Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 12 November 2020
Sec. Sleep Disorders

Characteristics of Sleep Structure Assessed by Objective Measurements in Patients With Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Meta-Analysis

\nSijie Cai,&#x;Sijie Cai1,2Tingting Li,&#x;Tingting Li1,2Li Zhang&#x;Li Zhang3Longhua Shi,Longhua Shi1,2Jingling Liao,Jingling Liao1,2Wenfang Li,Wenfang Li1,2Guangwen Cheng,Guangwen Cheng1,2Wei Tan,
Wei Tan4,5*Shuang Rong,
Shuang Rong1,2*
  • 1Department of Nutrition and Food Hygiene, School of Public Health, Medical College, Wuhan University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
  • 2Hubei Province Key Laboratory of Occupational Hazard Identification and Control, Wuhan University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
  • 3Department of Neurology, Hubei Provincial Hospital of Integrated Chinese & Western Medicine, Wuhan, China
  • 4Community Health Service Center of Qingling, Wuhan, China
  • 5Hospital of Wuhan University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China

Objectives: This study aims to explore the differences of sleep structure between patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and elderly people with normal cognition, which will help to provide evidence for the relationship between sleep disturbances and cognitive impairment.

Methods: A systematic review and meta-analysis were conducted on the literature on sleep parameters obtained by polysomnography or actigraphy in patients with aMCI. The PubMed and EMBASE databases were searched up to April 2020. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were established according to evidence-based medicine methods, and data of all eligible studies were meta-analyzed using the Review Manager 5.3 software.

Results: Among the 1,171 literature articles on sleep structure of patients with MCI, eight case-control studies met the inclusion criteria and were included in this meta-analysis. A total of 278 subjects were included, of which 103 were patients with aMCI and 175 were elderly people with normal cognition. The results showed that sleep efficiency (SE) and slow wave sleep (SWS) of patients with aMCI were significantly lower than those of healthy elderly people. Compared with the control group, the percentage of stage 1 of non-rapid eye movement (N1%) in the aMCI patients group increased, and the percentage of stage 2 of non-rapid eye movement (N2%) decreased.

Conclusions: Patients with aMCI may experience more severe sleep disturbances than normal cognitive elderly people. There were specific changes, especially in SE and SWS, in the sleep structure of patients with aMCI when compared to those with normal cognition.

Introduction

Around the world, there will be one new case of dementia every 3 seconds (1). In 2019, it was estimated that there were over 50 million people living with dementia globally, and this number is said to more than triple to 152 million by 2050 (2). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is the stage between the expected cognition decline of normal aging and the more serious decline of dementia (3). Between 10 and 20% of people older than 65 years were diagnosed with MCI in 2015 (4), and the rate of MCI patients who will go on to develop dementia is about 14.9% within 2 years (5). Not all forms of MCI evolve into Alzheimer's disease (AD). Amnestic MCI (aMCI) refers to impairment purely in one's ability to recall stored information, which is the most common type of MCI and has a higher risk of developing AD (6). In view of the lack of effective treatment for AD (7), early detection and prevention measures taken in the aMCI stage are of great importance.

Over the last decade, sleep disturbance has received mounting interest as another important risk factor for cognitive decline. Individuals with sleep problems had a 1.55 (95%CI: 1.25–1.93) and 1.65 (95%CI: 1.45–1.86) times higher risk of AD and cognitive impairment than individuals without sleep problems, respectively (8). Approximately 15% of AD in the population may be attributed to sleep problems (8). Meanwhile, sleep disorder is considered as a concurrent symptom of cognitive impairment, a review found that 60~70% of people with cognitive impairment or dementia have sleep disturbances (9). And in turn a drop in sleep quality also worsens cognitive impairment, especially the deterioration of memory, because the memory consolidation process is completed during sleep (10, 11). There may be a bidirectional relationship between sleep disturbance and cognitive impairment, which has been left unlinked.

Sleep disturbances can be evaluated by subjective and objective methods (12). Commonly used subjective assessment methods for sleep quality include the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (13), Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) (14, 15), and a sleep diary. In terms of objective methods for monitoring sleep state, polysomnography (PSG) and actigraphy are highly correlated with sleep state (12, 16). PSG is the agreed method for evaluating night time sleep duration and fragmentation and to differentiate sleep staging (17). PSG uses various methods to simultaneously and continuously record neurophysiologic, cardiopulmonary, and other physiologic parameters during an entire night. Actigraphy is quantitative but relies on rest-activity rhythms as a surrogate for sleep-wake activity.

