Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 27 August 2018
Sec. Multiple Sclerosis and Neuroimmunology

A Pilot Study Examining Speed of Processing Training (SPT) to Improve Processing Speed in Persons With Multiple Sclerosis

  • 1Neuropsychology and Neuroscience Laboratory, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ, United States
  • 2Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Medical School, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Newark, NJ, United States
  • 3Department of Occupational Therapy, New York University, New York, NY, United States
  • 4Department of Neurology, Medical School, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Newark, NJ, United States

Background: Individuals with Multiple Sclerosis (MS) have significant impairments in processing speed (PS) and such impairments may underlie other cognitive deficits common in MS and limit performance of everyday life activities.

Objective: To examine the efficacy of a computerized PS intervention, Speed of Processing Training (SPT), in persons with MS on PS, memory and everyday activities.

Methods: Twenty-one individuals with clinically definite MS and an objectively assessed impairment in PS were included in a controlled randomized clinical trial, randomly assigned to a treatment group or a control group. Participants were assessed prior to and within 1 week of completing the treatment. Outcome measures included traditional neuropsychological tests measuring PS and memory, and an assessment of PS in daily life activities.

Results: The treatment group showed a significant improvement on neuropsychological tests of PS and new learning and memory. A significant improvement was additionally noted in the treatment group on measures of PS in everyday life. These changes were not observed in the control group.

Conclusions: Results provide preliminary data in support of SPT in treating PS deficits in persons with MS. Additional research is needed with larger samples and more comprehensive outcome measures.

Introduction

Multiple Sclerosis (MS) is the most common nontraumatic neurological illness in young and middle-aged adults (1). Sensorial, motor, cognitive, and psychiatric problems are common, with high variability in symptom presentation (1). Processing speed (PS) is widely recognized as the single greatest cognitive deficit in MS (2, 3).

PS affects all cognitive domains and tends to be correlated with verbal abilities (4), working memory (WM) (5) and new learning and memory (NLM) (6, 7). Thus, a deficit in PS may be the source of deficits in other areas of cognition. As an example, PS has been shown to correlate with new learning abilities in persons with MS (8). The term new learning refers to the process by which new, to-be-learned information is initial acquired (9); this is in contrast to the term memory, referring to the retrieval of previously learned information at a later point in time (6).

Cognitive deficits in MS also negatively impact employment (10, 11), social activities (12), driving (13, 14), and the ability to complete everyday tasks (15, 16). Recent findings also indicate PS to play an important role in benefit from cognitive rehabilitation (CR) targeting other areas of cognition. For instance, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca (17) showed that persons with intact PS benefitted from memory treatment, while those with impaired PS did not (17). Thus an effective means of treating PS deficits in MS must be identified.

Existing CR programs in MS have been focused on attention, WM, communication skills and NLM [see (18, 19)]. However, only one study to our knowledge specifically addresses PS deficits in MS. Hancock et al. (20) provided preliminary evidence for an at-home CR program for PS deficits in persons with MS who self-reported cognitive impairment. The active training group improved on a PS/WM measure following treatment relative to controls. However, the study's high drop-out rate and inclusion of persons who self-reported PS cognitive deficits may present external and internal validity issues.

CR protocols designed to train multiple cognitive skills, including PS, have also been developed and tested (2124), with improved cognitive performance noted following treatment. For example, improvement has been shown in PS and other targeted cognitive domains following RehaCom, which persist over time [e.g., (25)]. However, since such rehabilitations programs were designed to train multiple cognitive skills, the relative focus on PS and the impact of treatment on PS specifically is unknown.

Speed of Processing Training (SPT) is a computerized treatment designed to improve PS, with substantial supporting evidence in normal aging on laboratory measures of PS and everyday functioning [e.g., (26)]. Ball et al. (26) examined data from six studies applying SPT to aging, demonstrating consistent improvements on lab-based tests of PS and everyday functioning, maintained at 2-year follow-up (27). Longitudinal analyses showed treatment effects to remain 5 and 10-years later (27). Given its efficacy in aging, we conducted an initial pilot study examining the efficacy of SPT in MS, hypothesizing that MS participants treated with SPT would show improved performance on neuropsychological tests of PS and NLM, as well as an objective test of daily functioning compared to a control group.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-one participants [Treatment (TX) n = 12, Control (CTL) n = 9] with clinically definite MS (28) were recruited from MS Clinics, advertisements and through the Kessler Foundation database of research participants. There were no significant differences between groups on demographic (age, education, gender, pre-morbid IQ) or disease related variables (disease duration, subtype; Table 1). Groups were also similar on pre-treatment PS and verbal abilities.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and disease characteristics by group (treatment vs. control).

