Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 19 October 2020
Sec. Dementia and Neurodegenerative Diseases
This article is part of the Research Topic Non-invasive Brain Stimulation for Neurodegenerative Disorders: From Investigation to Therapeutic Application View all 9 articles

A Systematic Review of Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Applications to Memory in Healthy Aging

  • School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom

It has long been acknowledged that memory changes over the course of one's life, irrespective of diseases like dementia. Approaches to mitigate these changes have however yielded mixed results. Brain stimulation has been identified as one novel approach of augmenting older adult's memory. Thus far, such approaches have however been nuanced, targeting different memory domains with different methodologies. This has produced an amalgam of research with an unclear image overall. This systematic review therefore aims to clarify this landscape, evaluating, and interpreting available research findings in a coherent manner. A systematic search of relevant literature was conducted across Medline, PsycInfo, Psycarticles and the Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, which uncovered 44 studies employing non-invasive electrical brain stimulation in healthy older adults. All studies were of generally good quality spanning numerous memory domains. Within these, evidence was found for non-invasive brain stimulation augmenting working, episodic, associative, semantic, and procedural memory, with the first three domains having the greatest evidence base. Key sites for stimulation included the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), temporoparietal region, and primary motor cortex, with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) holding the greatest literature base. Inconsistencies within the literature are highlighted and interpreted, however this discussion was constrained by potential confounding variables within the literature, a risk of bias, and challenges defining research aims and results. Non-invasive brain stimulation often did however have a positive and predictable impact on older adult's memory, and thus warrants further research to better understand these effects.

Introduction

With the average life expectancy amongst most Westernised countries consistently rising (1), it has been increasingly documented that ageing is associated with certain memory changes (2). Although deterioration in memory performance can arise as a consequence of dementia, not all memory deterioration is pathological. Indeed, memory changes are widely reported amongst healthy older adults, with myriad studies identifying memory changes in individuals ageing typically with evidence of no dementia (35). Consequently, older adults often have a considerable degree of worry over how their memory will change as they continue ageing (6) a concern which could be valid given the above evidence of age-associated memory changes.

Research also consistently demonstrates that memory is not a unitary structure. Critical contributions to the field include: Squire et al. (7), who provided a dichotomy between episodic memories (autobiographical memories of events bound within a certain temporal context) and semantic memories (general knowledge about the world that are not time bound); Baddeley (8), who evidenced the importance of working memory (short-term storage of information for immediate information processing and manipulation); and Suzuki's (9) definition of associative memory as the ability to learn and remember relationships between unrelated items. Furthermore, such divisions of memory appear to change differentially as we age (10, 11), suggesting that ageing does not fundamentally impair memory in its entirety.

Despite this, what is clear amongst older adults is that these memory changes are noticeable and, perhaps more importantly, aversive. For example, Parikh et al. (12) found healthy older adults often identify themselves as making common memory mistakes like forgetting names or faces, which causes them to feel upset, embarrassed, and could impact upon their ability to work. Further research has relayed a similar message, with as many as half of healthy older adults worrying about their everyday memory (13). Attention has therefore turned to identifying if it is possible to ameliorate, or indeed prevent, such age-related changes in memory performance. Approaches to doing this have been varied with mixed results, ranging from talking therapy (14), pharmacological interventions (15) to the use of nutritional supplements (16). Cognitive training interventions are a further example, which have shown some positive effects when using specific tasks and when assessing memory using certain tools [e.g., (17, 18)]. Research across these fields is hence rife, with all such approaches aiming to reduce the functional impact of memory changes over the lifespan.

Amongst this corpus of research, one approach gaining momentum is non-invasive brain stimulation. Whilst brain stimulation has historically involved invasive procedures, and thus often been targeted only at clinical populations, methodological advances have dramatically reduced the invasiveness of these procedures, making such research increasingly safe (19). Given this, brain stimulation has increasingly gained popularity due to its unique ability to transiently and non-invasively modulate neuronal activity, offering the potential to safely modulate underlying neural processes potentially contributing toward memory faculties (20).

Common non-invasive brain stimulation methodologies include repeated transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), and transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Whilst a full review of these is beyond the scope of this paper, a methodological review can be found within Miniussi et al. (21). Briefly, rTMS induces local neuronal depolarisation by inducing an electromagnetic field over a target area of the brain (via the scalp), which is thought to offer a high degree of temporal and spatial specificity (22). Contrarily, tDCS utilises a weak direct current over the scalp, modulating membrane potentials via voltage-gated ion channels. Such stimulation can either augment or inhibit local activity [with the association between polarity and excitation/inhibition being a matter of current debate, (23)], however typically with reduced spatial and temporal acuity to rTMS. tACS similarly induces a current via electrodes placed on the subjects' scalp however differs from tDCS by utilising an alternating current in which the electrical current periodically reverses direction. In doing so, tACS creates specific frequencies, enabling it to entrain brain oscillations and modulate associated cognitive functions (2426). Whilst the above briefly summarises three common methodologies other variants of these also exist.

Largely prefaced in research using deep brain stimulation and increasing attempts to map cognitive faculties to regions of the brain (27), the advent of non-invasive brain stimulation has led to numerous studies exploring the role of neurostimulation in modulating memory. However, given the relative primacy of this research area, there has been little co-ordination amongst research laboratories in stimulation methodologies and memory faculties assessed. This has led to a somewhat convoluted image, with different laboratories, using different methodologies, asserting contradictory findings, and espousing contradictory conclusions [e.g., (28, 29)]. Given the potential of non-invasive brain stimulation, this is unfortunate as these inconsistencies undermine the potential to support those with memory difficulties or concerns (13). Evidently, it is therefore necessary to systematically review this growing body of research to clarify what effect such stimulation may have on older adult's memory.

Efforts have previously been made to begin systematically exploring this body of research. For example, Hsu et al. (30) performed a systematic review with both healthy older adults and patients with Alzheimer's Disease, exploring primarily the growing evidence for tDCS in modulating cognition. Authors have subsequently reviewed numerous related areas, such as the role of tDCS in improving working memory amongst the general population (31), as well as less systematic reviews of the role non-invasive brain stimulation may have in Mild Cognitive Impairment (32). Nevertheless, the literature base for non-invasive brain stimulation has grown significantly within the past 5 years, with no extant review covering the unique effects non-invasive brain stimulation has upon different aspects of healthy older adult's memory. This is surprising, given the above evidence that older adults have specific concerns about their memory, are most likely to demonstrate memory issues (33), and are thus a logical future candidate for non-invasive brain stimulation should the research allude to this.

This systematic review therefore aims to explore the current literature regarding the efficacy of non-invasive brain stimulation in augmenting healthy older adult's memory faculties. Given the (current) relative primacy of this field, this systematic review has a deliberately broad scope, considering all methodologies utilising non-invasive brain stimulation and all memory modalities. In doing so, this review hopes to gleam a greater understanding into which aspects of memory are particularly amenable to modulation from non-invasive brain stimulation, what these effects look like, and which methodologies best create and capture these effects. This would serve to create a greater unity amongst researchers in their cumulative search to greater understand the nature of memory changes during the ageing process, and potentially how to intervene.

Methods

This systematic review has been conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (34).