The relationship between sleep structure evaluated by an objective method and aMCI is unclear. Some studies explored the changes of the sleep structure in patients with aMCI, but the conclusions were not consistent, especially in total sleep time (TST), slow wave sleep (SWS), and wake after sleep onset (WASO) (18, 19). Our study aimed to clarify the characteristic change of objective sleep structure in patients with aMCI by conducting a meta-analysis. We synthesized study results from sleep state objectively detected by the PSG and actigraphy, and attempted to clarify the differences of sleep structure between aMCI patients and normal cognitive elderly people.

Methods

Inclusion Criteria

(1) Original articles from observational studies (cross-sectional, case-control, and retrospective/prospective cohort) were included in this review; (2) there were clear criteria for the selection and diagnosis of participants. Patients should be diagnosed with aMCI, and the control group must contain healthy elderly people with normal cognition; (3) the detection methods of the outcomes were clear. The method for evaluating sleep quality should contain at least one objective measure, PSG or actigraphy; (4) the outcomes contained sleep structure and sleep parameters; (5) sample size, mean, and standard deviation were provided.

Exclusion Criteria

(1) The study was a duplicate report; (2) the research design was defective and of poor quality; (3) the outcomes were incomplete or unclear, and quantitative data cannot provide the mean and standard deviation; (4) the method of statistics was wrong and could not be corrected.

Data Sources and Search Strategy

We collected eligible studies that evaluated sleep disturbances in patients with aMCI compared to cognitively normal controls, and the measurement for monitoring sleep structure were required to contain PSG and/or actigraphy as the objective evidence. We conducted a systematic literature search using the PubMed and EMBASE databases, limited to articles published in English. The databases were searched up to April 2020. We also reviewed the references of the related studies to explore more potential candidates. Take the PubMed query as an example, the search terms included (“mild cognitive impairment[Title/Abstract]” OR “amnestic mild cognitive impairment[Title/Abstract]” OR “cognitive decline[Title/Abstract]” OR “cognitive dysfunction[Title/Abstract]”) AND (“sleep structure[Title/Abstract]” OR “sleep architecture[Title/Abstract]” OR “sleep parameter[Title/Abstract]” OR “sleep disturbance[Title/Abstract]” OR “sleep disorder[Title/Abstract]” OR “sleep wake disorder[Title/Abstract]”).

Review Selection and Data Extraction

In accordance with the formulated inclusion and exclusion criteria according to evidence-based medicine, firstly the titles were reviewed for obvious exclusions. Then we reviewed abstracts for further exclusions. If it was unclear whether the article should be excluded after the abstract was read, we would review the full text. Two reviewers (Cai SJ and Li TT) selected articles and extracted data independently. Finally, their lists of articles were compared to ensure that the same studies had been included. The two reviewers resolved the discrepancies by discussion. If the discrepancies could not be solved, the final decision was made by a third person (Rong S).

Literature Quality Assessment

The tool used for evaluating the quality of the studies was the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS), NOS is commonly used to evaluate the quality of case-control and cohort studies (20). Our study included eight case-control studies. Two reviewers (Cai SJ and Li TT) evaluated the included studies independently and they reached a consensus by discussion. The criteria of the NOS scale includes three categories of selection (four items, four stars), comparability (one item, two stars), and exposure (three items, three stars) for a total of nine stars. A total score of six stars or above was considered of high quality.

Data Synthesis and Statistical Analysis

RevMan 5.3 software was used in our meta-analysis. The standard mean difference (SMD) with 95%CI was applied. To begin, heterogeneity was assessed using I2 statistics. An I2 value of 75% and above indicates a high degree of heterogeneity (21). The fixed-effect model was used if the result of the heterogeneity test showed I2 <50%. Otherwise, heterogeneity was considered to exist in the included studies, and the random effect model would be used for the combined analysis. Subgroup analysis was used to analyze the sources of heterogeneity. A funnel plot was drawn to judge publication bias. The SMD as the x-axis and the SE (SMD) as the y-axis were used to examine publication bias. A p <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Search Results

By searching PubMed and EMBASE, we found 1,171 relevant studies. After excluding 951 studies that did not meet the inclusion criteria, abstracts of 220 studies were screened. Then, the full text of the 25 potentially eligible articles were read. Finally, eight case-control studies fulfilled the eligibility criteria and were included in this meta-analysis. The literature search method was conducted as shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart of searching and selection guidelines.

