- 1Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA, United States
- 2Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States
Introduction: One of the most challenging symptoms of aphasia is an impairment in auditory comprehension. The inability to understand others has a direct impact on a person's quality of life and ability to benefit from treatment. Despite its importance, limited research has examined the recovery pattern of auditory comprehension and instead has focused on aphasia recovery more generally. Thus, little is known about the time frame for auditory comprehension recovery following stroke, and whether specific neurologic and demographic variables contribute to recovery and outcome.
Methods: This study included 168 left hemisphere chronic stroke patients stroke patients with auditory comprehension impairments ranging from mild to severe. Univariate and multivariate lesion-symptom mapping (LSM) was used to identify brain regions associated with auditory comprehension outcomes on three different tasks: Single-word comprehension, yes/no sentence comprehension, and comprehension of sequential commands. Demographic variables (age, gender, and education) were also examined for their role in these outcomes. In a subset of patients who completed language testing at two or more time points, we also analyzed the trajectory of recovery in auditory comprehension using survival curve-based time compression.
Results: LSM analyses revealed that poor single-word auditory comprehension was associated with lesions involving the left mid- to posterior middle temporal gyrus, and portions of the angular and inferior-middle occipital gyri. Poor yes/no sentence comprehension was associated almost exclusively with the left mid-posterior middle temporal gyrus. Poor comprehension of sequential commands was associated with lesions in the left posterior middle temporal gyrus. There was a small region of convergence between the three comprehension tasks, in the very posterior portion of the left middle temporal gyrus. The recovery analysis revealed that auditory comprehension scores continued to improve beyond the first year post-stroke. Higher education was associated with better outcome on all auditory comprehension tasks. Age and gender were not associated with outcome or recovery slopes.
Conclusions: The current findings suggest a critical role for the posterior left middle temporal gyrus in the recovery of auditory comprehension following stroke, and that spontaneous recovery of auditory comprehension can continue well beyond the first year post-stroke.
Aphasia is a devastating language disorder that occurs in over 30% of patients who have a stroke (1, 2). Aphasia can impair an individual's ability to understand and produce language, leading to a number of social and functional challenges and decreased quality of life (3–5). Recovery from aphasia varies widely among individuals, and the factors predicting recovery are not well-understood. Current research on aphasia recovery has largely examined broad outcome measures such as overall aphasia severity, rather than specific aspects of speech and language (6–14). In particular, very little work has examined outcomes and recovery of auditory comprehension following stroke. This gap in knowledge is significant because comprehension deficits not only disrupt patients' functional abilities but can also compromise intervention and rehabilitation efforts. The present study examined the neurologic and demographic correlates of auditory comprehension outcome and recovery, using lesion-symptom mapping (LSM) and survival curve-based time compression.
Current knowledge on the recovery pattern of auditory comprehension following stroke is limited, as only a handful of studies have focused on this aspect of aphasia recovery. Moreover, when studies do focus on auditory comprehension, they typically only target the first several months post-stroke (note: in this paper, we use the term “recovery” to refer to spontaneous improvement over time and “outcome” to refer to a final chronic endpoint). Pickersgill and Lincoln (15) studied 56 chronic stroke patients (most of whom were an average of 3–4 months post-stroke) and found that most patients demonstrated significant recovery in auditory comprehension during the 2 months the study was conducted, with severely aphasic patients showing the greatest changes in auditory comprehension scores (15). Recovery after this 2-month assessment period was not examined. In another study, Mazzoni and colleagues assessed 45 stroke patients at 15 days, 1, 3–4, and once more at 6–7 months post-stroke. Findings indicated that spontaneous recovery of auditory comprehension occurred primarily within the first four months post-stroke (16). This finding is also supported by a small study with 22 mild-to-severe acute stroke patients who were given either the dictated commands test from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Exam or the sequential commands task of the Western Aphasia Battery at baseline and 90 days post-stroke. Results revealed that all of the patients had improved after 90 days, but no additional follow-ups were completed (17). In a more recent pilot study that assessed six patients at 2, 16, and 190 days post-stroke, results indicated that patients showed spontaneous recovery of auditory comprehension two-weeks post-stroke, but no significant changes were found at the 190-day/6-month assessment (18). Prins et al. (19) measured recovery of sentence comprehension and spontaneous speech up to a year post-stroke and found that patients continued to improve in comprehension (though not spontaneous speech) at each of the three assessments conducted over the year. In short, there is limited information on the trajectory of spontaneous recovery of auditory comprehension deficits associated with aphasia, particularly with respect to recovery after the first year, and the few results examining auditory comprehension recovery are mixed with respect to the degree and time course of recovery.
While many studies have attempted to identify the neural correlates of aphasia outcome and recovery, fewer studies have focused on the neural substrates of auditory comprehension specifically. In an early study by Selnes et al. (20), poor sentence comprehension outcome at 6 months post-stroke was associated with lesions in the left posterior superior temporal and supramarginal gyri. Similarly, Naeser et al. (21) found that sentence comprehension was most affected by posterior superior temporal gyrus lesions. This is supported by recent studies that similarly have found that the most critical regions for sentence comprehension were the left posterior superior temporal gyrus and inferior parietal cortex (22, 23). Dronkers et al. (24) found that comprehension of simple declarative sentences was dependent on the left posterior middle temporal gyrus; but a network of additional regions was critical for more complex syntactic processing, including left temporo-parietal cortex and lateral prefrontal regions. The importance of these regions for sentence comprehension are also supported in more recent lesion studies examining deficits in comprehension of complex sentences and syntactic structures (i.e., agrammatism). In these studies, patients with acute and chronic stroke similarly display more deficits in auditory sentencent studies that simila comprehension when damage was located in posterior middle temporal gyrus (25) and temporo-parietal cortex regions (26, 27), with one study also adding the importance of occipital and underlying white matter regions for completing auditory-visual sentence processing (28). With respect to single-word comprehension (29), Selnes et al. assessed patients at 1, 3, and 6 months post-stroke and found that even patients with damage to posterior superior temporal cortex showed good outcomes. Consistent with this finding, a recent study by Bonilha et al. (30) found rather that single-word comprehension deficits in chronic stroke patients were associated with lesions in the left inferior temporal and fusiform gyri and posterior temporal white matter. In sum, a number of different outcome studies have identified regions critical for single-word and sentence comprehension, but studies have typically focused on chronic outcome and either on single-word or sentence comprehension. Generally, sentence-level comprehension has been associated with left posterior temporo-parietal cortex and single-word comprehension with more inferior and sometimes anterior temporal lesions (31). In the current study, we had the opportunity to identify brain regions associated with both single-word auditory comprehension and two different types of sentence-level comprehension (sequential commands and yes/no questions) in a large group of patients, in order to determine the extent to which these different aspects of auditory comprehension are reliant on distinct brain regions.
