Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Neurol., 24 May 2018
Sec. Neuroprosthetics
This article is part of the Research Topic Sonification, perceptualizing biological information View all 15 articles

The Use of Footstep Sounds as Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation for Gait Rehabilitation in Parkinson’s Disease: A Randomized Controlled Trial

  • 1Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
  • 2Department of Pedagogy, Psychology, Philosophy, University of Cagliari, Cagliari, Italy
  • 3AOB “G. Brotzu” General Hospital, Cagliari, Italy
  • 4Department of Mechanical, Chemical and Materials Engineering, University of Cagliari, Cagliari, Italy

Background: The use of rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) has been proven useful in the management of gait disturbances associated with Parkinson’s disease (PD). Typically, the RAS consists of metronome or music-based sounds (artificial RAS), while ecological footstep sounds (ecological RAS) have never been used for rehabilitation programs.

Objective: The aim of this study was to compare the effects of a rehabilitation program integrated either with ecological or with artificial RAS.

Methods: An observer-blind, randomized controlled trial was conducted to investigate the effects of 5 weeks of supervised rehabilitation integrated with RAS. Thirty-eight individuals affected by PD were randomly assigned to one of the two conditions (ecological vs. artificial RAS); thirty-two of them (age 68.2 ± 10.5, Hoehn and Yahr 1.5–3) concluded all phases of the study. Spatio-temporal parameters of gait and clinical variables were assessed before the rehabilitation period, at its end, and after a 3-month follow-up.

Results: Thirty-two participants were analyzed. The results revealed that both groups improved in the majority of biomechanical and clinical measures, independently of the type of sound. Moreover, exploratory analyses for separate groups were conducted, revealing improvements on spatio-temporal parameters only in the ecological RAS group.

Conclusion: Overall, our results suggest that ecological RAS is equally effective compared to artificial RAS. Future studies should further investigate the role of ecological RAS, on the basis of information revealed by our exploratory analyses. Theoretical, methodological, and practical issues concerning the implementation of ecological sounds in the rehabilitation of PD patients are discussed.

Clinical Trial Registration: www.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier NCT03228888.

Introduction

Individuals affected by Parkinson’s disease (PD) typically exhibit motor (e.g., tremor, rigidity, postural instability, gait disturbance) and non-motor symptoms, which progressively affect their quality of life. In order to cope with motor symptoms, patients are generally treated with pharmacological therapies (e.g., l-DOPA, dopamine agonists). However, the symptoms tend to become more severe with the progression of the disease and, at the same time, they become more resistant to medication (1), determining the gradual increase of doses and, consequently, the onset of serious side effects. To optimize the use of medication and cope with patients’ impairments, pharmacological therapies are usually accompanied by physical therapy, which is essential for effectively contrasting the motor symptoms and (at least partially) restoring the motor functions. Given that the loss of motor functions increases the risk of falling and gradually affects patients’ independence, researchers have directed their attention to the methods enhancing the efficacy of physical therapy. One of them is the rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) developed by Thaut and colleagues (2) and widely studied in the past 20 years (36).

The RAS method consists in a gait training, in which patients’ gait is guided by an auditory rhythm. Typically, patients are provided with auditory rhythms (metronome or music), whose beats per minute (BPM) depends on patients’ cadence (steps per minute) at baseline. Usually the BPM is equal to one’s own cadence or slightly increased/decreased (e.g., ±5–10%), depending on the characteristics of the patients and on the methodological choices of the researchers (713). The logic of RAS can be understood by analyzing the source of gait disturbance in PD. The damages of basal ganglia, typical of PD, would compromise the functionality of patients’ internal clock, consequently affecting the coordination and the execution of movements (14, 15). Thus, the dysfunctions of the internal clock would be one of the causes of the scarce movement fluidity and of gait impairment. In order to reduce these symptoms, it is necessary to “guide” the internal clock and this can be done by using an external rhythm, that is, RAS. Hence, RAS would facilitate the activity of the internal clock and would help in regulating the fluidity of muscular activation, improving coordination, and facilitating the execution of automatic movements, such as walking. The neural mechanisms underpinning the effectiveness of RAS are not totally clear; it has been proposed that RAS would either rely on residual activity in cortico-striatal circuitry or facilitate compensation by bypassing the damaged areas and relying on alternative pathways (e.g., cerebello-thalamo-cortical circuitry) (16).

