- 1Laboratory of Neurobiology of Movement, Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
- 2Laboratory of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, Institute of Neurology Deolindo Couto, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
- 3Laboratory of Neurobiology II, Institute of Biophysics Carlos Chagas Filho, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Unilateral brachial plexus injury (BPI) impairs sensory and motor functions of the upper limb. This study aimed to map in detail brachial plexus sensory impairment both in the injured and the uninjured upper limb. Touch sensation was measured through Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments at the autonomous regions of the brachial plexus nerves, hereafter called points of exclusive innervation (PEIs). Seventeen BPI patients (31.35 years±6.9 SD) and 14 age-matched healthy controls (27.57 years±5.8 SD) were tested bilaterally at six selected PEIs (axillary, musculocutaneous, median, radial, ulnar, and medial antebrachial cutaneous [MABC]). As expected, the comparison between the control group and the brachial plexus patients' injured limb showed a robust difference for all PEIs (p ≤ 0.001). Moreover, the comparison between the control group and the brachial plexus uninjured limb revealed a difference for the median (p = 0.0074), radial (p = 0.0185), ulnar (p = 0.0404), and MABC (p = 0.0328) PEIs. After splitting the sample into two groups with respect to the dominance of the injured limb, higher threshold values were found for the uninjured side when it occurred in the right dominant limb compared to the control group at the median (p = 0.0456), radial (p = 0.0096), and MABC (p = 0.0078) PEIs. This effect was absent for the left, non-dominant arm. To assess the effect of the severity of sensory deficits observed in the injured limb upon the alterations of the uninjured limb, a K-means clustering algorithm (k = 2) was applied resulting in two groups with less or more severe sensory impairment. The less severely affected patients presented higher thresholds at the median (p = 0.0189), radial (p = 0.0081), ulnar (p = 0.0253), and MABC (p = 0.0187) PEIs in the uninjured limb in comparison with the control group, whereas higher thresholds at the uninjured limb were found only for the median PEI (p = 0.0457) in the more severely affected group. In conclusion, an expressive reduction in touch threshold was found for the injured limb allowing a precise mapping of the impairment caused by the BPI. Crucially, BPI also led to reduced tactile threshold in specific PEIs in the uninjured upper limb. These new findings suggest a superordinate model of representational plasticity occurring bilaterally in the brain after a unilateral peripheral injury.
Introduction
Brachial plexus injury (BPI) affects the sensory and motor functions of the upper limb to varying degrees resulting in complex patterns of sensorimotor dysfunction, often with very poor prognosis (1, 2). Affecting predominantly young male subjects [20–29 years, 89% male—(1)], BPI has major impacts on psychosocial well-being and quality of life (3, 4). A key aspect is that BPI often leads to loss of at least part of the upper limb movements (5) thus putatively compromising movements such as directional reaching, grasping and skilled manipulative movements with the upper limb. This is due not only to the loss of motor impairment, but also to the loss of sensation, known to be of particular importance in manipulative movements (6) [review in (7)]. Although most research in BPI has focused on motor impairment, relatively little has been done to further our understanding of its associated sensory dysfunction, despite its potential to inform both to clinicians and researchers about its severity and degree of impairment as well as to help improve strategies to increase the functionality of the upper limb.
Furthermore, current investigations of sensory dysfunction after a peripheral deafferentation consider the sensory deficit evaluation only in the injured limb and are often directed to a limited portion of the affected body surface (8–14). Given the heterogeneous nature of BPIs, it is important to make a complete investigation of all nerve territories of the BP, especially in its autonomous zones (2), corresponding to areas of skin in which each single nerve can be better assessed (15–17).
Although usually not evaluated after a peripheral nerve injury, changes in sensory function affecting the uninjured limb have been found in a variety of deafferentation models such as nerve block (18), nerve injury (19), amputation (20, 21), and in burned patients (22, 23). These changes have generally been interpreted as being the result of central nervous system adaptations occurring after the deafferentation.
Considering the lack of studies investigating the uninjured limb and possible sensory changes resulting from a BPI lesion, this study aimed to evaluate the sensory thresholds in BPI patients using Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments (SWM). We expected that a complete investigation of sensory thresholds in both upper limbs would allow not only the identification of the pattern of lesion-induced loss of sensation in the most affected limb but also reveal possible sensory deficits in the uninjured upper limb. This might expand the current knowledge on sensory changes after peripheral nerve lesions, providing novel approaches to clinical evaluation and also opening up new possibilities for the study of central reorganization after peripheral injury.
Methods
Participants
Seventeen BPI patients (2 females) with a mean age of 31.35 years ± 6.9 SD (19–40), were recruited at the Institute of Neurology Deolindo Couto (INDC-UFRJ). Fifteen patients were right handed (1 left handed and 1 ambidextrous) (24). Inclusion criteria were age equal to or above 18 years, preserved ability to communicate and unilateral traumatic BPI (any level and severity, pre- or post- nerve surgery, see Table 1), diagnosed through clinical and complementary exams such as electromyography and magnetic resonance imaging. Exclusion criteria were a previous history of primary or secondary central and peripheral nervous system disease. Participants were consecutively recruited between the years 2014 and 2015.
Table 1. BPI patient characteristics and time between injury and surgery and time between injury and the assessment.
Eighteen healthy right-handed participants were also evaluated, and from this sample fourteen age-matched healthy participants were included in the analysis (27.57 years ± 5.8 SD). The remaining four participants were excluded based on k-means clustering applied to homogenize the sample (see Statistics).