With respect to demographic predictors, previous studies that have assessed general aphasia recovery have found that variables such as younger age, being female, higher educational attainment, and lower initial stroke severity are associated with better recovery (5, 32–37). However, the role of these demographic factors in the recovery and outcome of auditory comprehension deficits in aphasia more specifically has not been well-studied and findings have been mixed. Pickersgill and Lincoln (15) reported small or no correlations between age and recovery of auditory comprehension in chronic aphasia patients, while Lazar et al. (17) found no age, education, or gender associations with outcome or recovery of auditory comprehension at baseline or 3 months post-stroke. In contrast, a larger study with 173 acute stroke patients tested on a battery of language tests, including a word-recognition auditory comprehension task, found that acute stroke patients with at least 12 years of education had higher auditory comprehension outcome scores than those with less than a high school education (34).
The present study addressed some of the gaps in the current literature on outcome and recovery of auditory comprehension by analyzing retrospective data from a large, well-characterized group of stroke patients with a wide range of auditory comprehension deficits. First, we utilized univariate and multivariate LSM, a well-established lesion mapping approach (38), to examine neuroanatomical associations of three different aspects of auditory comprehension, including both single-word and sentence-level comprehension. Second, in a subgroup of patients who were tested at multiple timepoints, we quantified the trajectory of auditory comprehension recovery using survival curve-based time compression. This analysis allowed us to capture patients' longitudinal trajectory of recovery during and beyond the first year post-stroke. Last, we analyzed the impact of demographic variables on outcome and recovery of auditory comprehension deficits following stroke.
Methods
Participants
Chronic Outcome Group
Language and brain imaging data were retrospectively analyzed from 168 patients in our stroke research database who met the following criteria: History of a single left hemisphere stroke; pre-morbidly right-handed; native English speaker; at least 12 months post-stroke; minimum eighth grade education, no other neurologic history; no severe psychiatric history (e.g., bipolar disorder, schizophrenia); concurrent brain imaging; no visual agnosia; normal/corrected-to-normal vision and hearing, and ability to comply with task instructions. The primary reasons for exclusion from this retrospective analysis were: (1) the presence of right hemisphere and/or multiple strokes, (2) no brain imaging data, and (3) no auditory comprehension subtest score. Patients with small right hemisphere lacunae were not excluded.
Based on the Western Aphasia Battery [WAB; (39)], the sample included 45 patients with Broca's aphasia, 14 patients with Wernicke's aphasia, 6 patients with conduction aphasia, 47 patients with anomic aphasia, 4 patients with global aphasia, 3 patients with transocrtical sensory aphasia, 1 patient with transcortical motor aphasia, and 48 patients who scored within-normal limits (WNL; see Table 1 for patient characteristics). WNL status is based on a cut-off score of 93.7/100 on the WAB. This cut-off score was taken from the WAB manual, which derived these scores by comparing stroke patients to a control sample. These WNL patients had a prior history of clinical aphasia and residual deficits, but their symptoms were too mild to be detected by the WAB.
Recovery Group
For the recovery analysis, we analyzed data from a group of 44 patients who were tested on the WAB on at least two different timepoints, separated by at least 1 month without any intervening treatment (see Table 1 for patient characteristics). Because this was a retrospective analysis, patients were not systematically tested during specific acute and chronic timepoints, but rather, this analysis made use of existing data from patients who were tested more than once at various points in their recovery. In this way, we were able to measure recovery curves across many months and years post-stroke. Patients who reported any new or additional neurologic events between behavioral test sessions were not included in this study. To avoid ceiling effects, we only included patients with auditory comprehension subscores <9 out of a possible 10 (as derived from the WAB manual) in their initial test session. The average time interval between patients' first and last WAB test session was 42.2 months (SD = 58.4; range 2–284). The average number of test sessions administered in this subgroup was 2.7 (SD = 1.0; range 2–6). Based on their final outcome scores, this subset included 9 patients with an anomic aphasia, 14 patients with Broca's aphasia, 6 patients with conduction aphasia, 1 patient with global aphasia, 2 patients with transcortical sensory aphasia, 10 patients with Wernicke's aphasia, and 2 WNL patients. For more details on the first and final WAB scores received by the recovery group, see Table 2.
Table 2. Participant WAB subtest scores (initial and final) for chronic outcome and recovery groups.
Informed consent was obtained from all patients in the study. The study was carried out in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and approved by the VA Northern California Institutional Review Board.
Materials and Procedures
Auditory Comprehension Testing
Patients were administered the WAB by a licensed speech-language pathologist or trained neuropsychologist as part of a larger neuropsychological battery for research purposes. The WAB is a speech and language battery that includes separate subtests that measure speech fluency, naming, repetition, and comprehension. Only patients who could comply with test instructions were included in these analyses. The present study analyzed data from the three WAB auditory comprehension subtests: (1) yes/no questions −20 items (e.g., “Is this a hospital?”), (2) single-word recognition −60 items (e.g., point to the cup), and (3) sequential commands −11 items (e.g., point to the book and the comb).