The first empirical evidence supporting the efficacy of RAS in the rehabilitation of PD patients was provided by Thaut and colleagues (2). In their study participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: RAS, self-paced training, and no training. In both RAS and self-paced training, participants performed daily walking and other exercises for 30 min, for 3 weeks. The only difference was that the RAS group did the exercises with the auditory stimulation, while the self-paced group had no external triggers. The results revealed that both training groups improved in terms of spatio-temporal parameters; however, the RAS group exhibited significantly better results than the other two groups in step cadence, gait speed, and stride length. Significant results were obtained also as concerns the EMG activity of the leg muscles.

In the subsequent years, the efficacy of RAS has been widely confirmed by many studies [for reviews see Ref. (36)]. For instance, it has been shown that during a RAS session there is a close synchronization between auditory rhythm and cadence in both PD and healthy participants, suggesting that rhythmic entrainment occurs even with damaged basal ganglia (17). Other studies investigated the immediate effects of RAS (1829) and its role in training protocols (8, 9, 1113, 3032), manipulating important variables (e.g., number of weeks and sessions, duration of each session, tempo of the stimuli), and the majority of them consistently reported positive effects of RAS. The improvements of training protocols have been observed in different kinds of variables: (1) clinical measures, such as unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS), freezing of gait questionnaire (FOGQ), Tinetti test, and timed up and go test (TUG) (10, 11); (2) spatio-temporal parameters of gait, in particular cadence and gait speed (9, 17); and (3) amplitude and timing of the muscular contraction, in particular with regards gastrocnemius, tibialis anterior, and vastus lateralis (2, 12). Recently, improvements have been observed also in terms of kinematics. Indeed, it has been shown that the typical gait of PD patients (33) would be modified by a rehabilitation program integrated with RAS, with the hip flexion–extension movement closer to the normality after rehabilitation (13).

Overall, the positive effects of RAS training based on metronome or music (artificial RAS) are quite well-established in the PD literature. Given that the tempo of RAS is usually determined on the basis of patient’s own cadence, in a certain degree RAS represents the perceptualization of biological information associated with gait. However, from the perceptual point of view, the experience of artificial sounds like metronome or music is quite far from the auditory experience of walking. Indeed, artificial RAS only provide rhythmic information, while more ecological stimuli such as footstep sounds would provide both rhythmic information and other gait-related information (e.g., posture, force, gait cycle). Surprisingly, to the best of our knowledge, nobody has explored whether RAS based on ecological footstep sounds can be advantageous, compared to RAS based on artificial sounds (e.g., metronome), within a PD rehabilitation program.

The effects of ecological versus artificial sounds on motor processes have been explored in various domains, such as breathing (34) and motor learning (35), with apparently contradictory results probably because of the different methods employed. In the domain of PD, the role of ecological sounds has been explored in laboratory experiments on walking, revealing interesting results. In one of these experiments, Rodger, Young, and Craig (36) found that the use of synthesized footstep sounds reduced the coefficient of variation of stride length and stride duration, compared to normal walk without sounds. In another study on PD patients (37), the same authors found that the variability of step length and step duration was lower when administering footstep sounds compared to metronome sounds, in a real-time imitation task. This evidence suggests that the complexity of ecological sounds can provide more information than simple beats and this pattern of information can be used as guidance for walking. Recently, it has been argued that the information conveyed by footstep sounds may have important implications for the enhancement of gait in PD (4, 38), and it has been questioned whether a rehabilitation program based on ecological sounds may be more advantageous compared to a metronome-based program.

The rationale of the hypothesized greater effects of the ecological sounds originates from both neurophysiological evidence and perceptual-motor theories. As regards the neurophysiological evidence, it is well-known that the neurons with mirror properties (39) are associated with imitation (40). Indeed, the main areas of the mirror system (i.e., inferior parietal lobule, precentral gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus) activate when humans see or perform an action, but even when they hear the same action (4144). Based on this evidence, it seems reasonable to hypothesize that listening to footstep sounds would activate some of the brain regions involved in the control of walking, possibly triggering imitative behaviors.

As regards the perceptual-motor theories, it has been suggested that the perceptual and the motor systems share a common representational organization, and continuously influence each other (45, 46). Thus, the footstep sounds would evoke a representation of walking in the common representational system, which would reinforce a representation of the same gesture already stored in memory, due to previous perceptual-motor experience. The same representation would not be triggered by the metronome sounds, because they are not intrinsically related to walk and would not be able to adequately resemble a human walking representation. As a consequence, footstep sounds would activate a powerful representation of human walking in the common representational system (45, 46), which with a higher probability would influence the corresponding motor outcomes (4, 34), namely, walking. In sum, while the artificial RAS (i.e., metronome) would provide patients with rhythmic cues only, the ecological RAS (i.e., footsteps) would provide patients with rhythmic cues, which are also meaningful and able to evoke a mental representation of walking.