Before the evaluation, all participants were asked whether they felt comfortable enough to be evaluated. They were then informed about the experimental procedures and provided written informed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the local ethics committee (Institute of Neurology Deolindo Couto—UFRJ, Brazil) and was in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki.
Semmes-Weinstein Monofilaments Assessment
In order to assess touch thresholds of the upper limb, a set of 20 Semmes-Weinstein Monofilaments (SWM, Bioseb, Vitrolles, France) were used. SWM are classified by the necessary force in grams to bend them against the skin, ranging from 0.007 to 160 g or, as expressed in log (10 × F; with F = force in milligrams), 1.84 to 6.20. The force values for each monofilament after the assessment using an analytical balance (Shimadzu Corp., Kyoto, Japan) were slightly different from the values specified by the manufacturer (see Supplementary Material). We decided to use the values we found rather than those of the manufacturer. Values for each monofilament are displayed in Supplementary Table 1.
Six stimulation points, called hereafter points of exclusive innervation (PEIs), were identified to perform the sensory test. The PEIs were within the five dermatomes (C5-T1) (25) of the BP and corresponded to the autonomous zones of six BP nerves: axillary, musculocutaneous, median, radial, ulnar and medial antebrachial cutaneous (MABC) (15) (Figure 1A).
Figure 1. (A) Illustration of the six Points of Exclusive Innervation (PEIs) in the upper limb. (B) Illustration of the experimental setup.
During the assessment, participants sat in a chair in a quiet room together with the experimenters BR and AS. The upper limb to be tested rested on a pillow. Each limb was assessed separately. A black curtain blocked the volunteer's view of the assessed limb (Figure 1B). Both experimenters were trained to conduct the assessment the same way and while one experimenter was applying the filaments the other one was taking notes about the evoked sensations and the threshold values. The experimenters were not blind regarding the subject's condition since all the patients had the injured arm with some degree of paralysis and hypotrophy. The uninjured upper limb of the BPI patients was assessed first, or the right upper limb, in the case of healthy volunteers. The order of PEI stimulation was pseudo-randomized for each upper limb. For each PEI, monofilaments were applied in ascending order—from the thinnest to the thickest. The thinnest filament detected by the participant in 3 out of 5 applications was considered as the tactile threshold for that PEI (26). All the procedures were explained and demonstrated to the participants in advance. The intervals between each stimulation were arbitrary to avoid the learning of the stimulation sequence and, consequently, false positives. When the participant was not able to feel even the thickest filament (6.20, see Supplementary Table 1) his touch threshold was established at this filament (6.20).
Statistical Analysis
Threshold values for each participant were log transformed (10 × force in mg—see Supplementary Table 1). For each PEI, the threshold values were described as medians.
K-means Clustering
Both for the control and the patients' group we used k-means cluster analysis (27) to assign individuals to clusters based on their threshold values for the six PEIs. The k-means algorithm partitions the sample into k clusters based on variables of interest. The algorithm uses a heuristic to find centroid seeds for k-means clustering and then computes the squared Euclidean distance from each observation to each centroid, assigning each observation to its closest centroid. The goal of this procedure is to minimize the within-cluster variance and maximize the between-cluster variance.
The D'agostino-Pearson test was then used to assess normality of the groups. Since data from some PEIs did not respect Gaussian distribution, non-parametric tests were applied.
Control Group
For setting the control group, the K-means clustering algorithm was applied and resulted in a cluster comprising 14 participants (four participants were excluded from further analyses). The comparison between each PEI with its counterpart in the same control participant (right arm vs. left arm) was performed by means of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Since the comparison between right and left upper PEIs revealed no difference (axillary, p = 0.5000; musculocutaneous, p = 0.7127; median, p = 0.3523; radial, p = 0.3493; ulnar, p = 1; MABC, p = 0.2123), the mean between the corresponding right and left PEIs from both upper limbs was calculated. This procedure was performed to establish a single threshold value per PEI for each participant.
To investigate if the sensory threshold differed among the six PEIs of the control group, the Kruskal-Wallis test was applied, followed by Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test to compare the six PEIs with each other—alfa = 0.05.
Control vs. Patients
The Mann-Whitney test was applied to compare PEIs between control participants and BPI patients (control × injured; control × uninjured upper limbs). For each patient, threshold values for all PEIs of the uninjured limb were normalized to the median values of the control group and these normalized data were used to calculate Spearman correlation coefficients employing the patients' age and the time interval between the injury and the assessment (T2 from Table 1).
To investigate the effect of the side of the injury, patients were separated into two groups comprising BPI in the dominant right (n = 7) or non-dominant left limb (n = 8). BPI03 and BPI04 were excluded from the sample because they were ambidextrous and left handed, respectively (Table 1). The Mann-Whitney test was performed to compare the threshold values of the uninjured side of both patient groups (dominant and non-dominant injury) with those of the control group. In addition, a K-means clustering algorithm was applied to the BPI group to separate patients into two groups as a function of their sensory impairment (k = 2). Thus, based on their PEI threshold values on the injured side, patients were grouped into more or less severely affected. Sensory thresholds of the uninjured side of both patient groups were then compared with the control group using the Mann-Whitney test. This analysis aimed to investigate if different levels of sensory deficits in the injured side would result in different patterns of sensory changes in the uninjured side.