Brain Imaging and Lesion Reconstructions
Patients underwent brain imaging at least 3 months post-stroke, when lesion site and size are stabilized. Patients' lesions were reconstructed from 3D MRI T1 scans, or 3D CT scans when MRI was contraindicated. Imaging was acquired close to the time of the first WAB test session. For 59 patients, high-resolution T1-weighted 3D MRI scans were obtained on a 1.5 T Phillips Eclipse scanner. T1-weighted images were acquired with a Spoiled Gradient Recall (SPGR) coronal sequence (TR/TE/FA = 15 ms /4.47 ms / 35°, FOV = 256 × 240 × 256, 0.94 × 1.3 × 0.94 mm3 voxels). For 14 patients, anatomical scans were obtained on a 3T Siemens Verio scanner (Syngo MR B17) with a 12-channel phased-array head coil. One high-resolution 3T T1 MPRage anatomical image was acquired for each subject (TR/TE/FA = 2,200 ms/1.62 ms/9°, FOV = 256 × 192 × 256 mm, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxels, inversion = 900 ms, bandwidth = 343 Hz/voxel, and GRAPPA factor = 2) along with a T2 image (TR/TE/FA = 3,000 ms/409 ms/120°, FOV = 256 × 192 × 256 mm, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxels, bandwidth = 751 Hz/voxel) and a FLAIR image (TR/TE/FA = 6,000 ms/388 ms/120°, FOV = 250 × 250 × 192 mm, 0.488 × 0.488 × 1 mm3 voxels, inversion = 2,100 ms, bandwidth = 781 Hz/voxel). Patient lesions were outlined directly on the patient's T1 digital MRI image using MRIcron (40) and then registered with the MNI template using the standard nonlinear spatial normalization procedure from SPM5 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging). A cost function masking procedure was used to avoid distortions due to the presence of the lesion (41). T2 and FLAIR images were yoked to the T1 images in MRIcron to verify the extent of the lesion.
When 3D digital MRI images were not available, lesions were drawn from hard-copy CT (n = 7) or MRI films (n = 88). The CT scanner was a Siemens Somatom Emotion 16 CT scanner with 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 imaging, and MRI images were collected on the 1.5 T scanner described above. Lesions were outlined onto an 11-slice, standardized template (based on the atlas by DeArmond et al. (42) by a board-certified neurologist who was blind to the patient's clinical presentation as well as the predictions of the study. Reliability with this technique has been previously demonstrated (43, 44). The brain templates were then digitized and non-linearly transformed into MNI space (45) with SPM5. This transformation was achieved using 50 control point pairs to match anatomical features on the two templates. Slices were then aligned using a local weighted mean transformation implemented using the cpselect, cp2tform, and imtransform Matlab imaging toolbox functions.
An overlay map of patients' lesions is shown in Figure 1. As illustrated, the extent of coverage in the analyses included much of the left cerebral hemisphere and underlying white matter, predominantly in the middle cerebral artery distribution. The average lesion volume for the sample was 121.8cc.
Figure 1. Overlay of patients' lesions in the chronic outcome LSM analyses. Color bar indicates number of patients with lesion overlap. Patients with right hemisphere lacunae were not excluded from our sample.
Data Analysis
Lesion-Symptom Mapping
To identify the gray and white matter correlates of auditory comprehension outcome scores, patients' behavioral data were related to their lesion reconstructions using univariate and multivariate lesion-symptom mapping (38) (software freely available at https://www.nitrc.org/projects/clsm/). With univariate LSM, linear regression functions are estimated at every voxel, comparing behavioral performance (here, auditory comprehension scores) in individuals with and without a lesion in each voxel. Analyses were limited to those voxels that had at least five individuals with a lesion in that voxel in order to minimize biased regression parameter estimates. Maximum-value permutation testing was used to determine the critical t-value threshold, based on 10,000 iterations and alpha set at 0.05 (46). This is a relatively conservative method to account for the large number of statistical comparisons across the brain. We also re-ran the analyses using multivariate LSM, using support vector regression (SVR) with the same parameters above, namely 10,000 iterations with alpha set at 0.05 to determine the critical t-value threshold (47, 48). SVR has high spatial accuracy and a spatially compact solution (38). All analyses included age, gender, months post-stroke, overall aphasia severity (WAB overall score), and lesion volume as covariates. Identification of the brain regions associated with the significant voxels in each map was made with the AAL atlas template in MRIcron.
Longitudinal Recovery Slopes
Using auditory comprehension data from multiple time points, each patient's recovery was computed as a single longitudinal slope using mixed-effects linear models (R, “lme4” package). Individual intercepts were used to search for the best transformation of auditory comprehension recovery time (of 10 different families). Best fit to the longitudinal data is reported, based on the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) model score. Slopes computed from the time transformation with the lowest BIC score were then used in the LSM and behavioral analyses of recovery.
Demographic Analysis
Associations between patient demographic variables and auditory comprehension outcome scores were examined with linear regressions, with outcome scores as the dependent variable and months post-stroke, age, gender, and education as independent variables. Associations between patient demographic variables and auditory comprehension recovery slopes were examined with linear mixed effects models, with longitudinal recovery slopes as the dependent variable and months post-stroke, age, gender, and education as fixed effects. Patients were entered as random effects.
Results
LSM Analysis of Auditory Comprehension
The univariate LSM analysis identified distinct brain regions that were critically associated with the chronic outcome scores of the different auditory comprehension tasks, where the critical t-threshold was surpassed at p < 0.05. As seen in Figure 2, poor outcome for single-word auditory comprehension was associated with lesions in the left mid- to posterior middle and inferior temporal gyri (t-max = 6.97 at −60, −38, −14, cluster size = 4,583, t-threshold = 4.46) as well as small portions of the left angular and inferior-middle occipital gyri. For comprehension of yes/no questions, poor outcome was associated with lesions involving the left mid- to posterior middle temporal gyrus (t-max = 7.73 at −66, −22, −16, cluster size = 3,377, t-threshold = 4.99), and included portions of the left posterior superior temporal gyrus and posterior inferior temporal gyrus. Poor outcome scores for comprehension of sequential commands was associated with lesions in the left posterior middle temporal gyrus (t-max = 6.21 at −64, −54, 14, cluster size =854, t-threshold = 4.45), with some involvement of the left superior temporal gyrus and angular gyrus. There was a small region of common overlap across these three auditory comprehension subtests, located in the left posterior middle temporal gyrus.