Quality Assessment of Included Studies

Quality assessment was conducted based on the criteria of NOS. All the articles scored more than six stars in terms of NOS, so all the articles were considered of high quality (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of NOS quality assessment of the eight included studies.

Baseline Characteristics of Studies and Samples

Information of the eight included studies (2229), such as publication time, countries, major criteria of aMCI, and characteristics of the aMCI and control samples are listed (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics and information of the eight studies and samples.

Data Analysis of Sleep Parameters

TST

Six (22, 24, 2629) included studies showed that there was no significant difference between the aMCI and NC groups (SMD = −0.51, 95%CI (−2.10~1.08), P = 0.53) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Meta-analysis of TST between the aMCI and NC groups.

Subgroup analysis of TST

Different measurements: three (22, 24, 27) studies provided detailed PSG data, while three (26, 28, 29) provided actigraphy data. In the actigraphy subgroup, no significant difference was observed between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = 1.22, 95%CI (−0.40~2.83), P = 0.14]. In the PSG subgroup, shorter TST was observed in the aMCI groups [SMD = −2.22, 95%CI (−2.79~-1.64), P < 0.00001] (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Subgroup analysis of different measurements in relation to TST.

Sleep Efficiency (SE)

Seven (2228) included studies showed that the NC group had better SE [SMD = −1.30, 95%CI (−2.21~-0.38), P =0.006] (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Meta-analysis of SE between the aMCI and NC groups.

Subgroup analysis of SE

Different measurements: five (2225, 27) studies provided detailed PSG data, while two (26, 28) provided actigraphy data. In the actigraphy subgroup, no significant difference was observed between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −0.00, 95%CI (−0.62~0.62), P = 1.00]. In the PSG subgroup, greater SE was observed in the NC group [SMD = −1.83, 95%CI (−2.99~-0.67), P = 0.002].

WASO

Seven (2227, 29) included studies showed that there was no significant difference between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = 0.18, 95%CI (−1.04~1.40), P = 0.77].

Subgroup analysis of WASO

Different measurements: five (2225, 27) studies provided detailed PSG data, while two (26, 29) provided actigraphy data. In the actigraphy subgroup, no significant difference was observed between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −4.26, 95%CI (−13.31~4.80), P = 0.36]. In the PSG subgroup, WASO in the NC group was shorter than that in aMCI group [SMD = 1.29, 95%CI (0.48~2.11), P = 0.002].

Sleep Latency (SL)

Four (23, 26, 27, 29) included studies showed that there was no significant difference between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −0.02, 95%CI (−1.13~1.09), P = 0.97].

Subgroup analysis of SL

Different measurements: two (23, 27) studies provided detailed PSG data, while two (26, 29) provided actigraphy data. Both in the actigraphy subgroup and in the PSG subgroup, no significant difference was observed between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −0.71, 95%CI (−3.26~1.85), P = 0.59; SMD = 0.53, 95%CI (−0.19~1.26), P = 0.15; respectively].

SWS

Only one (27) included study provided SWS, and the parameter was detected by PSG. The result showed that the NC group had great SWS than the aMCI group [SMD = −3.52, 95%CI (−4.96~2.08), P < 0.00001].

Percent of Stage 1 of Non-rapid Eye Movement (N1%)

Four (2224, 27) included studies showed that N1% in the aMCI group was greater than that in the NC group [SMD = 0.95, 95%CI (0.07~1.82), P = 0.03] (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Meta-analysis of N1% between the aMCI and NC groups.

Percent of Stage 2 of Non-rapid Eye Movement (N2%)

Four (2224, 27) included studies showed that N2% in the NC group was greater than that in the aMCI group [SMD = −0.91, 95%CI (−1.26~0.57), P < 0.00001].

Percent of Slow Wave Sleep (SWS%)

Four (2224, 27) included studies showed that there was no significant difference between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −2.05, 95%CI (−4.16~0.05), P = 0.06].

Percent of Rapid Eye Movement (REM%)

Four (2224, 27) included studies showed that there was no significant difference between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −0.68, 95%CI (−1.80~0.45), P = 0.24].

Arousal Index (AI)

Two (23, 25) included studies showed that there was no significant difference between the aMCI and NC groups [SMD = −0.19, 95%CI (−0.59~0.22), P = 0.36].

