Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 22 August 2019
Sec. Stroke

Accuracy of Individuals Post-hemiparetic Stroke in Matching Torques Between Arms Depends on the Arm Referenced

  • 1Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 2Department of Biomechanical Engineering, Delft University of Technology, Delft, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, United States

Background: Prior work indicates that 50–75% of individuals post-hemiparetic stroke have upper-extremity weakness and, in turn, inaccurately judge the relative torques that their arms generate during a bimanual task. Recent findings also reveal that these individuals judge the relative torques their arms generate differently depending on whether they reference their paretic vs. non-paretic arm.

Objective: Our goal was to determine whether individuals with hemiparetic stroke inaccurately matched torques between arms, regardless of the arm that they referenced.

Methods: Fifteen participants with hemiparetic stroke and 10 right-hand dominant controls matched torques between arms. Participants performed this task with their right arm referencing their left arm, and vice versa. Participants generated (1) 5 Nm and (2) 25% of their reference elbow's maximum voluntary torque (MVT) in flexion and extension using their reference arm while receiving audiovisual feedback. Then, participants matched the reference torque using their opposite arm without receiving feedback on their matching performance.

Results: Participants with stroke had greater magnitudes of error in matching torques than controls when referencing their paretic arm (p < 0.050), yet not when referencing their non-paretic arm (p > 0.050). The mean magnitude of error when participants with stroke referenced their paretic and non-paretic arm and controls referenced their dominant and non-dominant arm to generate 5 Nm in flexion was 9.4, 2.6, 4.2, and 2.5 Nm, respectively, and in extension was 5.3, 2.8, 2.5, and 2.3 Nm, respectively. However, when the torques generated at each arm were normalized by the corresponding MVT, no differences were found in matching errors regardless of the arm participants referenced (p > 0.050).

Conclusions: Results demonstrate the importance of the arm referenced, i.e., paretic vs. non-paretic, on how accurately individuals post-hemiparetic stroke judge their torques during a bimanual task. Results also indicate that individuals with hemiparetic stroke judge torques primarily based on their perceived effort. Finally, findings support the notion that training individuals post-hemiparetic stroke to accurately perceive their self-generated torques, with a focus of their non-paretic arm in relation to their paretic arm, may lead to an improved ability to perform bimanual activities of daily living.

1. Introduction

Inaccurately judging the relative torques that one generates with their arm(s) can make it difficult to perform bimanual activities of daily living, such as carrying a tray, pushing a grocery cart, and caring for an infant (1, 2). An estimated 500,000 individuals who survive a hemiparetic stroke each year in the USA inaccurately judge the relative torques that they generate between arms (311). Even so, a gap remains in our understanding of the extent to which these erroneous judgments occur. Our goal in this work was to determine whether individuals with hemiparetic stroke inaccurately match torques between arms when referencing each arm while flexing and extending about their elbow.

Assessments of one's ability to accurately judge their self-generated torques often request an individual to generate symmetric torques between their limbs without receiving feedback on their matching performance (4, 5, 719). Based on findings from such assessments, the current understanding is that individuals perceive their self-generated torques by combining peripheral and central information (1, 15, 2026). Peripheral information arises from muscle and cutaneous mechanoreceptors (22, 27). Muscle mechanoreceptors include Golgi tendon organs (2831) and muscle spindles (32, 33), and cutaneous mechanoreceptors include slow-adapting and fast-adapting units (3437). Central information arises from an individual's perceived effort, or the extent to which an individual perceives that their arm muscles are being driven in relation to the maximum. Findings from ongoing research indicate that the greater an individual's strength asymmetry, the poorer their ability to accurately perceive torques between limbs (4, 711, 1319, 38, 39). In turn, the general understanding is that: (i) perceived effort information generated in the cortex is weighed more heavily than sensory information arising from the periphery (2) and (ii) matching errors occur because individuals do not adapt their perceived effort at their affected limb to its weakened state. Findings from the literature have also provided controversial results as to whether arm dominance affects an individual's judgment of their self-generated forces (10, 12, 16, 19, 4044).

We know that, post-stroke, portions of an individual's motor pathways (e.g., corticospinal tracts) may be damaged (45), leading to reduced motor unit rate modulation and mean firing rates (46), as well as muscular atrophy (47). In turn, individuals post-hemiparetic stroke may generate a maximal effort at their paretic arm that results in a lower torque output than a maximal effort at their non-paretic arm. Researchers have suggested that individuals with hemiparetic stroke are unable to adapt their perceived effort in their paretic limb to its weakened state and, in turn, do not generate symmetric torques in the absence of visual and corrective feedback (4, 5, 711). Our preliminary results, however, challenge this understanding by suggesting that the ability to accurately judge self-generated torques may depend on whether individuals with hemiparetic stroke reference their paretic vs. non-paretic arm (10). Moreover, Yen and Li (11) reported that the accuracy with which torques are matched between arms changes depending on the arm that individuals reference.

In this work, we assessed an individual's ability to accurately judge their self-generated torques between arms when referencing each arm. We ran this assessment during elbow flexion and extension tasks so that we could determine the extent to which impairments in individuals with stroke depend on the direction of the applied force. We compared the accuracy of individuals with stroke in matching torques between arms to individuals without neurological impairments (i.e., controls) so that we could gain an improved understanding of the degree to which their judgment may be erroneous. By using a mechatronic system to execute our assessment, we could control the user's interaction through automated audiovisual cues, such that the delivery of the assessment was highly repeatable. Additionally, we could quantify the user's ability to accurately match torques between arms to a high degree of resolution, such that the outcome measures were objective and highly sensitive. Based on our preliminary work in this area (10), we hypothesized that individuals with hemiparetic stroke would match torques between arms with a similar accuracy as controls if they referenced their non-paretic arm, yet inaccurately if they referenced their paretic arm. This asymmetry in their ability to accurately match torques between arms would support our hypothesis that individuals with hemiparetic stroke do not learn post-injury how to perceive their non-paretic arm in relation to their paretic arm.