Database Searches

To identify relevant papers, Medline (1966–11 November 2019), PsycInfo (1967–11 November 2019), Psycarticles (1988–11 November 2019), and Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection (1965–11 November 2019) were evaluated using EBSCO host. This was set to include both published research and research pending full publication, and time frames were based upon the earliest articles for each journal available on EBSCO host to the present day. Search criteria used were [Non-invasive Brain Stimulation OR tDCS OR tACS OR TMS OR TES] AND [Older Adults OR Elderly OR Seniors OR Geriatrics] AND [Memory] to assess primary research exploring the impact of non-invasive brain stimulation on healthy older adult's memory. Search terms were set to search the entire body of academic papers for these terms to identify the greatest possible number of hits. Two of the authors of this paper first screened each paper for its relevancy by analysing titles or abstracts, which was followed by a review of each paper's full text. One additional paper was included following running this search (35), as the research laboratory were aware this had just been released, matched the search criteria, contained pertinent findings, however was not yet identifiable on EBSCO host due to its recent publication. Reinhart and Nguyen had no affiliation to the authors of this systematic review.

Studies selected for the final review were written in English in the above databases, meaning any articles not translated by said databases could not be identified by this review. To enhance the scope of this review, research was included irrespective of if they were published within a journal or thesis. Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (a) were primary research (i.e., not a review, systematic review, meta-analysis); (b) measured healthy older adults not diagnosed with dementia, psychiatric illness, ongoing physical health issue, or Mild Cognitive Impairment; (c) utilised any form of non-invasive brain stimulation: (d) actively measured memory performance, and; (e) utilised a control condition, or another mean, of assessing the unique effects of stimulation on older adults memory performance.

Data Extraction

Using the above protocol, studies were selected independently by two authors, with discrepancies submitted to the third author for review. Following extracting relevant papers, research quality was assessed independently by two authors using the Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP) framework for assessing randomised controlled trials (36). This was used to address questions such as the validity and risk of bias within and across those papers included. Stimulation methodology and outcomes as assessed by at least one performance-based memory test were subsequently extracted, including significance testing and effect sizes. Forty-four original studies were identified using the above strategy, as illustrated in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA chart for included studies.

Results

Study Selection and Characteristics

Using the search protocol, 192 studies were initially identified matching the search criteria, which reduced to 129 after the removal of duplicates. After screening, this was reduced to 44 papers assessed as applicable for this review's inclusion criteria. Papers were subsequently screened for their quality and risk of bias using CASP, of which each papers score (spanning 1–9, with 9 being of best quality) are presented in Tables 15. Quality of reports were all generally good, with none included scoring below 7.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Working memory research.

All included papers recruited older adult populations, which were assessed as healthy using multiple approaches. For clarity, results have been divided into the memory domain assessed. This was due to the high degree of overlap between certain study's methodology/outcomes, with methodologies used clustered together within each sub-section. Online stimulation refers to stimulation delivered during the task(s) of interest, while offline stimulation refers to delivery outside the task(s) of interest, generally before the task and at rest.

Working Memory

Working memory, as defined as the ability to store, process, and manipulate short-term information was assessed within 17 studies. These are summarised in Table 1. By far the most common stimulation method was tDCS (14 papers), of which 10 studies found significant benefits on working memory performance (28, 37, 38, 40, 42, 44, 4649), and 4 did not (29, 41, 43, 45). Across all studies the region most commonly stimulated was the prefrontal cortex (PFC).

Deldar et al. (28) and Stoynova et al. (49) both reported positive findings when targeting the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) with online tDCS during working memory tasks; Stoynova et al. evidenced enhanced self-confidence on an auditory addition task across 12 stimulation sessions, and Deldar et al. evidenced a decreased mean reaction time on a modified N-Back task over 1 stimulation session compared to sham. Cespón et al. (40) also identified greater accuracy on the N-Back task, however DLPFC stimulation here was done immediately before the task.

Di Rosa et al. (42) found faster reaction times during and after stimulation of the left PFC on a visuo-spatial working memory paradigm, which corresponded with increased hemodynamic activity bilaterally in the PFC. Arciniega et al. (37) found that right PFC-posterior parietal (PPC) online stimulation (as compared to sham or bilateral PFC stimulation) improved visual working memory in an item location paradigm. Stephens (47) stimulated the right PFC over 5 cognitive training sessions, and found that tDCS uniquely improved performance on novel working memory tasks up to a month afterwards. Finally, Park et al. (46) identified that bilateral PFC stimulation can benefit verbal working memory, with tDCS significantly augmenting cognitive training for up to 4 weeks on verbal working memory paradigms.

Other studies have evidenced the effects of tDCS to be less tied to region-specific stimulation. Stephens et al. (48) identified that just one session of tDCS across different sites augmented working memory performance. Moreover, this research went further in identifying a particular variant of the COMT gene predicted tDCS efficacy, with effects lasting up to 1 month post-stimulation and training. Jones et al. (44) similarly found offline non-site-specific stimulation was sufficient to improve visuo-spatial working memory, with improved performance both after 10 sessions of training and after a 1 month delay. A final nuanced effect was observed by Berryhill and Jones (38), as although these authors stimulated frontal regions offline, significant improvements on the N-back task were only observed amongst participants identified as being more highly educated.

Four papers failed to identify a significant effect of tDCS on working memory performance. Cespón et al. (41) failed to identify a significant change in reaction time on the N-back task amongst healthy older adults immediately following DLPFC stimulation, and found no unique memory benefits following stimulation [a result that appears to oppose their previous findings; (40)]. That said, anodal tDCS did result in increased P300 amplitudes (a marker of cognitive processing measured using electroencephalography), which correlated with improved accuracy. Emonson et al. (43) failed to identify a significant improvement on the N-Back task following one session of DLPFC stimulation, however used a weak current of 0.7 mA (lower than the above studies). In two studies, Nilsson et al. (29, 45) failed to find an improvement in working memory performance during or after up to 20 rounds of tDCS stimulation of the left DLPFC.

Three studies used other forms of brain stimulation. Yamanaka et al. (50) used rTMS to assess working memory via a delayed match-to-sample task, with stimulation of either the left or right parietal regions [P3 and P4, respectively, based on the electroencephalography (EEG) International system; (78)]. No significant improvements were found when stimulation was delivered in 5 Hz pulses during the task, however P3 stimulation did significantly reduce reaction times relative to P4.

Reinhart and Nguyen (35) utilised online individually-tuned theta-frequency tACS in a phase-synchronous manner over temporal and frontal regions. In doing so, this study identified significant improvements in older adults visual working memory both during and for up to 50 min post-stimulation. In addition, EEG recordings showed increased post-stimulation phase-amplitude coupling in frontotemporal regions, however only when stimulation was phase-synchronised across fronto-temporal sites and performed at theta frequency. Finally, Borghini et al. (39) showed that online alpha-tACS uniquely augmented performance on a visual working memory paradigm (compared to theta or gamma frequency), increasing performance to a level comparable with healthy young adults.

Associative Memory

Associative memory, as defined as the ability to learn and remember the relationship between previously unrelated items, was assessed within 14 original papers (presented in Table 2). Anodal tDCS was the most common mode of modulating brain activity, with 8 papers employing this approach. Of these, 4 identified a statistically significant result (51, 52, 55, 58), whilst 4 did not observe a main effect of tDCS on memory performance (43, 56, 59, 62).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associative memory research.

Amongst the 4 tDCS studies that identified statistically significant results, both Antonenko et al. (51) and Flöel et al. (55) utilised comparable stimulation protocols over the right temporoparietal region during 2–3 learning sessions, and both reported improved object-location pairing learning. However, Antonenko et al. (51) identified incremental increases in performance over a 3-day period of object-location learning, whilst Flöel et al. (55) found improved free recall of object-location pairings 1-week post-learning.

Antonenko et al. (52) showed that stimulation of the left temporoparietal cortex during learning improved both immediate and delayed recall of object-pseudo-word associations, persisting for up to 20 min post-stimulation. Notably, participants who received tDCS also showed a faster learning curve of object-pseudo-word pairings, and evidenced augmented hippocampo-temporoparietal functional connectivity, suggesting network-level effects of stimulation.