Inclusion Criteria

Participants were 18–65 years of age and free of exacerbations and steroid use for at least 1 month. All participants demonstrated baseline impaired PS, defined as performance 1.5 standard deviations or more below the mean of published normative data on the oral Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (29). Participants were excluded if they had a major psychiatric disorder, substance abuse, evidence of significant vision impairment from diplopia, nystagmus or scotomas upon testing (corrected vision in worse eye >20/60 assessed with Snellen Eye Test) or impaired language comprehension on the Token Test.

Design

This RCT used a 5-week, parallel groups design (Figure 1). Potential participants underwent a 2-part screening consisting of initial telephone screen for age, injury type and date, neurological history, and current medications; and in-person screening for psychiatric and substance abuse history, visual acuity, language comprehension and PS abilities.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental overview.

The groups were assigned via 1:1 randomization using a computerized random number generator. Treatment allocation was concealed. The individual responsible for group assignment was not otherwise involved in data collection, and group assignment was verified by a second individual via duplicate copy of the randomization table generated prior to the initiation of data collection. Only the person administering treatment knew group assignment. All other study personnel were blinded, assured through several mechanisms (Therapists and evaluators were always different and were not able to communicate directly about participants). Participants were informed that they had a 50% chance of being assigned to treatment.

Once qualifying, participants underwent baseline evaluation, including neuropsychological (NP) assessment and the Timed Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Test [TIADL (30)], performed in one session. Tests were administered in a standard order for all participants. Within 1 week of completing treatment, participants completed the same evaluation with alternate forms. The same evaluator conducted baseline and immediate follow-up evaluations wherever possible.

Treatment Protocol

The Treatment Condition consisted of 10 computerized training sessions over a 5-week period similar to the work of Ball et al. (31). Initial sessions involve practice on three types of tasks presented on a computer (simple speed of processing, divided attention, and selective attention), with three different central demands (detection, identification, same/different). Training is customized to each participant's ability; an individual's entry point into SPT is determined by current level of PS, evaluated as the speed of stimulus presentation at which the person can correctly identify the stimulus 75% of the time. If this threshold is 30 ms or greater, SPT begins at the most basic level. Training sessions lasted approximately 30–45 min each depending on self-reported fatigue or an observable drop in performance.

In Task I

Level I participants practice a single discrimination task at progressively faster speeds. The task is composed of either target present or absent, target identification, or same/different judgments. Training continues with increasingly more complex discrimination tasks until the participant can perform the target identification task correctly 75% of the time, at exposure duration of 17 ms, at which time the participant progresses to Level II.

Task II

Level II requires the participant to perform one of the discrimination tasks described above, and simultaneously locate a peripheral target. The demand of the center target can be varied, as described above, and the peripheral task demand is changed by decreasing or increasing the distance of the peripheral target from the center target (32). The process of progressing from near peripheral targets to far targets is repeated, at faster speeds, and with increasing difficulty of the center task. This is repeated until the participant can perform both the foveal identification task and the peripheral localization task (at the furthest eccentricity) with 75% accuracy, at a speed of 50 ms or less. Once this level is achieved, the participant progresses to Level III.

Task III

Level III (selective attention training) requires the participant to perform a discrimination task and locate a peripheral target embedded among distracters. It begins with a display duration near threshold, with the peripheral target placed at a near eccentricity. When the participant is able to perform the selected task correctly 75% of the time, a more demanding task is introduced by manipulating the complexity of the discrimination task, the display duration and/or target eccentricity. Practice continues until 75% correct performance is achieved at a 120 ms exposure, with peripheral targets at the most extreme eccentricity.

The Control Condition was a no treatment control condition.

Outcome Measures

Neuropsychological assessments were scheduled according to the participants' availability, the majority of which were scheduled for either 10 a.m. or 1 p.m. Every effort was made to schedule a given participant's assessments (baseline and follow-up) at the same time of day.

