Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 29 October 2018
Sec. Neuro-Ophthalmology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Pupil: Behavior, Anatomy, Physiology and Clinical Biomarkers View all 25 articles

Influence of Strategic Cortical Infarctions on Pupillary Function

\r\nCostanza Peinkhofer,,Costanza Peinkhofer1,2,3Pernille MartensPernille Martens4Johannes GrandJohannes Grand5Thomas TruelsenThomas Truelsen1Gitte M. Knudsen,,Gitte M. Knudsen1,2,6Jesper KjaergaardJesper Kjaergaard5Daniel Kondziella,,*Daniel Kondziella1,6,7*
  • 1Department of Neurology, Copenhagen University Hospital Rigshospitalet, Denmark
  • 2Neurobiology Research Unit, Denmark
  • 3Medical Faculty, University of Trieste, Italy
  • 4Department of Radiology, Copenhagen University Hospital Rigshospitalet, Denmark
  • 5Department of Cardiology, Copenhagen University Hospital Rigshospitalet, Denmark
  • 6Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 7Department of Neuroscience, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Norway

Objective: Cortical activity, including cognitive and emotional processes, may influence pupillary function. The exact pathways and the site of cortical pupillary innervation remain elusive, however. We investigated the effects of select cortical strokes, i.e. ischemic infarcts affecting the insular cortex and prefrontal eye field, on pupillary function.

Methods: Seventy-four patients with acute ischemic stroke, consecutively admitted to our institution from March to July 2018, were assessed 24 h after endovascular recanalization therapy (i.e., day 2 after the stroke), using automated pupillometry. Stroke location and volume and clinical severity (estimated by the Alberta Stroke Program Early CT Score and National Institute of Health Stroke Scale) were recorded. We excluded patients with posterior circulation stroke, intracranial pathology other than ischemic stroke, midline shift on computed tomography exceeding 5 millimeters or a history of eye disease. Pupillometry data from 25 neurologically normal patients with acute myocardial infarction were acquired for control.

Results: Fifty stroke patients after thrombectomy were included for analysis. Twenty-five patients (50%) had insular cortex or prefrontal eye field involvement (group 1, strategic infarcts); 25 patients had infarcts located in other cerebral areas (group 2, other infarcts). The pupillary light reflex, as measured by constriction velocity and maximal/minimal pupillary diameters, was within physiological limits in all patients, including controls. However, while pupillary size and constriction velocities were correlated in all subjects, the correlation of size and dilatation velocity was absent in right-hemispheric infarcts (left hemisphere infarcts, group 1 (r2 = 0.15, p = 0.04), group 2 (r2 = 0.41, p = 0.0007); right hemisphere infarcts, group 1 (r2 = 0.008, p = 0.69); group 2 (r2 = 0.12, p = 0.08); controls (r2 = 0.29, p ≤ 0.0001).

Conclusions: Cortical infarcts of the prefrontal eye field or insula do not impair the pupillary light reflex in humans. However, subtle changes may occur when the pupils dilate back to baseline, probably due to autonomic dysfunction. Replication is needed to explore the possible influence of hemispheric lateralization. We suggest that endovascular therapy for acute ischemic stroke may serve as a clinical research model for the study of acquired cortical lesions in humans.

Introduction

The pupillary light reflex is a polysynaptic reflex that requires cranial nerves II and III, as well as central brainstem connections (1). Light falling into one eye stimulates retinal photoreceptors, bipolar cells and subsequently retinal ganglion cells, whose axons form the optic nerve. Some of these axons terminate in the pretectum of the mesencephalon; and pretectal neurons project further to the Edinger-Westphal nuclei. Then, preganglionic parasympathetic axons synapse with ciliary ganglion neurons which in turn send postganglionic axons to innervate the pupillary constrictor muscles of both eyes (1).

Although the pupillary light reflex is part of the routine neurological examination, its physiological background is less well-understood than most clinicians are aware of. In addition to the pathways outlined above, there is also a cortical component of pupillary innervation. For instance, emotional responses and cognitive processes such as decision making and mental arithmetic may produce pupillary dilatation (28). Further, electrical stimulation of the frontal eye field in monkeys leads to pupillary dilatation (9, 10). Via connections with the intermediate layer of the superior colliculus, the frontal eye field appears to be able to modulate pupillary diameters, resulting in pupillary dilation during cognitive processes (10). Another gray matter region that may contribute to pupillary function is the insular cortex. An important region for arousal and autonomic control (11, 12), the insular cortex is involved [together with the anterior cingulate cortex (13, 14)] in the control of the locus coeruleus, the noradrenergic brainstem center (15), and may thereby influence pupillary function via sympathetic-parasympathetic innervation (13, 16, 17). However, the exact pathways of cortical modulation of human pupillary function remain elusive.