Finally, Leach et al. (58) hypothesised that older adults may as a group have diminished face-name learning, and be specifically impaired by left inferior PFC tDCS. Their findings appeared to corroborate this theory, with one session of online tDCS causing a reduction in accuracy during recognition trials.

Four tDCS studies did not observe significant effects. Leach et al. (59) used a face-name association paradigm and found that stimulation of the left DLPFC during the task improved recognition and recall in younger but not in older adults. Emonson et al. (43) utilised a weak current over the left DLPFC, and was unable to identify any significant changes in older adult's subsequent performance on an object-location association paradigm. Külzow et al. (56) used a similar stimulation paradigm, targeting instead the right temporoparietal region, and found that tDCS-paired object-location learning did initially increase after the first day of training, however this did not persist after 3 trials and overall performance did not significantly differ from sham stimulation. Prehn et al. (62) also found no significant effect of tDCS over the right temporoparietal area on an object-location association task. Prehn et al. did however find a significant effect when utilising both Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRI's) and tDCS concurrently on this task, however this was only achieved via pooling older and younger adults together.

An additional 4 studies utilised an alternative form of tDCS known as slow oscillatory tDCS (sotDCS), assessing the effects of stimulation during slow-wave sleep on associative memory performance. Of these studies, Westerberg et al. (63) used sotDCS during a nap after learning word pairings, and stimulated participants at a frequency of 0.75 Hz in 5 blocks of 5 min after the onset of stage 2 sleep. Such stimulation was targeted toward bilateral frontal lobe regions at locations F7 and F8 according to the international 10–20 EEG system (78). Whilst word recall increased across both sham and sotDCS following a nap, the increase was significantly greater amongst those who received sotDCS.

Three sotDCS studies however reported no significant improvement in associative memory (54, 57, 61). Whilst these used similar memory tasks to Westerberg et al. (63), they differed in stimulation location by applying stimulation bilaterally to the frontal lobes but with electrodes placed in areas F3 and F4 (international 10–20 EEG system). Eggert et al. (54) found that sotDCS during early non-REM sleep did not improve word-pair memory consolidation, and that across conditions memory generally deteriorated following sleep. Ladenbauer et al. (57) and Paßmann et al. (61) both found that participants recalling lists of word-pairs or object-location pairings generally performed worse following a nap, with no significant differences between sham and sotDCS.

A final 2 studies examined the effects of rTMS on associative memory performance (53, 60). Manenti et al. (60) applied rTMS (20 Hz) to either the left or right DLPFC during encoding or recalling of word-pairs and found that stimulation significantly impeded word-pair recall accuracy. By subsequently breaking down test performance into high performers and low performers, the authors also reported that online stimulation of either the left or right DLPFC impeded performance in high performers, whilst lower performers were significantly more affected by stimulation to the left than right DLPFC.

Davis et al. (53) used rTMS at lower frequencies of either 1 or 5 Hz in the left DLPFC and did not identify any subsequent changes in word-pair association memory. Using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Davis et al. did however find changes in neural activity following differing stimulation conditions. 1 Hz stimulation decreased local success-related activity, causing distributed bilateral PFC activity instead, whereas 5 Hz stimulation increased success-related local activity, resulting in increased local connectivity within the PFC.

Episodic Memory

Episodic memory, defined as the ability to recall time-bound, personally relevant experiences, was assessed within 11 research papers (Table 3). The most commonly cited form of non-invasive brain stimulation was anodal tDCS (9 papers), of which 7 cited improved episodic memory recall (6467, 6971), and 2 did not (37, 68).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Episodic memory research.

The most common means of assessing episodic memory was via word learning and recall tasks. Manenti et al. (65) identified that tDCS over the left DLPFC and parietal cortex during retrieval augmented recognition of words 5 min after being ambiguously presented with a list of words. Notably, bilateral stimulation improved recognition in younger subjects, whereas only left hemisphere stimulation augmented older adult performance. Brambilla et al. (64), utilising the same sample as Manenti et al. (65), extended this by using fMRI data to identify that high performing older adults on the task utilised bilateral parietal cortex/ DLPFC during retrieval, whilst low performers asymmetrically utilised the left hemisphere.

In a series of experiments, Sandrini et al. (69) utilised a word learning task which, after 24 h, was followed by participants seeing contextual reminders and subsequently receiving tDCS to the left DLPFC. This paradigm resulted in improved word recall after a delay of both 3 and 30 days. This finding was further corroborated by Manenti et al. (66), which used a similar paradigm and identified significant improvements in word recognition after a 30 day interval. Sandrini et al. (70) stimulated the left DLPFC whilst participants initially learned a series of words, and found that relative to sham tDCS improved recall accuracy 3 days post-learning, however not after 30 days. Sandrini et al. (71) tweaked this methodology, shifting left DLPFC tDCS to immediately after word encoding for 15 min. This resulted in no significant differences between sham and active stimulation 3 days post-learning, however significantly better performance after 30 days post-stimulation.

Medvedeva et al. (67) stimulated instead the left Ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) during word encoding, hypothesising that this is similarly utilised during word encoding. In line with their hypothesis, Medvedeva et al. found significantly enhanced word learning 24 h post stimulation when comparing tDCS to sham.

Two tDCS protocols were not able to identify a significant effect of tDCS on episodic memory. Arciniega et al. (37) exposed participants to a series of visual scenes whilst having either bilateral or right PFC stimulation, and found no significant benefits of stimulation on recognition of familiar scenes after a short delay. Peter et al. (68) stimulated the left DLPFC for 20 min whilst learning a series of words, however found 20 min post-learning there was no significant differences between sham and DLPFC stimulation in recall amongst older adults.

Two further paradigms explored the effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on episodic memory using sotDCS (57, 61), of which both found significant effects. Ladenbauer et al. (57), during the previously cited visuo-spatial associative memory task, found that bilateral F3 and F4 stimulation during a nap significantly improved recognition memory of visual scenes presented prior to falling asleep (as compared to no stimulation). This was hypothesised to relate to boosted slow oscillatory activity during early sleep. Conversely, Paßmann et al. (61), using a similar methodology, identified that sotDCS during early sleep impaired consolidation of visual memories, significantly reducing participants accuracy when recognising visual scenes presented prior to sleep.

Semantic Memory

Semantic memory, defined as recollection of facts and general knowledge about the world, was assessed within 3 original pieces of research. Of these papers, all used tDCS, and all 3 found significant effects of tDCS augmenting semantic memory recollection (7274). These papers are summarised in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Semantic memory research.

Ross et al. (74) asked participants to name age-matched famous people or landmarks whilst concurrently having tDCS to either the left or right anterior temporal lobe (vs. sham). Whilst this research found that tDCS alone did not significantly improve recognition, on trials where participants took longer to respond, stimulation to the left anterior temporal lobe significantly improved face recognition, whilst stimulation to the right anterior lobe significantly improved landmark recognition.

Meinzer et al. (73) assessed the effects of either left or bilateral tDCS to the primary motor cortex during a semantic word generation task. Both stimulation paradigms significantly improved accuracy vs. sham, with neither significantly outperforming the other. Martin et al. (72) also assessed the effects of tDCS to the primary motor cortex during a semantic word generation task. They found that whilst performance significantly improved for both younger and older adults, for older adults this increment was more pronounced. Moreover, in older adults such stimulation uniquely caused greater left laterality in processing, modulating network-level neural dynamics.

Procedural Memory

Procedural memory, which is the ability to learn and remember motor skills, typically outside of conscious awareness, was assessed within 6 papers (summarised in Table 5). Of these papers, 2 utilised anodal tDCS, and both identified significant effects of tDCS in augmenting procedural memory (75, 76).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Procedural memory research.