The primary outcome was a measure of PS, Digit Symbol Coding Subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) (33). Subjects are presented with a key of nine digit-symbol pairings at the top of a page and given 120 s to write the corresponding symbol for 133 digits. The raw score is the total number of correct pairings. The subtest has high test-retest reliability (r = 0.84) and good construct validity.

Secondary outcomes were (1) additional measures of PS, Letter Comparison (LC) and Pattern Comparison (PC)(34) (2) the California Learning Verbal Test II (CVLT-II) (35), measuring the impact of changes in PS on NLM and (3) the TIADL, an objective test of speeded everyday life tasks.

LC is a measure of perceptual speed requiring the examinee to determine whether two strings of printed letters (3, 6, or 9 characters) are identical or different on two 30-s trials of 21 items. PC reflects the same task with non-verbal stimuli, two printed geometric line patterns. Examinees respond orally in LC and PC, with “s” for same or “d” for different.

The CVLT-II examines verbal NLM. A list of 16 words from four semantic categories is presented orally over five trials for free recall. Two alternate forms minimize carryover between testing sessions. The dependent variables (DV) were the learning slope across the 5 learning trials (LS) and Short Delay Free Recall (SDFR)(35).

The TIADL is a performance-based measure of functional activities uses real everyday items comprising five tasks sampling common instrumental activities of daily living: (1) communication: finding a number in a phone book, (2) finance: counting change using coins, (3) nutrition: locating and reading ingredients from a food can, (4) shopping: locating items on a shelf, and (5) medicine: locating and reading directions from medicine bottles. Scoring was determined by both accuracy and time to completion. To minimize the effects of motor deficits, stimulus materials are placed in the participant's hand immediately before the task begins (when applicable), and tasks require very simple motor ability. Each task has a maximum time limit of 2 min, with the exception of the communication task, which has a limit of 3 min. If a participant does not complete a task within the time limit, the task is terminated and maximum time is assigned. The TIADL has excellent test-retest reliability (r = 0.85) and criterion validity (19). A positive relationship has also been noted between TIADL performance and PS (30, 36).

Statistical Analyses

Due to the pilot nature of the current study an intend-to-treat design was not utilized. Analysis examining treatment effects utilized an ANCOVA with baseline performance as the covariate in each analysis. All analyses were performed with SPSS version 18 software. No interim data analyses were performed.

Ethics and Registration

All procedures were approved by an institutional review board and all participants provided written informed consent. The RCT is registered with clinicaltrials.gov (NCT01838824).

Results

Recruitment ran from 1/31/2012-2/2/13. The trial ended with completion of external funding. 21 participants with clinically definite MS were randomly assigned to TX (n = 12) or CTL (n = 9), with no dropout.

Treatment Efficacy

Changes on Neuropsychological Tasks of PS

Significant improvement was noted from pre- to post-treatment on the primary outcome, the WAIS-III Coding Subtest, in the treatment group only. Treated participants completed the WAIS-III Coding tasks faster following treatment than prior to treatment. No change was noted in the control group [F(1, 21) = 2.72, p = 0.05, one tailed; partial Eta2 = 0.133, large effect; Figure 2]. Interestingly, 25% of our treated sample moved from an impaired score on the Coding subtest to a not impaired score following treatment, while 0% of the participants in the control group showed this change.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Changes on the WAIS-III coding subset scaled scores pre to post treatment by group (treatment vs. control; group means plus standard error of the mean).

Albeit non-significant, a medium-large effect size was noted from pre- to post treatment on PC [F(1, 21) = 2.16, p = 0.08, one tailed; partial Eta2 = 0.107, medium-large effect]; such that the treatment group showed more correct responses after treatment while the control group showed no change. No significant difference was noted from pre- to post-treatment on LC (partial Eta2 = 0.025; small effect) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Performance on the neuropsychological tests.

Changes in Learning and Memory Abilities

A significant improvement was noted from pre- to post-treatment on the CVLT-II SDFR in the treatment group only. That is, participants who completed treatment were able to recall more words following treatment than prior to treatment [F(1, 21) = 4.93, p = 0.015, one tailed; partial Eta2 = 0.215, large effect; Figure 3]. No change was noted in the control group.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Changes on the California Learning Verbal Test II short delay Free Recall (CVLT SDFR) from ore to post treatment by group (treatment vs. control group means plus standard error of the mean).

There were no significant differences between the groups on the CVLT learning slope from pre- to post-treatment (partial Eta2 = 0.029; small effect) (Table 2).

