Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 10 December 2020
Sec. Movement Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroimaging in Parkinson’s Disease and Parkinsonism View all 27 articles

Changes in Prefrontal Cortical Activity During Walking and Cognitive Functions Among Patients With Parkinson's Disease

  • 1TS2-LESCOT, Univ Gustave Eiffel, IFSTTAR, Univ Lyon, Lyon, France
  • 2Service de Médecine Physique et de Réadaptation Neurologique, Hôpital Henry-Gabrielle, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • 3Department of Physical Therapy and Rehabilitation Science, School of Health Professions, The University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS, United States
  • 4EuroMov Digital Health in Motion, Univ Montpellier, IMT Mînes Ales, Montpellier, France
  • 5Inserm UMR-S 1028, CNRS UMR 529, ImpAct, Center de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Université Lyon-1, Bron, France
  • 6Université de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Lyon, France
  • 7Center de Neurosciences Cognitives, Service de Neurologie C, Hôpital Neurologique Pierre Wertheimer, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon I, Lyon, France

Background: Walking becomes more and more degraded as Parkinson's Disease (PD) progresses. Previous research examined factors contributing to this deterioration. Among them, changes in brain cortical activity during walking have been less studied in this clinical population.

Objectives: This study aimed to: (1) investigate changes in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activation during usual walking and dual-task walking conditions in patients with PD; (2) examine the association between cortical activity and behavioral/cognitive outcomes; and (3) explore which factors best predict increased activation of the DLPFC during usual walking.

Methods: Eighteen patients with early stage PD and 18 controls performed 4 conditions: (1) standing while subtracting, (2) usual walking, (3) walking while counting forward, and (4) walking while subtracting. Cortical activity in DLPFC, assessed by changes in oxy-hemoglobin (ΔHbO2) and deoxy-hemoglobin (ΔHbR), was measured using functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Gait performance was recorded using wearables sensors. Cognition was also assessed using neuropsychological tests, including the Trail Making Test (TMT).

Results: DLPFC activity was higher in patients compared to controls during both usual walking and walking while subtracting conditions. Patients had impaired walking performance compared to controls only during walking while subtracting task. Moderate-to-strong correlations between ΔHbO2 and coefficients of variation of all gait parameters were found for usual walking and during walking while counting forward conditions. Part-B of TMT predicted 21% of the variance of ΔHbO2 during usual walking after adjustment for group status.

Conclusions: The increased DLPFC activity in patients during usual walking suggests a potential compensation for executive deficits. Understanding changes in DLPFC activity during walking may have implications for rehabilitation of gait in patients with PD.

Introduction

Walking in everyday life may be a complex task for individuals with Parkinson's Disease (PD). Patients with PD have gait problems, such as reduced gait speed and step length (1), that worsen as the disease progresses. These gait problems may be exacerbated during dual-task (DT) walking combining both motor and cognitive activities. This may lead to a higher risk of falls (2), reduced mobility and quality of life (3).

Previous studies have investigated behavioral (e.g., cognitive, motor) and neurophysiological measures [e.g., functional magnetic resonance imagery (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG)] that could predict impaired walking or falls risk in patients with PD (2, 46). Among neurophysiological measures, changes in brain cortical activity during walking have been less studied in patients with PD (7). Different neurophysiological techniques have been proposed to better examine changes in brain activation during walking in PD.

Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imagery (fMRI) suggest that patients with PD had higher activation in frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes than healthy older adults during imagined usual walking (6). The increased cortical activation in patients with PD may reflect a compensatory mechanism to overcome inefficient neural activation. Although fMRI has adequate spatial resolution, changes in brain activation during real walking conditions in patients with PD are unfeasible to capture in such neuroimaging studies. Portable electroencephalography (EEG) or functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) techniques have allowed investigation of cortical activity during real-time walking (8). Using EEG, specific changes in electrical brain activity during DT walking were found in patients with PD (9). Patients with PD showed reduced P300 amplitude during DT walking compared to standing. This reduced P300 amplitude reflects unsynchronized activation of various resources (lower timing and coordination between different brain regions), suggesting less neural recruitment during DT walking. EEG has a good temporal resolution compared to other neuroimaging techniques. However, the spatial resolution of EEG is relatively low compared to fNIRS and neck muscles as well as artifacts such as eye movements can affect the quality of the recordings. With recent advances in technology, fNIRS allows an indirect evaluation of brain activation by measuring changes in brain blood oxygen levels. The fNIRS technique emits light with different wavelengths that is partly absorbed by the chromophores such as oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2 and HbR, respectively). Increased brain activation induces an intensified blood flow in the active brain regions leading to an increase in HbO2 and decrease of HbR (10).

To date, few studies have explored cortical activity during walking in patients with PD, using fNIRS (6, 1118). Some studies reported both HbO2 and HbR (12) and only one study used reference channels to correct for superficial hemodynamic interferences (15). Furthermore, the associations between neurophysiological and behavioral outcomes during usual and DT walking tasks were poorly understood (19) in patients with PD (20, 21). Under challenging tasks [e.g., obstacle negotiation (20) or DT walking while subtracting (12, 14)], patients showed an increased activation of prefrontal cortex (PFC) that may affect walking performance. An explanation to this increased activation in the PFC could be that basal ganglia dysfunction leads to reduced movement automaticity, which increases reliance on executive resources to control movements. The PFC, specifically the DLPFC plays a major role in executive functions, essential for the management of cognitive functions including planning, working memory and cognitive flexibility (22). Compared to healthy older adults, patients with PD showed a higher activation in the PFC compared to controls, even during usual walking (12, 20). This may reduce the functional reserve needed during more demanding tasks which may contribute to high prevalence of falls and DT difficulties among patients with PD (6). The role of DLPFC activity during usual walking should therefore better be explored in individuals with PD.