The present study aimed at investigating cortical modulation of pupillary reflex pathways by using routinely collected data in a clinical setting. To this end, we correlated automated pupillometry with cerebral infarct locations in stroke patients after endovascular thrombectomy, which served as a paradigm for pupillary changes caused by select cerebral lesions. We hypothesized that patients with strategic infarcts localized to the prefrontal eye field (Broadman area 8) and/or the insular cortex on either side would have pupillary abnormalities compared to stroke patients without infarcts in these areas. Pupillometry data from neurologically normal patients with acute but clinically stable myocardial infarction, investigated after percutaneous coronary intervention, served as control group.

Materials and Methods

Inclusion Criteria

We assessed pupillometry data from stroke patients (aged ≥18 years) with an anterior circulation stroke (i.e., affecting internal carotid artery, middle cerebral artery and/or anterior cerebral artery territories) consecutively admitted for acute endovascular thrombectomy to the Department of Neurology, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen University Hospital, during the period from March to July 2018.

Exclusion Criteria

Patients with a history of eye disease (e.g., following cataract operation), relevant structural pathology on CT other than ischemic stroke (e.g., tumors), and mass effects on CT exceeding a midline shift of 5 mm (measured at the level of the pineal gland) were excluded. In addition, we excluded patients with evidence of posterior circulation strokes (acute or chronic) to avoid lesions involving the brainstem and occipital cortex.

Procedures

For automated pupillometry, we used the NPi®-200 Pupillometer (NeurOptics, Laguna Hills, CA 92653 USA), a portable, handheld, monocular, infrared device, which allows quantitative measurements of the pupillary response. The pupillometer releases a flash of white light (duration 0.8 s, pulse intensity 121 uW) to stimulate the pupillary light reflex. Light calibration is performed by the manufacturer and does not require any periodic re-calibration. The pupillometer digitally registers the pupillary light response as a video (sampling rate 30 Hz) and displays numeric results on a screen (Table 1, Figure 1). For an illustration of the NPi®-200 pupillometer, please consider https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EjlZ5oocl0g&frags=pl%2Cwn. Measurements were performed once in both eyes as part of the routine clinical evaluation 24 h after the endovascular treatment (i.e., on the second day of stroke) immediately before the CT scan control. During pupillometry measurement of each eye the opposite eye was covered to minimize the consensual light reflex and its effect on the pupillary baseline diameter. National Institute of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS) scores 24 h after stroke onset were also collected as part of the clinical routine. Twenty-four hours control computed tomography (CT) of the brain was assessed by a trained neuroradiologist for infarctions localized in the prefrontal eye field (Brodmann area 8), the insular cortex and/or the thalamus in either hemisphere (Figure 1). In addition, CT was evaluated for overall stroke volume using the Alberta Stroke Program Early CT Score (ASPECTS) (18). We also recorded whether infarcts occurred in the thalamus, as this region is an important relay station of the visual pathway (19) (although typically supplied by the posterior, not anterior, circulation).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Variables assessed by pupillometry.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. We performed a clinical practice study investigating the cortical modulation of pupillary function following strategic cerebral strokes. This figure depicts CT of the brain from 2 exemplary stroke patients 24 h following endovascular therapy for large vessel occlusive stroke. Strategic ischemic infarctions are seen in the left prefrontal eye field (Left) and right insular cortex (Central). Using automated pupillometry [(Right); courtesy of https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/Main_Page], we collected pupillometry data of patients with strategic infarcts in the prefrontal eye field and/or insular cortex (group 1) and compared them to data from stroke patients without infarcts in these areas (group 2) and to data from patients with myocardial infarcts but without clinical evidence of brain injury (control group).

Pupillometry, CT, and NIHSS data were dichotomized according to stroke location: Group 1 included stroke patients with infarcts in the prefrontal eye field and insular cortex in either hemisphere (strategic infarcts); group 2 included stroke patients without infarcts in these regions. The control group consisted of patients with acute, clinically stable myocardial infarction after percutaneous coronary intervention from the Department of Cardiology, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen University Hospital. The latter patients underwent pupillometry as described, but neither NIHSS nor CT.