Parikh (75) stimulated the primary motor cortex approximately over the hand region (M1) as participants practiced completing a pegboard motor task. Whilst practice improved performance independent of stimulation condition, only those who received M1 tDCS maintained these gains after a delay of 35 min across numerous measures of fine motor control on this task.

Rumpf et al. (76) stimulated the left primary or premotor cortex (vs. sham) either immediately or after a short period following completing a finger tapping sequence task. This identified that stimulation to only the left primary motor cortex immediately following the task significantly improved performance after 8 h or 1 day.

A further 3 papers used sotDCS, of which all stimulated bilateral F3/F4 regions and all failed to find a significant effect of sotDCS on procedural memory tasks (54, 57, 61). Both Eggert et al. (54) and Ladenbauer et al. (57) used sotDCS during a short nap after participants completed a finger tapping sequence task, and whilst neither found a significant effect of sotDCS on procedural memory, interestingly Eggert et al. found a significant decay in performance following sleep whilst Ladenbauer et al. found a significant improvement in performance following sleep. Paßmann et al. (61) similarly failed to evidence sotDCS significantly augmenting performance on a finger tapping sequence task, although did find performance increased significantly independent of condition following a full night sleep.

Rumpf et al. (77) modulated primary motor cortex activity using tACS at different frequencies immediately following a finger tapping sequence task. Performance was subsequently re-assessed 6 h after completion of the task. Interestingly, this protocol found that alpha-tACS significantly reduced performance after the 6-h interval when compared to either sham or beta-frequency tACS, suggesting a frequency specific effect of stimulation on procedural memory consolidation.

Discussion

This systematic review aimed to explore the role of non-invasive brain stimulation in modulating different aspects of memory functioning in healthy older adults. Research was generally of a good quality, and many papers were able to generate pertinent findings. Below is a brief summary of the key findings uncovered within each memory domain, followed by a more general discussion of these findings, limitations of the current literature, and suggestions for future research.

Much non-invasive brain stimulation research focused on improving working memory competencies, with many paradigms successfully augmenting performance. Research in this domain consistently targeted prefrontal regions, with a particular preference for dorsolateral regions (presented in Figure 2). This is grounded in prior research linking this region with numerous working memory abilities during the ageing process (79). Lateral PFC regions are key areas within the frontoparietal control network (FCN), which has been shown to critically mediate working memory and attention demanding tasks (80). As such, it was unsurprising that this was a key target area along with, albeit to a lesser degree, the parietal nodes of the FCN (e.g., 50). However, not all studies showed enhanced working memory performance following DLPFC stimulation alone, with some non-significant paradigms using large samples of older adults (e.g., 29). Whether it is more efficient to target one or multiple nodes of the FCN is therefore an interesting question. The evidence from this systematic review points toward the latter, with studies targeting two regions within this network all demonstrating significant effects on working memory performance (35, 37, 39, 46), while those aiming at one specific node showing mixed results. Another pertinent question relates to the impact of stimulation delivery timing (online vs. offline). Overall, studies conducting online stimulation reported positive effects, while offline studies were less consistent.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Targeted brain areas across studies are overlaid in the brain (neurological convention is used). Colour code bars indicate the number of studies that targeted a particular brain area. This figure is for illustrative purposes only.

The role of non-invasive brain stimulation on associative memory yielded more of a convoluted image. Amongst this domain, prefrontal stimulation appeared to have little, and more of an inhibitory (60), effect than with working memory tasks. More popular stimulation targets included those along the temporoparietal cortex (Figure 2), with a variety of face-name or object-location association tasks being employed. In terms of stimulation montages, those that aimed at one area were favoured in relation to those targeting several nodes within a network. Some evidence did appear to exist for stimulation of temporoparietal regions augmenting performance, which appeared to last for up to a week (e.g., 53). This is consistent with the right temporoparietal junction being particularly involved with integrating sensory and contextual stimuli (81), hence making it a likely neuroanatomical correlate for associative memory. Whilst half of the anodal tDCS or rTMS studies failed to identify this main effect, it is worth noting that of these some did identify a trend toward improvement (e.g., 55). Stimulation on sleeping participants identified less of a consistent effect, with most studies evidencing non-significant outcomes when targeting prefrontal regions; this could suggest that associative memory is less influenced by offline consolidation processes or is less amenable to improvement (61). Nevertheless, targeting frontal regions during sleep did evince some improvements in associative memory (63), providing limited support for theories that frontal regions facilitate recollection of stabilized (i.e., well-learned) memory traces (82).

Thirdly, this review explored the current state of research exploring episodic memory augmentation. Most studies uncovered a significant effect of tDCS in augmenting episodic memory, particularly in relation to word learning and recall. Most successful research in this domain targeted either the DLPFC or VLPFC (Figure 2) and appeared to identify a left hemispheric bias in processing material (e.g., 65). This appears congruent with pre-established knowledge of left prefrontal lateralisation of verbal episodic memories (83), however builds upon this by evidencing the efficacy of augmenting activity within this region in older adults. It also builds on this by adding credence to theories that the Default Mode Network (DMN) contributes toward episodic memory functioning (71), as the DMN is known to be modulated by lateral PFC sites, which are thought to support sustained attention throughout memory tasks (84). Thus, the consistent effect of tDCS in lateral PFC regions supports models of episodic memory being modulated by DMN activity and that this is susceptible to transient external modulation.

A fourth, smaller strand of research has explored whether brain stimulation could augment older adults accessing/recollecting semantic memories, primarily in the form of word generation, or landmark/celebrity naming. Interestingly all of these evidenced a significant effect, with left lateralised stimulation for verbal material proving most effective (72). This appears to be of particular relevance to older adult populations, wherein semantic recall has been shown to increasingly recruit right lateralised neural structures in later life, which is associated with worse performance on such tasks (85). Therefore, this provides evidence of older adults being less able to efficiently recruit specialized neural networks within the dominant hemisphere during memory tasks (86). Specifically, in the context of the above findings, as well as known networks of processing semantic information, it appears as though older adults were less able to recruit frontal and medial temporal lobe networks within the left hemisphere when accessing semantic memories (85). As such, external modulation of these networks could be one method of reversing this age-related trend (87). That said, it is likely that this hemispheric bias exists primarily for verbal material, as research by Ross et al. (74) found that stimulation within the right hemisphere significantly improved location recognition, suggesting a right hemispheric bias for visual learning. Interestingly, all studies focused on this memory domain opted for online stimulation protocols.

A fifth and final area assessed was the role brain stimulation may have in augmenting procedural memories. Of these studies, finger sequence tapping techniques were most commonly used, with stimulation primarily to the primary motor cortex (Figure 2). This research suggested that stimulation during or immediately after the task was most successful in augmenting performance for a period ranging from 35 min to 1 day (75, 76). It has been suggested that older adults procedural memory progressively deteriorates (88), which is corroborated by fMRI evidence of older adults being less able to effectively recruit motor cortical networks within their brain during motor tasks (89). As such, external modulation may again be one method of strengthening activity within such motor networks sub-serving motor memory. This effect may also be frequency specific, with some evidence documented here of M1 alpha-tACS specifically impeding performance (77), suggesting a significance of this frequency in M1 network connectivity and hence consolidation. Stimulation of these regions during sleep appeared to have little effect on motor performance, suggesting this network is most pertinent either during or immediately after learning motor commands (90).