The objectives of this study were (1) to investigate changes in DLPFC activation during usual walking and DT walking conditions in patients with PD compared to controls, (2) to examine the association between cortical activity and behavioral/cognitive outcomes, and (3) to explore which factors best predict increased activation of the DLPFC during usual walking.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Patients were recruited from the Henry-Gabrielle Hospital, Lyon and healthy older adults (controls) were recruited through advertisements between September 1, 2018 to December 17, 2019.

General inclusion criteria were: 50 years old and above and able to walk unassisted for at least 20 mins. All participants had normal-to-corrected vision. All participants had no other visual, neurological (other than PD for the PD group), internal or psychiatric conditions that may interfere with walking. Patients with PD were excluded if they had Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA) scores ≤ 15, other parkinsonian syndromes, severe dyskinesia, deep brain stimulation, unpredictable motor fluctuations, or ocular diseases causing significant visual impairment.

Fear of falling was assessed by the Falls Efficacy Scale International (FES-I) and symptoms of depression were measured by the Beck Depression Inventory scale. Total daily levodopa-equivalent dose was calculated for each PD patient (23). All patients were medicated and tested in the “on” medication state. The “on” state was defined by a doctor (MC or TD) during the medical examination that preceded the experiment. Patients were tested between 1 and 3 h after taking their medication.

The study was approved by the French biomedical ethics committee on March, 9 2018 (Comité de Protection des Personnes Nord Ouest III Réf. CPP: 2018-01 N° ID RCB: 2017-A03187-46). Informed written consent was obtained from all participants.

Protocol

Participants performed in a single session 4 conditions: (1) standing while subtracting, (2) usual walking, (3) walking while counting forward, and (4) walking while subtracting. In the standing while subtracting condition, participants were required to stand still while subtracting 7 to a 3-digit number out loud. In the usual walking condition, participants were asked to walk at their “normal pace.” In the walking condition while counting forward, they were instructed to walk while counting forward out loud from a random 3-digit number. In the walking condition while subtracting, they were asked to subtract 7 from a random 3-digit number out loud. No instructions about priorization were given for the DT walking conditions. Each condition included 5 trials of 30 s. The duration of rest periods between trials ranged from 25 to 35 s in order to avoid anticipation of block onset. During rest periods, participants were instructed to stand still quietly. During the standing while subtracting and the two DT walking conditions, cognitive performance was measured and defined as the number of correct operations. The order of each condition was randomized between participants. Each condition started with 45 s of standing quietly (resting state period), with the instruction to refrain from talking and moving the head. The walking path configuration was an oval shape whose dimensions are displayed in Figure 1A. In order to assist participants to walk on the path, there were markings on the floor. Participants were allowed to take breaks between conditions. Furthermore, all experiments took place in the morning, the fatigue being less pronounced than in the afternoon.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Walking path configuration. (B) Optodes configuration. The right panels show the optodes with respect to the Fz locations of the international 10-10 system. Red circles represent the sources and blue circles represent the detectors. The numbers in gray represent the channels. Channels 9 and 10 represent the short separation channels.

Gait was assessed using two wearable sensors placed on the shoes (Physiolog®5, Gait Up, Switzerland). Walking performance parameters included mean gait speed (m/s), cadence (step/min), stride length (m), gait cycle time (s), and their coefficient of variation (CV) calculated as (standard deviation/mean)*100. For each condition, gait outcome measures were averaged over the 5 trials.

Relative concentration changes of HbO2 and HbR (ΔHbO2 and ΔHbR in μmol/L, respectively) in the DLPFC were measured using a wireless continuous waves fNIRS device (NIRSport, NIRx Medical Technologies) with 16 optodes (8 sources and 8 detectors).

Functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy

An increase of ΔHbO2 associated with a slight decrease of ΔHbR reflects a functional activation for the task (24). Fourteen optodes, corresponding to the 8 sources and 6 detectors separated by ~30 mm, were placed on the DLPFC according to the modified international EEG 10-10 system. Two short separation channels (channels 9 and 10, see Figure 1B) with an interoptode distance of 15 mm were used in order to account for peripheral tissue signals. The near infrared light was emitted by sources with wavelengths of 760 and 850 nm at a sampling rate of 7.81 Hz. Textile EEG caps in 3 different sizes (i.e., circumference of 54, 56, 58 cm) were used in order to fix the sources and detectors on the participant's head. An overcap was used to prevent ambient-light contamination. Raw intensities were recorded using the software provided by the manufacturer (NIRStar, version 15.1 and 15.2).

