti B, Marinelli L, Sanguineti V, Bove M, Feigin A, et al. Basal ganglia and kinematics modulation: insights from Parkinson's and Huntington's diseases. Park Relat Disord. (2011) 17:642–4. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ghilardi MF, Tatti E, Quartarone A. Beta power and movement-related beta modulation as hallmarks of energy for plasticity induction: implications for Parkinson's disease. Park Relat Disord. (2021) 88:136–9. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2021.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Ballanger B, Thobois S, Baraduc P, Turner RS, Broussolle E, Desmurget M. “Paradoxical kinesis” is not a hallmark of Parkinson's disease but a general property of the motor system. Mov Disord. (2006) 21:1490–5. doi: 10.1002/mds.20987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Shiner T, Seymour B, Symmonds M, Dayan P, Bhatia KP, Dolan RJ. The effect of motivation on movement: a study of bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e47138. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Froment J, Gardère H. Paradoxic parkinsonian kinesis, striatal paradoxes and disturbances of the function of stabilisation. Arch Neurol Psychiatry. (1926) 15:820–1.

65. Chen C, Turnbull DM, Reeve AK. Mitochondrial dysfunction in Parkinson's disease—cause or consequence? Biology. (2019) 8:38. doi: 10.3390/biology8020038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Rango M, Arighi A, Bonifati C, Bresolin N. Increased brain temperature in Parkinson's disease. Neuroreport. (2012) 23:129–33. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834e8fac

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Braak H, Bohl JR, Müller CM, Rüb U, de Vos RAI, Del Tredici K. Stanley Fahn Lecture 2005: the staging procedure for the inclusion body pathology associated with sporadic Parkinson's Disease Reconsidered. Mov Disord. (2006) 21:2042–51. doi: 10.1002/mds.21065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Braak H, Ghebremedhin E, Rüb U, Bratzke H, Del Tredici K. Stages in the development of Parkinson's disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Res. (2004) 318:121–34. doi: 10.1007/s00441-004-0956-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Braak H, Del Tredici K. Poor and protracted myelination as a contributory factor to neurodegenerative disorders. Neurobiol Aging. (2004) 25:19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. van der Knaap MS, Valk J, Bakker CJ, Schooneveld M, Faber JA, Willemse J, et al. Myelination as an expression of the functional maturity of the brain. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1991) 33:849–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14793.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Nieuwenhuys R. Structure and organization of fibre systems. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar JH, Nicholson C, editors. The Nervous System of Vertebrates, Vol. 1. Berlin: Springer (1999). p. 113–57.

Google Scholar

72. Surmeier DJ. Calcium, ageing, and neuronal vulnerability in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. (2007) 6:933–8. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70246-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Bolam JP, Pissadaki EK. Living on the edge with too many mouths to feed: why dopamine neurons die. Mov Disord. (2012) 27:1478–83. doi: 10.1002/mds.25135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Pissadaki E, Bolam JP. The energy cost of action potential propagation in dopamine neurons: clues to susceptibility in Parkinson's disease. Front Comput Neurosci. (2013) 7:13. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Jenner P. Oxidative stress in Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. (2003) 53(Suppl. 3):26–38. doi: 10.1002/ana.10483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Marinelli L, Trompetto C, Ghilardi MF. Brain temperature as a measure of misfolded proteins metabolism. Med Hypotheses. (2017) 101:11. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2017.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Shannon CE. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst Tech J. (1948) 27:379–423. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Delgado-Bonal A, Marshak A. Approximate entropy and sample entropy: a comprehensive tutorial. Entropy. (2019) 21:541. doi: 10.3390/e21060541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Laughlin SB, De Ruyter Van Steveninck RR, Anderson JC. The metabolic cost of neural information. Nat Neurosci. (1998) 1:36–41. doi: 10.1038/236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Sengupta B, Stemmler MB, Friston KJ. Information and efficiency in the nervous system-a synthesis. PLoS Comput Biol. (2013) 9:e1003157. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Pincus SM. Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (1991) 88:2297–301. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.6.2297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Longo G, Miquel PA, Sonnenschein C, Soto AM. Is information a proper observable for biological organization? Prog Biophys Mol. Biol. (2012) 109:108–14. doi: 10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Coates L, Shi J, Rochester L, Din SD, Pantall A. Entropy of real-world gait in Parkinson's disease determined from wearable sensors as a digital marker of altered ambulatory behavior. Sensors (Basel). (2020) 20:2631. doi: 10.3390/s20092631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Pantall A, Del Din S, Rochester L. Longitudinal changes over thirty-six months in postural control dynamics and cognitive function in people with Parkinson's disease. Gait Posture. (2018) 62:468–74. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Vaillancourt DE, Newell KM. The dynamics of resting and postural tremor in Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurophysiol. (2000) 111:2046–56. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00467-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Meigal AY, Rissanen SM, Tarvainen MP, Georgiadis SD, Karjalainen PA, Airaksinen O, et al. Linear and nonlinear tremor acceleration characteristics in patients with Parkinson's disease. Physiol Meas. (2012) 33:395–412. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/33/3/395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Sturman MM, Vaillancourt DE, Metman LV, Bakay RAE, Corcos DM. Effects of subthalamic nucleus stimulation and medication on resting and postural tremor in Parkinson's disease. Brain. (2004) 127:2131–43. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Kovacs N, Balas I, Illes Z, Kellenyi L, Doczi TP, Czopf J, et al. Uniform qualitative electrophysiological changes in postoperative rest tremor. Mov Disord. (2006) 21:318–24. doi: 10.1002/mds.20712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Sanghera ODMK, Alam M, Krauss JK, Friehs G, Jankovic J, Simpson RK, et al. Entropy measurements in pallidal neurons in dystonia and Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2012) 27(Suppl 1):S212–3. doi: 10.1002/mds.25051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Ostrem JL, Racine CA, Glass GA, Grace JK, Volz MM, Health SL. Subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in primary cervical dystonia. Neurology. (2011) 76:870–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820f2e4f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Lafreniere-Roula M, Darbin O, Hutchison WD, Wichmann T, Lozano AM, Dostrovskyet JO. Apomorphine reduces subthalamic neuronal entropy in parkinsonian patients. Exp Neurol. (2010) 225:455–8. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2010.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Syrkin-Nikolau J, Koop MM, Prieto T, Anidi C, Afzal MF, Velisar A, et al. Subthalamic neural entropy is a feature of freezing of gait in freely moving people with Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Dis. (2017) 108:288–97. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2017.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Pezard L, Jech R, RuŽička E. Investigation of non-linear properties of multichannel EEG in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurophysiol. (2001) 112:38–45. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00512-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Darbin O, Dees D, Martino A, Adams E, Naritoku D. An entropy-based model for basal ganglia dysfunctions in movement disorders. Biomed Res Int. (2013) 2013:742671. doi: 10.1155/2013/742671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Pothakos K, Kurz MJ, Lau YS. Restorative effect of endurance exercise on behavioral deficits in the chronic mouse model of Parkinson's disease with severe neurodegeneration. BMC Neurosci. (2009) 10:6. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Dorval AD, Grill WM. Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus reestablishes neuronal information transmission in the 6-OHDA rat model of parkinsonism. J Neurophysiol. (2014) 111:1949–59. doi: 10.1152/jn.00713.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Hohwy J. The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2013). 294 p.