Figure 2. Univariate (A) and multivariate (B) LSM maps showing neural correlates of yes/no questions (in dark blue), single-word comprehension (in green), and sequential commands (in red). Regions overlapping between single-word comprehension and yes/no questions are in light blue, and regions overlapping between single-word comprehension and sequential commands are in magenta. Regions overlapping across all three subtests are in yellow. Color bars reflect t-values.
Multivariate LSM analysis using SVR identified similar regions to the univariate LSM results, again where the critical t-threshold was surpassed at p < 0.05, for yes/no comprehension (t-max = 10.0 at −66, −22, −14, cluster size = 4,913, t-threshold = 6.43) and sequential commands (t-max = 10.0 at −64, −54, 14, cluster size = 770, t-threshold = 7.40). Single word auditory comprehension still fell in the posterior middle temporal gyrus, although the focus was more posterior than that identified with univariate LSM. The cluster size was also smaller (t-max = 10.0 at −40, −76, 20, cluster size = 1,866, t-threshold = 7.27). The region of common overlap across the auditory comprehension subtests was in the left posterior middle temporal gyrus as seen in the univariate LSM analyses.
Additional LSM analyses were also conducted on each of the three comprehension outcome scores, with the two other comprehension tasks included as covariates (e.g., examining single-word comprehension with scores from yes/no questions and sequential commands added as covariates), but there was not enough variability to generate any significant voxels.
Neither univariate nor multivariate LSM identified any significant voxels associated with the recovery slopes, likely due to the small sample size in the recovery group. Univariate LSM typically requires a minimum of 50 participants to generate significant, reliable findings (depending on effect size), and multivariate LSM typically requires 60–70 participants (38).
Analysis of Recovery Slopes
Longitudinal recovery of overall auditory comprehension (collapsed across all three subtests) was examined using survival curve-based time compression with ten different time compressions (see Table 3). Exponential log, weibull, lomax, and log-logistic classes produced the models with the best fit (BIC = 464.4–464.8). The overall estimated best slope for the log-logistic model was 3.71 (SD = 0.60) The log-logistic normalized time suggests that approximately 60% of improvement in auditory comprehension occurred by 12 months post-stroke, and approximately 80% of improvement occurred by 36 months post-stroke (see Figure 3). There was approximately the same amount of improvement (~7%) in auditory comprehension between 18 and 24 months post-stroke as there was between 60 and 120 months post-stroke.
Figure 3. WAB auditory comprehension subscores over time shown as distinct lines for all 44 individuals. Months post-stroke are reported above each asterisk (*). The log-logistic time compression provided an optimal transformation for examining recovery of auditory comprehension, using the following equation, where MPO represents months post-onset: .
We also attempted the same survival curve-based analysis using separate recovery slopes for the three individual auditory comprehension subtest scores (yes/no questions, single-word comprehension, and sequential commands), but the optimal models had poor model fit (i.e., unclear group recovery slopes), likely due to increased noise in these subtests relative to the overall comprehension score. This in turn reduced the models' ability to effectively transform the data and accurately detect longitudinal slopes.
Impact of Demographic Variables on Outcome and Recovery
First, we analyzed the relationship between demographic variables (age, gender, education, and months post-onset) and chronic outcome scores for the three different comprehension subtests. Regression analyses revealed that higher education was positively associated with higher yes/no question scores [B = 0.61, SE = 0.29, β = 0.16, p = 0.041, 95% CI = (0.02–1.18)], single-word recognition scores [B = 1.26, SE = 0.42, β = 0.22, p = 0.003, 95% CI = (0.43–2.10)], sequential commands [B = 2.57, SE = 0.72, β = 0.27, p < 0.001, 95% CI = (1.15–3.99)], and the overall auditory comprehension score [B = 0.22, SE = 0.07, β = 0.25, p < 0.001, 95% CI = (0.09–0.35)]. While age trended toward significance across all measures (p < 0.14), none of the other demographic variables were associated with any of the comprehension subtest outcome scores.
Secondly, we assessed the relationship between demographic variables and patients' recovery curves. Longitudinal recovery curves were significantly associated with time post-stroke [B = 0.007, SE = 0.003, β = 2.43, p = 0.02, 95% CI = (0.41–0.80)], as greater chronicity was associated with better recovery. Gender [β = −1.41, SE = 0.74, t(40) = −1.90, p = 0.06, 95% CI = (−2.98, 0.03)], and age [β = −0.04, SE = 0.02, t(42) = −1.78, p = 0.08, 95% CI = (−0.09, −0.01)] trended toward significance, with male gender being associated with better recovery and higher age associated with poorer recovery. In contrast to the outcome scores, education showed no association with longitudinal recovery [β = −0.12, SE = 0.13, t(40) = −0.93, p = 0.36, 95% CI = (−0.42, 0.10)].
Discussion
Auditory comprehension is often negatively impacted in stroke-related aphasia, but prior recovery studies of aphasia have typically focused on broad measures, such as overall aphasia severity. Also, very few studies have investigated recovery beyond the first year post-stroke (35). In the current study, we specifically examined recovery and outcome of auditory comprehension over an extended time period, including the impact of lesion site and demographic variables. First, we used univariate and multivariate lesion-symptom mapping (LSM) to identify the neural correlates of outcome for three different aspects of auditory comprehension in a large sample of 168 stroke patients (38). Lesion correlates for all three tasks overlapped in a small portion of left posterior middle temporal cortex, but there was substantial divergence across the tasks as well: Single-word auditory comprehension was uniquely associated with inferior temporal cortex as well as inferior middle-occipital regions; answering yes/no questions was associated primarily with mid- to posterior middle temporal gyrus; and comprehending sequential commands was associated with the left posterior middle temporal gyrus, with some involvement of left superior temporal and angular gyri. These findings provide context for discrepant results in the literature on auditory comprehension that may arise from the use of different types of tasks. Consistent with previous findings (30), tasks involving picture/object stimuli were associated with more inferior temporal cortical regions, while sentence-level tasks (which likely engage verbal working memory) tapped into more superior temporal and inferior parietal cortices.