2. Methods

We refer the reader to van der Helm et al. (10) for a more in-depth description of the experimental methods.

2.1. Participants

This study was approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board (STU00021840), and all participants provided written informed consent. Non-Northwestern University employees were monetarily compensated. Participants were required to: have the capacity to provide informed consent; understand and be able to execute the task; and not have any serious upper-extremity or peripheral nerve injury that may interfere with their torque generation/perception. Participants were assessed for their arm dominance using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (48).

Participants with stroke were screened by a licensed physical therapist (co-author Dr. Justin Drogos, PT/DPT/NCS) for potentially relevant impairments. This therapist assessed these participants for motor and sensory impairments using the upper-extremity Fugl-Meyer Motor Assessment (UE FMA) (49) and revised Nottingham Sensory Assessment (rNSA) (50), respectively. In addition to the recruitment criteria listed above, participants with stroke were required to have: a single unilateral brain lesion greater than 6 months prior to testing; lesion(s) located above the brainstem, yet not in the cerebellum; not used antispastic agents (e.g., baclofen) in the past 6 months that may impact their task performance; and no comorbid neurological impairments. Lesion location(s) was determined from medical records and T1/T2 MRI scans.

2.2. Experimental Setup

The experimental setup used to quantify how accurately and precisely participants matched torques between arms is shown in Figure 1. The participant's self-generated elbow torque was measured at each arm using a one-degree-of-freedom load sensor (OMEGA Engineering Inc. LCM201-300; Stamford, CT, USA). A monitor provided the participant with a visual indicator of the magnitude of their self-generated torque at their reference arm, and speakers provided the participant with auditory cues indicating the actions to execute with each arm throughout the duration of each trial. Trial-related data were stored at 1 kHz.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup and protocol. (A) Shown is the mechatronic system that was used to assess each participant's accuracy and precision in matching torques between arms when referencing each arm. The participant received visual feedback on the monitor about their reference arm's applied torque (red circle), the target torque (dark blue circle), and the allowable range of applied torques (cyan inner and outer circles). (B) The participant generated a reference torque in flexion or extension with their reference arm (light purple) while receiving visual feedback, and then matched this torque using their opposite, i.e., indicator, arm (dark purple). Written informed consent was obtained for the publication of the image. Images are adapted from van der Helm et al. (10) ©IEEE.

2.3. Torque-Matching Trial

The participant was instructed to match, between arms, sub-maximal isometric torques that they generated about their elbow joints as accurately as possible. Automated audio cues were played to encourage that the events occurring throughout a trial were consistent across trials and participants.

Figure 2 visually depicts the torques that the participant generated at each arm throughout a trial, along with the corresponding automated audio cues. At the start of each trial, an automated auditory cue, “In” or “Out,” instructed the participant to flex or extend, respectively, their reference forearm about the elbow joint. The monitor visually portrayed the target torque, τtarget, using a dark blue circle. The participant generated a torque with their reference arm to reach the target torque, and their applied torque was visually conveyed using a red circle. Once the participant maintained for 3 consecutive seconds their applied torque within an acceptable range of torques (0.8·τtarget<τ^user<1.2·τtarget), as visually depicted using two cyan circles, an automated auditory cue, “Match,” instructed the participant to use their opposite indicator arm to match the reference torque. No feedback was provided about the torque applied by the indicator arm. Once the participant perceived that the torque generated by each arm was the same, the participant stated aloud “Target.” At this time, the experimenter pressed a designated keyboard key to indicate that the torques were matched. Following, an automated auditory cue, “Hold,” instructed the participant to maintain the same torque in each arm. After 1 s, the automated auditory cue, “Relax,” instructed the participant to relax both arms, marking completion of the trial. No feedback was provided to the participant about their accuracy in matching torques between arms.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Trial timeline. Visually depicted are the torques generated and corresponding events occurring throughout an example trial when each participant matched torques between arms. The automated audio cues are indicated by the vertical dashed lines. Participant verbal feedback, indicating when the torques are matched, is indicated by the vertical dashed line with the italicized word “Target.” The red thick horizontal lines identify the 0.5 s of data extracted to calculate the reference and indicator torque that the participant generated.

2.4. Experimental Protocol

The participant was instructed to not exercise the day prior to and of each testing session to avoid fatigue. Each testing session was conducted on a separate day.

During the first testing session, the maximum voluntary torque (MVT) that the participant could generate about the elbow joint of each arm was quantified in flexion and extension. During the second testing session, the accuracy and precision with which the participant could match sub-maximal isometric torques between arms when referencing each arm was evaluated. The procedures followed for each session are provided in more detail in van der Helm et al. (10); we outline the latter procedures here.

At the beginning of the second testing session, the participant's body was secured to a Biodex chair (System 3 Pro™; Shirley, NY, USA), using straps, such that trunk and shoulder movements were restricted. The participant's forearms were casted to: (i) permit a rigid connection between their isometric movements and recordings from the isometric measurement devices and (ii) encourage that, when applying torques to each isometric device, the participant was cutaneously stimulated evenly across the length of each forearm. The participant was situated in each isometric measurement device with shoulder abduction angles of 85°, shoulder flexion adduction angles of 40°, and elbow flexion angles of 90°. The 90° elbow flexion angle corresponds to the approximate configuration at which the MVT about the elbow joint can be generated (51).

Prior to initiating the testing trials, we determined whether the participant could generate and hold each arm within the desired range of torques. The participant was required to flex and extend each arm to 20 and 40% of their paretic arm's MVT in flexion and extension, and to continue holding the desired torque within the required range of torques (i.e., 0.8·τtarget<τ^user<1.2·τtarget) for 3 consecutive seconds. As such, we could determine whether the participant's motor impairments would interfere with their ability to match torques on our task.