Changes in Everyday Life Functioning

A significant improvement was noted from pre- to post-treatment on the TIADL with the treatment group showing improved accuracy and speed on the TIADL following treatment compared to the control group [F(1, 19) = 8.4. p = 0.01, Eta2 = 0.33], noted specifically on the finding a number in a phonebook task [F(1, 19) = 6.3. p = 0.02, Eta2 = 0.27] (Figure 4). In addition, treatment participants were able to locate and read ingredients on a can of food significantly faster and more accurately following treatment than prior to treatment compared to the control group [F(1, 19) = 5.55, p = 0.03; partial Eta2 = 0.246; large effect; Table 3].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Changes on the timed instrumental activities of daily living phonebook task from pre to post treatment (mean seconds to complete task; lower score is better; bars represent standard error of the mean).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Performance on the timed instrumental activities of daily living (scores incorporates speed and accuracy) by group.

There were no significant group differences on any other TIADL metric.

Discussion

Results indicated that relative to the control group, the group receiving SPT showed significant improvements on neuropsychological tests of PS, NLM and daily life. These findings support our hypotheses and emphasize the importance of assessing the impact of a CR protocol not only within the domain targeted for treatment, but also on higher order cognitive functions reliant on basic cognition and on cognitive functioning in daily life. While CR studies continue to accumulate in MS, few studies examine the impact of treatment on daily life (19). This is despite the fact that multiple experts have stressed the importance of evaluating post-CR changes on daily life in addition to the gold standard of neuropsychological testing [e.g., (37)].

A significant treatment effect was noted from before to after treatment on the primary PS outcome, the Coding subtest of the WAIS-III. Albeit non-significant, a large effect size was also observed on a secondary measure of PS ability, PC. There are several possible explanations for these positive findings. Costa et al. (3) proposed the tri-factor model of PS in MS, hypothesizing that PS difficulties in MS are associated with three independent deficits: a sensorial speed deficit (sensorial processing), a cognitive speed deficit (information manipulation), and a motor speed deficit (oral or written motor response, or performance of eye movements for a visual search). Related to this theory, performance on Coding and PC is dependent on how quickly and accurately an individual is able to execute these three information processing stages: (1) process visual information; (2) manipulate the information and plan a response; (3) provide a response (written on Coding and oral on PC) and subsequently move on to the next target stimulus (perform saccadic eye movements). It is well known that these three components of information processing are often impaired in MS (3). The SPT trains these three stages individually. In SPT Level I, individuals are trained to process visual information at faster presentation rates (visual speed training); in SPT Levels II and III individuals are trained to manipulate information within shorter periods of time (cognitive speed training) and perform faster saccadic eye movements toward the periphery (motor speed training). Thus, one can hypothesize that SPT is effective for the rehabilitation of PS deficits in MS because it trains sensorial, cognitive and motor speed, all of which are fundamental to the performance of visual PS tests and often impaired in MS [see (3)]. More research is needed to explore the impact of SPT on visual, cognitive and motor speed individually and fully understand its impact on performance of visual PS tasks.

Importantly, the current results add to the growing body of literature supporting the efficacy of cognitive training in persons with MS (19). Given the frequency with which slowed PS is noted in the MS population, it is vital that we identify effective means for addressing this deficit specifically (3). Shatil et al. (38), compared a group completing training with CogniFit to a no treatment control group, noting the cognitive training to be associated with increased naming speed and speed of information recall. Vogt et al. (39) noted that 45 MS participants completing training with BrainStim saw improved fatigue, working memory abilities and PS post-treatment. Perez-Martin et al. (40) similarly showed an improvement in memory, fluency and PS following treatment with a computer assisted neuropsychological training program in 30 treated participants as compared with 32 participants assigned to a control condition. The RehaCom cognitive training software has similarly demonstrated efficacy in its ability to treat PS specifically in several samples of persons with MS [e.g., (4144)]. Similarly, Covey et al. (45) showed a n-back training program to results in improved functioning on working memory, PS, complex attention and reasoning. Pedulla et al. (46) utilized an at-home training program, the COGNI-TRAcK in 28 persons with MS, noting significant improvement on verbal learning and recall, verbal fluency, sustained attention, concentration and PS. Substantial gains are thus noted following cognitive rehabilitation for PS specifically.