The only brain region common to all three auditory comprehension tasks was a small region of posterior middle temporal gyrus, a highly-interconnected region that we and others have shown to be a critical hub for lexical-semantics (25, 49–52). While some studies have suggested that single-word comprehension is not dependent on posterior temporal cortex (29), our findings are consistent with recent studies by Fridriksson et al. and Kristinsson et al. (25, 53). Fridriksson and colleagues reported that both single-word and sentence comprehension were significantly impacted by lesions in the posterior superior and middle temporal gyri in chronic stroke patients, while Kristinsson and colleagues found that damage to the posterior middle temporal gyrus was the strongest predictor of overall auditory sentence comprehension performance in acute stroke patients. Studies in acute stroke patients have also implicated the posterior superior temporal gyrus in single-word comprehension, though not the posterior middle temporal gyrus as we did (54). Other studies, particularly with cases of progressive aphasia, have suggested that more anterior temporal regions are critical for single-word comprehension (31, 55). We were not able to directly address the role that the anterior temporal lobe plays in auditory comprehension due to the predominance of middle cerebral artery strokes in our patient sample.
Our study is one of the first to examine the longitudinal recovery of auditory comprehension specifically, and our results revealed that recovery occurs over a longer period than typically thought or previously investigated. Our findings revealed that, counter to common clinical wisdom, spontaneous recovery of auditory comprehension (i.e., without intervention) can continue well-beyond the first year post-stroke. Prior studies of auditory comprehension recovery have largely examined recovery over the span of only a few months post-stroke (16, 18, 19), while even studies examining language recovery more broadly only followed chronic stroke patients up to a year post-stroke. We also found that chronicity was predictive of the slope of recovery of auditory comprehension, but other factors such as age, gender, and education were not predictive (or trend only). By expanding the time window to include many years post-stroke, our study was able to demonstrate a longer course of recovery. Understanding the time course of auditory comprehension recovery has implications for treatment, as it supports the idea that patients can still show spontaneous improvement beyond the first year of recovery and indirectly supports the growing use of speech-language interventions during the chronic phase of stroke (56, 57).
Notably, recovery in our sample was not due to any intervening speech and language treatment, as this was a part of our exclusion criteria. Patients' spontaneous recovery over time thus likely reflected (at least in part) natural regeneration and the recruitment and activation of undamaged or compensatory neural regions (58, 59). Regarding the mechanisms that may undergird recovery of auditory comprehension specifically, few studies have examined non-speech related treatments that may contribute to improvement. The literature that is available in this area suggests that aspects such as exercise (60–62), access to quality medical care and fewer comorbidities (62, 63), managing psychiatric sequelae (64, 65) and social support (66–68) can all impact stroke patients' overall cognition and functioning. Thus, it is possible that such factors contributed to the spontaneous improvement of auditory comprehension in this sample, but additional work is necessary to explore this possibility.
Although there was evidence of recovery in auditory comprehension beyond the first year post-stroke, our data also showed that some elderly patients many years post-stroke sometimes exhibited declines in auditory comprehension. This diminution could be due to medical factors such as progressive cognitive decline that are more common in this aging population (69, 70). Future studies should devote effort to understanding these longer-term recovery patterns and the individual factors that can lead to continued or suspended long-term recovery from aphasia. Our use of longitudinal recovery slopes provides a way to examine auditory comprehension in a large sample of patients at various points in their stroke recovery. Our analyses revealed that the exponential log, weibull, lomax, and log-logistic time compressions best captured recovery over time, with log-logistic compression providing the most stable and interpretable results. This method also reveals additional avenues for assessing variables associated with aphasia recovery. By providing an approach for examining retrospective data, we hope this work will facilitate future collaborations and lead to larger aggregate datasets that can readily capture the neuroanatomical regions that are most critical for auditory comprehension recovery.
With respect to outcome, education was strongly associated with higher outcome scores on each of the three auditory comprehension tasks. This finding builds upon a prior study that found that acute stroke patients (within 24 h of stroke onset) with at least 12 years of education had higher scores on an auditory comprehension word-recognition task (34). However, studies have also found no associations between education and auditory comprehension outcome/recovery (15, 17), possibly due to a lack of statistical power. Identifying demographic factors associated with outcome and recovery is an important area of future research, as understanding which people may be more vulnerable to poor auditory comprehension recovery can help provide more accurate prognoses and lead to earlier intervention.
Strengths of the current study include our examination of both neuroanatomical and demographic factors associated with outcome scores of auditory comprehension in a large sample of well-characterized patients. We identified brain regions associated with auditory comprehension using both univariate and multivariate LSM. Recent empirical studies have suggested that the tandem use of both types of LSM is optimal for confirming critical foci (38). Both methods were consistent with respect to the critical foci identified in left mid-posterior temporal cortex, with the only exception being a somewhat more posterior middle temporal focus identified with multivariate LSM for single-word comprehension. Another strength of our study includes our analysis of recovery slopes many months and years post-stroke, which allowed us to demonstrate that spontaneous recovery continues beyond the first year.
One limitation in the current study was the smaller sample size of our recovery subgroup, which was not sufficiently large enough to utilize LSM to identify brain regions significantly associated with patients' recovery slopes (38). Also, because this was a retrospective analysis, patients were not consistently tested at specific timepoints during acute and chronic phases of stroke. Such evidence has not been forthcoming in previous longitudinal studies due to the difficulty and time required to follow a sufficiently large group of stroke patients over many years. Current efforts are underway at our site and others to amass large-scale stroke samples that include patients tested in both acute and chronic phases of stroke (71). Another consideration is that we cannot rule out the possibility that practice effects played a role in increasing scores on the WAB over time. However, practice effects in language testing are minimal, including the WAB (35, 36). In terms of demographics, the recovery group differed from the outcome group in their level of education, so it is possible that with a larger sample, education may be found to play a larger role in the recovery of auditory comprehension as well. The current study also did not address variables associated with language recovery in response to specific speech-language interventions, as we focused here on spontaneous recovery and outcome [for a review of intervention-based recovery, see (72)]. Another limitation is that we did not have tractography or functional imaging data available to address, for example, the role of the right hemisphere in supporting recovery of auditory comprehension (73, 74). Saur et al. (13) studied auditory comprehension recovery with fMRI in a group of 14 aphasic patients and found evidence of distinct patterns of activation associated with recovery during different phases of recovery. Last, LSM analyses of individual auditory comprehension subtests did not detect significant voxels when we covaried for the other comprehension subtests (e.g., examining single-word auditory comprehension with yes/no questions and sequential command subtests included as covariates). The high level of spatial overlap between the three tests made it difficult for the permutation tests to set an effective threshold, a statistical challenge made more difficult by adding the other two highly correlated tests as covariates. Nonetheless, the primary LSM analyses did show spatial divergence across tasks, as well as regions of common overlap.