Taken together, the above provides some commonalities. For example, stimulation of the DLPFC (Figure 2) appeared to provide numerous memory benefits for healthy older adults, memory research has generally been successful in identifying an effect of non-invasive brain stimulation in improving memory performance (with perhaps the exception of associative memory), and that certain neural networks (like the DMN and FCN) may constitute better targets for improving memory performance than focusing on separate nodes. Nevertheless, this review also identified some contradictory results. This comes within a context of research being of generally very good quality, using randomisation, blinding, and minimising experimenter effects. It is therefore apparent that some of the classical assumptions underlying much of this research, such as stimulation of the DLPFC alone improving memory (91), may warrant reconsideration. For instance, one way of re-interpreting this could be that the DLPFC is a vital site for attention rather than memory, which has accrued recent interest amongst brain stimulation research (92). Stimulation of this site may therefore improve one's ability to concentrate, hence this site indirectly supporting memory as information can only be encoded if attended to. This could therefore explain inconsistent findings amongst such research, as healthy individuals have been shown to vary in attentional capacity considerably later in life (93), which was not controlled for within the above studies.

Amongst the papers evaluated, numerous additional theories were posited to explain the above findings. For example, Stephens et al. (48) articulated that certain variants of the COMT genotype result in greater or diminished working memory faculties in older adults. Moreover, those with an enhanced working memory capacity COMT genotype responded preferentially to tDCS, whilst those with a diminished working memory capacity COMT genotype responded poorly to tDCS. It may therefore be plausible that variants of the COMT genotype differentially influence amenability to non-invasive brain stimulation. Such exploration of genotype-brain stimulation interactions could be extended by also exploring the role other pertinent genes play in memory. For example, the APOE gene has been recognised as potentially mediating long-term memory functioning (94), and it is interesting to note that different versions of the APOE gene result in strikingly different neural responses to brain stimulation (95). Thus, it is evident that participants may respond differently to brain stimulation as a function of gene expression and other physiological traits.

Additionally, the efficacy of brain stimulation could relate to degree of education, as Berryhill and Jones (38) identified tDCS selectively augmented working memory amongst older adults who were highly educated, whilst having little to no effect amongst those less educated. This was hypothesised to relate to better educated older adults recruiting different neural structures during working memory tasks, and would again explain individual differences in response to transcranial stimulation. These examples illustrate that both experience and genetics could impact individual's responsiveness to brain stimulation, which should be considered when assessing different outcomes observed from different samples.

A further source of inconsistency may derive from definitions of different memory modalities, both within this literature review itself as well as within primary research. Indeed, there were some discrepancies between what constituted as a test of working, associative, and episodic memory, which could result in articles being mis-represented or mis-reported. This is to be perhaps expected, given that myriad definitions of working memory alone already exist (96). Moreover, classifying memory itself can be troublesome, as memory domains can themselves be sub-divided. For example, episodic memory could be divided into verbal and visual domains which, as cited above, may each have unique neural correlates (97). Thus, it is possible that more nuanced effects exist than given credit for in this article. Nevertheless, if a semblance of order is to be given to the above research some degree of coherent memory classification will be necessary moving forward.

One alternative explanation for the above results could relate to the ageing brain functionally changing over the course of the lifespan. Increasing evidence suggests that the hemispheric biases reported earlier become less apparent as we age, with increased bilateral activity in the ageing brain (98). Brain imaging and memory research suggests that previously superior hemispheres increasingly lose their enhanced ability to process certain information over the lifespan, causing contralateral structures to increasingly activate to compensate for this loss (99). Indeed, this was explicitly highlighted within the research above. For example, older adults increasingly relied on bilateral de-differentiated neural networks during semantic memory tasks (72), instead of left hemispheric frontal-temporal networks specifically sub-serving semantic knowledge in younger individuals (85). Further evidence of more globalised activity comes from Di Rosa et al.'s (42) finding that left PFC tDCS increased bilateral PFC activity (which subsequently improved working memory performance), whilst Manenti et al. (60) showed that high performing older adults on episodic memory tasks responded equally to bilateral (rather than lateral) DLPFC stimulation. Together, this suggests bilateral hemispheric recruitment could act as a compensatory plastic strategy to support the ageing brain, in contrast to younger adult's memory faculties which might place more reliance on specialized localised circuits (100). This would also explain why paradigms such as Leach et al. (59) uncovered significant improvements in memory functioning following tDCS only with younger adults, with the above functional reorganization likely resulting in the two groups benefitting from different forms of stimulation. As such, it is possible that older adults memory networks qualitatively differ from those seen amongst younger adults.

As mentioned, there were limitations when conducting this systematic review. Firstly, due to the nature of extant literature, sample sizes included in this review were often small, which increased the risk of insufficient statistical power to yield a significant result, biased effect sizes, and unreliable results (101). This appeared to permeate the literature and may contribute toward some of the inconsistent findings observed. Furthermore, it is possible that the searching strategy missed relevant articles by its choice of terms used. Whilst this research chose to search for the term “memory” within the body of articles selected, and used similar terms to those successfully used elsewhere [e.g., (102)], it is possible that other less relevant terms, such as “learning,” could have identified a limited number of additional results. This choice equally meant papers written in other languages were excluded. Furthermore, due to the volume of included articles it has not been possible to cite all potential confounds or methodological questions within the body of this paper. Whilst quality checks were performed and summarised within this article to give an indication of the credibility of those findings reported, it is possible (although not anticipated) that bias exists within those papers cited that has not been captured here. Finally, it has been noted elsewhere that a publication bias may exist amongst studies of non-invasive brain stimulation in augmenting memory amongst those with dementia (30). This should be considered when assessing the above findings with healthy older adults, as this could distort the true landscape of brain stimulation research.

That said, the findings above do have numerous implications. Firstly, numerous research paradigms appeared to evidence a significant effect of non-invasive brain stimulation in augmenting working memory and episodic memory, both of which appear to be significantly impacted by a range of organic dementias (103). Whilst the above was conducted amongst healthy older adults, it is possible these same findings could be extrapolated to support those with early signs of pathological memory loss. Indeed, many of the same regions and mechanisms targeted by the above research are similarly implicated in Alzheimer's disease, with preliminary research showing these areas remain sensitive to non-invasive brain stimulation amongst those with dementia (104). Furthermore, whilst research included here involved “healthy” participants, many studies did not assess participants for prodromal biomarkers of dementia. It is therefore possible that many included participants may already have a predisposition to develop a dementia (105), similarly suggesting that brain stimulation may be a useful early intervention in supporting cognitive functioning. As such, an exciting future avenue for brain stimulation research may be to study if these same effects hold true amongst people with early signs of memory loss, potentially providing an opportunity to reduce the impact such changes may be having on their lives.

The above research also does much to prove that non-invasive brain stimulation is a safe, well-tolerated, and often effective approach for mitigating age-related changes in memory performance. Whilst effects varied in length, it was of particular interest to see that some paradigms were able to induce long lasting effects in memory performance. For example, Jones et al. (44) identified that 10 sessions of tDCS was sufficient to induce changes in working memory performance for up to 1-month post stimulation. Given that memory changes are something which are commonplace and distressing amongst older adults (3, 6), the potential to reduce these concerns for up to a month at a time is something which holds utility when considering the functional well-being of the ageing population. Nevertheless, it is likely that these same effects would not be possible with fewer rounds of brain stimulation, with the above research largely failing to find long-standing effects after only single doses of tDCS (e.g., 69). It is unclear as yet what exact mechanisms are associated with this dose-response curve and how dose-response will translate into risk and benefit. Nevertheless, numerous theories have been put forward to explain long-term effects, mostly related to long-term potentiation within neural networks (106). This emphasizes the need to investigate dose-response relationships when supporting maximal cognitive functioning in older adults.