Data Processing

The data were analyzed using the open-access software Homer 2 within MATLAB (R2019b, Mathworks). The first processing step was to convert raw data into optical density. Then, a low pass filter with a cut-off of 0.1 Hz was applied to attenuate respiration and cardiac activity and high frequency noise (25). The next step was a motion artifact correction using wavelet-based filters (iqr = 1.5) (26, 27). After motion correction, optical density was converted into relative concentration changes of HbO2 and HbR using the modified Beer-Lambert law with constant differential path length factors (DPF) values of 6 (28). Finally, contribution of short separation channels was removed from the signal using a Kalman filter dynamic estimator (29). Using the block average method, mean amplitude difference of ΔHbO2 and ΔHbR for each channel was obtained using the last 5 s of the resting state before each trial and 30 s after the “start” instruction. Signal quality of each channel was visually checked to ensure divergence between the HbO2 and HbR traces (15) and extreme values were detected using the box-plot method. When the number of extreme values were inferior or equal to 3 (≤10%) for a channel, values were replaced by mean values of the group in the specific condition. Otherwise, the channel was removed for all participants. For the subtraction task, C5 and C12 were therefore removed for all participants. Signals from channels were averaged over the PFC (channels 1–8; 11–18), in line with previous research (21). Recommendations and good practices were followed according to a recent consensus paper (30).

Neuropsychological Tasks

Participants completed a series of tests assessing global cognition (MOCA) (31), inhibition (Stroop test; inhibition cost) (32), psychomotor speed, working memory, and attentional shift [Trail Making Test (TMT) part A and B, TMT (B-A)] (33), attention and processing speed [Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)] (34), mental flexibility (Plus Minus Task; shift cost) (35) and visuospatial abilities (Bells Test) (36). The order of neuropsychological tasks was randomized between participants. They were administered before or after the walking task.

Statistical Analyses

Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests were used to determine the normality of variables. For demographic, clinical, and neuropsychological variables, between-group differences were examined using Fisher's Exact tests, independent student t-tests or Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, as appropriate. For neuropsychological tests, p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni adjustment. A score is significant only if the corresponding p-value is ≤0.05/5 (p ≤ 0.01).

Repeated measures univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA) with group (patients vs. controls) as between subject factor and condition (standing while subtracting, usual walking and the two DT walking) as within subject factor, and group by condition as interaction effect, were applied on fNIRS data, as recommended in a previous study (37). Dependent variables were ΔHbO2 and ΔHbR.

Repeated measures multivariate ANOVA with group as a between subject factor and condition as within subject factor, and group by condition, were applied on gait parameters (gait speed, cadence, stride length and gait cycle time). Dependent variables were mean gait parameters and CV of gait parameters, respectively.

For each ANOVA, effect sizes (η2) were reported and were interpreted as small (0.02), medium (0.13), and large (0.26) (38). Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was applied during the post-hoc analyses.

In all participants, Pearson or Spearman rank correlations were analyzed to investigate associations between cortical activity (ΔHbO2) and behavioral performance (gait and neuropsychological outcomes) within condition. Pearson correlations (r) were used when data was distributed normally whereas Spearman rank correlations (ρ) were used when data was not distributed normally. Correlations (ρ or r) were considered weak below 0.10, moderate between 0.10 and 0.49 and strong between 0.50 and 1.00 (39). To determine which factors (age, gender, years of education, BDI, FES-I or/and neuropsychological factors) were the best predictors of HbO2 levels during usual walking, stepwise linear regression model was employed with group as a covariate. Only variables that significantly correlated with ΔHbO2 during usual walking and did not show strong intercorrelations (r < 0.80) were selected for entry in the model. P < 0.05 were considered significant. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS, version 26.0.

Results

Participants

Eighteen patients with early stage PD and 18 controls matched for age, sex and education level were included (Table 1). Patients were categorized into 3 clinical subtypes: 3 patients were classified as postural instability and gait difficulty-predominant disease, 8 patients were classified as tremor-dominant disease, and 7 patients were rigidity-dominant disease. Scores on BDI and FES-I scale were significantly higher in patients with PD, compared to controls. Patients were in early stages of the disease, based on disease duration, the Hoehn and Yahr scale and the motor section of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS). Three patients had a Hoehn and Yahr rating at stage 1, 13 were at stage 2, and 2 were at stage 3.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics for all participants.

Changes in Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Activation

ΔHbO2 and ΔHbR

Group Effect

Patients had higher ΔHbO2 than controls [F(1, 34) = 7.11, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.17]. No between-group differences were found for ΔHbR [F(1, 34) = 0.54, p = 0.47, η2 = 0.02].

Condition Effect

Furthermore, significant differences in ΔHbO2 and in ΔHbR between conditions [F(3, 102) = 7.71, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.19; F(3, 102) = 8.91, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21, respectively] were found. ΔHbO2 significantly increased during walking while counting forward compared to usual walking (p = 0.02). ΔHbO2 tended to increase during walking while subtracting condition compared to usual walking (p = 0.06). ΔHbO2 also increased during both DT walking conditions compared to standing while subtracting condition (p < 0.01). ΔHbR significantly decreased during walking while subtracting compared to usual walking (p < 0.001) and walking while counting (p < 0.001) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean (SD) changes (Δ) in HbO2 and HbR during the 4 conditions in both groups. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. P-values adjusted for Bonferroni correction. Red bars represent ΔHbO2 and blue bars represent ΔHbR. Red lines represent significant within-group differences in ΔHbO2 between 2 conditions.

Group* Condition Interaction

In addition, the interaction between condition and group on ΔHbO2 was significant [F(3, 102) = 3.34, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.09]. Patients had higher ΔHbO2 than controls during usual walking (p = 0.001) and in DT-walking while subtracting (p = 0.02) (Figure 2). In controls, ΔHbO2 increased during both DT walking conditions compared to usual walking, which was not the case for the patients (p = 1.00). Only in patients, ΔHbO2 was significantly higher during the 2 DT walking conditions compared to the standing while subtracting condition. No significant interaction between condition and group was found on ΔHbR [F(3, 102) = 1.65, p = 0. 18, η2 = 0.05]. The Figure 3 shows the averaged time courses of HbO2 and HbR in the DLPFC for the 4 conditions in both groups. For all conditions, the average relative concentration changes of HbO2 increased after starting the task. The average relative concentration changes of HbR showed slight reductions or remained relatively stable during the task.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Averaged time courses of HbO2 and HbR in the DLPFC for each condition in both groups. Vertical dashed lines indicate start of the task. Rel. concentrations, Relative concentrations.