Conclusions
This study was one of the first to examine neuroanatomic and demographic variables associated with recovery and outcome of auditory comprehension after stroke. LSM analyses revealed that different aspects of auditory comprehension, including both single-word and sentence-level comprehension, were associated with distinct portions of left mid-posterior temporal cortex, with a common region of overlap in the left posterior middle temporal gyrus. These findings provide context for discrepant results in the literature on auditory comprehension that may arise from the use of different types of tasks. We found that tasks involving picture/object stimuli were associated with more inferior temporal cortical regions, while sentence-level tasks (which likely engage verbal working memory) tap more superior temporal and inferior parietal cortices. Higher education was also found to be associated with higher auditory comprehension outcome scores, while age and gender were not. Our findings also revealed that spontaneous recovery of auditory comprehension continues beyond the first year post-stroke.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because these data were collected at a VA (Veterans Affairs) facility, which does not readily allow data-sharing. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to juliana.baldo@va.gov.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by VA Northern California Health Care System Institutional Review Board. All participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Note
JB, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA. SL, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA. TH, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA. BC, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA. MI, University of California, Berkeley. KS, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, CA. ND, University of California, Berkeley.
Author Contributions
JB, ND, and MI contributed to conception and design of the study. TH, BC, KS, and SL organized the database. SL and TH performed the statistical analyses. SL wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JB, KS, and TH wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs Research & Development (award I01CX001290-01A1) and by NIH/NIDCD grant R01 DC016345. The contents reported/presented within do not represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
We thank the research participants for their involvement in this project. The information in this manuscript and the manuscript itself are new and original and have never been published either electronically or in print. The authors of this manuscript had no financial or other relationship to any entity that would pose a conflict of interest.
References
1. Engelter ST, Gostynski M, Papa S, Frei M, Born C, Ajdacic-Gross V, et al. Epidemiology of aphasia attributable to first ischemic stroke: incidence, severity, fluency, etiology, and thrombolysis. Stroke. (2006) 37:1379–84. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000221815.64093.8c
2. Pedersen PM, Stig Jørgensen H, Nakayama H, Raaschou HO, Olsen TS. Aphasia in acute stroke: incidence, determinants, and recovery. Ann Neurol. (1995) 38:659–66. doi: 10.1002/ana.410380416
3. Angeleri F, Angeleri VA, Foschi N, Giaquinto S, Nolfe G. The influence of depression, social activity, and family stress on functional outcome after stroke. Stroke. (1993) 24:1478–83. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.24.10.1478
4. Cruice M, Worrall L, Hickson L, Murison R. Finding a focus for quality of life with aphasia: social and emotional health, psychological well-being. Aphasiology. (2003) 17:333–53. doi: 10.1080/02687030244000707
5. Watila MM, Balarabe SA. Factors predicting post-stroke aphasia recovery. J Neurol Sci. (2015) 352:12–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.03.020
6. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebr Cortex. (2009) 19:2767–96. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055
7. Catani M, Mesulam MM, Jakobsen E, Malik F, Martersteck A, Wieneke C, et al. A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. (2013) 136:2619–28. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt163
8. Forkel SJ, Catani M. Lesion mapping in acute stroke aphasia and its implications for recovery. Neuropsychologia. (2018) 115:88–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.036
9. Holland A, Fromm D, Forbes M, MacWhinney B. Long-term recovery in stroke accompanied by aphasia: a reconsideration. Aphasiology. (2017) 31:152–65. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2016.1184221
10. Maas MB, Lev MH, Ay H, Singhal AB, Greer DM, Smith WS, et al. The prognosis for aphasia in stroke. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. (2012) 21:350–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2010.09.009
11. Marsh EB, Hillis AE. Recovery from aphasia following brain injury: the role of reorganization. Prog Brain Res. (2006) 157:143–56. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)57009-8
12. Ramsey LE, Siegel JS, Lang CE, Strube M, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Behavioural clusters and predictors of performance during recovery from stroke. Nat Human Behav. (2017) 1:0038. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0038
13. Saur D, Lange R, Baumgaertner A, Schraknepper V, Willmes K, Rijntjes M, et al. Dynamics of language reorganization after stroke. Brain. (2006) 129:1371–84. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl090
14. Stefaniak JD, Halai AD, Lambon Ralph MA. The neural and neurocomputational bases of recovery from post-stroke aphasia. Nat Rev Neurol. (2020) 16:43–55. doi: 10.1038/s41582-019-0282-1
15. Pickersgill MJ, Lincoln NB. Prognostic indicators and the pattern of recovery of communication in aphasic stroke patients. J Neurol Neuro Psychiatry. (1983) 46:130–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.46.2.130