This literature review also does much to identify areas where current non-invasive brain stimulation literature could expand further. Firstly, little research with healthy older adults appears to have used modalities other than tDCS, despite there being increasing evidence of the success of targeting neural networks using other approaches such as tACS (107). The application of tACS, for example, offers greater potential for individualized approaches that target networks using subject-specific tuned-frequencies [e.g., (35)]. It is also of interest to see so few articles using rTMS; this is surprising, given the extensive literature on rTMS applications to neurological and psychiatric disorders [e.g., (108, 109)]. In terms of memory domains, an area to be explored further is associative memory, as this was one domain generating particularly inconclusive findings. Whilst this does appear to be particularly susceptible to the ageing process (110, 111), many studies have failed to identify statistically significant methods of augmenting this form of memory. As such, protocols to target associative memory should be explored further to identify if it is indeed possible to reliably augment this domain. Finally, for the opposite reason, it would be of interest to explore further the impact of non-invasive brain stimulation on semantic memory, as this an area which appears to have not been explored greatly but uncovered a generally consistent effect.

To conclude, this systematic literature review has aimed to bring some degree of clarity into the current state of research into non-invasive brain stimulation in modulating healthy older adult's memory. In doing so it has uncovered that numerous approaches have been used to target five areas of memory, and that with the exception of associative memory these have uncovered a degree of consistency in effects on memory performance. Particularly established methods of augmenting memory performance included using tDCS to improve working and episodic memory performance, whilst semantic memory appeared to be similarly susceptible although less thoroughly researched. The above should be used to guide future researchers in their endeavours to better understand how to support older adults as their memory changes.

Author Contributions

RG: conception of review, literature review, and wrote the manuscript. JR: literature review, table, and figure construction. IV: conception of review, support writing manuscript, and figure construction. All authors contributed to manuscript revisions, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

IV was funded by the BBSRC (Ref: BB/S008314/1). This systematic review was unfunded and completed as part of a doctorate in Clinical Psychology at the University of Surrey, UK.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Leon DA. Trends in European life expectancy: a salutary view. Int J Epidemiol. (2011) 40:271–7. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Reid LM, MacLullich AM. Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dementia Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2000) 22:471–85. doi: 10.1159/000096295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Craik FI, Luo L, Sakuta Y. Effects of aging and divided attention on memory for items and their contexts. Psychol Aging. (2010) 25:968–79. doi: 10.1037/a0020276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. James LE, Fogler KA, Tauber SK. Recognition memory measures yield disproportionate effects of aging on learning face-name associations. Psychol Aging. (2008) 23:657–664. doi: 10.1037/a0013008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rosen AC, Prull MW, O'Hara R, Race EA, Desmond JE, Glover GH, et al. Variable effects of aging on frontal lobe contributions to memory. Ageing. (2002) 13:2425–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200212200-00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Dark-Freudeman A, West RL, Viverito KM. Future selves and aging: older adults' memory fears. Educ Gerontol. (2006) 32:85–109. doi: 10.1080/03601270500388125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Squire LR, Knowlton B, Musen G. The structure and organization of memory. Annual Rev Psychol. (1993) 44:453–95. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.002321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Baddeley A. Working memory. Science. (1992) 255:556–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1736359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Suzuki WA. Integrating associative learning signals across the brain. Hippocampus. (2007) 17:842–50. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Harada CN, Love MC, Triebel K. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. (2013) 29:737–52. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Nilsson L. Memory function in normal aging. Acta Neurol Scand. (2003) 107:7–13. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.107.s179.5.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Parikh PK, Troyer AK, Maione AM, Murphy KJ. The impact of memory change on daily life in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Gerontologist. (2015) 56:877–85. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnv030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Jonker C, Geerlings MI, Schmand B. Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatr. (2000) 15:983–91. https://doi.org/10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11%3C983::AID-GPS238%3E3.0.CO;2-5

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

14. Gonçalves DC, Albuquerque PB, Paul C. Aging clinical and experimental research. Aging Clin Exp. (2009) 21:369–71. doi: 10.1007/BF03324931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

15. Crook TH, Ferris SH, Alvarez XA, Laredo M, Moessler H. Effects of N-PEP-12 on memory among older adults. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. (2005) 20:97–100. doi: 10.1097/00004850-200503000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Macpherson H, Ellis KA, Sali A, Pipingas A. Memory improvements in elderly women following 16 weeks treatment with a combined multivitamin, mineral and herbal supplement. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 220:351–65. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2481-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Belleville S, Gilbert B, Fontaine F, Gagnon L, Ménard É, Gauthier S. Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dementia Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2006) 22:486–99. doi: 10.1159/000096316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Richmond LL, Morrison AB, Chein JM, Olson IR. Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychol Aging. (2011) 26:813–22. doi: 10.1037/a0023631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kadosh RC. (ed.). The Stimulated Brain: Cognitive Enhancement Using Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation. London: Elsevier (2014).

Google Scholar

20. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Ruff CC. Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat Neurosci. (2018) 21:174–87. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Miniussi C, Harris JA, Ruzzoli M. Modelling non-invasive brain stimulation in cognitive neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37:1702–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Zimerman M, Hummel FC. Non-invasive brain stimulation: Enhancing motor and cognitive functions in healthy old subjects. Front Aging Neurosci. (2010) 2:149. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Li LM, Violante IR, Leech R, Ross E, Hampshire A, Opitz A, et al. Brain state and polarity dependent modulation of brain networks by transcranial direct current stimulation. Hum Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:904–15. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Herrmann CS, Rach S, Neuling T, Strüber D. Transcranial alternating current stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Front Human Neurosci. (2013) 7:279. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Krause MR, Vieira PG, Csorba BA, Pilly PK, Pack CC. Transcranial alternating current stimulation entrains single-neuron activity in the primate brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2019) 116:5747–55. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1815958116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Violante IR, Li LM, Carmichael DW, Lorenz R, Leech R, Hampshire A, et al. Externally induced frontoparietal synchronization modulates network dynamics and enhances working memory performance. ELife. (2017) 6:e22001. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fried I. Brain stimulation and memory. Brain. (2015) 138:1766–7. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Deldar Z, Rustamov N, Blanchette I, Piché M. Improving working memory and pain inhibition in older persons using transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci Res. (2017) 148:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2018.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nilsson J, Lebedev AV, Rydström A, Lövdén M. Direct-current stimulation does little to improve the outcome of working-memory training in older adults. Psychol Sci. (2017) 28:907–20. doi: 10.1177/0956797617698139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hsu W, Ku Y, Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Effects of noninvasive brain stimulation on cognitive function in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging. (2015) 36:2348–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hill AT, Fitzgerald PB, Hoy KE. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory: a systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimulation. (2016) 9:197–208. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Birba A, Ibáñez A, Sedeño L, Ferrari J, García AM, Zimerman M. Non-invasive brain stimulation: a new strategy in mild cognitive impairment? Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:16. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Park DC, Festini SB. Theories of memory and aging: a look at the past and a glimpse of the future. J Gerontol Seri. (2017) 72:82–90. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Antes G, Atkins D, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Int J Surg. (2010) 8:336–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2010.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Reinhart RM, Nguyen JA. Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits. Nat Neurosci. (2019) 22:820–7. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0371-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Critical Appraisal Skills Programme. CASP Randomised Controlled Trial Checklist. (2018) Available online at: https://casp-uk.net/casp-tools-checklists/ (accessed 11 December, 2019).