Behavioral Performance

Gait Performance

Group Effect

Patients had reduced stride length compared to controls (p < 0.05) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Repeated analyses of variance for gait parameters.

Condition Effect

Speed, cadence, stride length, as well as gait cycle time were altered as the walking task difficulty increased (see Table 2). CV of cadence, speed and gait cycle time were greater during the 2 DT walking conditions than during the usual walking (CV of cadence, speed, and gait cycle time, p ≤ 0.001). CV of stride length was greater during walking while subtracting condition than during walking while counting (p = 0.04) or during usual walking (p < 0.001).

Group*Condition Interaction

No interaction between group and condition was found in any of the mean gait parameters (p > 0.12) (Table 2). However, two interactions between group and condition on CV of speed (p = 0.04) and CV of gait cycle time (p = 0.007) were significant. In patients, CV of speed increased during both DT walking conditions compared to usual walking whereas no pairwise comparisons for the CV of speed were found for the controls. Furthermore, patients had greater CV of speed than controls during the walking while subtracting condition (p = 0.02) (Figure 4). In patients, CV of gait cycle time increased as walking task difficulty increased whereas in controls, CV of gait cycle time increased during DT walking while counting forward compared to usual walking (p = 0.03). Patients also had a greater CV of gait cycle time than controls during the DT walking while subtracting (p = 0.04).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Gait performance in patients and controls. 1 = Usual walking; 2 = Walking while counting forward; 3 = Walking while subtracting. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01; P-values adjusted for Bonferroni correction. Only significant between-group differences are presented in the figure.

Cognitive Performance

No significant between-group differences in cognitive performance were found during the standing while subtracting and walking while subtracting conditions (standing while subtracting: Controls: 5.61 ± 3.19; patients: 5.91 ± 3.22, p = 0.83; walking while subtracting: Controls: 4.85 ± 2.74; patients: 5.28 ± 3.11, p = 0.67). Patients were significantly better than controls in the walking while counting forward condition (27.31 ± 5.60 vs. 22.42 ± 7.17, p = 0.02).

Patients were significantly slower than controls to perform the Stroop test (word reading conditions), the TMT (part A and B), and the PMT (the addition list) (see Table 3). They performed worse than controls on the DSST.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of performance on neuropsychological tests between PD patients and controls.

Correlations Between ΔHbO2 and Behavioral Measures

Strong correlations between ΔHbO2 during usual walking and CV of cadence, stride length and gait cycle time were found (Table 4). Moderate correlations were also found between ΔHbO2 during walking while counting forward and CV of all gait parameters. No significant correlations between ΔHbO2 during walking while subtracting condition and gait outcomes were found. A higher number of significant correlations between ΔHbO2 and cognitive performance was found for the usual walking condition than for the other conditions. Increased ΔHbO2 during usual walking was negatively correlated with worse performance on TMT, DSST, and Stroop test (color and word conditions). Note that no significant correlations between ΔHbO2 and behavioral measures were found when conducting correlation analyses in each group (data available upon request).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Associations between ΔHbO2 and behavioral performance.

Linear Regression Analysis

Among demographic variables, age and years of education were also significantly correlated with ΔHbO2 during usual walking (age: r = 0.37, p = 0.03; level of education: r = −0.39, p = 0.02).

Age, years of education, time to complete the color condition of the Stroop test, times to complete part A and B of the TMT, CV of cadence, CV of stride length were included in the model, with the group as a covariate. Stepwise linear regression revealed that part B of TMT was the most significant predictor of ΔHbO2 during usual walking. This test explained 21% of the variance. Years of education was then introduced in addition to part B of TMT, explaining an additional 6% of the variance (Table 5). Note that part B of TMT was the most significant predictor, explaining 27% of the variance when the stepwise linear regression was performed in PD patients only.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Predictors of ΔHbO2 during usual walking.

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to investigate changes in cortical activity of the DLPFC during usual walking and DT walking conditions in patients with PD in comparison with controls. Associations between cortical activity in the DLPFC during usual and DT walking and behavioral/cognitive outcomes were also examined. A third objective was to determine the best predictors of an increased activation in the DLPFC during usual walking. The novelty of this research is that we further explored the determinants of an increased cortical activity during usual walking. This may help to better understand falls risk or gait deficits before they emerge.

Increased Brain Activity in DLPFC in Patients With PD During Walking

Usual Walking

Moderate to strong associations were found during usual walking between increased cortical activity and poorer performance in neuropsychological tests assessing executive functions, speed of processing and psychomotor speed (i.e., TMT, DSST, and color and word conditions of the Stroop test). These findings suggest that normal walking is already a complex process that requires input from executive functions (40), that is carried out by DLPFC with other projections to various cortical and subcortical regions (41). As expected, patients had executive deficits and processing speed slowness compared to controls.