A second major finding of the current study is the significant improvement of treated participants on a NLM test, the CVLT-II, with a significant treatment effect noted on SDFR. It is important to note that the same pattern of results was noted on the CVLT-II slope, albeit non-significant, with the treatment group showing an improvement post treatment and the control group showing a decline. It is notable that a CR protocol directly addressing PS resulted in a change in NLM. This highlights the role of PS as a more basic level of processing that clearly impacts NLM (3). In fact, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca (17) demonstrated a memory rehabilitation treatment to be effective in persons with MS with intact PS abilities, but not persons with MS with impaired PS abilities, hypothesizing two distinct groups of MS memory impaired participants–one with a hippocampally-mediated memory deficit and the other with a memory deficit resulting from impaired PS. The current pattern of results lends support to this hypothesis, highlighting the intricate connection between PS and learning and memory in MS.

Finally, results also indicated a significant impact of treatment on performance of the TIADL total score and subtests of the phonebook task and locating and reading ingredients on a can of food. Locating and reading instructions on a medicine bottle did not reach significance, but showed a medium effect size. These three tasks are similar because all three require participants to quickly scan the visual field in order to locate the required information. The noted improvement may thus have been impacted by improved visual scanning post-treatment in addition to improved cognitive PS, consistent with the tri-factor model of PS in MS (3).

Despite the positive impact of SPT on PS, NLM and speed-based tasks of daily life in this study, there are some limitations. First, generalization is limited by the small sample. Future work would benefit from the inclusion of larger samples to further document the impact of treatment on daily life. Second, this study is limited by the outcome measures utilized. To truly understand the impact of SPT on PS in MS, one must include more outcome measures focused on PS without a motor component. Measures of multiple higher order cognitive functions (e.g., memory and executive functioning) would be a goal for future research. Third, future work should include an active control group to control for non-specific factors related to participation in treatment. Future work should additionally evaluate the impact of SPT on daily life from the perspective of both the patient and the significant other. While self-report measures have been criticized for being biased by depression, awareness and other symptomatology, they provide important information about the perception of the patient and family. Finally, the maintenance of the treatment effect over time is important to evaluate for any cognitive rehabilitation program and should be considered in future work.

The current findings support the application of SPT to treat PS deficits in MS. A particular strength of the current study is the use of a well-studied technique previously shown to improve PS in healthy aging with participants diagnosed with MS who have objectively verified PS deficits. This study is a first step in designating a CR program directed to treat PS in MS.

Author Contributions

NC acquired funding, directed study execution, conducted data analysis, led interpretation and drafted manuscript. YG assisted with data analysis, interpretation and manuscript preparation specifically in regard to everyday life data (TIADL). SC assisted with data collection, analysis, interpretation, and manuscript preparation. JD assisted with obtaining funding, analysing data, interpreting data, and manuscript preparation.

Funding

The authors would like to acknowledge grant support from the National Multiple Sclerosis Society (pilot grant RG 4607-A) to NC and funding by the Kessler Foundation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

CLT, Control; CR, Cognitive rehabilitation; CVLT II, California Learning Verbal Test-2nd Edition; DV, Dependent variable; LC, Letter comparison; LS, Learning trials; Ms, Milliseconds; MS, Multiple Sclerosis; NLM, New learning and memory; NP, Neuropsychological; PC, Pattern comparison; PS, Processing speed; SDFR, Short Delay Free Recall; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; SPT, Speed of Processing Training; TIADL, Timed Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Test; TX, Treatment; WAIS–III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III.

References

1. Chelune GJ, Stott H, Pinkston J. Multiple Sclerosis. In: JE Morgan and JH Ricker, editors. Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis (2008). p. 599-15

Google Scholar

2. Benedict RH, DeLuca J, Phillips G, LaRocca N, Hudson LD, Rudick R, et al. Validity of the symbol digit modalities test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2017) 23:721–33. doi: 10.1177/1352458517690821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Costa SL M, Genova H, DeLuca J, Chiaravalloti ND. Information processing speed in multiple sclerosis: past, present, and future. Mult Scler. (2017) 23:772–89. doi: 10.1177/1352458516645869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sherman EMS, Strauss E, Spellacy F. Validity of the paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT) in adults referred for neuropsychological assessment after head injury. Clin Neuropsychol. (1997) 11:34–45. doi: 10.1080/13854049708407027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Chiaravalloti ND, Christodoulou C, Demaree HA, DeLuca J. Differentiating simple versus complex processing speed: influence on new learning and memory performance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2003) 25:489–501. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.4.489.13878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. DeLuca J, Barbieri-Berger S, Johnson SK. The nature of memory impairments in multiple sclerosis: acquisition versus retrieval. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1994) 16:183–9. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. DeLuca J, Gaudino EA, Diamond BJ, Christodoulou C, Engel RA. Acquisition and storage deficits in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1998) 20:376–90. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.3.376.819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Demaree HA, Gaudino EA, DeLuca J, Ricker JH. Learning impairment is associated with recall ability in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2000) 22:856–73.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Hannay HJ, Fischer JS. Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2004).