37. Arciniega H, Gözenman F, Jones KT, Stephens JA, Berryhill ME. Frontoparietal tDCS benefits visual working memory in older adults with low working memory capacity. Front Aging Neurosci. (2018) 10:57. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Berryhill ME, Jones KT. tDCS selectively improves working memory in older adults with more education. Neurosci Lett. (2012) 521:148–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Borghini G, Candini M, Filannino C, Hussain M, Walsh V, Romei V, et al. Alpha oscillations are causally linked to inhibitory abilities in ageing. J Neurosci. (2018) 38:4418–29. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1285-17.2018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cespón J, Rodella C, Rossini PM, Miniussi C, Pellicciari MC. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation promotes frontal compensatory mechanisms in healthy elderly subjects. Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:420. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Cespón J, Rodella C, Miniussi C, Pellicciari MC. Behavioural and electrophysiological modulations induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy elderly and Alzheimer's disease patients: a pilot study. Clin Neurophysiol. (2019) 130:2038–52. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.08.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Di Rosa E, Brigadoi S, Cutini S, Tarantino V, Dell'Acqua R, Mapelli D, et al. Reward motivation and neurostimulation interact to improve working memory performance in healthy older adults: a simultaneous tDCS-fNIRS study. NeuroImage. (2019) 202:116062. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Emonson MRL, Fitzgerald PB, Rogasch NC, Hoy KE. Neurobiological effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in younger adults, older adults and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia. (2019) 125:51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jones KT, Stephens JA, Alam M, Bikson M, Berryhill ME. Correction: longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0129751. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Nilsson J, Lebedev AV, Lövdén M. No significant effect of prefrontal tDCS on working memory performance in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. (2015) 7:230. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Park SH, Seo JH, Kim YH, Ko MH. Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. NeuroReport. (2014) 25:122–6. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Stephens JA. Enhancing everyday cognition in healthy older adults via non-invasive neurostimulation and memory training: An integration of neuroscience and rehabilitation science. [doctoral dissertation]. [Reno (NV)]: University of Nevada (2015).

Google Scholar

48. Stephens JA, Jones KT, Berryhill ME. Task demands, tDCS intensity, and the COMT val158met polymorphism impact tDCS-linked working memory training gains. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:13463. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14030-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

49. Stoynova N, Laske C, Plewnia C. Combining electrical stimulation and cognitive control training to reduce concerns about subjective cognitive decline. Brain Stimul. (2019) 12:1083–5. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Yamanaka K, Tomioka H, Kawasaki S, Noda Y, Yamagata B, Iwanami A, et al. Effect of parietal transcranial magnetic stimulation on spatial working memory in healthy elderly persons - comparison of near infrared spectroscopy for young and elderly. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e102306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Antonenko D, Külzow N, Sousa A, Prehn K, Grittner U, Flöel A. Neuronal and behavioral effects of multi-day brain stimulation and memory training. Neurobiol Aging. (2018) 61:245–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Antonenko D, Hayek D, Netzband J, Grittner U, Flöel A. tDCS-induced episodic memory enhancement and its association with functional network coupling in older adults. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:2273. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-38630-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Davis SW, Luber B, Murphy DLK, Lisanby SH, Cabeza R. Frequency-specific neuromodulation of local and distant connectivity in aging and episodic memory function. Hum Brain Mapp. (2017) 38:5987–6004. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Eggert T, Dorn H, Sauter C, Nitsche MA, Bajbouj M, Danker-Hopfe H. No effects of slow oscillatory transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on sleep-dependent memory consolidation in healthy elderly subjects. Brain Stimulation. (2013) 6:938–45. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Flöel A, Suttorp W, Kohl O, Kürten J, Lohmann H, Breitenstein C, et al. Non-invasive brain stimulation improves object-location learning in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging. (2012) 33:1682–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Külzow N, Cavalcanti de Sousa AV, Cesarz M, Hanke JM, Günsberg A, Harder S, et al. No effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on multiple sessions of object-location-memory training in healthy older adults. Front Neurosci. (2018) 11:746. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Ladenbauer J, Külzow N, Passmann S, Antonenko D, Grittner U, Tamm S, et al. Brain stimulation during an afternoon nap boosts slow oscillatory activity and memory consolidation in older adults. NeuroImage. (2016) 142:311–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Leach RC, McCurdy MP, Trumbo MC, Matzen LE, Leshikar ED. Transcranial stimulation over the left inferior frontal gyrus increases false alarms in an associative memory task in older adults. Healthy Aging Res. (2016) 5:1–6. doi: 10.1097/01.HXR.0000491108.83234.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Leach RC, McCurdy MP, Trumbo MC, Matzen LE, Leshikar ED. Differential age effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on associative memory. J Gerontol. (2018) 74:1163–73. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Manenti R, Cotelli M, Miniussi C. Successful physiological aging and episodic memory: a brain stimulation study. Behav Brain Res. (2011) 216:153–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.07.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Paßmann S, Külzow N, Ladenbauer J, Antonenko D, Grittner U, Tamm S, et al. Boosting slow oscillatory activity using tDCS during early nocturnal slow wave sleep does not improve memory consolidation in healthy older adults. Brain Stimulation. (2016) 9:730–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Prehn K, Stengl H, Grittner U, Kosiolek R, Ölschläger A, Weidemann A, et al. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation and serotonergic enhancement on memory performance in young and older adults. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:551–61. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Westerberg CE, Florczak SM, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM, Marshall L, Zee PC, et al. Memory improvement via slow-oscillatory stimulation during sleep in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. (2015) 36:2577–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Brambilla M, Manenti R, Ferrari C, Cotelli M. Better together: left and right hemisphere engagement to reduce age-related memory loss. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 293:125–33. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Manenti R, Brambilla M, Petesi M, Ferrari C, Cotelli M. Enhancing verbal episodic memory in older and young subjects after non-invasive brain stimulation. Front Aging Neurosci. (2013) 5:49. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Manenti R, Sandrini M, Gobbi E, Cobelli C, Brambilla M, Binetti G, et al. Strengthening of existing episodic memories through non-invasive stimulation of prefrontal cortex in older adults with subjective memory complaints. Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:401. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Medvedeva A, Materassi M, Neacsu V, Beresford-Webb J, Hussin A, Khan N, et al. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex on episodic memory formation and retrieval. Cereb Cortex. (2018) 29:657–65. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Peter J, Neumann-Dunayevska E, Geugelin F, Ninosu N, Plewnia C, Klöppel S. Reducing negative affect with anodal transcranial direct current stimulation increases memory performance in young—but not in elderly—individuals. Brain Structure Funct. (2019) 224:2973–82. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01946-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Sandrini M, Brambilla M, Manenti R, Rosini S, Cohen LG, Cotelli M. Noninvasive stimulation of prefrontal cortex strengthens existing episodic memories and reduces forgetting in the elderly. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:289. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Sandrini M, Manenti R, Brambilla M, Cobelli C, Cohen LG, Cotelli M. Older adults get episodic memory boosting from noninvasive stimulation of prefrontal cortex during learning. Neurobiol Aging. (2016) 39:210–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Sandrini M, Manenti R, Gobbi E, Rusich D, Bartl G, Cotelli M. Transcranial direct current stimulation applied after encoding facilitates episodic memory consolidation in older adults. Neurobiol Learn Memory. (2019) 163:107037. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Martin AK, Meinzer M, Lindenberg R, Sieg MM, Nachtigall L, Flöel A. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on neural networks structure in young and older adults. J Cognit Neurosci. (2017) 29:1817–28. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Meinzer M, Lindenberg R, Sieg MM, Nachtigall L, Ulm L, Flöel A. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex improves word-retrieval in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:253. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Ross LA, McCoy D, Coslett HB, Olson IR, Wolk DA. Improved proper name recall in aging after electrical stimulation of the anterior temporal lobes. Front Aging Neurosci. (2011) 3:16. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Parikh PJ. Handling Objects in Old Age. [doctoral thesis]. [Iowa City (IA)]: University of Iowa (2012).