Outcome Measures

Outcome measures included the pupillary diameter before and after light exposure, percentage change of pupillary diameters, and pupillary constriction and dilatation velocities, as well as the neurological pupil index (NPi), which is a proprietary pupillometry sum score (i.e., a composite of quantitative pupillary parameters and a measure of the briskness of the pupil light reflex) from 0 to 5, with ≥3 indicating physiological limits (including a maximal difference between the 2 eyes of < 0.7) (20, 21) (Table 1). In addition, strategic infarcts were evaluated as outlined above (Figure 1).

Statistics

Statistical tests were performed using Prism 7 software (GraphPad Software; La Jolla, CA, USA). Baseline characteristics, dichotomized ASPECT (18) scores and NIHSS (22) and all pupillary parameters were first compared between group 1 (strategic strokes) and group 2 (other strokes), and then between group 1 and controls using either Fisher's exact test or unpaired or paired two tailed Student's t-tests. Where required, the Holm-Sidak method was performed to adjust results for multiple testing. In addition, the relationship between constriction and dilation velocities, respectively, maximum size before constriction and minimum size after constriction (i.e., diameter at maximal constriction) was examined using Pearson's correlation coefficient and linear regression for left and right eye in group 1 and controls; for left and right hemisphere infarcts in all three groups. A statistically significant difference was defined by a value of p < 0.05.

Ethics

All measurements were performed as part of routine clinical assessment. Data were anonymized and handled according to the European Union's Data Protection Law. The Ethics Committee of the Capital Region of Denmark approved the study concept and waived the need for written consent because risks were deemed negligible.

Results

Patients

Seventy-four patients were admitted for acute endovascular thrombectomy from March 1 to July 11, 2018. Twenty-four patients fulfilled exclusion criteria and their data were omitted. Fifty patients with an anterior circulation stroke were included for analysis [26 (52%) females; mean age 71.8 years, SD ± 10.8]. All stroke patients received endovascular treatment, followed by a control CT, NIHSS score and an assessment of pupillary function 24 h later. The control group consisted of 25 age- and sex-matched patients [11 (44%) females; mean age 67.8 years, SD ± 13.6] with acute, clinically stable myocardial infarction and without known cerebral stroke. Clinical baseline data are provided in Table 2. For raw data (clinical, radiological and pupillometric data) see Supplementary Table 1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Clinical baseline characteristics.

Location of Cerebral Infarcts and Stroke Burden

Of 25 patients with strategic infarctions (group 1), 17 had an involvement of the insular cortex alone, 2 of the prefrontal eye field, and 5 of both areas (Figure 1). Only 1 patient had a thalamic infarct, together with both insular and prefrontal eye field lesions. Eleven patients (44%) had a left hemispheric stroke, 14 (56%) a right-side hemispheric stroke. Twenty-five patients had lesions in other brain areas, 12 (48%) had a left sided stroke, 13 (52%) a right hemispheric stroke (group 2).

As expected, infarct volumes (estimated with the ASPECT score) were correlated with the presence of a strategic stroke. Patients with an ASPECT score ≤ 7 (n = 20 or 80% in group 1; n = 3 or 12% in group 2), indicating higher stroke volumes, had a significantly higher chance of having an insular or prefrontal eye field involvement compared to those with ASPECT >7 (Fisher's exact test; p < 0.0001).

The clinical severity, as revealed by the NIHSS score, was also associated with stroke location. NIHSS scores >10 (n = 15 or 56% in group 1; n = 6 or 24% in group 2) correlated with a higher probability of strategic infarctions (Fisher's exact test; p = 0.01).

Pupillometry

General pupillary function was normal in the 3 groups: The NPi index was >3 in all 75 patients (i.e., 150 eyes examined). Likewise, NPi differences between left and right eyes were always within physiological limits (< 0.7). Maximal and minimal pupillary diameters, percentage changes in pupillary sizes, latency of pupillary constrictions, as well as constriction and dilation velocities were also similar between group 1 (strategic infarcts) and group 2 (other infarcts) (Table 3). There were neither any differences of these parameters between group 1 and controls following adjustment for multiple testing (Table 3). In addition, the relative amplitude (initial diameter–minimum size) of the pupillary light reaction was calculated for group 1 and compared between left and right eyes (paired t-test p = 0.25, effectiveness of pairing r = 0.63, p = 0.0003), indicating normal consensual pupillary reactions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pupillometry data from stroke patients with and without strategic infarctions, and controls.