To quantify the accuracy and precision with which torques were matched between arms, the participant completed eight consecutive testing trials for each of the eight testing conditions (i.e., 2 reference arms × 2 directions × 2 target torques). Thus, the participant completed eight blocks of eight testing trials, where each block was comprised of a single testing condition. The participant rested for a minimum of 20 s between every trial to permit their involuntary muscle activity (i.e., hypertonicity) to subside (52). Presentation order of the eight testing conditions was randomized across participants using a latin square design.

A target torque of 5 Nm (i.e., τtarget = 5) was included in the testing to identify participant perception during a situation that resembles lifting an object that has a fixed mass. We refer to this task as a fixed torque task. A target torque of 25% of MVT (i.e., τtarget = 0.25 · τMVT : ref) was included to account for the fact that generating 5 Nm of elbow torque corresponds to a different degree of challenge across participants, due to variations in their strength. We refer to this task as a percentage of MVT task.

2.5. Data Analyses

2.5.1. Strength Asymmetry

We quantified the asymmetry in participant strength between arms for each direction (i.e., flexion, extension) using the MVTRatio, which we defined as the MVT of the paretic arm, τMVT : par, divided by the MVT of the non-paretic arm, τMVT : nonPar, in participants with stroke and the MVT of the non-dominant arm, τMVT : nonDom, divided by the MVT of the dominant arm, τMVT : dom, in controls. An MVTRatio of 1.0 indicates that the strength is identical in each arm. An MVTRatio less than 1.0 indicates that the paretic arm in participants with stroke and non-dominant arm in controls is weaker than the non-paretic arm in participants with stroke and dominant arm in controls, respectively. An MVTRatio greater than 1.0 indicates that the paretic arm in participants with stroke and non-dominant arm in controls is stronger than the non-paretic arm in participants with stroke and dominant arm in controls, respectively.

2.5.2. Torque-Matching Ability

We assessed the accuracy and precision with which each participant matched sub-maximal isometric torques between arms for each of the eight testing conditions (i.e., 2 reference arms×2 directions×2 target torques). First, we identified for every trial, i, the indicator torque, τ^ind,i, and reference torque, τ^ref,i. These torques were defined as the average of 0.5 s of torque data from the indicator arm and reference arm, respectively, immediately after the experimenter pressed a keyboard key in response to the participant stating that the torques were matched (see Figure 2 for a visual depiction). Next, we identified the torque-matching error, τerr, i, for every trial, i, or the difference between the indicator torque, τ^ind,i, and reference torque, τ^ref,i. A negative and positive torque-matching error corresponded to the participant undershooting and overshooting the reference torque, respectively. We visually inspected participant torque-matching errors as a function of trial, for each testing condition, to verify that learning, fatigue, or boredom, as indicated by trends of increasing or decreasing errors, did not occur.

Following, we identified participant accuracy using the constant error and absolute error, and participant precision using the variable error (10, 53, 54). Constant error, absolute error, and variable error were determined for each of the eight testing conditions (i.e., 2 reference arms×2 directions×2 target torques) based on the eight testing trials of every condition. Constant error, CE, is the mean torque-matching error across the eight testing trials and identifies whether the participant consistently underestimated (CE < 0) or overestimated (CE > 0) the reference torque. Absolute error, AE, is the mean magnitude of the torque-matching error across the eight testing trials and identifies whether the participant consistently perfectly matched (AE = 0) or poorly matched (AE >> 0) the reference torque. Variable error, VE, is the standard deviation of the torque-matching error across the eight testing trials and identifies whether the participant matched using consistently the same torque (VE = 0) or highly variable torques (VE >> 0).

We also wanted to address whether torques were matched between arms based on the effort that the participant perceived was required to activate each limb rather than the magnitude of the torques that they generated. As such, we normalized the torque about each elbow by the corresponding MVT and identified the normalized torque-matching error, τnorm, err, i, as a percentage, e.g.,:

τnorm,err,i=100×(τ^ind,iτ^MVT,ind-τ^ref,iτ^MVT,ref).    (1)

We determined the mean normalized error across the eight testing trials for each condition (i.e., 2 reference arms×2 directions×2 target torques).

2.6. Statistical Analyses

We determined whether accuracy and precision in matching torques differed depending on the arm participants referenced (i.e., controls: dominant/non-dominant; participants with stroke: paretic/non-paretic) for each tested direction and target torque. This analysis was achieved using linear-mixed effects models (55, 56), where reference arm was a fixed effect and participant was a random effect. Outcome measures were CE, AE, and VE, as well as the mean normalized error. For models with a significant main effect, we identified significantly differing levels (57), using a Holm correction to account for the multiple comparisons (58). Mean (μ), standard deviation (σ), and standard error (SE) values are reported.

3. Results

Data were collected from fifteen participants with hemiparetic stroke and ten participants without neurological impairments (i.e., controls). Tables 1, 2 provide demographic, clinical, and experimental information about each participant.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant clinical and experimental information.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participant demographic information.

3.1. Motor Impairments

Participant strength in each arm during elbow flexion and extension, as well as UE FMA scores, are reported in Table 1. The strength of controls was nearly symmetric in each arm in elbow flexion [μ ± σ (range) MVTRatio: 0.96 ± 0.06 (0.88−1.05)] and extension [0.95 ± 0.06 (0.86−1.08)]. The strength of participants with stroke tended to be greater in their non-paretic arm than their paretic arm in elbow flexion [0.64 ± 0.24 (0.18−0.97)] and extension [0.70 ± 0.28 (0.25−1.21)]. The severity of motor impairments for the participants with stroke, according to their UE FMA score, spanned from mild to severe [μ ± σ (range) UE FMA: 37 ± 15 (12−62)].

For Stroke 7 and 12, the MVT of their paretic arm was less than 25% of the MVT of their non-paretic arm. As such, these participants would not be able to generate enough torque with their paretic arm to match 25% of the MVT of their non-paretic arm. To ensure that these participants could match the torque generated by their non-paretic arm, we set the target torques for Stroke 7 and 12 to 15% and 20% of the MVT of their reference arm, respectively.