Publication Bias

We used RevMan 5.3 to make the funnel plots. The figures appeared to have bilateral symmetry and we can conclude that no publication bias was evident.

Discussion

Main Findings and Discussions

In this study, we conducted a meta-analysis to clarify the differences of sleep structure as assessed by objective measures between aMCI patients and normal cognitive elderly people. We found that, SE of aMCI patients is significantly lower than that of normal cognitive elderly people, which is one of the most important indicators of sleep quality. Moreover, the SWS is significantly shorter in patients with aMCI compared with normal cognitive elderly people. However, compared with the normal control group, N1% in aMCI patients group increased, and N2% decreased. Nevertheless, there was no significant difference in TST, SL, AI, SWS%, REM%, and WASO between the two groups.

Previous studies have demonstrated that sleep disturbances and cognitive impairment have a bidirectional relationship (30, 31). Mounting epidemiologic evidence implicates disturbed sleep or lack of sleep as one of the risk factors for AD (8). Progressive worse changes in sleep quality, architecture, and neural regulation may constitute a contributing factor to cognitive decline (32). On the other hand, sleep disorders are extremely prevalent in neurodegenerative diseases. With cognitive impairment, there are further reductions of sleep quality, SWS, and percent REM, along with a profound disruption of circadian rhythmicity (32). Previous studies have confirmed that the characteristic sleep changes of AD patients are significantly decreased SE and reduced SWS (33, 34). In addition, studies have found that patients with MCI often suffer from sleep disorders. A review of 18 studies indicated that 14~59% of patients with MCI had sleep disturbances (35). However, generally speaking, epidemiologic studies used subjective sleep measures including questionnaire tools (such as PSQI and ESS) and self-reports (such as sleep diaries) to evaluate sleep quality (36). These subjective measures rely on the ability of patients and their caregivers to accurately recall sleep states (37). Indeed, subjective measures have poor concordance with objective measures and are subject to considerable recall limitations (38).

Hu et al. had conducted a meta-analysis of certain objective sleep parameter alterations in patients with MCI (39). But only one study providing data of an aMCI group was included. As one of the main subtypes of MCI, aMCI patients were our research objects in this study. Several parameters of sleep architecture were identified in our study. When compared with normal cognitive elderly controls, aMCI patients expressed lower SE and different sleep stage distribution including increased N1% and decreased SWS and N2%. Some epidemiological studies suggested that SWS is associated with better cognitive function, consistent with our results. A case-control study indicated that insufficiency of SWS may be the underlying mechanism of the association between insomnia and deficits in the executive control of attention (40). Memory impairment in aging is also indicated to be related to suppressed slow waves (41, 42). A trial demonstrated that enhancing slow wave activity conducted by acoustic stimulation was associated with improved morning word recall in individuals with aMCI (43). Accumulation of amyloid-β and tau aggregates, two histopathological markers of AD in the brain, are demonstrated to correlate with decreased SWS (44). What is more, a recent study suggests that slow wave loss could be associated with decreased cerebrospinal fluid flow, leading to lower protein aggregate clearance (45). However, SWS had significant differences while SWS% had none, the reason of which may be that the TST of patients with aMCI parallelly decreased, but without statistical significance.

The non-REM stage of aMCI patients had a distinctive change, with N1% increasing and N2% decreasing in aMCI patients. Targa A et al. found that some biomarkers changed with different sleep states in mild-moderate AD patients (46). For instance, there was a positive correlation between neurofilament light and N1%, and chitinase 3-like 1 was negatively correlated with N2%, SWS, and SE (46). Participants aged >65 years with cognitive impairment had significantly more N1 than those with no cognitive impairment, but the significance disappeared after adjustments for confounding variables (47). SE is another important index of sleep parameters. A cross-sectional study investigating the relationship between sleep and cognition in 2,932 elderly community members also found an association between low SE and MCI (48). Participants with SE <85% calculated using actigraphy showed a significantly decreased cognitive performance compared with those with SE ≥85% (49). With the decrease of SE, the accuracy of actigraphy decreased slightly (50). For mean WASO more than 30 min, actigraphy underestimated WASO compared to PSG (50). This possible mechanism may be that decreased SE is associated with decreased non-REM slow wave activity, leading to worse memory performance in those with amyloid deposition (51). In the future, improving SWS and SE to reduce the risk of memory loss or even AD is promising.