A positive correlation between maximum size and constriction velocity was found for both right and left eyes. The Pearson coefficient for left eyes was r = 0.62 (p = 0.0009) in group 1 (strategic infarcts) and r = 0.83 (p < 0.0001) in controls, and for right eyes the coefficient was r = 0.76 (p < 0.0001) in group 1 and r = 0.85 (p < 0.0001) in controls (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Pupillometry data from patients with strategic cerebral infarcts and controls: Maximal pupillary diameters and constriction velocities. Solid lines denote the best fit from linear regression analysis. (Left) Relationship between maximum diameter and constriction velocity of the left eye, group 1 in green (Y = 0.5945X+2.2, r2 = 0.38, p = 0.0009); controls in red (Y = 0.8142X + 1.772, r2 = 0.69, p < 0.0001). (Right) Relationship between maximum diameter and constriction velocity of the right eye, group 1 in green (Y = 0.6748X + 1.975, r2 = 0.57, p < 0.0001); controls in red (Y = 0.9028X +1.671, r2 = 0.72, p < 0.0001).

A weak correlation was also found for minimum size and dilation velocity in controls but not in patients with strategic infarctions. Thus, the coefficient for left eyes in group 1 was r = 0.25 (p = 0.23) and in controls r = 0.47 (p = 0.018, adjusted p = 0.054), and in group 1 for right eyes r = 0.065 (p = 0.75) and in controls r = 0.60 (p = 0.0013, adjusted p = 0.005) (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Pupillometry data from patients with strategic cerebral infarcts and controls: Minimal pupillary diameters and dilatation velocities. Solid lines denote the best fit from linear regression analysis. (Left) Relationship between minimum diameter and dilatation velocity of the left eye, group 1 in green (Y = 0.3976X + 1.977, r2 = 0.063, p = 0.23); controls in red (Y = 0.5857X + 1.83, r2 = 0.22, p = 0.018, adjusted p = 0.054). (Right) Relationship between minimum diameter and dilation velocity of the right eye, group 1 in green (Y = 0.07548X + 2.263, r2 = 0.004, p = 0.75); controls in red (Y = 0.6646X + 1.738, r2 = 0.37, p = 0.0013, adjusted p = 0.005).

Linear regression analysis of constriction velocities, plotted against maximum pupillary diameters, was unaffected by hemispheric stroke lateralization (Figures 4, 5), but the correlation of dilatation velocities with minimal pupillary diameters was lost with strategic infarcts in the right hemisphere [group 1 (r2 = 0.008, p = 0.69); group 2 (r2 = 0,12, p = 0.0821); controls (r2 = 0.29, p ≤ 0.0001)] (Figure 5).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Pupillometry data from patients with left, respectively, right hemisphere cerebral infarcts (plotted against pupillometry data from controls without hemispheric strokes): Maximal pupillary diameters and constriction velocities. Solid lines denote the best fit from linear regression analysis. (Left) Left hemisphere infarcts, group 1 in green (Y = 0.9538X + 1.322, r2 = 0.67, p ≤ 0.0001); group 2 in blue (Y = 0.9257 X + 1.428, r2 = 0.7, p < 0.0001); controls in red (Y = 0.8583X + 1.721, r2 = 0.7, p ≤ 0.0001). (Right) Right hemisphere infarcts, group 1 in green (Y = 0.3189X + 2.861, r2 = 0.3, p = 0.0077); group 2 in blue (Y = 0.6064X + 2.086, r2 = 0.4, p = 0.0005), controls in red (identical values as above).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Pupillometry data from patients with left, respectively, right hemisphere cerebral infarcts (plotted against pupillometry data from controls without hemispheric strokes): Minimal pupillary diameters and dilatation velocities. Solid lines denote the best fit from linear regression analysis, showing the relationship between minimum diameter and dilation velocity. (Left) Left hemisphere infarcts, group 1 in green (Y = 0.5528X + 1.705, r2 = 0.15, p = 0.04); group 2 in blue (Y = 0.8756X + 1.538, r2 = 0.41, p = 0.0007); controls in red (Y = 0.6312 X 1.78, r2 = 0.3, p ≤ 0.0001). (Right) Right hemisphere infarcts, group 1 in green (Y = −0.1018 X+2.549, r2 = 0.008, p = 0.69); group 2 in blue (Y = 0.5222X + 2.023, r2 = 0.12, p = 0.0821), controls in red (identical values as above).