Patients with PD had also higher levels of DLPFC activation in comparison with controls, even during usual walking, which is consistent with previous studies (6, 11, 13, 21, 42, 43). However, patients did not show decreased walking performance compared to controls during usual walking. These results are not consistent with previous studies reporting decreased walking performance in patients already during the usual walking condition (20, 44). An explanation could be due to clinical and demographic characteristics of our group of patients. Our patients were at the earliest stages of PD, based on disease duration and motor score of UPDRS. They were also younger than those recruited in previous studies (20, 44). Furthermore, they were all volunteers to participate in this study and physically active. As in most research, there is a recruitment bias: it is often the fittest patients who agree to participate. It is therefore not excluded that these patients represent a special category, not quite representative of PD patients' population. It is also possible that our path configuration with markings on the floor helped patients to walk. Other measures, such as measures of arm swing while walking could be used in future studies, to better discriminate patients with PD from controls. Indeed, this measure has been shown as a new prodomal marker of PD (45).

Finally, this increased cortical activity in the DLPFC in absence of gait impairments during usual walking may reflect a compensatory mechanism to overcome executive deficits, processing speed slowness and deficits in movement automaticity in early stage PD (46).

DT Walking

No significant increase of DLPFC activity in patients was found from usual walking to DT walking conditions whereas in controls, significant increase appeared from usual walking to DT walking conditions. Findings on patients are consistent with previous studies that used the same type of DT walking (20). These results suggest that patients are unable to further increase DLPFC activity, due to a reduction of their cognitive resources and their limited cognitive capacity. Results in controls are consistent with previous studies reporting a significant increase in PFC, as a compensatory mechanism in healthy older adults during DT walking (e.g., walking while serially subtracting 3s) (4750). This is also in line with the cognitive resource theories of aging which postulated that brain activity will be more pronounced with increasing task demands in older adults (51). Increased prefrontal activity in older adults may also be due to the fact that most of healthy older adults in the present study were younger than 70s. A recent study suggested that healthy older adults in the 70s were unable to further increase prefrontal cortex activity, leading to a decrease of walking performance (52).

During walking while counting forward, no significant between-group differences were observed for DLPFC activity and gait measures. Furthermore, patients performed better in the counting forward condition while walking than controls. It is possible that patients prioritize the counting task at the expense of the walking task. Patients are globally slower, with a reduced cadence and shorter stride length than controls during this condition although no significant between-group differences in gait performance were found. Another explanation could be that patients had sufficient cognitive resources to perform both the counting forward condition and the walking task. Association between cortical activity in the DLPFC and cognitive measures revealed that increased DLPFC activity during walking while counting forward condition is rather associated with psychomotor slowness than executive functions. This result also highlights the major role of this cortical area on psychomotor functioning (53). The lack of significant differences between groups in cortical activity and gait measures may also be due to a high heterogeneity in the performance.

Patients with PD had higher levels of DLPFC activation than controls during walking while subtracting condition. As expected, increased DLPFC activity during walking while subtracting condition was found to be associated with executive deficits and psychomotor slowness. Furthermore, patients performed as well as controls the subtraction task while walking. However, they walked slower and had shorter stride length than controls. Patients were also more irregular than controls, as reflected by a greater variability of speed and gait cycle time. Based on the concept that attentional capacity is limited, it is possible that patients allocated their available cognitive resources to the subtraction task at the expense of the walking task. Increased DLPFC activation in patients during this complex DT condition is therefore insufficient to compensate for motor and executive deficits and may lead to decreased walking performance (12).

Associations Between Cortical Activity and Gait Parameters

Associations between increased activation in the DLPFC and higher gait variability in all participants were already found during usual walking. Similar findings were found in previous studies in older adults during obstacle negotiation (48). These authors discussed the fact that increased activation in the DLPFC may reflect a compensatory attempt to overcome inefficient neural activation in the face of demanding tasks. The inefficiency was unable to cope with the motor needs. During the walking while counting forward condition, increased cortical activity was associated with lower variability of their cadence and stride length as well as a greater variability of speed and gait cycle time. A possible explanation of the lower variability of cadence and stride length is that counting forward may induce a rhythm on which participants paced their walking. However, higher variability in their speed and gait cycle time seems to be moderately associated with increased cortical activity in the DLPFC during walking while counting forward. The lack of significant correlations between HbO2 and gait parameters in the DT walking while subtracting condition could be explained by the fact that patients with PD are unable to further increase their cortical activity in the DLPFC.

In the literature, measures of gait variability were found to be closely related to instability and fall risk (54). Strong relationships between high gait variability and increased cortical activity during usual walking supports the notion that subtle variations in the gait during usual walking, particularly in older individuals with PD, may precede disturbances in DT walking, leading to a higher risk of falls. To our knowledge, few studies explored associations between cortical activity and behavioral measures in walking tasks in older individuals with or without PD (52, 55). Further studies with larger sample size are needed to better understand the relationships between cortical activity and gait parameters in this population.

Best Predictors of Increased Cortical Activity During Usual Walking

Poorer performances on part B of TMT, and a low level of education significantly contributed to explain increased cortical activity in the DLPFC during usual walking. Particularly, the TMT-B explained 21% of the variance after adjusting for group status. These findings highlight the major role of TMT-B in predicting cortical activity of the DLPFC during usual walking. This complex test requires executive functions (updating information in working memory, shifting attention between letters and number as well as inhibit non-relevant information), which is affected by PD. This test in clinic may prove useful to determine difficulties in activities of daily living in patients with PD, as previously reported in a driving context (56). Yet, caution with the interpretation is warranted as the sample of the study was relatively small. Finally, a lower level of education also contributed to increased cortical activity in the DLPFC which is in accordance with the evidence that better executive functions are related to an increased number of education years (57). Further studies should therefore consider the level of education of patients with PD when exploring changes in brain activity in the DLPFC.