For the aMCI group in our study, not all sleep parameters were changed, and there was no significant difference in TST, SL, AI, REM%, and WASO. Previous studies have found that people with MCI have longer SL and shorter TST than healthy elderly people (39). In a 4-year longitudinal study of the relationship between cognitive aging and dementia in 2,238 healthy elderly people and 655 patients with MCI, increased SL could be an early clinical indicator of cognition decline in cognitively normal elderly people or patients with MCI (52). A large prospective study of sleep patterns measured by actigraphy reported that increased sleep fragmentation augmented the risk of AD and the rate of cognitive decline in older adults (53). Moreover, in the subgroup analysis, TST, SE, and WASO recorded by PSG between patients with aMCI and healthy participants were significantly different, while there was no difference in the actigraphy group. The different accuracy of objective sleep monitoring methods caused this distinction. PSG can provide information on the physiologic changes occurring in many different organ systems in relation to sleep stages and wakefulness. Actigraphy evaluates sleep state by recording human activities so that it is sensitive and reliable for predicting TST, SL, and SE. As compared with PSG, actigraphy is known to overestimate sleep and underestimate wake time (54).

Strengths and Limitations

The strengths of this study mainly include the following three aspects. Firstly, aMCI patients are at a higher risk of developing AD, which deserves special attention. Secondly, the sleep parameters of the studies included in our study were measured by PSG and actigraphy, in order to avoid subjective assertion. Thirdly, our study summarized several case-control studies' outcomes to reach a more accurate agreement by the meta-analysis. Nevertheless, this study also has some limitations. Firstly, this study only searched literature in English, and there may be publication bias. Secondly, because the sleep data of objective measures are not easily obtained, the small sample size is also one of the limitations.

Conclusion

Our study showed that patients with aMCI have significantly lower SE and shorter SWS, which are key points in elucidating the relationship between sleep disturbances and cognitive decline. Well-designed prospective studies and mechanism research are needed to provide more evidence in the future.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material.

Author Contributions

SR has primary responsibility for final content and is the study guarantor. SC, TL, and LZ together wrote this paper. TL and SC selected articles and extracted data. LZ, WT, and SR did study design and interpretation of results. LS, JL, WL, and GC helped with review selection and manuscript revision.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Alzheimer's Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2019: Attitudes to dementia. (2019). Available online at: https://www.alz.co.uk/research/world-report-2019 (accessed June 20, 2020)

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. World Health Organization. Towards a Dementia Plan: A Who Guide. (2018). Available online at: https://www.who.int/mental_health/neurology/dementia/policy_guidance/en/ (accessed June 20, 2020)

Google Scholar

3. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. (1999) 56:303–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kirova AM, Bays RB, Lagalwar S. Working memory and executive function decline across normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Biomed Res Int. (2015) 2015:748212. doi: 10.1155/2015/748212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Petersen RC, Lopez O, Armstrong MJ, Getchius TSD, Ganguli M, Gloss D, et al. Practice guideline update summary: mild cognitive impairment: report of the guideline development, dissemination, and implementation subcommittee of the american academy of neurology. Neurology. (2018) 90:126–35. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, Petersen RC, Ritchie K, Broich K, et al. Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet. (2006) 367:1262–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68542-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Anderson RM, Hadjichrysanthou C, Evans S, Wong MM. Why do so many clinical trials of therapies for Alzheimer's disease fail? Lancet. (2017) 390:2327–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32399-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bubu OM, Brannick M, Mortimer J, Umasabor-Bubu O, Sebastiao YV, Wen Y, et al. Sleep, cognitive impairment, and alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep. (2017) 40:zsw032. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wennberg AMV, Wu MN, Rosenberg PB, Spira AP. Sleep disturbance, cognitive decline, and dementia: a review. Semin Neurol. (2017) 37:395–406. doi: 10.1055/s-0037-1604351a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rauchs G, Schabus M, Parapatics S, Bertran F, Clochon P, Hot P, et al. Is there a link between sleep changes and memory in Alzheimer's disease? Neuroreport. (2008) 19:1159–62. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32830867c4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rauchs G, Harand C, Bertran F, Desgranges B, Eustache F. Sleep and episodic memory: a review of the literature in young healthy subjects and potential links between sleep changes and memory impairment observed during aging and Alzheimer's disease. Rev Neurol. (2010) 166:873–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2010.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Landry GJ, Best JR, Liu-Ambrose T. Measuring sleep quality in older adults: a comparison using subjective and objective methods. Front Aging Neurosci. (2015) 7:166. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mollayeva T, Thurairajah P, Burton K, Mollayeva S, Shapiro CM, Colantonio A. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index as a screening tool for sleep dysfunction in clinical and non-clinical SAMPLES: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. (2016) 25:52–73. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Buysse DJ, Hall ML, Strollo PJ, Kamarck TW, Owens J, Lee L, et al. Relationships between the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS), and clinical/polysomnographic measures in a community sample. J Clin Sleep Med. (2008) 4:563–71. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.27351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Spira AP, Beaudreau SA, Stone KL, Kezirian EJ, Lui LY, Redline S, et al. Reliability and validity of the pittsburgh sleep quality index and the epworth sleepiness scale in older men. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2012) 67:433–9. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Blood ML, Sack RL, Percy DC, Pen JC. A comparison of sleep detection by wrist actigraphy, behavioral response, and polysomnography. Sleep. (1997) 20:388–95.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