Discussion

The human pupillary light reflex, as assessed by the speed of pupillary constriction and diameters before and after constriction, does not seem to be affected by strategic infarcts of the prefrontal eye field or insular cortex. This finding does not support the hypothesis of strategic strokes altering pupillary function, probably because the present model is a model of cortical lesioning as opposed to cortical activation. Hence, cortical activation may lead to pupillary dilation (5, 9) [or, more rarely, constriction (2326)], but the absence of cortical input to the pupils following cortical damage does not appear to affect the light reflex. To our knowledge, this is the first systematic human study investigating cortical modulation of pupillary function in a true-to-life clinical setting.

Subtle changes in pupillary function, however, may still be possible immediately after the light reflex, i.e., when the light stimulus is over, and the pupils dilate back to baseline. Thus, while we observed a robust correlation between pupillary size and constriction velocity [confirming previous studies (27, 28)], minimum size and dilation velocity were still correlated in controls but no longer in patients with strategic infarctions. In addition, in patients with strategic infarcts the correlation of dilatation velocities with pupillary diameters was weak with left hemispheric strokes and lost with right-sided strokes. In contrast, this correlation was still robust in controls and in stroke patients without strategic infarcts, irrespective of hemispheric lateralization (Figure 4). Several explanations are possible. First, strategic infarctions in the prefrontal eye field and/or insular cortex may indeed influence pupillary function in subtle ways, perhaps by impaired sympathetic control or reduced parasympathetic inhibition, resulting in decreased pupillary dilation velocity (27). This is consistent with the insular cortex being an important center of autonomic control. Indeed, strokes affecting the insular cortex are associated with significant autonomic dysfunction (11, 12, 29, 30), in particular ischemic stroke events involving the right-sided insular cortex (31). Second, the observed difference could be related to infarct volume. Patients with strategic infarcts had larger stroke volumes and more severe neurological deficits compared to patients without strategic infarcts, and, although strokes producing mass effects were excluded, this may have influenced pupillary function either by accumulative neuronal loss or damage to still unidentified neuronal groups that may be important for pupillary function. Third, the results may be flawed due to the relatively small sample size. However, if these findings can be replicated with larger numbers, loss of cortical innervation would, indeed, seem to produce subtle pupillary changes as we excluded strokes with mass effect and posterior circulations strokes, i.e., the occipital visual areas and the subcortical neuronal innervation of the pupils were intact.

Limitations to our study, besides the modest sample size, include the use of CT instead of Magnetic Resonance Imaging to estimate stroke location and the use of the ASPECT score which is a crude measure of stroke volume (18). Also, we did not adjust for stroke volumes but, as stated, excluded significant mass effects, ensuring integrity of brainstem pathways. Lastly, the NPi is an index based on a proprietary algorithm, and although it is commonly used in the clinical setting e.g., (20, 21, 32, 33), it cannot be publicly verified, which limits its intrinsic value to the scientific community (We contacted the manufacturer but were unable to receive information about how the NPi is computed).

On the positive side, all pupillometry data besides the NPi are based on objective and well-known indices (Table 1), and this is one of the few systematic studies investigating cortical modulation of pupillary function in humans. Moreover, we worked within a true-to-life clinical research setting, using noninvasive and easily available tools; we included controls adequately matched in terms of sex, vascular co-morbidity and age (34, 35). Further, we excluded confounding factors such as eye diseases, posterior circulation strokes and intracranial structural pathologies other than ischemic stroke.

Of note, we also introduced a new cortical lesion model in humans (which are very rare for obvious reasons), using strategic infarctions in patients after endovascular thrombectomy for acute ischemic stroke. Given the rapidly increasing use of endovascular stroke therapy (36), this seems to be a feasible way to recruit patients within a comparatively short timeframe and to systematically study the neuronal effects of select cortical lesions in a real-world setting.