Results
Control Values for Absolute Touch Threshold—“Typical” Index
A typical index per PEI was calculated for control subjects. The comparison among the six PEI threshold values in the control group revealed a significant difference (p < 0.0001). Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test (alfa = 0.05) revealed that the most proximal PEIs had lower threshold values than the most distal ones (Figure 2). Median (25th and 75th percentiles) PEI threshold values are presented in Table 2.
Figure 2. Comparison between the six PEIs of the control group revealed a significant difference (Kruskal-Wallis test—p < 0.0001). Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test (alfa = 0.05) applied to compare all pairs of PEIs revealed that the most proximal PEIs (axillary and musculocutaneous) had lower thresholds than the distal PEIs (median, radial and ulnar). *** p < 0.05 at the Dunn's Multiple Comparison Test.
Table 2. Median (Q1–Q3) threshold values for the control group and the injured and uninjured upper limbs of the BPI patients.
Control vs. Injured Side
The comparison between the control group and the BPI patients' injured side showed a clear difference for all evaluated PEIs (Figure 3). It confirms the important sensory deficit in the latter group. Touch thresholds on the injured side were highly variable among patients, ranging from 1.84 to 6.20 (Table 2). For several patients, even the thickest monofilament was not detectable in some PEIs.
Figure 3. Individual sensory threshold values in the 6 PEIs of the control group compared to the BPI patients' injured upper limb. Lines represent the median values of each group with the interquartile ranges. P-values of the statistical difference between groups (control × injured) are presented in each graph. The broken line represents the top limit of the set of monofilaments used (6.20). ● = control (n = 14) and ■ = BPI patients (n = 17).
Control vs. Uninjured Side
The comparison between the control group and the patients' uninjured side is shown in Figure 4. Statistical analysis showed differences for the median, radial, ulnar, and MABC PEIs, indicating that the uninjured side is also affected by the BPI. Moreover, sensory threshold values from the patients uninjured limb were globally considerably more variable than those of the control group, with several patient values lying above the control group range (Table 2).
Figure 4. Individual sensory threshold values of the 6 PEIs of the control group compared to the BPI patients' uninjured upper limb. Lines represent the median values and the interquartile ranges of each group. P-values of the difference between groups (control × uninjured) are presented in each graph. The broken line represents the top limit of the set of monofilaments (6.20). ● = control (n = 14) and ■ = BPI patients (n = 17).
Spearman correlation coefficients analysis showed no significant correlation between the normalized values of each uninjured PEI and patient age or for the time interval between the injury and the assessment (p > 0.1).
Side of Injury and Sensory Deficits in the Uninjured Side
After excluding the left handed and the ambidextrous patients we found that patients with BPI in the dominant right limb presented higher threshold values for the uninjured side compared to the control group at the median (p = 0.0456), radial (p = 0.0096) and MABC (p = 0.0078) PEIs (axillary: p = 0.0732; musculocutaneous: p = 0.6531; ulnar: p = 0.0628). Conversely, patients with BPI in the non-dominant left limb showed no PEI threshold difference from those of the control group (axillary: p = 0.3762; musculocutaneous: p = 0.1157; median: p = 0.0568; radial: p = 0.2722; ulnar: p = 0.4035; MABC: p = 0.6539). Table 3 presents median threshold values of both groups. With respect to the ambidextrous and the left handed patients we cannot take any conclusion regarding the small sample size.
Table 3. Median (Q1–Q3) threshold values for the injured and uninjured upper limbs after a BPI in the dominant and non-dominant limb.
Severity of Injury and Sensory Deficits in the Uninjured Side
A K-means clustering algorithm applied to the BPI group (k = 2) resulted in the formation of two groups: one with less severe sensory impairment (n = 10: BPI01, BPI02, BPI03, BPI07, BPI08, BPI09, BPI10, BPI11, BPI13, and BPI14) and the other with more severe BPI impairment of sensory function (n = 7: BPI04, BPI05, BPI06, BPI12, BPI15, BPI16, and BPI17). For graphs depicting the patients' individual assessment of both limbs, see Supplementary Figures 1, 2.
The comparison between the control group and the uninjured side of the less severely affected patients (n = 10) revealed higher thresholds for patients at the median (p = 0.0189), radial (p = 0.0081), ulnar (p = 0.0253), and MABC (p = 0.0187) PEIs (axillary: p = 0.6482; musculocutaneous: p = 0.3864). Similar results were obtained after excluding the ambidextrous patient from the sample, with higher thresholds for the uninjured side in the less severely affected patients (n = 9) compared to the control group (axillary: p = 0.5261; musculocutaneous: p = 0.4581; median: p = 0.0277; radial: p = 0.0161; ulnar: p = 0.0466; and MABC: p = 0.0367).
From this sample of less severely affected patients, six have undergone ulnar to musculocutaneous nerve transfer. For these 6 patients the difference between the uninjured side and the control group was found only for the ulnar nerve (p = 0.0332) (axillary: p = 0.5500; musculocutaneous: p = 0.1745; median: p = 0.0791; radial: p = 0.1582; MABC: p = 0.2452).
The comparison between the uninjured side of the more severely affected patients and the control group showed statistical difference only for the median PEI (p = 0.0457) (axillary: p = 0.3976; musculocutaneous: p = 0.3958; radial: p = 0.3109; ulnar: p = 0.3434; MABC: p = 0.3288). This group comprised 6 right-handed and one left-handed patients. When the left handed patient was excluded from this group, no difference between the uninjured side and the control group was observed (axillary: p = 0.7989; musculocutaneous: p = 0.1745; median: p = 0.1033; radial: p = 0.3002; ulnar: p = 0.6440; MABC: p = 0.4543).