16. Mazzoni M, Vista M, Pardossi L, Avila L, Bianchi F, Moretti P. Spontaneous evolution of aphasia after ischaemic stroke. Aphasiology. (1992) 6:387–96. doi: 10.1080/02687039208248609
17. Lazar RM, Speizer AE, Festa JR, Krakauer JW, Marshall RS. Variability in language recovery after first-time stroke. J Neurol Neuro Psychiatry. (2008) 79:530–4. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.122457
18. Mattioli F, Ambrosi C, Mascaro L, Scarpazza C, Pasquali P, Frugoni M, et al. Early aphasia rehabilitation is associated with functional reactivation of the left inferior frontal gyrus: a pilot study. Stroke. (2014) 45:545–52. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.003192
19. Prins RS, Snow CE, Wagenaar E. Recovery from aphasia: spontaneous speech versus language comprehension. Brain Lang. (1978) 6:192–211. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(78)90058-5
20. Selnes OA, Knopman DS, Niccum N, Rubens AB, Larson D. Computed tomographic scan correlates of auditory comprehension deficits in aphasia: a prospective recovery study. Ann Neurol. (1983) 13:558–66. doi: 10.1002/ana.410130515
21. Naeser MA, Helm-Estabrooks N, Haas G, Auerbach S, Srinivasan M. Relationship between lesion extent in'Wernicke's area'on computed tomographic scan and predicting recovery of comprehension in wernicke's aphasia. Arch Neurol. (1987) 44:73–82. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1987.00520130057018
22. Den Ouden D, Malyutina S, Basilakos A, Bonilha L, Gleichgerrcht E, Yourganov G, et al. Cortical and structural-connectivity damage correlated with impaired syntactic processing in aphasia. Human Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:2153–73. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24514
23. Lukic S, Thompson CK, Barbieri E, Chiappetta B, Bonakdarpour B, Kiran S, et al. Common and distinct neural substrates of sentence production and comprehension. NeuroImage. (2021) 224:117374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117374
24. Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RD Jr, Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition. (2004) 92:145–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002
25. Kristinsson S, Thors H, Yourganov G, Magnusdottir S, Hjaltason H, Stark BC, et al. Brain damage associated with impaired sentence processing in acute aphasia. J Cogn Neurosci. (2020) 32:256–71. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01478
26. Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R, Makris N. Deficit-lesion correlations in syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Brain Lang. (2016) 152:14–27. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.10.005
27. Thothathiri M, Kimberg DY, Schwartz MF. The neural basis of reversible sentence comprehension: evidence from voxel-based lesion symptom mapping in aphasia. J Cogn Neurosci. (2012) 24:212–22. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00118
28. Magnusdottir S, Fillmore P, Den Ouden DB, Hjaltason H, Rorden C, Kjartansson O, et al. Damage to left anterior temporal cortex predicts impairment of complex syntactic processing: a lesion-symptom mapping study. Human Brain Mapp. (2013) 34:2715–23. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22096
29. Selnes OA, Niccum N, Knopman DS, Rubens AB. Recovery of single word comprehension: CT-scan correlates. Brain Lang. (1984) 21:72–84. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(84)90037-3
30. Bonilha L, Hillis AE, Hickok G, Den Ouden DB, Rorden C, Fridriksson J. Temporal lobe networks supporting the comprehension of spoken words. Brain. (2017) 140:2370–80. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx169
31. Mesulam M-M, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ. The wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. (2015) 138:2423–37. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv154
32. Cocquyt E-M, De Ley L, Santens P, Van Borsel J, De Letter M. The role of the right hemisphere in the recovery of stroke-related aphasia: a systematic review. J Neuroling. (2017) 44:68–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.03.004
33. Forkel SJ, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell'Acqua F, Kalra L, Murphy DGM, Williams SCR, et al. Anatomical predictors of aphasia recovery: a tractography study of bilateral perisylvian language networks. Brain. (2014) 137:2027–39. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu113
34. González-Fernández M, Davis C, Molitoris JJ, Newhart M, Leigh R, Hillis AE. Formal education, socioeconomic status, and the severity of aphasia after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2011) 92:1809–13. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.05.026
35. Johnson L, Basilakos A, Yourganov G, Cai B, Bonilha L, Rorden C, et al. Progression of aphasia severity in the chronic stages of stroke. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (2019) 28:1–11. doi: 10.1044/2018_AJSLP−18-0123
36. Kertesz A, McCabe P. Recovery patterns and prognosis in aphasia. Brain. (1977) 100:1–18. doi: 10.1093/brain/100.1.1
37. Martins IP, Fonseca J, Morgado J, Leal G, Farrajota L, Fonseca AC, et al. Language improvement one week after thrombolysis in acute stroke. Acta Neurol Scandinav. (2017) 135:339–45. doi: 10.1111/ane.12604
38. Ivanova VM, Herron TJ, Dronkers NF, Baldo JV. An empirical comparison of univariate versus multivariate methods for the analysis of br1ain–behavior mapping. Human Brain Mapp. (2021) 42:1070–101. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25278
40. Rorden C, Brett M. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav Neurol. (2000) 12:191–200. doi: 10.1155/2000/421719
41. Brett M, Leff AP, Rorden C, Ashburner J. Spatial normalization of brain images with focal lesions using cost function masking. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:486–500. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0845
42. DeArmond MM, Fusco SJ, Dewey MM. Structure of the Human Brain: A Photographic Atlas. 3rd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1977).