17. Kushida CA, Littner MR, Morgenthaler T, Alessi CA, Bailey D, Coleman J, et al. Practice parameters for the indications for polysomnography and related procedures: an update for 2005. Sleep. (2005) 28:499–521. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.4.499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Jiang B, Ding C, Yao G, Yao C, Zhang Y, Ge J, et al. Polysomnographic abnormalities in patients with vascular cognitive impairment-no dementia. Sleep Med. (2013) 14:1071–5. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Maestri M, Carnicelli L, Tognoni G, Di Coscio E, Giorgi FS, Volpi L, et al. Non-rapid eye movement sleep instability in mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Sleep Med. (2015) 16:1139–45. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Stang A. Critical evaluation of the newcastle-ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol. (2010) 25:603–5. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gorgoni M, Lauri G, Truglia I, Cordone S, Sarasso S, Scarpelli S, et al. Parietal fast sleep spindle density decrease in alzheimer's disease and amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Neural Plast. (2016) 2016:8376108. doi: 10.1155/2016/8376108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Brayet P, Petit D, Frauscher B, Gagnon JF, Gosselin N, Gagnon K, et al. Quantitative EEG of rapid-eye-movement sleep: a marker of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2016) 47:134–41. doi: 10.1177/1550059415603050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Reda F, Gorgoni M, Lauri G, Truglia I, Cordone S, Scarpelli S, et al. In search of sleep biomarkers of alzheimer's disease: K-complexes do not discriminate between patients with mild cognitive impairment and healthy controls. Brain Sci. (2017) 7:51. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7050051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Terpening Z, Lewis SJ, Yee BJ, Grunstein RR, Hickie IB, Naismith SL. Association between sleep-disordered breathing and neuropsychological performance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. (2015) 46:157–65. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Westerberg CE, Lundgren EM, Florczak SM, Mesulam MM, Weintraub S, Zee PC, et al. Sleep influences the severity of memory disruption in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: results from sleep self-assessment and continuous activity monitoring. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2010) 24:325–33. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181e30846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Westerberg CE, Mander BA, Florczak SM, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM, Zee PC, et al. Concurrent impairments in sleep and memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2012) 18:490–500. doi: 10.1017/S135561771200001X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wams EJ, Wilcock GK, Foster RG, Wulff K. Sleep-wake patterns and cognition of older adults with amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI): a comparison with cognitively healthy adults and moderate alzheimer's disease patients. Curr Alzheimer Res. (2017) 14:1030–41. doi: 10.2174/1567205014666170523095634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hayes TL, Riley T, Mattek N, Pavel M, Kaye JA. Sleep habits in mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2014) 28:145–50. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Guarnieri B, Sorbi S. Sleep and cognitive decline: a strong bidirectional relationship. it is time for specific recommendations on routine assessment and the management of sleep disorders in patients with mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Eur Neurol. (2015) 74:43–8. doi: 10.1159/000434629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Zhang F, Zhong R, Li S, Chang RC, Le W. The missing link between sleep disorders and age-related dementia: recent evidence and plausible mechanisms. J Neural Transm. (2017) 124:559–68. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1696-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RM. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation in healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2015) 25:307–30. doi: 10.1007/7854_2014_300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bliwise DL. Sleep in normal aging and dementia. Sleep. (1993) 16:40–81. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.1.40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Peter-Derex L, Yammine P, Bastuji H, Croisile B. Sleep and Alzheimer's disease. Sleep Med Rev. (2015) 19:29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Hudon C. Sleep disturbances in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. (2009) 21:654–66. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209009120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sun Q, Luo L, Ren H, Wei C, Xing M, Cheng Y, et al. Semantic clustering and sleep in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment or with vascular cognitive impairment-no dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. (2016) 28:1493–502. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216000739