Taken together, these results suggest that more severe injuries are associated with minor sensory dysfunction in the uninjured upper limb. In contrast, for the less severe group, the unaffected upper limb presented a higher number of affected PEIs. Table 4 presents median threshold values of both groups.
Table 4. Median (Q1–Q3) threshold values for the injured and uninjured upper limbs of the less and more severe BPI.
Discussion
By applying Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments, we were able to characterize, for the first time, superficial sensibility deficits in points of exclusive innervation (PEI) both in the injured and the uninjured upper limbs in BPI patients as compared to an age-paired and sex-matched control group. The comparison between the control group and the BPI patients' injured side showed higher thresholds for all PEIs in the affected limb. Interestingly, the comparison between the sensory thresholds of the control group and those of the BPI patients' uninjured side also revealed higher thresholds for the median, radial, ulnar and MABC PEIs. After splitting the sample of right handed patients with respect to the dominance of the injured limb, we found higher threshold values for the uninjured side in the BPI patients in the right dominant limb compared to the control group at the median, radial and MABC PEIs. On the other hand, in patients with BPI in the left non-dominant arm, this effect was absent. Using the K-means clustering algorithm, the patient sample was then split into two groups based on the threshold values of the injured side. Thresholds of the uninjured side of the resulting two groups of patients were compared to those of the control group. Less severely affected BPI patients (n = 10) had higher thresholds for the median, radial, ulnar and MABC PEIs. Conversely, more severe BPI patients showed higher thresholds only for the median PEI. These results suggest that both the laterality and the degree of sensory impairment of the injured arm associate to higher sensory thresholds in the uninjured side. These findings expand the current knowledge on sensory changes after peripheral nerve lesions, providing novel approaches to clinical evaluation and also opening up new possibilities for the study of central reorganization after peripheral injury.
“Typical” Threshold Values for the Upper Limb
In this study, it was possible to establish the control group's “typical” threshold for each PEI. The lack of difference between the sensory thresholds for the left and right upper limb in the control subjects corroborates previous findings (28). A “typical” threshold value has been previously described in healthy subjects (29). Many studies have employed this threshold value to evaluate all dermatomes of the upper limb (hand/arm/forearm) (11, 12, 14). Nevertheless, we found lower threshold values in proximal PEIs (axillary and musculocutaneous) than in distal PEIs (median, radial and ulnar). One possible explanation for this discrepancy may be related to the hairiness of the evaluated skin. Proximal PEIs are in hairy skin regions while the distal ones are in glabrous skin (30). The hair follicle shaft has collars of mechanoreceptor terminals, including at least three low threshold mechanoreceptor subtypes (31). Therefore, hair deflection might be associated with a lower threshold in the proximal PEIs. In this case, applying the normative threshold value of 2.85, which is higher than the “typical” threshold found for this population, to assess the entire upper limb could induce the detection of false negative sensory deficits.
Threshold Values in the Injured Upper Limb After a BPI
The assessment of the injured upper limb of the BPI patients at the six selected PEIs agreed with their clinical diagnoses. Indeed, in the majority of our sample BPI had been documented as affecting mainly the superior and middle trunk of the brachial plexus. Accordingly, axillary, musculocutaneous and the median PEIs presented the highest sensory thresholds when compared to the control group. The huge variability amongst threshold values found for most PEIs in the injured limb (Figure 2) can be attributed to the highly variable degree of severity that is commonly seen for BPI (1).
Unilateral BPI Induces Bilateral Touch Threshold Impairment
BPI led to increased tactile threshold in specific PEIs of the uninjured upper limb. These results are in agreement with a variety of deafferentation models that also found changes in sensory processing in the uninjured limb (18–23, 32). Both in burn patients (22, 23) and lower limb amputees (20), the assessment of cutaneous threshold presented higher values for the uninjured limb as compared to controls.
The sensory impairment in the uninjured limb found in BPI patients suggests that BPI leads to central modifications in the hemisphere contralateral to the uninjured limb, as shown in different models of deafferentation (33–41). As reviewed by Wall et al. (42), chronic nerve injuries (peripheral nerve injury, root injury, spinal injury, and amputation) in humans and primates promote not only cortical but also subcortical changes at all levels of the somatosensory system. In animal models, unilateral injuries also lead to bilateral changes in the somatosensory ascending pathway (43–46).
For contralateral alterations to be reflected in the ipsilateral hemibody there must be an interhemispheric transfer of information at some level in the brain. Sensory input is ordinarily processed in the contralateral primary somatosensory cortex but there is also to some extent an ipsilateral cortical response to peripheral stimulation (47, 48). As shown by Iwamura et al. (47), this ipsilateral response seems to depend on the opposite hemisphere via transcallosal connections between homotopic areas. In addition, as described by Preuss and Goldman-Rakic (49) in primates, the midline thalamic nuclei project bilaterally to the prefrontal cortex, an important path of top-down regulation of attention to a given stimulus (50). Thus, the altered ascending information coming from the injured limb could lead to reduced activity in the intralaminar nuclei and reduce the attentional network associated with the detection of ascending tactile stimuli, both for the injured and the uninjured limb.