3.2. Matching of Torques in Flexion

Stroke 9 was unable to successfully match torques in elbow flexion due to an inability to hold the reference torque for 3 s. Therefore, this participant's data were not included in the following analyses. The accuracy and precision with which the remaining participants matched torques between arms in flexion as a function of their reference arm are summarized in Table 3 and Figure 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Statistical results in flexion.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Participant accuracy and precision in matching torques in flexion. Mean (bar height) and standard error (error bars) of participants' constant errors, absolute errors, variable errors, and mean normalized errors when referencing each arm to match a (Left) fixed torque and (Right) percentage of their MVT. Significant differences are indicated by solid black horizontal lines with a star above.

3.2.1. Fixed Torque Task

First, we address whether the errors when matching torques in flexion differed depending on the arm that participants referenced for the fixed torque task (Figure 3, Left). Constant error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p < 0.001). Participants with stroke had a greater constant error when referencing their paretic arm than when referencing their non-paretic arm (p < 0.001) and than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.002) and dominant arm (p = 0.028). Absolute error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p < 0.001). Participants with stroke had a greater absolute error when referencing their paretic arm than when referencing their non-paretic arm (p < 0.001) and than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.001) and dominant arm (p = 0.017). Variable error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.035). However, post-hoc comparisons did not reveal any significant differences (p > 0.050).

Next, we indicate the results for the normalized errors when participants matched torques in flexion during the fixed torque task (Figure 3, Left, Bottom). The mean normalized error did not significantly differ depending on the arm that participants referenced (p > 0.050).

3.2.2. Percentage of MVT Task

First, we address whether the errors when matching torques in flexion differed depending on the arm that participants referenced during the percentage of MVT task (Figure 3, Right). Constant error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p < 0.001). Participants with stroke had a greater constant error when referencing their paretic arm than when referencing their non-paretic arm (p < 0.001) and than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.013). Also, participants with stroke when referencing their non-paretic arm had a constant error that was less than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.043) and dominant arm (p = 0.001). Absolute error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.004). Participants with stroke had a greater absolute error when referencing their paretic arm than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.022) and dominant arm (p = 0.022). Variable error was not significantly affected by the arm that participants referenced (p> 0.050).

Next, we indicate the results for the normalized errors when participants matched torques in flexion during the percentage of MVT task (Figure 3, Right, Bottom). The mean normalized error did not significantly differ depending on the arm that participants referenced (p> 0.050).

3.3. Matching of Torques in Extension

3.3.1. Fixed Torque Task

Stroke 10, 13, and 14 were unable to successfully match the fixed torque in elbow extension due to an inability to hold the reference torque for 3 s. Therefore, these participants' data were not included in the following analyses. The accuracy and precision with which the remaining participants matched torques between arms in extension as a function of their reference arm are summarized in Table 4 (Top) and Figure 4 (Left).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Statistical results in extension. Mean±standard deviation are reported for every data entry. μtorque, mean magnitude of reference torque.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Participant accuracy and precision in matching torques in extension. Mean (bar height) and standard error (error bars) of participants' constant errors, absolute errors, variable errors, and mean normalized errors when referencing each arm to match a (Left) fixed torque and (Right) percentage of their MVT. Significant differences are indicated by solid black horizontal lines with a star above.

First, we address whether the errors when matching torques in extension differed depending on the arm that participants referenced during the fixed torque task. Constant error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.005). Participants with stroke had a greater constant error when referencing their paretic arm than when referencing their non-paretic arm (p = 0.004). Absolute error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.013). However, post-hoc comparisons did not reveal any significant differences (p> 0.050). Variable error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.003). Participants with stroke had a greater variable error when referencing their paretic arm than when referencing their non-paretic arm (p = 0.021) and than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.003).

Next, we indicate the results for the normalized errors when participants matched torques in extension during the fixed torque task (Figure 4, Left, Bottom). The mean normalized error did not significantly differ depending on the arm that participants referenced (p> 0.050).

3.3.2. Percentage of MVT Task

Stroke 10 and 13 were unable to successfully match torques in elbow extension due to an inability to hold the reference torque for 3 s. Therefore, these participants' data were not included in the following analyses. The accuracy and precision with which the remaining participants matched torques between arms in extension as a function of their reference arm are summarized in Table 4 (Bottom) and Figure 3 (Right).

First, we address whether the errors when matching torques in extension differed depending on the arm that participants referenced during the percentage of MVT task. Constant error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p < 0.001). Participants with stroke had a greater constant error when referencing their paretic arm than when referencing their non-paretic arm (p < 0.001) and than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.050) and dominant arm (p = 0.050). Absolute error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.021). Participants with stroke had a greater absolute error when referencing their paretic arm than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm (p = 0.049) and dominant arm (p = 0.007). Variable error was affected by the arm that participants referenced (p = 0.041). However, post-hoc comparisons did not reveal any significant differences (p> 0.050).

Next, we indicate the results for the normalized errors when participants matched torques in extension during the percentage of MVT task (Figure 3, Right, Bottom). The mean normalized error did not significantly differ depending on the arm that participants referenced (p> 0.050).

4. Discussion

The main findings of this work are that the participants with hemiparetic stroke: (i) had magnitudes of error similar to controls when referencing their non-paretic arm; (ii) had magnitudes of error greater than controls when referencing their paretic arm; and (iii) had similar results to controls, regardless of the arm referenced, when the torque that they generated at each arm was normalized by its corresponding MVT.

4.1. Torque-Matching Results

To determine whether individuals with hemiparetic stroke accurately matched torques on our task, we first needed to identify how accurately and precisely a group of individuals without neurological impairments matched torques between arms. Therefore, we first discuss the results of our controls, and then we discuss the results of our participants with stroke.

4.1.1. Controls

We assessed whether our right-hand dominant controls matched torques differently depending on their arm dominance by quantifying their accuracy and precision in matching torques between arms when referencing each arm. Our results did not find controls to differ in their accuracy and precision in matching torques between arms depending on the arm referenced (i.e., dominant vs. non-dominant; p > 0.050). We acknowledge that a significant effect may have been obtained had a larger number of controls been tested. However, we point out that the effect size would likely have been relatively small, at least when compared to the degree of error observed in our participants with stroke. We cannot indicate whether accuracy and precision in matching torques would be affected by arm dominance in left-hand dominant individuals since we only tested right-hand dominant controls.