Google Scholar

10. Kornblith AB, La Rocca NG, Baum HM. Employment in individuals with multiple sclerosis. Int J Rehabil Res. (1986) 9:155–66. doi: 10.1097/00004356-198606000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Larocca N, Kalb R, Scheinberg L, Kendall P. Factors associated with unemployment of patients with multiple sclerosis. J Chronic Dis. (1985) 38:203–10. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(85)90093-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rao SM, Leo GJ L, Unverzagt F. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. I. Frequency, Patterns, and Prediction. Neurology (1991) 41:685–91.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

13. Schultheis MT, Garay E, DeLuca J. The influence of cognitive impairment on driving performance in multiple sclerosis. Neurology (2001) 56:1089–94. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.8.1089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Schultheis MT, Garay E, Millis SR, Deluca J. Motor vehicle crashes and violations among drivers with multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2002) 83:1175–8. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.34279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kessler HR, Cohen RA, Lauer K, Kausch DF. The relationship between disability and memory dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Int J Neurosci. (1992) 62:17–34.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

16. Cicerone KD, Langenbahn DM, Braden C, Malec JF, Kalmar K, Fraas M, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2011) 92:519–30. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. The influence of cognitive dysfunction on benefit from learning and memory rehabilitation in MS: a sub-analysis of the MEMREHAB Trial. Mult Scler. (2015) 21:1575–82. doi: 10.1177/1352458514567726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. O'Brien AR, Chiaravalloti N, Goverover Y, Deluca J. Evidenced-based cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: a review of the literature. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2008) 89:761–9. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Goverover Y, Chiaravalloti ND, O'Brien AR, DeLuca J. Evidenced-based cognitive rehabilitation for persons with multiple sclerosis: an updated review of the literature from 2007 to 2016 Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2018) 99:390–407. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hancock LM, Bruce JM, Bruce AS, Lynch SG. Processing speed and working memory training in multiple sclerosis: a double-blind randomized controlled pilot study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2015) 37:113–27. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.989818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bonavita S, Sacco R, Della Corte M, Esposito S, Sparaco M, d'Ambrosio A, et al. Computer-aided cognitive rehabilitation improves cognitive performances induces brain functional connectivity changes in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients: an exploratory study. J Neurol. (2015) 262:91–100. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7528-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gich J, Freixanet J, Garcia R, Vilanova JC, Genis D, Silva Y, et al. A randomized, controlled, single-blind, 6-month pilot study to evaluate the efficacy of MS-Line!: a cognitive rehabilitation programme for patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2015) 21:1332–43. doi: 10.1177/1352458515572405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Rilo O, Pena J, Ojeda N, Antiguedad AR, Bilbao MM, Gastiasoro AG, et al. Integrative group-based cognitive rehabilitation efficacy in multiple sclerosis: a randomized clinical trial. Disabil Rehabil. (2016) 1–9. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2016.1250168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mattioli F, Stampatori C, Bellomi F, Danni M, Compagnucci L, Uccelli A, et al. A RCT comparing specific intensive cognitive training to aspecific psychological intervention in RRMS: the smict study. Front Neurol. (2014) 5:278. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Mattioli F, Bellomi F, Stampatori C, Provinciali L, Compagnucci L, Uccelli A, et al. Two years follow up of domain specific cognitive training in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis: a randomized clinical trial. Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 10:28. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ball K, Edwards JD, Ross LA. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2007) 62:19–31. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.special_issue_1.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Rebok GW, Ball K, Guey LT, Jones RN, Kim HY, King JW, et al. Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2014) 62:16–24. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Polman C, Reingold SC, Banwell B, Clanet M, Cohen JA, Filippi M, et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald Criteria. Ann Neurol. (2011) 69:292–302. doi: 10.1002/ana.22366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Smith A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Edited by The Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services (1982).