BPIs of the Dominant Right Limb Are Linked to a Large Sensory Impairment in the Uninjured Side
When the injury occurred in the dominant right limb, higher sensory thresholds were observed in the non-dominant left uninjured limb. Thus, higher thresholds were found for the uninjured limb when the limb was the dominant one. With respect to the ambidextrous and the left handed patients we cannot take any conclusion due to the small sample size.
A behavioral study of tactile perception after bilateral hand transplant showed that the somatosensory perception of the right hand was largely reduced when the right side of the face was concurrently stimulated. The same was not true for the left side (51). After a bilateral hand allograft, Vargas et al. (52) showed that the motor cortical reorganization was faster and more extensive for the non-dominant hand than for the dominant hand. One possibility raised by these authors was that the hemisphere contralateral to the dominant hand is more “hardwired” (and thus less plastic) in the context of a bilateral hand allograft. Interestingly, the amputation of the dominant upper limb elicits more errors and slower responses in motor imagery and handedness judgment tasks (53).
Greater cortical functional reorganization is observed in patients with BPI when the dominant limb is affected as compared to the non-dominant limb (54). This suggests that a relatively more extensive adaptive process may occur following an injury to the dominant hand. The long-standing loss and/or disuse of the dominant limb may degrade the sensorimotor efficiency of both the dominant and the non-dominant upper limb. If this is correct, then it is possible that a dominant side BPI has a greater impact over sensorimotor representations, with reduced plasticity in the hemisphere contralateral to the dominant hand.
Reduced Sensory Impairments in the Injured Side Are Linked to Greater Sensory Impairment in the Uninjured Side
The group of patients with less severe sensory impairment presented a higher number of affected PEIs in their uninjured upper limb, while the more severe lesions were associated with lower sensory dysfunction in the uninjured upper limb. These results suggest that the severity of injury is inversely related to the impaired threshold detection observed in the uninjured upper limb. One possibility is that the less severe injury is associated with a higher sensorimotor disorganization in the hemisphere contralateral to the injured limb. This, in turn, would affect the representations of the uninjured limb to a greater extent. Indeed, Jain et al. (55) showed in owl monkeys that an incomplete unilateral cervical (C3–C4) dorsal column section leads to expanded cortical representations in the contralateral cortical somatosensory area 3b. Interestingly, the remaining parts of the hand in the deafferented cortical areas present larger receptive fields and abnormal response properties compared to complete lesions. It was also shown that the combination of median+radial nerve injury leads to far more silent regions in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) than that of the median+ulnar lesion (56–58). These results suggest that both the type and extent of peripheral lesion matter when it comes to the cortical reorganization of S1 topographical maps. The manner by which these changes reflect modifications of the representation of the unaffected limb is unknown. The abnormal reorganization taking place in the less severe injuries can thus contribute to higher touch thresholds in the contralateral hemibody due to changes in interhemispheric communication of cortical maps (40, 41).
The group of patients classified as more severely impaired corresponded to those with extended lesions (superior, medium, and inferior trunks, SMI), most of them having undergone an intercostal to musculocutaneous nerve transfer. This surgery has already been proven to produce changes in the biceps cortical representation (59, 60). However, the small sample of patients with this type of surgery recruited in the present study precluded any further analysis on the effect of this nerve transfer upon sensory threshold impairment in the uninjured limb.
The group of less severely affected patients comprised those having undergone ulnar to musculocutaneous nerve transfer. Contrasting with the prevailing effect of higher threshold values found for the uninjured side, for these patients the difference between the uninjured side and the control group was found only for the ulnar nerve. As discussed above, a less severe injury would be associated with a higher sensorimotor disorganization in the hemisphere contralateral to the injured limb. This in turn would lead to a change in the cortical representation of the uninjured limb. In this context, the ulnar to musculocutaneous nerve transfer, already associated to functional improvement of the affected limb (61) and possibly of cortical representation restoration (62), could play a role by reducing the tactile threshold impairment of the uninjured limb through callosal modulatory effects (38).
Conclusion
This is the first report of superficial sensibility deficit in both injured and the uninjured upper limbs in BPI patients. Furthermore, the higher sensory deficits in the uninjured side were associated to BPI of the dominant limb and the lower severity of sensory deficits in the injured side. These findings corroborate previous results reported after other peripheral injuries such as in amputees and suggest a superordinate model of representational plasticity occurring bilaterally in the brain after unilateral peripheral deafferentation. Indeed, peripheral nerve lesion causes continuous, time-dependent adaptation in the cortical network (63, 64). Among the limitations of the current study we highlight the small sample size as well as its heterogeneity regarding the lesion extent, the occurrence and type of surgical intervention and the time elapsed from lesion. Expanding the knowledge on sensory changes after peripheral nerve lesions might provide novel approaches to understand and treat BPI.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.
Ethics Statement
Participants were informed about the experimental procedures and provided written informed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the ethics committee of the Institute of Neurology Deolindo Couto—UFRJ, Brazil (number: 298.925 from June 10th, 2013), and was in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki.
Author Contributions
BR: conception, data collection, analysis, and interpretation and drafting. MR: data collection, analysis, and interpretation and drafting. AS: conception and data collection. FE and CV: conception, data analysis, and interpretation and revision.