Methodological Considerations

Methodological considerations of this study included the use of short-separation reference channels (1.5 cm apart) to remove for peripheral hemodynamic response (i.e., artifact caused by breathing, cardiac cycle or other error related to movement) which has been used only once in patients with PD (15). Furthermore, both Hb species were reported. ΔHbO2 was found to be the most sensitive indicator of regional cerebral blood flow in NIRS measurements (58) whereas ΔHbR better reflects the match between oxygen supply and demand (59). In this study, fNIRS processing was the same for patients with PD and healthy older individuals. Although a recent study showed few effects of different processing methods on fNIRS signals assessed during active walking tasks in older adults (60), future studies applying specific fNIRS processing (e.g., age-dependent DPF values) are warranted. Furthermore, it would have been interesting to include a standing while counting forward condition in order to determine different patterns of task prioritization between groups (61). However, as the duration of the experiment was already long, we preferred to limit the number of conditions. The small sample size including volunteers with well-preserved cognitive abilities among PD participants limits the generalizability of the results. One limitation of the present study is that only the activity of DLPFC cortex is recorded. Further studies should assess multiple cognitive regions while walking to provide a greater understanding of the contribution of brain areas to walking and dual tasking (62). Finally, except the fear-of-falling inventory, no direct measures on the number of falls in patients with PD was reported in this study. Further studies should include a detailed questionnaire reporting the number of falls.

Clinical Implications

These findings may have important clinical implications for training program: it is possible that cognitive intervention decreases DLPFC activity and therefore improve the ability to walk in usual and complex situations. However, the increased cortical activity in the DLPFC is also due to deficits in automaticity (46). Multimodal intervention that targets both motor and cognitive aspects should therefore be developed to improve cognition and movement automaticity and promote mobility in individuals with PD. As a consequence, a reduction in the DLPFC activity while walking after the intervention should be expected. Further studies investigating the effect of training programs on cortical activity in DLPFC in patients with PD are therefore warranted.

Conclusions

This present study shows that patients with early stage PD increased their DLPFC activity, already during usual walking to compensate for subcortical dysfunction and executive deficits. Furthermore, strong relationships between cortical activity in the DLPFC during usual walking and behavioral measures were found in all participants. Finally, longer times to complete part B of TMT, and lower number of education years were found to be the best predictors of an increased of ΔHbO2 during usual walking.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité de Protection des Personnes Nord Ouest III Réf. CPP: 2018-01 No. ID RCB: 2017-A03187-46. All participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MR, IH, MC, JL, TD, and LP-F conceived and designed the study. MR, IH, MC, RD, TD, and LP-F contributed to data collection. MR, IH, RD, and LP-F contributed to data analysis and interpretation. IH, MC, RD, HD, SP, JL, TD, and LP-F contributed to the critical revision for important intellectual content. MR wrote the first draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work is part of the PARACHUTE project that was supported by the National Research Agency under the Future Investment Programme (ref. ANR-16-IDEX-0003), managed by the French government.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to M. F. Boyer for the inclusion of healthy older adults. We also would like to thank all the participants for their willingness to volunteer for this study. We are grateful to Biomechanics and Impact Mechanics Laboratory for the material provided.

References

1. Mirelman A, Bonato P, Camicioli R, Ellis TD, Giladi N, Hamilton JL, et al. Gait impairments in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. (2019) 18:697–708. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30044-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Fasano A, Canning CG, Hausdorff JM, Lord S, Rochester L. Falls in Parkinson's disease: a complex and evolving picture. Mov Disord. (2017) 32:1524–36. doi: 10.1002/mds.27195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Montero-Odasso M, Bherer L, Studenski S, Gopaul K, Oteng-Amoako A, Woolmore-Goodwin S, et al. Mobility and cognition in seniors. Report from the 2008 institute of aging (CIHR) mobility and cognition workshop. Can Geriatr J. (2015) 18:159–67. doi: 10.5770/cgj.18.188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Mak MK, Wong A, Pang MY. Impaired executive function can predict recurrent falls in Parkinson's disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2014) 95:2390–5. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Canning CG, Paul SS, Nieuwboer A. Prevention of falls in Parkinson's disease: a review of fall risk factors and the role of physical interventions. Neurodegener Dis Manag. (2014) 4:203–21. doi: 10.2217/nmt.14.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Maidan I, Rosenberg-Katz K, Jacob Y, Giladi N, Deutsch JE, Hausdorff JM, et al. Altered brain activation in complex walking conditions in patients with Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 25:91–6. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kahya M, Moon S, Lyons KE, Pahwa R, Akinwuntan AE, Devos H. Pupillary response to cognitive demand in Parkinson's disease: a pilot study. Front Aging Neurosci. (2018) 10:90. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Perrey S. Possibilities for examining the neural control of gait in humans with fNIRS. Front Physiol. (2014) 5:204. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Maidan I, Fahoum F, Shustak S, Gazit E, Patashov D, Tchertov D, et al. Changes in event-related potentials during dual task walking in aging and Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurophysiol. (2019) 130:224–30. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Villringer A, Planck J, Hock C, Schleinkofer L, Dirnagl U. Near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): a new tool to study hemodynamic changes during activation of brain function in human adults. Neurosci Lett. (1993) 154:101–4. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90181-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Gazit E, Mirelman A, Hausdorff J, Giladi N. Increased activation of the frontal lobe is associated with freezing of gait in patients with parkinson's disease: an fNIRS study (P6.074). Neurology. (2015) 84.