Google Scholar

98. Clark A. Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2015). 425 p.

Google Scholar

99. Friston KJ, Stephan KE. Free-energy and the brain. Synthese. (2007) 159:417–58. doi: 10.1007/s11229-007-9237-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. van Gunsteren WF, Daura X, Mark AE. Computation of free energy. Helv Chim Acta. (2002) 85:3113–29. doi: 10.1002/1522-2675(200210)85:10<3113::AID-HLCA3113>3.0.CO;2-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci. (2010) 11:127–38. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Hohwy J. Self-supervision, normativity and the free energy principle. Synthese. (2020). doi: 10.1007/s11229-020-02622-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Tse L, Barr AM, Scarapicchia V, Vila-Rodriguez F. Neuroleptic malignant syndrome: a review from a clinically oriented perspective. Curr Neuropharmacol. (2015) 13:395–406. doi: 10.2174/1570159x13999150424113345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Toru M, Matsuda O, Makiguchu K, Sugano K. Neuroleptic malignant syndrome like state following a withdrawal of antiparkinsonian drugs. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1981) 169:324–7. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198105000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Figa Talamanca L, Gualandi C, Di Meo L, Di Battista G, Neri G, Lo Russo F. Hyperthermia after discontinuance of levodopa and bromocriptine therapy: impaired dopamine receptors a possible cause. Neurology. (1985) 35:258–61. doi: 10.1212/WNL.35.2.258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Keyser DL, Rodnitzky RL. Neuroleptic malignant syndrome in Parkinson's disease after withdrawal or alteration of dopaminergic therapy. Arch Intern Med. (1991) 151:794–6. doi: 10.1001/archinte.151.4.794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Ueda M, Hamamoto M, Nagayama H, Otsubo K, Nito C, Miyazaki T, et al. Susceptibility to neuroleptic malignant syndrome in Parkinson's disease. Neurology. (1999) 52:777–81. doi: 10.1212/WNL.52.4.777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Gordon PH, Frucht SJ. Neuroleptic malignant syndrome in advanced Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2001) 16:960–2. doi: 10.1002/mds.1166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Mizuno Y, Takubo H, Mizuta E, Kuno S. Malignant syndrome in Parkinson's disease: concept and review of the literature. Park Relat Disord. (2003) 9:3–9. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(02)00125-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Gibb WRG, Griffith DNW. Levodopa withdrawal syndrome identical to neuroleptic malignant syndrome. Postgrad Med J. (1986) 62:59–60. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.62.723.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Harris CM, Wolpert DM. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature. (1998) 394:780–5. doi: 10.1038/29528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson's disease, energy, dopamine, weight loss, thermodynamics

Citation: Kempster PA and Perju-Dumbrava L (2021) The Thermodynamic Consequences of Parkinson's Disease. Front. Neurol. 12:685314. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.685314

Received: 24 March 2021; Accepted: 04 August 2021;
Published: 26 August 2021.

Edited by:

Luigi Bubacco, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Nico Diederich, Centre Hospitalier de Luxembourg, Luxembourg
Lucio Marinelli, University of Genoa, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Kempster and Perju-Dumbrava. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Perju-Dumbrava, lauraperju@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.