Funding
This work was funded by the Fundação de amparo à pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP) project NeuroMat (grant 2013/07699-0), Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa (CNPq) (grants 306817/2014-4 and 309560/2017-9), Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro FAPERJ (grants E-26/111.655/ 2012 and E26/010.002902/2014), and Financiadora de Estudos e Projetos—FINEP (Pró-infra Hospitalar - 0.1.12.0308.00).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank José Fernando Guedes Correa, Paulo Leonardo Tavares, José Vicente Martins, and Michelle Miranda for their contribution to this work and Karen Reilly for her comments on a previous version of this manuscript.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2019.00872/full#supplementary-material
Supplementary Figure 1. Individual sensory threshold values in the 6 PEIs of each BPI patients' injured (dark gray) and uninjured (light gray) upper limbs of the more severe group. The black line represents the median values of the control group (n = 14). The top dashed line represents the higher threshold value possible to assess with our filaments set (6.20). ϕ = PEIs in which the subjects were unable to feel even the thickest filament assessed (6.20).
Supplementary Figure 2. Individual sensory threshold values in the 6 PEIs of each BPI patients' injured (dark gray) and uninjured (light gray) upper limbs of the less severe group. The black line represents the median values of the control group (n = 14). The top dashed line represents the higher threshold value possible to assess with our filaments set (6.20). ϕ = PEIs in which the subjects were unable to feel even the thickest filament assessed (6.20).
Supplementary Table 1. Calibration measures of the set of 20 SWM used. Forces in grams (g) and the log of the force (log) (10 × force in mg) needed to bend the filament (90° to the skin) in a “C” shape according to the manufacturer (Manufacturer's information) and after filaments calibration (Mean of the Calibrations).
Abbreviations
BPI, Brachial plexus injury; MABC, medial antebrachial cutaneous nerve; PEI, Point of exclusive innervation; SWM, Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments.
References
1. Midha R. Epidemiology of brachial plexus injuries in a multitrauma population. Neurosurgery. (1997) 40:1182–9. doi: 10.1097/00006123-199706000-00014
2. Moran SL, Steinmann SP, Shin AY. Adult brachial plexus injuries: mechanism, patterns of injury, and physical diagnosis. Hand Clin. (2005) 21:13–24. doi: 10.1016/j.hcl.2004.09.004
3. Mancuso CA, Lee SK, Dy CJ, Landers ZA, Model Z, Wolfe SW. Expectations and limitations due to brachial plexus injury: a qualitative study. Hand. (2015) 10:741–9. doi: 10.1007/s11552-015-9761-z
4. Franzblau L, Chung KC. Psychosocial outcomes and coping after complete avulsion traumatic brachial plexus injury. Disabil Rehabil. (2015) 37:135–43. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2014.911971
5. Thatte MR, Babhulkar S, Hiremath A. Brachial plexus injury in adults: diagnosis and surgical treatment strategies. Ann Indian Acad Neurol. (2013) 16:26–33. doi: 10.4103/0972-2327.107686
6. Johansson RS, Westling G. Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp Brain Res. (1984) 56:550–64. doi: 10.1007/BF00237997
7. Johansson RS, Flanagan JR. Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:345–59. doi: 10.1038/nrn2621
8. Novak CB. Evaluation of the nerve-injured patient. Clin Physiol. (2003) 30:127–38. doi: 10.1016/S0094-1298(02)00098-6
9. Bentolila V, Nizard R, Bizot P, Sedel L. Complete traumatic brachial plexus palsy. J Bone Jt Surg. (1999) 81A:20–8. doi: 10.2106/00004623-199901000-00004
10. Doi K, Muramatsu K, Hattori Y, Otsuka K, Tan S-H, Nanda V, et al. Restoration of prehension with the double free muscle technique following complete avulsion of the brachial plexus. J Bone Jt Surg. (2000) 82A:652–66. doi: 10.2106/00004623-200005000-00006
11. Htut M, Misra P, Anand P, Birch R, Carlstedt T. Pain phenomena and sensory recovery following brachial plexus avulsion injury and surgical repairs. J Hand Surg Am. (2006) 31B:596–605. doi: 10.1016/J.JHSB.2006.04.027
12. Bertelli JA, Ghizoni MF, Loureiro Chaves DP. Sensory disturbances and pain complaints after brachial plexus root injury: a prospective study involving 150 adult patients. Microsurgery. (2011) 31:93–7. doi: 10.1002/micr.20832
13. Bertelli JA, Ghizoni MF. Very distal sensory nerve transfers in high median nerve lesions. J Hand Surg Am. (2011) 36:387–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jhsa.2010.11.049
14. Bertelli JA. Distal sensory nerve transfers in lower-type injuries of the brachial plexus. J Hand Surg Am. (2012) 37A:1194–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jhsa.2012.02.047
15. Russel SM (ed). The diagnostic anatomy of the brachial plexus. In: Examination of Peripheral Nerve Injuries: An Anatomical Approach. New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers Inc. (2006). p. 66–89.
16. Highet WB. Procaine nerve block in the investigation of peripheral nerve injuries. J Neurol Psychiatry. (1942) 5:101–16. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.5.3-4.101
17. Haymaker W, Woodhall B. Causes and manifestations of peripheral nerve injuries. In: Peripheral Nerve Injuries: Principles of Diagnosis. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders (1953). p. 138–41.