Google Scholar

12. Al-Yahya E, Mahmoud W, Meester D, Esser P, Dawes H. Neural substrates of cognitive motor interference during walking; peripheral and central mechanisms. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 12:536. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM, Mirelman A. When is higher level cognitive control needed for locomotor tasks among patients with Parkinson's disease? Brain Topogr. (2017) 30:531–8. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0564-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nieuwhof F, Reelick MF, Maidan I, Mirelman A, Hausdorff JM, Olde Rikkert MGM, et al. Measuring prefrontal cortical activity during dual task walking in patients with Parkinson's disease: feasibility of using a new portable fNIRS device. Pilot Feasibil Stud. (2016) 2:59. doi: 10.1186/s40814-016-0099-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Stuart S, Belluscio V, Quinn JF, Mancini M. Pre-frontal cortical activity during walking and turning is reliable and differentiates across young, older adults and people with Parkinson's disease. Front Neurol. (2019) 10:536. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Orcioli-Silva D, Vitório R, Nóbrega-Sousa P, da Conceição NR, Beretta VS, Lirani-Silva E, et al. Levodopa facilitates prefrontal cortex activation during dual task walking in Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2020) 34:589–9. doi: 10.1177/1545968320924430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Orcioli-Silva D, Vitório R, Beretta VS, da Conceição NR, Nóbrega-Sousa P, Oliveira A de, SC, et al. Is cortical activation during walking different between Parkinson's disease motor subtypes? J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2020) 16:glaa174. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glaa174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Stuart S, Morris R, Giritharan A, Quinn J, Nutt JG, Mancini M. Prefrontal cortex activity and gait in Parkinson's disease with cholinergic and dopaminergic therapy. Mov Disord. (2020). doi: 10.1002/mds.28214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Doi T, Makizako H, Shimada H, Park H, Tsutsumimoto K, Uemura K, et al. Brain activation during dual-task walking and executive function among older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a fNIRS study. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2013) 25:539–44. doi: 10.1007/s40520-013-0119-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Maidan I, Nieuwhof F, Bernad-Elazari H, Reelick MF, Bloem BR, Giladi N, et al. The role of the frontal lobe in complex walking among patients with Parkinson's disease and healthy older adults: an fnirs study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2016) 30:963–71. doi: 10.1177/1545968316650426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Stuart S, Vitorio R, Morris R, Martini DN, Fino PC, Mancini M. Cortical activity during walking and balance tasks in older adults and in people with Parkinson's disease: a structured review. Maturitas. (2018) 113:53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2018.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Panikratova YR, Vlasova RM, Akhutina TV, Korneev AA, Sinitsyn VE, Pechenkova EV. Functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contributes to different components of executive functions. Int J Psychophysiol. (2020) 151:70–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2649–53. doi: 10.1002/mds.23429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Villringer A. Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and imaging of human brain function. Trends Neurosci. (1997) 20:435–42. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01132-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Huppert TJ. Commentary on the statistical properties of noise and its implication on general linear models in functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neurophotonics. (2016) 3:010401. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.3.1.010401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Cooper R, Selb J, Gagnon L, Phillip D, Schytz HW, Iversen HK, et al. A systematic comparison of motion artifact correction techniques for functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Front Neurosci. (2012) 6:147. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Molavi B, Dumont GA. Wavelet-based motion artifact removal for functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Physiol Meas. (2012) 33:259–70. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/33/2/259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Herold F, Wiegel P, Scholkmann F, Thiers A, Hamacher D, Schega L. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy in movement science: a systematic review on cortical activity in postural and walking tasks. Neurophotonics. (2017) 4:041403. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.4.4.041403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Gagnon L, Perdue K, Greve DN, Goldenholz D, Kaskhedikar G, Boas DA. Improved recovery of the hemodynamic response in diffuse optical imaging using short optode separations and state-space modeling. NeuroImage. (2011) 56:1362–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Menant JC, Maidan I, Alcock L, Al-Yahya E, Cerasa A, Clark DJ, et al. A consensus guide to using functional near-infrared spectroscopy in posture and gait research. Gait Posture. (2020) 82:254–65. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hoops S, Nazem S, Siderowf AD, Duda JE, Xie SX, Stern MB, et al. Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease. Neurology. (2009) 73:1738–45. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c34b47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Azouvi P, Didic-Hamel C, Fluchaire I, Godefroy O, Hoclet E, Le Gall D, et al. L'évaluation des fonctions exécutives en pratique clinique. Revue Neuropsychol. (2001) 11:383–433.