43. Friedrich FJ, Egly R, Rafal RD, Beck D. Spatial attention deficits in humans: a comparison of superior parietal and temporal-parietal junction lesions. Neuropsychology. (1998) 12:193–207. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.2.193
44. Knight RT, Scabini D, Woods DL, Clayworth C. The effects of lesions of superior temporal gyrus and inferior parietal lobe on temporal and vertex components of the human AEElectroencephalography P. Clin Neurophysiol. (1988) 70:499–509. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90148-4
45. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized talairach space. J Comp Assis Tomog. (1994) 18:192–205. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199403000-00005
46. Kimberg DY, Coslett HB, Schwartz MF. Power in voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci. (2007) 19:1067–80. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1067
47. DeMarco AT, Turkeltaub PE. A multivariate lesion symptom mapping toolbox and examination of lesion-volume biases and correction methods in lesion-symptom mapping. Hum Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:4169–82. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24289
48. Zhang Y, Kimberg DY, Coslett HB, Schwartz MF, Wang Z. Multivariate lesion-symptom mapping using support vector regression. Human Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:5861–76. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22590
49. Baldo VJ, Arévalo A, Patterson JP, Dronkers NF. Grey and white matter correlates of picture naming: evidence from a voxel-based lesion analysis of the boston naming test. Cortex. (2013) 49:658–67. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.001
50. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM, Prieto T. Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. (1997) 17:353–62. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00353.1997
51. Ivanova VM, Isaev DY, Dragoy VO, Akinina YS, Petrushevskiy AG, Fedina ON, et al. Diffusion-tensor imaging of major white matter tracts and their role in language processing in aphasia. Cortex. (2016) 85:165–81. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.019
52. Turken A, Dronkers NF. The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Front Syst Neurosci. (2011) 5:1. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001
53. Fridriksson J, den Ouden D.-B., Hillis AE, Hickok G, Rorden C, et al Anatomy of aphasia revisited. Brain. (2018) 141:848–62. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx363
54. Hillis AE, Rorden C, Fridriksson J. Brain regions essential for word comprehension: drawing inferences from patients. Ann Neurol. (2017) 81:759–768. doi: 10.1002/ana.24941
55. Wilson SM. Lesion-symptom mapping in the study of spoken language understanding. Lang Cogn Neurosci. (2017) 32:891–9. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1248984
56. Breitenstein C, Grewe T, Flöel A, Ziegler W, Springer L, Martus P, et al. Intensive speech and language therapy in patients with chronic aphasia after stroke: a randomised, open-label, blinded-endpoint, controlled trial in a health-care setting. Lancet. (2017) 389:1528–38. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30067-3
57. Marcotte K, Adrover-Roig D, Damien B, de Preaumont M, Genereux S, Hubert M, et al. Therapy-induced neuroplasticity in chronic aphasia. Neuropsychologia. (2012) 50:1776–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.001
58. Murphy TH, Corbett D. Plasticity during stroke recovery: from synapse to behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:861–72. doi: 10.1038/nrn2735
59. Stockert A, Wawrzyniak M, Klingbeil J, Wrede K, Kümmerer D, Hartwigsen G, et al. Dynamics of language reorganization after left temporo-parietal and frontal stroke. Brain. (2020) 143:844–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa023
60. Pyöri,ä O, Talvitie U, Nyrkk,ö H, Kautiainen H, Pohjolainen T, Kasper V. The effect of two physiotherapy approaches on physical and cognitive functions and independent coping at home in stroke rehabilitation. A preliminary follow-up study. Disabil Rehabil. (2007) 29:503–11. doi: 10.1080/09638280600902497
61. Rand D, Eng JJ, Liu-Ambrose T, Tawashy AE. Feasibility of a 6-month exercise and recreation program to improve executive functioning and memory in individuals with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neur Rep. (2010) 24:722–9. doi: 10.1177/1545968310368684
62. Winstein CJ, Stein J, Arena R, Bates B, Cherney LR, Cramer SC, et al. Guidelines for adult stroke rehabilitation and recovery: a guideline for healthcare professionals from the American heart association/American stroke association. Stroke. (2016) 47:e98–169. doi: 10.1161/STR.0000000000000098
63. Mohamed W, Bhattacharya P, Shankar L, Chaturvedi S, Madhavan R. Which comorbidities and complications predict ischemic stroke recovery and length of stay? Neurologist. (2015) 20:27–32. doi: 10.1097/NRL.0000000000000040
64. Lewin A, Jöbges M, Werheid K. The influence of self-efficacy, pre-stroke depression and perceived social support on self-reported depressive symptoms during stroke rehabilitation. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2013) 23:546–62. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2013.794742
65. White JH, Attia J, Sturm J, Carter G, Magin P. Predictors of depression and anxiety in community dwelling stroke survivors: a cohort study. Disabil Rehabil. (2014) 36:1975–82. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2014.884172
66. Grohn B, Worrall LE, Simmons-Mackie N, Brown K. The first 3-months post-stroke: what facilitates successfully living with aphasia? Int J Speech Lang Pathol. (2012) 14:390–400. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2012.692813
67. Northcott S, Moss B, Harrison K, Hilari K. A systematic review of the impact of stroke on social support and social networks: associated factors and patterns of change. Clin Rehabil. (2016) 30:811–31. doi: 10.1177/0269215515602136
68. Tsouna-Hadjis E, Vemmos KN, Zakopoulos N, Stamatelopoulos S. First-stroke recovery process: the role of family social support. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2000) 81:881–7. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2000.4435
69. Qiu C, Fratiglioni L. A major role for cardiovascular burden in age-related cognitive decline. Nat Rev Cardiol. (2015) 12:267. doi: 10.1038/nrcardio.2014.223
70. Wilson RS, Wang T, Yu L, Bennett DA, Boyle PA. Normative cognitive decline in old age. Ann Neurol. (2020) 87:816–29. doi: 10.1002/ana.25711
71. Seghier ML, Patel E, Prejawa S, Ramsden S, Selmer A, Lim L, et al. The PLORAS database: a data repository for predicting language outcome and recovery after stroke. Neuroimage. (2016) 124:1208–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.083
72. Hartwigsen G, Saur D. Neuroimaging of stroke recovery from aphasia–Insights into plasticity of the human language network. NeuroImage. (2019) 190:14–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.056
73. Crinion J, Price CJ. Right anterior superior temporal activation predicts auditory sentence comprehension following aphasic stroke. Brain. (2005) 128:2858–71. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh659
Keywords: stroke, recovery, comprehension, temporal lobe, MRI, outcome
Citation: Lwi SJ, Herron TJ, Curran BC, Ivanova MV, Schendel K, Dronkers NF and Baldo JV (2021) Auditory Comprehension Deficits in Post-stroke Aphasia: Neurologic and Demographic Correlates of Outcome and Recovery. Front. Neurol. 12:680248. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.680248
Received: 13 March 2021; Accepted: 09 July 2021;
Published: 12 August 2021.
Edited by:
Swathi Kiran, Boston University, United StatesReviewed by:
Erin L. Meier, Northeastern University, United StatesShihui Xing, Sun Yat-sen University, China
Copyright © 2021 Lwi, Herron, Curran, Ivanova, Schendel, Dronkers and Baldo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Juliana V. Baldo, juliana.baldo@va.gov