18. Werhahn KJ, Mortensen J, Van Boven RW, Zeuner KE, Cohen LG. Enhanced tactile spatial acuity and cortical processing during acute hand deafferentation. Nat Neurosci. (2002) 5:936–8. doi: 10.1038/nn917
19. De La Llave-Rincón AI, Fernández-De-Las-Peñas C, Fernández-Carnero J, Padua L, Arendt-Nielsen L, Pareja JA. Bilateral hand/wrist heat and cold hyperalgesia, but not hypoesthesia, in unilateral carpal tunnel syndrome. Exp Brain Res. (2009) 198:455–63. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1941-z
20. Kavounoudias A, Tremblay C, Gravel D, Iancu A, Forget R. Bilateral changes in somatosensory sensibility after unilateral below-knee amputation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2005) 86:633–640. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.10.030
21. Wahren LK. Changes in thermal and mechanical pain thresholds in hand amputeres. A clinical and physiological long term follow-up. Pain. (1990) 42:269–77. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(90)91139-A
22. Malenfant A, Forget R, Amsel R, Papillon J, Frigon JY, Choinière M. Tactile, thermal and pain sensibility in burned patients with and without chronic pain and paresthesia problems. Pain. (1998) 77:241–51. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00096-7
23. Hermanson A, Jonsson C-E, Lindblom U. Sensibility after burn injury. Clin Physiol. (1986) 6:507–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-097X.1986.tb00784.x
24. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
25. Gardner EP, Johnson KO. The somatosensory system: receptors and central pathways. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, Siegelbaum SA, Hudspeth AJ, editors. Principles of Neural Science. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill (2013). p. 475–97.
26. Gardner EP, Johnson KO. Sensory coding. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, Siegelbaum SA, Hudspeth AJ, editors. Principles of Neural Science. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill (2013). p. 449–74.
27. Hartigan JA, Wong MA. Algorithm AS 136 : a K-means clustering algorithm. J R Stat Soc. (1979) 28:100–8. doi: 10.2307/2346830
28. Jain S, Muzzafarullah S, Peri S, Ellanti R, Moorthy K, Nath I. Lower touch sensibility in the extremities of healthy Indians: further deterioration with age. J Peripher Nerv Syst. (2008) 13:47–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-8027.2008.00157.x
29. Bell-Krotoski JA, Fess EE, Figarola JH, Hiltz D. Threshold detection and Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments. J Hand Ther. (1995) 8:155–62. doi: 10.1016/S0894-1130(12)80314-0
30. Garn SM. Types and distribution of the hair in man. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1951) 53:498–507. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1951.tb31952.x
31. Zimmerman A, Bai L, Ginty DD. The gentle touch receptors of mammalian skin. Science. (2014) 346:950–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1254229
32. Konopka KH, Harbers M, Houghton A, Kortekaas R, van Vliet A, Timmerman W, et al. Bilateral sensory abnormalities in patients with unilateral neuropathic pain; A quantitative sensory testing (QST) study. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e37524. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037524
33. Capaday C, Richardson MP, Rothwell JC, Brooks DJ. Long-term changes of GABAergic function in the sensorimotor cortex of amputees. A combined magnetic stimulation and 11C-flumazenil PET study. Exp Brain Res. (2000) 133:552–6. doi: 10.1007/s002210000477
34. Werhahn KJ, Mortensen J, Kaelin-Lang A, Boroojerdi B, Cohen LG. Cortical excitability changes induced by deafferentation of the contralateral hemisphere. Brain. (2002) 125:1402–13. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf140
35. Fraiman D, Miranda MF, Erthal F, Buur PF, Elschot M, Souza L, et al. Reduced functional connectivity within the primary motor cortex of patients with brachial plexus injury. NeuroImage Clin. (2016) 12:277–84. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.07.008
36. Calford MB, Tweedale R. Interhemispheric transfer of plasticity in the cerebral cortex. Science. (1990) 249:805–7. doi: 10.1126/science.2389146
37. Elbert T, Sterr A, Flor H, Rockstroh B, Knecht S, Pantev C, et al. Input-increase and input-decrease types of cortical reorganization after upper extremity amputation in humans. Exp Brain Res. (1997) 117:161–4. doi: 10.1007/s002210050210
38. Simoes EL, Bramati I, Rodrigues E, Franzoi A, Moll J, Lent R, et al. Functional expansion of sensorimotor representation and structural reorganization of callosal connections in lower limb amputees. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:3211–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4592-11.2012
39. Pawela CP, Biswal BB, Hudetz AG, Li R, Jones SR, Cho YR, et al. Interhemispheric neuroplasticity following limb deafferentation detected by resting-state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging (fcMRI) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Neuroimage. (2010) 49:1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.054
40. Hsieh J-C, Cheng H, Hsieh H-M, Liao K-K, Wu Y-T, Yeh T-C, et al. Loss of interhemispheric inhibition on the ipsilateral primary sensorimotor cortex in patients with brachial plexus injury: fMRI study. Ann Neurol. (2002) 51:381–5. doi: 10.1002/ana.10149
41. Liu B, Li T, Tang WJ, Zhang JH, Sun HP, Xu WD, et al. Changes of inter-hemispheric functional connectivity between motor cortices after brachial plexuses injury: a resting-state fMRI study. Neuroscience. (2013) 243:33–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.048
42. Wall JT, Xu J, Wang X. Human brain plasticity: an emerging view of the multiple substrates and mechanisms that cause cortical changes and related sensory dysfunctions after injuries of sensory inputs from the body. Brain Res Rev. (2002) 39:181–215. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00192-3
43. Behera D, Behera S, Jacobs KE, Biswal S. Bilateral peripheral neural activity observed in vivo following unilateral nerve injury. Am J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. (2013) 3:282–90.