4.1.2. Participants With Stroke

To begin, our results did corroborate the findings of other researchers when our participants with stroke referenced their paretic arm (4, 7, 8, 11). That is, our participants with hemiparetic stroke could not accurately match torques that they generated about their non-paretic elbow to those they generated about their paretic elbow. The accuracy of our participants with hemiparetic stroke in matching torques between arms when referencing their paretic arm was worse than when referencing their non-paretic arm, and than controls when referencing their non-dominant arm and dominant arm (p < 0.050). For the fixed torque task, the mean magnitude of error (i.e., absolute error) for our participants with stroke when referencing their paretic arm was 9.4 Nm in flexion and 5.3 Nm in extension. This magnitude of error was two to three times greater when participants with stroke referenced their paretic arm than when they referenced their non-paretic arm and than when controls referenced their dominant arm and non-dominant arm (see Tables 3, 4). Furthermore, Stroke 13, who was classified according to the rNSA as having absent elbow kinaesthesia and pressure sense in the paretic arm (see Table 1), matched torques between arms when referencing their paretic arm with an accuracy that was much greater than our controls. When referencing their paretic arm, this participant's mean magnitude of error on the fixed torque task in flexion was 16.4 Nm. Stroke 13 was not able to control their torques during the extension task so these results are not reported. We also highlight that the average torque-matching errors for our participants with stroke reached a 7:1 difference. Intuitively, this difference implies that a mass of 1 kg in the paretic arm is perceived as equivalent to a mass of 7 kg in the non-paretic arm. Therefore, our findings indicate that the errors in accurately judging the torques that one generates between arms can be substantial in individuals with hemiparetic stroke.

On the other hand, the magnitude of errors of our participants with hemiparetic stroke in matching torques between arms, when referencing their non-paretic arm, did not differ from the magnitude of errors of our controls when referencing their non-dominant arm and dominant arm (p > 0.050). For the fixed torque task, the mean magnitude of torque-matching error (i.e., absolute error) when participants with stroke referenced their non-paretic arm and controls referenced their dominant and non-dominant arm was 2.6, 4.2, and 2.5 Nm, respectively, in flexion and 2.8, 2.5, and 2.3 Nm, respectively, in extension. We also highlight that Stroke 13, who was classified according to the rNSA as having absent elbow kinaesthesia and pressure sense in the paretic arm (see Table 1), could match torques between arms when referencing their non-paretic arm with an accuracy that was similar to our controls. When referencing their non-paretic arm, this participant's mean magnitude of error in matching torques in flexion was 1.6 Nm. This finding that individuals with hemiparetic stroke can match torques between arms with a similar accuracy as individuals without neurological impairments is in contrast to what is known in the existing literature (4, 7, 8, 11). Even so, our finding that accuracy in matching depends on whether the non-paretic arm vs. paretic arm is referenced has been supported by other groups. Yen and Li (11) found that the accuracy in matching torques differed depending on whether individuals with hemiparetic stroke referenced their non-paretic arm vs. paretic arm. Likewise, Hirayama et al. (59) showed that the accuracy in locating one's thumb in space differed depending on whether individuals with lesions referenced their non-paretic arm vs. paretic arm.

As discussed above, the magnitude of errors (i.e., absolute error) did not significantly differ between the participants with stroke, when their paretic arm matched their non-paretic arm, and controls. That is, our participants with stroke, when referencing their non-paretic arm, matched torques with a similar degree of accuracy as our controls. Even so, our participants with stroke tended to undershoot the target torque when referencing their non-paretic arm and overshoot when referencing their paretic arm, whereas our controls, regardless of which arm they referenced, tended to overshoot to a lesser degree than the participants with stroke. Hence, while the magnitude of errors was similar, the torques generated by each arm differed in our participants with stroke and controls, as can be seen by the constant error.

Analyses based on the mean normalized errors did not reveal any differences regardless of the arm that participants with stroke and controls referenced (p > 0.050). Moreover, Stroke 13, who was classified according to the rNSA as having absent elbow kinaesthesia and pressure sense in the paretic arm (see Table 1), had nearly the same mean normalized error when referencing their paretic arm (15.3%) and non-paretic arm (15.7%) for the fixed torque task in flexion. Therefore, while the magnitude of errors differed depending on the arm referenced, the analyses that were run to investigate whether participants may match based on their perceived effort did not reveal a difference.

We recognize that our tested participants with stroke are heterogeneous in terms of lesioned hemisphere/location/size and arm dominance. We inspected our results according to these parameters, and we did not observe trends depending on any of these variables. Therefore, our findings indicate that, regardless of lesioned hemisphere/location/size and arm dominance, accuracy in matching torques was more severely affected when our participants with hemiparetic stroke referenced their paretic arm than their non-paretic arm.

4.2. Explaining the Asymmetry in Accurately Matching Torques

The most probable explanation to address why accuracy in matching torques between arms depends on whether individuals with hemiparetic stroke reference their paretic vs. non-paretic arm is aligned with the literature. Specifically, our findings support the notion that individuals with hemiparetic stroke match torques primarily based on their perceived effort. Post-hemiparetic stroke, an individual's paretic arm is weaker than their non-paretic arm. By rearranging the right-hand side of Equation (1), we can see that if the participant were to match the effort required to generate a torque at their non-paretic arm, τ^non-paretic,i, to their paretic arm, τ^paretic,i, then the matching torque would correspond to τ^non-paretic,i ~ (1MVTRatio)·τ^paretic,i. Applying the same rationale, if the participant were to match the effort required to generate a torque at their paretic arm, τ^paretic,i, to their non-paretic arm, τ^non-paretic,i, then the matching torque would be less, corresponding to τ^paretic,i ~ MVTRatio·τ^non-paretic,i. Typically, 1MVTRatio>1 and 1MVTRatio<1 in individuals with hemiparetic stroke. Hence, this explanation that our participants with hemiparetic stroke matched torques based on their perceived effort is reasonable for indicating why the magnitude of errors were greater and positive when their paretic arm was referenced, and smaller and negative when their non-paretic arm was referenced. We also provide an explanation to indicate why the mean normalized error was almost always greater than zero for each participant and arm referenced. We determined MVTs during a unimanual task and accuracy in matching torques during a bimanual task. Prior research has demonstrated that MVTs can differ depending on whether the task is unimanual vs. bimanual (60). Therefore, the mean normalized errors may have been closer to zero had we quantified MVTs during a bimanual task.