Conclusions

Overall pupillary function is unaffected by prefrontal eye field or insular cortex strokes in humans. Subtle changes, perhaps related to autonomic dysfunction, may still occur immediately after the light reflex when the pupils dilate back to baseline. Replication using a larger sample size is needed to further explore the possible influence of hemispheric lateralization. We suggest that endovascular therapy for acute ischemic stroke due to occlusive large vessel disease may serve as a pragmatic and realistic clinical research model for the study of acquired cortical lesions in humans.

Author Contributions

CP: acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation, writing of the manuscript, critical revision for important intellectual content, approval of final manuscript; PM, JG: acquisition of data, critical revision for important intellectual content, approval of final manuscript; TT, GK: analysis and interpretation, critical revision for important intellectual content, approval of final manuscript; JK: study concept, analysis and interpretation, critical revision for important intellectual content, approval of final manuscript; DK: study concept, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation, writing of the manuscript, critical revision for important intellectual content, approval of final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2018.00916/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Kawasaki A. Physiology, assessment, and disorders of the pupil. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. (1999) 10:394–400. doi: 10.1097/00055735-199912000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Damsma A, van Rijn H. Pupillary response indexes the metrical hierarchy of unattended rhythmic violations. Brain Cogn. (2017) 111:95–103. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kloosterman NA, Meindertsma T, van Loon AM, Lamme VAF, Bonneh YS, Donner TH. Pupil size tracks perceptual content and surprise. Eur J Neurosci. (2015) 41:1068–78. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Stoll J, Chatelle C, Carter O, Koch C, Laureys S, Einhäuser W. Pupil responses allow communication in locked-in syndrome patients. Curr Biol. (2013) 23:R647–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Steinhauer SR, Condray R, Kasparek A. Cognitive modulation of midbrain function: task-induced reduction of the pupillary light reflex. Int J Psychophysiol. (2000) 39:21–30. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00119-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. de Gee JW, Knapen T, Donner TH. Decision-related pupil dilation reflects upcoming choice and individual bias. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2014) 111:E618–25. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317557111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Einhäuser W, Koch C, Carter OL. Pupil dilation betrays the timing of decisions. Front Hum Neurosci. (2010) 4:18. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Simpson HM, Hale SM. Pupillary changes during a decision-making task. Percept Mot Skills. (1969) 29:495–8. doi: 10.2466/pms.1969.29.2.495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Becket Ebitz R, Moore T. Selective modulation of the pupil light reflex by microstimulation of prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. (2017) 37:5008–18. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2433-16.2017

CrossRef Full Text

10. Lehmann SJ, Corneil BD. Transient pupil dilation after subsaccadic microstimulation of primate frontal eye fields. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:3765–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4264-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Cechetto DF. Cortical control of the autonomic nervous system. Exp Physiol. (2014) 99:326–31. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2013.075192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gasquoine PG. Contributions of the insula to cognition and emotion. Neuropsychol Rev. (2014) 24:77–87. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9246-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Joshi S, Li Y, Kalwani RM, Gold JI. Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron (2016) 89:221–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Geva R, Zivan M, Warsha A, Olchik D. Alerting, orienting or executive attention networks: differential patters of pupil dilations. Front Behav Neurosci. (2013) 7:145. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Samuels E, Szabadi E. Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part i: principles of functional organisation. Curr Neuropharmacol. (2008) 6:235–53. doi: 10.2174/157015908785777229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Hall CA, Chilcott RP. Eyeing up the future of the pupillary light reflex in neurodiagnostics. Diagnostics (2018) 8:E19. doi: 10.3390/diagnostics8010019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mcmahon T, Zijl PCM, Van Gilad AA. Autonomic control of the eye. Cell Tissue Res. (2015) 80:132–40. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c140014.Autonomic

CrossRef Full Text

18. Barber PA, Demchuk AM, Zhang J, Buchan AM. Validity and reliability of a quantitative computed tomography score in predicting outcome of hyperacute stroke before thrombolytic therapy. Lancet (2000) 355:1670–4. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02237-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kimura S, Shoumura K, Ichinohe N, Yun S. Neural mechanisms of pupillary abnormality following thalamic lesions: experimental lesion and stimulation studies in cats, and consideration of pupillary findings in thalamic vascular lesions. J Hirnforsch. (1992) 33:565–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