Google Scholar

30. Owsley C, Sloane M, Jr McGwin G, Ball K. Timed instrumental activities of daily living tasks: relationship to cognitive function and everyday performance assessments in older adults. Gerontology (2002) 48:254–65. doi: 10.1159/000058360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA (2002) 288:2271–81. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK, Wadley VG, Edwards JD. Speed-of-processing and driving simulator training result in improved driving performance. Hum Factors (2003) 45:218–33. doi: 10.1518/hfes.45.2.218.27241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale. 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (1997).

Google Scholar

34. Fristoe NM, Salthouse TA, Woodard JL. Examination of age-related deficits on the wisconsin card sorting test. Neuropsychology (1997) 11:428–36. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.3.428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test. 2nd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (2000).

Google Scholar

36. Goverover Y, Genova HM, Hillary FG, DeLuca J. The Relationship between neuropsychological measures and the timed instrumental activities of daily living task in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2007) 13:636–44. doi: 10.1177/1352458506072984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Piras F, Borella E, Incoccia C, Carlesimo GA. Evidence-based practice recommendations for memory rehabilitation. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. (2011) 47:149–75.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

38. Shatil EA, Metzer O, Horvitz Miller A. Home-based personalized cognitive training in ms patients: a study of adherence and cognitive performance. NeuroRehabilitation (2010) 26:143–53. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2010-0546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Vogt A, Kappos L, Calabrese P, Stöcklin M, Gschwind L, Opwis K, et al. Working memory training in patients with multiple sclerosis - comparison of two different training schedules. Restor Neurol Neurosci. (2009) 27:225–35. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2009-0473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Perez-Martin MY, Platas MG, Del Rio PE, Elias CC, Sosa AJ. Efficacy of a short cognitive training program in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2017) 13:245–52. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S124448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Messinis L, Nasios G, Kosmidis MH, Zampakis P, Malefaki S, Ntoskou K, et al. Efficacy of a computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation intervention in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients: a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Behav Neurol. (2017) 2017:5919841. doi: 10.1155/2017/5919841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Mattioli F, Stampatori C, Zanotti D, Parrinello G, Capra R. Efficacy and specificity of intensive cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive functions in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci. (2010) 288:101–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Filippi M, Riccitelli G, Mattioli F, Capra R, Stampatori C, Pagani E, Valsasina P, et al. Multiple sclerosis: effects of cognitive rehabilitation on structural and functional MR imaging measures–an explorative study. Radiology (2012) 262:932–40. doi: 10.1148/radiol.11111299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Amato MP, Goretti B, Viterbo RG, Portaccio E, Niccolai C, Hakiki B, et al. Computer-assisted rehabilitation of attention in patients with multiple sclerosis: results of a randomized, double-blind trial. Mult Scler. (2014) 20:91–8. doi: 10.1177/1352458513501571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Covey TJ, Shucard JL, RH, enedict, Guttman BW, Shucard DW. Improved cognitive performance and event-related potential changes following working memory training in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler J Exp Transl Clin. (2018) 4:2055217317747626. doi: 10.1177/2055217317747626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Pedulla L, Brichetto G, Tacchino A, Vassallo C, Zaratin P, Battaglia MA, et al. Adaptive vs. non-adaptive cognitive training by means of a personalized app: a randomized trial in people with multiple sclerosis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2016) 13:88. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0193-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive rehabilitation, cognitive functions, daily life activities, multiple sclerosis, disease course

Citation: Chiaravalloti ND, Goverover Y, Costa SL and DeLuca J (2018) A Pilot Study Examining Speed of Processing Training (SPT) to Improve Processing Speed in Persons With Multiple Sclerosis. Front. Neurol. 9:685. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00685

Received: 27 April 2018; Accepted: 30 July 2018;
Published: 27 August 2018.

Edited by:

Letizia Leocani, San Raffaele Hospital (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Moussa Antoine Chalah, Hôpitaux Universitaires Henri Mondor, France
Ralf Lürding, University of Regensburg, Germany

Copyright © 2018 Chiaravalloti, Goverover, Costa and DeLuca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nancy D. Chiaravalloti, bmNoaWFyYXZhbGxvdGlAa2Vzc2xlcmZvdW5kYXRpb24ub3Jn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.