Additionally, we provide our original hypothesis to explain why we believed that individuals post-hemiparetic stroke inaccurately judge the torques that they generate between arms since this new hypothesis cannot be ruled out. Even so, we find the explanation that our participants with stroke were matching torques based on their perceived effort more probable. To begin, we point out that joint torque generation between the arms of a healthy adult is normally symmetric (10). However, post-hemiparetic stroke, joint torque generation becomes asymmetric (4, 7, 8, 10, 11, 61). Since the paretic arm lacks the control and strength of the non-paretic arm, during bimanual tasks individuals with hemiparetic stroke almost exclusively use their paretic arm to assist their non-paretic arm, not vice versa (62). Given this asymmetry when executing bimanual tasks, we hypothesized that individuals with hemiparetic stroke never learn to perceive their non-paretic arm in relation to their paretic arm (63). Consequently, their perception is only affected when using their non-paretic arm to assist their paretic arm to execute bimanual tasks. Given the motor control limitations occurring post-stroke in their paretic arm, we also hypothesized that these individuals learn to rely more heavily on their non-paretic arm than their paretic arm to perform activities of daily living. As such, they utilize their paretic arm only to the extent that is needed to achieve a similar degree of accuracy as prior to the stroke, resulting in their paretic arm undershooting the target torque.

4.3. Study Limitations

Limitations of this work include that participants were assessed on a single-degree-of-freedom isometric task, which is potentially an unfamiliar task. We chose this task since we wanted to: (i) avoid assessing participants on a multi-joint coordination task, which may give results that are more difficult to interpret, and (ii) isolate errors in judging torques from errors in judging movements, which may occur during an isotonic task. Future work could determine the ability of individuals with stroke to match torques between arms on multi-joint tasks and isotonic tasks when positional cues are included.

Another limitation of this study is that our results are summarized for a range of MVTs spanning between 4 and 40% at the paretic elbow. The literature provides conflicting evidence as to whether our findings will extend to MVTs beyond 40% (4, 19, 64). We underscore that a challenge faced in assessing larger percentages of MVT is fatigue, especially in individuals with stroke.

An additional limitation of this work is that our results are based on a sample size of fifteen individuals with stroke who have different levels of motor impairments. Future work could strengthen our results by identifying a quicker, and likely less precise assessment approach that would make it feasible to quantify the accuracy with which individuals can match torques between arms in a much larger group of participants.

4.4. Clinical Relevance

Rehabilitation practice focuses on movement limitations without considering how accurately an individual perceives the torques that they generate. Based on our results, we propose that the ability to accurately judge the torques that one generates may be important to study since it may hinder the ability of individuals with stroke to safely deal with unfamiliar or potentially dangerous bimanual tasks, such as carrying a platter with hot drinks, pushing a grocery cart, and caring for an infant. Perceiving that the torques generated in the non-paretic arm are as much as seven times greater than the torques generated in the paretic arm may contribute to the challenges that individuals with stroke face in controlling their upper limb movements during bimanual tasks, particularly when the task is novel.

Our results also support the notion that the erroneous judgment corresponds to how weak the paretic arm is in relation to the non-paretic arm. Consequently, the findings corroborate the idea that a large number of the 50–75% of stroke survivors who have hemiparesis (6, 65) inaccurately judge their self-generated torques, particularly when referencing their paretic arm during bimanual tasks.

Given the magnitude of the errors we observed, we propose that training individuals with hemiparetic stroke to more accurately perceive the torques that they generate may enable them to more successfully execute bimanual activities of daily living and avoid potentially dangerous situations. Strength training of the paretic arm could be one approach to reduce the extent of the erroneous judgment. However, this approach may not be very effective due to the losses in motor neural resources individuals post-hemiparetic stroke have that are necessary to drive their paretic upper limb (4547). Based on the understanding that judgment errors are more evident when individuals post-hemiparetic stroke reference their paretic arm than their non-paretic arm, we propose that training these individuals to coordinate movements while focusing on their paretic arm may make them more aware and capable of rehabilitating their erroneous judgment. This proposed training protocol is novel when considering bimanual training approaches that have been developed to-date for individuals post-stroke [e.g., (5, 6668)]. Future work can test the neuroplasticity of the erroneous judgment by determining whether training individuals with hemiparetic stroke to perceive their non-paretic arm in relation to their paretic arm during bimanual coordination tasks mitigates the degree of the errors. To conclude, we propose that an improved ability to accurately perceive the torques that one generates between arms may lead to greater usage of the paretic arm, resulting in survivors of a stroke gaining confidence and ability to engage in daily activities.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board.

Author Contributions

NG, NvdH, JMD, and JPD: study design, data interpretation, and manuscript editing. NG, NvdH, and JMD: data acquisition. NG and NvdH: data analysis. NG: manuscript preparation.

Funding

This study was supported by the grants: K25HD096116 (PI: NG) and NIH R01HD039343 (PI: JPD).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer AS declared a shared affiliation, with no collaboration, with the authors to the handling editor at time of review.

Acknowledgments

We thank Lauren Balmert for her assistance with the statistical analyses, Di Zhang for his assistance with the hardware development, the participants for partaking in this experiment, and the reviewers for their helpful feedback.