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lyons BE, Austin D, Seelye A, Petersen J, Yeargers J, Riley T, et al. Pervasive computing technologies to continuously assess alzheimer's disease progression and intervention efficacy. Front Aging Neurosci. (2015) 7:102. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Naismith SL, Mowszowski L. Sleep disturbance in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of recent findings. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2018) 31:153–9. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hu M, Zhang P, Li C, Tan Y, Li G, Xu D, et al. Sleep disturbance in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of objective measures. Neurol Sci. (2017) 38:1363–71. doi: 10.1007/s10072-017-2975-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Li Y, Liu H, Weed JG, Ren R, Sun Y, Tan L, et al. Deficits in attention performance are associated with insufficiency of slow-wave sleep in insomnia. Sleep Med. (2016) 24:124–30. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.07.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Arico D, Drago V, Foster PS, Heilman KM, Williamson J, Ferri R. Effects of NREM sleep instability on cognitive processing. Sleep Med. (2010) 11:791–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, Saletin JM, Lindquist JR, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci. (2013) 16:357–64. doi: 10.1038/nn.3324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Papalambros NA, Weintraub S, Chen T, Grimaldi D, Santostasi G, Paller KA, et al. Acoustic enhancement of sleep slow oscillations in mild cognitive impairment. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. (2019) 6:1191–201. doi: 10.1002/acn3.796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Wang C, Holtzman DM. Bidirectional relationship between sleep and alzheimer's disease: role of amyloid, tau, and other factors. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 45:104–20. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0478-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fultz NE, Bonmassar G, Setsompop K, Stickgold RA, Rosen BR, Polimeni JR, et al. Coupled electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in human sleep. Science. (2019) 366:628–31. doi: 10.1126/science.aax5440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Targa A, Dakterzada F, Benitez I, Lopez R, Pujol M, Dalmases M, et al. Decrease in sleep depth is associated with higher cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light levels in alzheimer's disease patients. Sleep. (2020). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa147. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Haba-Rubio J, Marti-Soler H, Tobback N, Andries D, Marques-Vidal P, Waeber G, et al. Sleep characteristics and cognitive impairment in the general population: the hypnolaus study. Neurology. (2017) 88:463–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Schneider JL, Cauley JA, Hillier TA, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: the study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2006) 61:405–10. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.4.405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Miyata S, Noda A, Iwamoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. (2013) 22:535–41. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Marino M, Li Y, Rueschman MN, Winkelman JW, Ellenbogen JM, Solet JM, et al. Measuring sleep: accuracy, sensitivity, and specificity of wrist actigraphy compared to polysomnography. Sleep. (2013) 36:1747–55. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Molano JRV, Roe CM, Ju YS. The interaction of sleep and amyloid deposition on cognitive performance. J Sleep Res. (2017) 26:288–92. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Suh SW, Han JW, Lee JR, Byun S, Kwon SJ, Oh SH, et al. Sleep and cognitive decline: a prospective nondemented elderly cohort study. Ann Neurol. (2018) 83:472–82. doi: 10.1002/ana.25166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lim AS, Kowgier M, Yu L, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Sleep fragmentation and the risk of incident alzheimer's disease and cognitive decline in older persons. Sleep. (2013) 36:1027–32. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, Pollak CP. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep. (2003) 26:342–92. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: amnestic mild cognitive impairment, sleep structure, polysomnography, actigraphy, meta-analysis

Citation: Cai S, Li T, Zhang L, Shi L, Liao J, Li W, Cheng G, Tan W and Rong S (2020) Characteristics of Sleep Structure Assessed by Objective Measurements in Patients With Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Neurol. 11:577126. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.577126

Received: 02 July 2020; Accepted: 08 September 2020;
Published: 12 November 2020.

Edited by:

Liborio Parrino, University of Parma, Italy

Reviewed by:

Michelangelo Maestri, University of Pisa, Italy
Anna Szucs, Semmelweis University, Hungary

Copyright © 2020 Cai, Li, Zhang, Shi, Liao, Li, Cheng, Tan and Rong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shuang Rong, rongshuangwust@yeah.net; Wei Tan, tanwei63317@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.