44. Verge VM, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, Hökfelt T. Cholecystokinin in mammalian primary sensory neurons and spinal cord: in situ hybridization studies in rat and monkey. Eur J Neurosci. (1993) 5:240–50.
45. Chew DJ, Carlstedt T, Shortland PJ. A comparative histological analysis of two models of nerve root avulsion injury in the adult rat. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol. (2011) 37:613–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2990.2011.01176.x
46. Koltzenburg M, Wall PD, McMahon SB. Does the right hand know what the left hand is doing? Trends Neurosci. (1999) 22:122–7. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01302-2
47. Iwamura Y, Taoka M, Iriki A. Bilateral activity and callosal connections in the somatosensory cortex. Neuroscientist. (2001) 7:419–29. doi: 10.1177/107385840100700511
48. Hansson T, Brismar T. Tactile stimulation of the hand causes bilateral cortical activation: a functional magnetic resonance study in humans. Neurosci Lett. (1999) 271:29–32. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00508-X
49. Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Crossed corticothalamic and thalamocortical connections of macaque prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol. (1987) 257:269–81. doi: 10.1002/cne.902570211
50. Hagen MC, Zald DH, Thornton TA, Pardo JV. Somatosensory processing in the human inferior prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. (2002) 88:1400–6. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1400
51. Farnè A, Roy AC, Giraux P, Pawson T, Sirigu A. Face or hand, not both: perceptual correlates of reafferentation in a former amputee. Curr Biol. (2002) 12:1342–6. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01018-7
52. Vargas CD, Aballéa A, Rodrigues EC, Reilly KT, Mercier C, Petruzzo P, et al. Re-emergence of hand-muscle representations in human motor cortex after hand allograft. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2009) 106:7197–202. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809614106
53. Nico D, Daprati E, Rigal F, Parsons L, Sirigu A. Left and right hand recognition in upper limb amputees. Brain. (2004) 127:120–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh006
54. Feng JT, Liu HQ, Xu JG, Gu YD, Shen YD. Differences in brain adaptive functional reorganization in right and left total brachial plexus injury patients. World Neurosurg. (2015) 84:702–8. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2015.04.046
55. Jain N, Catania KC, Kaas JH. Deactivation and reactivation of somatosensory cortex after dorsal spinal cord injury. Nature. (1997) 386:495–8. doi: 10.1038/386495a0
56. Merzenich MM, Kaas JH, Wall J, Nelson RJ, Sur M, Felleman D. Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation. Neuroscience. (1983) 8:33–55. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(83)90024-6
57. Garraghty PE, Kaas JH. Large-scale functional reorganization in adult monkey cortex after peripheral nerve injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (1991) 88:6976–80. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.16.6976
58. Garraghty PE, Hanes DP, Florence SL, Kaas JH. Pattern of peripheral deafferentation predicts reorganizational limits in adult primate somatosensory cortex. Somatosens Mot Res. (1994) 11:109–17. doi: 10.3109/08990229409028864
59. Malessy MJ, van der Kamp W, Thomeer RT, van Dijk JG. Cortical excitability of the biceps muscle after intercostal-to-musculocutaneous nerve transfer. Neurosurgery. (1998) 42:787–94. doi: 10.1097/00006123-199804000-00062
60. Malessy MJ, Bakker D, Dekker AJ, Van Duk JG, Thomeer RT. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and control over the biceps muscle after intercostal-musculocutaneous nerve transfer. Neurosurgery. (2003) 98:261–8. doi: 10.3171/jns.2003.98.2.0261
61. Oberlin C, Beal D, Leechavengvongs S, Salon A, Dauge MC, Sarcy JJ. Nerve transfer to biceps muscle using part of ulnar nerve for C5-C6 avulsion of the brachial plexus: anatomical study and report of four cases. J Hand Surg. (1994) 19:232–7. doi: 10.1016/0363-5023(94)90011-6
62. de Sousa AC, Guedes-Corrêa JF. Post-Oberlin procedure cortical neuroplasticity in traumatic injury of the upper brachial plexus. Radiol Bras. (2016) 49:201–2. doi: 10.1590/0100-3984.2015.0082
63. Jiang S, Li Z-Y, Hua X-Y, Xu W-D, Xu J-G, Gu Y-D. Reorganization in motor cortex after brachial plexus avulsion injury and repair with the contralateral C7 root transfer in rats. Microsurgery. (2010) 30:314–20. doi: 10.1002/micr.20747
Keywords: Semmens-Weinstein monofilaments, sensory threshold, brachial plexus neuropathy, impairment, light touch sensation, deafferentation, uninjured
Citation: Ramalho BL, Rangel ML, Schmaedeke AC, Erthal FS and Vargas CD (2019) Unilateral Brachial Plexus Lesion Impairs Bilateral Touch Threshold. Front. Neurol. 10:872. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00872
Received: 31 July 2018; Accepted: 26 July 2019;
Published: 13 August 2019.
Edited by:
Giorgio Sandrini, University of Pavia, ItalyReviewed by:
Lukas Rasulić, Clinical Center of Serbia, SerbiaAlessandro Picelli, University of Verona, Italy
Copyright © 2019 Ramalho, Rangel, Schmaedeke, Erthal and Vargas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Bia Lima Ramalho, cmFtYWxob2JzbCYjeDAwMDQwO2dtYWlsLmNvbQ==