References

1. Cole JD, Sedgwick EM. The perceptions of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck. J Physiol. (1992) 449:503–15. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1992.sp019099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Cole J. Pride and a Daily Marathon. Cambridge: The MIT Press (1995).

Google Scholar

3. Benjamin EJ, Blaha MJ, Chiuve SE, Cushman M, Das SR, Deo R, et al. Heart disease and stroke statistics–2017 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. (2017) 135:e1–458. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0000000000000491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bertrand AM, Mercier C, Shun PLW, Bourbonnais D, Desrosiers J. Effects of weakness on symmetrical bilateral grip force exertion in subjects with hemiparesis. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 91:1579–85. doi: 10.1152/jn.00597.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Li S, Durand-Sanchez A, Latash ML. Inter-limb force coupling is resistant to distorted visual fedback in chronic hemiparetic stroke. J Rehabil Med. (2014) 46:206–11. doi: 10.2340/16501977-1256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lawrence ES, Coshall C, Dundas R, Stewart J, Rudd AG, Howard R, et al. Estimates of the prevalence of acute stroke impairments and disability in a multiethnic population. Stroke. (2001) 32:1279–84. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Lodha N, Coombes SA, Cauraugh JH. Bimanual isometric force control: asymmetry and coordination evidence post stroke. Clin Neurophysiol. (2012) 123:787–95. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Mercier C, Bertrand AM, Bourbonnais D. Differences in the magnitude and direction of forces during a submaximal matching in hemiparetic subjects. Exp Brain Res. (2004) 157:32–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.06.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Simon AM, Kelly BM, Ferris DP. Sense of effort determines lower limb force production during dynamic movement in individuals with poststroke hemiparesis. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2009) 23:811–8. doi: 10.1177/1545968308331163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. van der Helm NA, Gurari N, Drogos JM, Dewald JPA. Task directionality impacts the ability of individuals with chronic hemiparetic stroke to match torques between arms: preliminary findings. In: 2017 International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics. London: IEEE (2017). p. 714–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

11. Yen JT, Li S. Altered force perception in stroke survivors with spastic hemiplegia. J Rehabil Med. (2015) 47:917–23. doi: 10.2340/16501977-2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Adamo DE, Scotland S, Martin BJ. Asymmetry in grasp force matching and sense of effort. Exp Brain Res. (2012) 217:273–85. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2991-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Jones LA. Matching force: constant errors and differential thresholds. Perception. (1989) 18:681–7. doi: 10.1068/p180681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Jones LA. The senses of effort and force during fatiguing contractions. Fatigue. (1995) 384:305–13.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

15. Lafargue G, Paillard J, Lamarre Y, Sirigu A. Production and perception of grip force without proprioception: is there a sense of effort in deafferented subjects? Eur J Neurosci. (2003) 17:2741–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02700.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Scotland S, Adamo DE, Martin BJ. Sense of effort revisited: relative contributions of sensory feedback and efferent copy. Neurosci Lett. (2014) 561:208–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Simon AM, Ferris DP. Lower limb force production and bilateral force asymmetries are based on sense of effort. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 187:129–38. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1288-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Takarada Y, Nozaki D, Taira M. Force overestimation during tourniquet-induced transient occlusion of the brachial artery and possible underlying neural mechanisms. Neurosci Res. (2006) 54:38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Weerakkody N, Percival P, Morgan DL, Gregory JE, Proske U. Matching different levels of isometric torque in elbow flexor muscles after eccentric exercise. Exp Brain Res. (2003) 149:141–50. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1341-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Jones LA, Hunter IW. Effect of fatigue on force sensation. Exp Neurol. (1983) 81:640–50. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(83)90332-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Jones LA. Perception of force and weight: theory and research. Psychol Bull. (1986) 100:29–42. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Matthews PBC. Where does Sherrington's “muscular sense" originate? Muscles, joints, corollary discharges. Annu Rev Neurosci. (1982) 5:189–218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.05.030182.001201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. McCloskey DI. Corollary discharges: motor commands and perception. In: Comprehensive Physiology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (2011). Available online at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/cphy.cp010232

Google Scholar

24. Proske U, Gandevia SC. The kinaesthetic senses. J Physiol. (2009) 587:4139–46. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.175372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Proske U, Gandevia SC. The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol Rev. (2012) 92:1651–97. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Roland PE. Sensory feedback to the cerebral cortex during voluntary movement in man. Behav Brain Sci. (1978) 1:129. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00060118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Jones LA. Kinestheti. In: Human and Machine Haptics. Cambridge (2000).