PubMed Abstract

33. Reitan RM. Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Motor Skills. (1958) 8:271–6. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lafont S, Marin-Lamellet C, Paire-Ficout L, Thomas-Anterion C, Laurent B, Fabrigoule C. The Wechsler digit symbol substitution test as the best indicator of the risk of impaired driving in Alzheimer disease and normal aging. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2010) 29:154–63. doi: 10.1159/000264631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gauthier L, Dehaut F, Joanette Y. The bells test: a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. Int J Clin Neuropsychol. (1989) 11:49–54. doi: 10.1037/t28075-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Sato T, Ito M, Suto T, Kameyama M, Suda M, Yamagishi Y, et al. Time courses of brain activation and their implications for function: a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy study during finger tapping. Neurosci Res. (2007) 58:297–304. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bakeman R. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behav Res Methods. (2005) 37:379–84. doi: 10.3758/BF03192707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. (1992) 112:155–9. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Holtzer R, Verghese J, Xue X, Lipton RB. Cognitive processes related to gait velocity: results from the Einstein aging study. Neuropsychology. (2006) 20:215–23. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Leh SE, Petrides M, Strafella AP. The neural circuitry of executive functions in healthy subjects and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2010) 35:70–85. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Vitorio R, Stuart S, Rochester L, Alcock L, Pantall A. fNIRS response during walking — artefact or cortical activity? A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 83:160–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Gazit E, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM, Mirelman A. Changes in oxygenated hemoglobin link freezing of gait to frontal activation in patients with Parkinson disease: an fNIRS study of transient motor-cognitive failures. J Neurol. (2015) 262:899–908. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7650-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Yamada P de, A, Amaral-Felipe KM, Spinoso DH, Abreu DCC de Stroppa-Marques AEZ, Faganello-Navega FR. Everyday tasks impair spatiotemporal variables of gait in older adults with Parkinson's disease. Hum Mov Sci. (2020) 70:102591. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mirelman A, Bernad-Elazari H, Thaler A, Giladi-Yacobi E, Gurevich T, Gana-Weisz M, et al. Arm swing as a potential new prodromal marker of Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2016) 31:1527–34. doi: 10.1002/mds.26720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, Coomans D, Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, et al. Freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease: disturbances in automaticity and control. Front Hum Neurosci. (2012) 6:356. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Beurskens R, Helmich I, Rein R, Bock O. Age-related changes in prefrontal activity during walking in dual-task situations: a fNIRS study. Int J Psychophysiol. (2014) 92:122–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Mirelman A, Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Shustack S, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Effects of aging on prefrontal brain activation during challenging walking conditions. Brain Cogn. (2017) 115:41–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Holtzer R, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, Izzetoglu K, Onaral B, Verghese J. fnirs study of walking and walking while talking in young and old individuals. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2011) 66A:879–87. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mirelman A, Maidan I, Bernad-Elazari H, Nieuwhof F, Reelick M, Giladi N, et al. Increased frontal brain activation during walking while dual tasking: an fNIRS study in healthy young adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2014) 11:85. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Steffener J, Stern Y. Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve in aging. Biochim Biophys Acta. (2012) 1822:467–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Nóbrega-Sousa P, Gobbi LTB, Orcioli-Silva D, Conceição NR da, Beretta VS, Vitório R. Prefrontal cortex activity during walking: effects of aging and associations with gait and executive function. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2020) 34:915–24. doi: 10.1177/1545968320953824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Schrijvers D, Hulstijn W, Sabbe BGC. Psychomotor symptoms in depression: a diagnostic, pathophysiological and therapeutic tool. J Affect Disord. (2008) 109:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hausdorff JM. Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2005) 2:19. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-2-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

55. Stuart S, Mancini M. Prefrontal cortical activation with open and closed-loop tactile cueing when walking and turning in parkinson disease: a pilot study. J Neurol Phys Ther. (2020) 44:121–31. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Ranchet M, Paire-Ficout L, Marin-Lamellet C, Laurent B, Broussolle E. Impaired updating ability in drivers with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. (2011) 82:218–23. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.203166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Grasset L, Jacqmin-Gadda H, Proust-Lima C, Pérès K, Amieva H, Dartigues J-F, et al. Temporal trends in the level and decline of cognition and disability in an elderly population: the PAQUID study. Am J Epidemiol. (2018) 187:2168–76. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwy118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Hoshi Y, Kobayashi N, Tamura M. Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J Appl Physiol. (2001) 90:1657–62. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.1657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Elwell CE, Cope M, Edwards AD, Wyatt JS, Delpy DT, Reynolds EO. Quantification of adult cerebral hemodynamics by near-infrared spectroscopy. J Appl Physiol. (1994) 77:2753–60. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1994.77.6.2753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Izzetoglu M, Holtzer R. Effects of processing methods on fnirs signals assessed during active walking tasks in older adults. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. (2020) 28:699–709. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2020.2970407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Plummer P, Eskes G. Measuring treatment effects on dual-task performance: a framework for research and clinical practice. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 9:225. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Stuart S, Alcock L, Rochester L, Vitorio R, Pantall A. Monitoring multiple cortical regions during walking in young and older adults: dual-task response and comparison challenges. Int J Psychophysiol. (2019) 135:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson's disease, fNIRS, walking, cognition, dual-task walking, gait

Citation: Ranchet M, Hoang I, Cheminon M, Derollepot R, Devos H, Perrey S, Luauté J, Danaila T and Paire-Ficout L (2020) Changes in Prefrontal Cortical Activity During Walking and Cognitive Functions Among Patients With Parkinson's Disease. Front. Neurol. 11:601686. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.601686

Received: 23 September 2020; Accepted: 16 November 2020;
Published: 10 December 2020.

Edited by:

Edward Ofori, Arizona State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Rodrigo Vitório, Oregon Health & Science University, United States
Ji Hyun Ko, University of Manitoba, Canada

Copyright © 2020 Ranchet, Hoang, Cheminon, Derollepot, Devos, Perrey, Luauté, Danaila and Paire-Ficout. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maud Ranchet, bWF1ZC5yYW5jaGV0JiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pdi1laWZmZWwuZnI=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.