20. Larson MD, Behrends M. Portable infrared pupillometry: a review. Anesth Analg. (2015) 120:1242–53. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000000314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Chen J, Gombart Z, Rogers S, Gardiner S, Cecil S, Bullock R. Pupillary reactivity as an early indicator of increased intracranial pressure: the introduction of the neurological pupil index. Surg Neurol Int. (2011) 2:82. doi: 10.4103/2152-7806.82248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Kondziella D, Cortsen M, Eskesen V, Hansen K, Holtmannspötter M, Højgaard J, et al. Update on acute endovascular and surgical stroke treatment. Acta Neurol Scand. (2013) 127:1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2012.01702.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Barbur JL, Harlow AJ, Sahraie A. Pupillary responses to stimulus structure, colour and movement. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. (1992) 12:137–41.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. Sahraie A, Weiskrantz L, Trevethan C, Cruce R, Murray A. Psychophysical and pupillometric study of spatial channels of visual processing in blindsight. Exp Brain Res. (2002) 143:249–56. doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0989-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Weiskrantz L, Cowey A, Le Mare C. Learning from the pupil: a spatial visual channel in the absence of V1 in monkey and human. Brain (1998) 121(Pt 6):1065–72.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

26. Link B, Jünemann A, Rix R, Sembritzki O, Brenning A, Korth M, et al. Pupillographic measurements with pattern stimulation: the pupil's response in normal subjects and first measurements in glaucoma patients. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. (2006) 47:4947–55. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-0021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ellis CJ. The pupillary light reflex in normal subjects. Br J Ophthalmol. (1981) 65:754–9. doi: 10.1136/bjo.65.11.754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bremner FD. Pupillometric evaluation of the dynamics of the pupillary response to a brief light stimulus in healthy subjects. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. (2012) 53:7343–7. doi: 10.1167/iovs.12-10881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Barron SA, Rogovski Z, Hemli J. Autonomic consequences of cerebral hemisphere infarction. Stroke (1994) 25:113–6.

PubMed Abstract

30. Xiong L, Leung HHW, Chen XY, Han JH, Leung TW, Soo YO, et al. Comprehensive assessment for autonomic dysfunction in different phases after ischemic stroke. Int J Stroke (2013) 8:645–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2012.00829.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Meyer S, Strittmatter M, Fischer C, Georg T, Schmitz B. Lateralization in autonomic dysfunction in ischemic stroke involving the insular cortex. Neuroreport (2004) 15:357–61. doi: 10.100001756-200402090-00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Olson DM, Fishel M. The Use of Automated Pupillometry in Critical Care. Crit Care Nurs Clin North Am. (2016) 28:101–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cnc.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Emelifeonwu JA, Reid K, Rhodes JK, Myles L. Saved by the pupillometer! - a role for pupillometry in the acute assessment of patients with traumatic brain injuries? Brain Inj. (2018) 32:675–7. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2018.1429021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Tekin K, Sekeroglu MA, Kiziltoprak H, Doguizi S, Inanc M, Yilmazbas P. Static and dynamic pupillometry data of healthy individuals. Clin Exp Optom. (2018) 101:659–65. doi: 10.1111/cxo.12659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bitsios P, Prettyman R, Szabadi E. Changes in autonomic function with age: a study of pupillary kinetics in healthy young and old people. Age Ageing (1996) 25:432–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

36. Fisher M, Saver JL. Future directions of acute ischaemic stroke therapy. Lancet Neurol. (2015) 14:758–67. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00054-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: endovascular stroke therapy, insula, prefrontal eye field, pupils, pupillometry, pupillary light reflex, stroke, mechanical thrombectomy

Citation: Peinkhofer C, Martens P, Grand J, Truelsen T, Knudsen GM, Kjaergaard J and Kondziella D (2018) Influence of Strategic Cortical Infarctions on Pupillary Function. Front. Neurol. 9:916. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00916

Received: 13 August 2018; Accepted: 09 October 2018;
Published: 29 October 2018.

Edited by:

Andrew J. Zele, Queensland University of Technology, Australia

Reviewed by:

Helmut Wilhelm, University Eye Hospital, Germany
Alexander Erich Hartmann, Kliniken der Stadt Köln, Germany

Copyright © 2018 Peinkhofer, Martens, Grand, Truelsen, Knudsen, Kjaergaard and Kondziella. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniel Kondziella, ZGFuaWVsX2tvbmR6aWVsbGFAeWFob28uY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.