PubMed Abstract

28. Anderson JH. Dynamic characteristics of Golgi tendon organs. Brain Res. (1974) 67:531–7. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90501-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Jami L. Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. Physiol Rev. (1992) 72:623–66. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1992.72.3.623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Roland PE, Ladegaard-Pedersen H. A quantitative analysis of sensations of tensions and of kinaesthesia in man. Brain. (1977) 100:671–92. doi: 10.1093/brain/100.4.671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stauffer EK, Stephens JA. Responses of Golgi tendon organs to ramp-and-hold profiles of contractile force. J Neurophysiol. (1977) 40:681–91. doi: 10.1152/jn.1977.40.3.681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Luu BL, Day BL, Cole JD, Fitzpatrick RC. The fusimotor and reafferent origin of the sense of force and weight. J Physiol. (2011) 589:3135–47. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.208447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Monjo F, Shemmell J, Forestier N. The sensory origin of the sense of effort is context-dependent. Exp Brain Res. (2018) 236:1997–2008. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5280-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Birznieks I, Macefield VG, Westling G, Johansson RS. Slowly adapting mechanoreceptors in the borders of the human fingernail encode fingertip forces. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:9370–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0143-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Johansson RS, Vallbo AB. Tactile sensibility in the human hand: relative and absolute densities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in glabrous skin. J Physiol. (1979) 286:283–300. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Johansson RS, Landström U, Lundström R. Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements. Brain Res. (1982) 244:17–25. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90899-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Westling G, Johansson RS. Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans. Exp Brain Res. (1987) 66:128–40. doi: 10.1007/BF00236209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Cafarelli E, Bigland-Ritchie B. Sensation of static force in muscles of different length. Exp Neurol. (1979) 65:511–25. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(79)90040-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Proske U, Gregory JE, Morgan DL, Percival P, Weerakkody NS, Canny BJ. Force matching errors following eccentric exercise. Hum Mov Sci. (2004) 23:365–78. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2004.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gueugnon M, Torre K, Mottet D, Bonnetblanc F. Asymmetries of bilateral isometric force matching with movement intention and unilateral fatigue. Exp Brain Res. (2014) 232:1699–706. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3862-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Park WH, Leonard CT, Li S. Finger force perception during ipsilateral and contralateral force matching tasks. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 189:301–10. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1424-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Sleimen-Malkoun R, Temprado JJ, Thefenne L, Berton E. Bimanual training in stroke: how do coupling and symmetry-breaking matter? BMC Neurol. (2011) 11:11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-11-11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wang J, Sainburg RL. Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 92:349–60. doi: 10.1152/jn.00960.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Wang J, Przybyla A, Wuebbenhorst K, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Aging reduces asymmetries in interlimb transfer of visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 210:283–90. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2631-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Stinear CM, Barber PA, Smale PR, Coxon JP, Fleming MK, Byblow WD. Functional potential in chronic stroke patients depends on corticospinal tract integrity. Brain. (2007) 130:170–80. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gemperline JJ, Allen S, Walk D, Rymer WZ. Characteristics of motor unit discharge in subjects with hemiparesis. Muscle Nerve. (1995) 18:1101–14. doi: 10.1002/mus.880181006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ryan AS, Dobrovolny CL, Smith GV, Silver KH, Macko RF. Hemiparetic muscle atrophy and increased intramuscular fat in stroke patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2002) 83:1703–7. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.36399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fügl-Meyer AR, Jääskö L, Leyman I, Olsson S, Steglind S. The post-stroke hemiplegic patient. 1. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand J Rehabil Med. (1975) 7:13–31.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

50. Lincoln NB, Jackson JM, Adams SA. Reliability and revision of the Nottingham Sensory Assessment for stroke patients. Physiotherapy. (1998) 84:358–65. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9406(05)61454-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Murray WM, Delp SL, Buchanan TS. Variation of muscle moment arms with elbow and forearm position. J Biomech. (1995) 28:513–25. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(94)00114-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. McPherson JG, Ellis MD, Heckman CJ, Dewald JPA. Evidence for increased activation of persistent inward currents in individuals with chronic hemiparetic stroke. J Neurophysiol. (2008) 100:3236–43. doi: 10.1152/jn.90563.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Henry FM. Variable and constant performance errors within a group of individuals. J Motor Behav. (1974) 6:149–54. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1974.10734991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Schutz RW, Roy EA. Absolute error: the devil in disguise. J Motor Behav. (1973) 5:141–53. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1973.10734959

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Laird NM, Ware JH. Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics. (1982) 38:963–74. doi: 10.2307/2529876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-Plus. New York, NY: Springer (2000).

Google Scholar

57. Lenth R. Using lsmeans. (2015). p. 1–28. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lsmeans/vignettes/using-lsmeans.pdf.

Google Scholar

58. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. (1979) 6:65–70.

Google Scholar

59. Hirayama K, Fukutake T, Kawamura M. ‘Thumb localizing test' for detecting a lesion in the posterior column–medial lemniscal system. J Neurol Sci. (1999) 167:45–9. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00136-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. DeJong SL, Lang CE. The bilateral movement condition facilitates maximal but not submaximal paretic-limb grip force in people with post-stroke hemiparesis. Clin Neurophysiol. (2012) 123:1616–23. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Kang N, Cauraugh JH. Force control in chronic stroke. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 52:38–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Bailey RR, Klaesner JW, Lang CE. Quantifying real-world upper-limb activity in nondisabled adults and adults with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2015) 29:969–78. doi: 10.1177/1545968315583720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Rode G, Rossetti Y, Boisson D. Inverse relationship between sensation of effort and muscular force during recovery from pure motor hemiplegia: a single-case study. Neuropsychologia. (1996) 34:87–95. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00065-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Chang SH, Durand-Sanchez A, DiTommaso C, Li S. Interlimb interactions during bilateral voluntary elbow flexion tasks in chronic hemiparetic stroke. Physiol Rep. (2013) 1:e00010. doi: 10.1002/phy2.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Roger VL, Go AS, Lloyd-Jones DM, Benjamin EJ, Berry JD, Borden WB, et al. Heart disease and stroke statistics–2012 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. (2012) 125:e2–220. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e31823ac046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Lum SP, Lehman SL, Reinkensmeyer DJ. The bimanual lifting rehabilitator: an adaptive machine for therapy of stroke patients. IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng. (1995) 3:166–74. doi: 10.1109/86.392371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Rose DK, Winstein CJ. Bimanual training after stroke: are two hands better than one? Top Stroke Rehabil. (2004) 11:20–30. doi: 10.1310/XAUM-LPBM-0RXD-RLDK

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Sainburg RL, Good D, Przybyla A. Bilateral synergy: a framework for post-stroke rehabilitation. J Neurol Transl Neurosci. (2013) 1:1025.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: perception, torque, stroke, evaluation methodology, mechatronics

Citation: Gurari N, van der Helm NA, Drogos JM and Dewald JPA (2019) Accuracy of Individuals Post-hemiparetic Stroke in Matching Torques Between Arms Depends on the Arm Referenced. Front. Neurol. 10:921. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00921

Received: 20 July 2018; Accepted: 08 August 2019;
Published: 22 August 2019.

Edited by:

Ping Zhou, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States

Reviewed by:

Annie Simon, Shirley Ryan AbilityLab, United States
Sheng Li, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States

Copyright © 2019 Gurari, van der Helm, Drogos and Dewald. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Netta Gurari, netta.gurari@northwestern.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.