Google Scholar

76. Rumpf JJ, Wegscheider M, Hinselmann K, Fricke C, King BR, Weise D, et al. Enhancement of motor consolidation by post-training transcranial direct current stimulation in older people. Neurobiol Aging. (2017) 49:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Rumpf JJ, Barbu A, Fricke C, Wegscheider M, Classen J. Posttraining alpha transcranial alternating current stimulation impairs motor consolidation inElderly People. Neural Plasticity. (2019) 2019:2689790. doi: 10.1155/2019/2689790

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Klem GH, Lüders HO, Jasper HH, Elger C. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. (1999) 52:3–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

79. Fregni F, Boggio PS, Nitsche M, Bermpohl F, Antal A, Feredoes E, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp Brain Res. (2005) 166:23–30. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Keller JB, Hedden T, Thompson TW, Anteraper SA, Gabrieli JD, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Resting-state anticorrelations between medial and lateral prefrontal cortex: association with working memory, aging, and individual differences. Cortex. (2015) 64:271–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Jakobs O, Langner R, Caspers S, Roski C, Cieslik EC, Zilles K, et al. Across-study and within-subject functional connectivity of a right temporo-parietal junction subregion involved in stimulus–context integration. Neuroimage. (2012) 60:2389–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Takashima A, Nieuwenhuis IL, Rijpkema M, Petersson KM, Jensen O, Fernández G. Memory trace stabilization leads to large-scale changes in the retrieval network: a functional MRI study on associative memory. Learn Memory. (2007) 14:472–9. doi: 10.1101/lm.605607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Kelley WM, Miezin FM, McDermott KB, Buckner RL, Raichle ME, Cohen NJ, et al. Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron. (1998) 20:927–36. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80474-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Sambataro F, Murty VP, Callicott JH, Tan HY, Das S, Weinberger DR, et al. Age-related alterations in default mode network: impact on working memory performance. Neurobiol Aging. (2010) 31:839–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Meinzer M, Flaisch T, Wilser L, Eulitz C, Rockstroh B, Conway T, et al. Neural signatures of semantic and phonemic fluency in young and old adults. J Cogn Neurosci. (2009) 21:2007–18. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Rajah MN, D'Esposito M. Region-specific changes in prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. Brain. (2005) 128:1964–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Meinzer M, Lindenberg R, Antonenko D, Flaisch T, Flöel A. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation temporarily reverses age-associated cognitive decline and functional brain activity changes. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:12470–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5743-12.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Roig M, Ritterband-Rosenbaum A, Lundbye-Jensen J, Nielsen JB. Aging increases the susceptibility to motor memory interference and reduces off-line gains in motor skill learning. Neurobiol Aging. (2014) 35:1892–900. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.02.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ni Z, Isayama R, Castillo G, Gunraj C, Saha U, Chen R. Reduced dorsal premotor cortex and primary motor cortex connectivity in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. (2015) 36:301–3. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Muellbacher W, Ziemann U, Wissel J, Dang N, Kofler M, Facchini S, et al. Early consolidation in human primary motor cortex. Nature. (2002) 415:640–4. doi: 10.1038/nature712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Javadi AH, Walsh V. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates declarative memory. Brain Stimulation. (2012) 5:231–41. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Brosnan MB, Wiegand I. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a dynamic cortical area to enhance top-down attentional control. J Neurosci. (2017) 37:3445–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0136-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Duchek JM, Balota DA, Tse CS, Holtzman DM, Fagan AM, Goate AM. The utility of intraindividual variability in selective attention tasks as an early marker for Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychol. (2009) 23:746–58. doi: 10.1037/a0016583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Rajah MN, Wallace LM, Ankudowich E, Yu EH, Swierkot A, Patel R, et al. Family history and APOE4 risk for Alzheimer's disease impact the neural correlates of episodic memory by early midlife. NeuroImage. (2017) 14:760–74. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Peña-Gomez C, Solé-Padullés C, Clemente IC, Junqué C, Bargalló N, Bosch B, et al. APOE status modulates the changes in network connectivity induced by brain stimulation in non-demented elders. PloS ONE. (2012) 7:e51833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Cowan N. The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bull Rev. (2017) 24:1158–70. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1191-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Zammit AR, Ezzati A, Zimmerman ME, Lipton RB, Lipton ML, Katz MJ. Roles of hippocampal subfields in verbal and visual episodic memory. Behav Brain Res. (2017) 317:157–62. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Dolcos F, Rice HJ, Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: Right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2002) 26:819–25. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00068-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Collins K, Mohr C. Performance of younger and older adults in lateralised right and left hemisphere asymmetry tasks supports the HAROLD model. Laterality. (2013) 18, 491–512. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2012.724072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Pascual-Leone A, Freitas C, Oberman L, Horvath JC, Halko M, Eldaief M, et al. Characterizing brain cortical plasticity and network dynamics across the age-span in health and disease with TMS-EEG and TMS-fMRI. Brain Topogr. (2011) 24:302–15. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0196-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2013) 14:365–76. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Brunoni AR, Vanderhasselt MA. Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. (2014) 86:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. van Geldorp B, Heringa SM, Van Den Berg E, Olde Rikkert MG, Biessels GJ, Kessels RP. Working memory binding and episodic memory formation in aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2015) 37:538–48. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1037722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Ahmed MA, Darwish ES, Khedr EM, Ali AM. Effects of low versus high frequencies of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cognitive function and cortical excitability in Alzheimer's dementia. J Neurol. (2012) 259:83–92. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6128-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Wagner M, Wolf S, Reischies FM, Daerr M, Wolfsgruber S, Jessen F, et al. Biomarker validation of a cued recall memory deficit in prodromal Alzheimer disease. Neurology. (2012) 78:379–86. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318245f447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Elsner B, Kwakkel G, Kugler J, Mehrholz J. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving capacity in activities and arm function after stroke: a network meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. J Neuroengineer Rehabil. (2017) 14:95. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0301-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Bender M, Romei V, Sauseng P. Slow theta tACS of the right parietal cortex enhances contralateral visual working memory capacity. Brain Topogr. (2019) 32:477–81. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00702-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Eldaief MC, Press DZ, Pascual-Leone A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in neurology: a review of established and prospective applications. Neurology. (2013) 3:519–26. doi: 10.1212/01.CPJ.0000436213.11132.8e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Wassermann EM, Lisanby SH. Therapeutic application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: a review. Clin Neurophysiol. (2001) 112:1367–77. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00585-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Naveh-Benjamin M, Guez J, Kilb A, Reedy S. The associative memory deficit of older adults: Further support using face-name associations. Psychol Aging. (2004) 19:541–6. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Old SR, Naveh-Benjamin M. Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging. (2008) 23:104–18. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: non-invasive brain stimulation, ageing, older adult's, memory, transcranial magnetic stimulation, transcranial direct current stimulation, transcranial alternating current stimulation, systematic review

Citation: Goldthorpe RA, Rapley JM and Violante IR (2020) A Systematic Review of Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Applications to Memory in Healthy Aging. Front. Neurol. 11:575075. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.575075

Received: 22 June 2020; Accepted: 11 September 2020;
Published: 19 October 2020.

Edited by:

Francesco Di Lorenzo, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Alberto Benussi, University of Brescia, Italy
Asli Demirtas-Tatlidede, Bahçeşehir University, Turkey

Copyright © 2020 Goldthorpe, Rapley and Violante. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robin A. Goldthorpe, R.Goldthorpe@surrey.ac.uk; Ines R. Violante, Ines.Violante@surrey.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.