In this study, we aim to investigate whether the hypothesized superiority of the ecological over artificial RAS has an actual impact in a PD rehabilitation program. In particular, we intend to better understand whether the type of sound (i.e., footsteps or metronome) is relevant in a typical rehabilitation protocol including a gait training with RAS. We hypothesize that patients treated with footstep sounds would improve more than those treated with metronome sounds. Indeed, we expect that both groups would benefit from the rhythmic information of the stimuli, but the former would also take advantage of the priming effect elicited by the ecological information of footstep sounds.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-eight individuals affected by PD participated in this study and 32 of them (Mage = 68.2 years; SD = 10.5) completed it; see Figure 1 for participant flow diagram and Table 1 for baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of participants. Patients were enrolled by RP, CC, and GC between December 2014 and February 2015 at “G. Brotzu” General Hospital (Cagliari, Italy), where they were informed about the present study. The sample size was calculated by means of the G*Power software (parameters: alfa = 0.05; power = 0.80; effect size = 0.25), and the result was 28 participants. All patients included in the study met the following criteria: diagnosis of PD according to the UK Brain Bank criteria (47) ability to walk independently; absence of relevant hearing impairments which could prevent the correct perception of the auditory cues (i.e., ability to have a regular conversation with medical doctors during interview without the use of hearing aids and without the doctor shouting); absence of significant cognitive impairment [i.e., mini-mental status examination (MMSE) >24; frontal assessment battery (FAB) >13]; absence of psychiatric or severe systemic illnesses; mild-to-moderate disability assessed by means of the modified Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) staging scale (1.5 ≤ H&Y ≤ 3); and no engagement in any rehabilitative program in the 3 months before the beginning of the study. When participants were recruited, all of them were treated with l-DOPA and five of them were also taking dopamine agonists. The experimental protocol was approved by local ethics committee (Prot. PG/2014/19654). Written informed consent was obtained by all participants.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Participant flow diagram.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics for each group.

Stimuli

Footsteps Recording

The recordings were carried out in a soundproof room with a parquet floor. Footstep sounds were recorded by means of a fixed-cardioid, large-diaphragm condenser sE2200A microphone. The microphone was fixed on an elastic shock mount in order to isolate it from mechanically transmitted noise; the elastic shock, in turn, was fixed on a stick. The microphone was connected to a M-AUDIO Fast Track Ultra 8R external sound card, which was connected to a laptop running the Logic Pro X software.

A database of footstep sounds was created using the following procedure. Fourteen healthy young adults (7F, 7M) participated in the recording phase. These volunteers were recruited on the basis of their weight. Specifically, females’ weight ranged from 45 to 75 kg with intervals of 5 kg, while males’ weight ranged from 60 to 90 kg, always with 5 kg intervals. The volunteers were required to wear garments free of synthetic fabrics (to avoid potential side noises) and a pair of their own sneakers with a rubber sole. Each volunteer was required to take six steps of 70 cm at the pace of 100 BPM. A set of strips were marked on the floor to provide the correct distance, while the pace was provided by means of earplugs conveying a metronome sound from a portable MP3 player. The recordings were carried out by an experimenter following the walk of the volunteer from a side, without walking, by using the microphone stick.

Stimuli Editing

Two kinds of RAS stimuli (i.e., ecological and artificial) were created. Ecological stimuli consisted of footstep recordings taken from the above-described database; artificial stimuli consisted of metronome sounds. Each patient was provided with one stimulus, either ecological or artificial (depending on the assigned condition); the stimuli were personalized for each patient. In this regard, ecological stimuli were assigned to patients on the basis of their own gender and weight, thus providing patients with sounds similar to those produced by themselves. Moreover, for both ecological and artificial stimuli, the BPM of the soundtracks provided to patients were calculated considering one’s own cadence measured before the beginning of the rehabilitation program (at T0, see “experimental design” paragraph), and the cadence of healthy individuals of the same age (48, 49). In particular, the BPM were calculated following the procedure of Pau and colleagues (13), namely: (a) if a patient’s cadence was below the normality, the BPM of the stimulus was set at a value of 10% higher than one’s own cadence; (b) if a patient’s cadence was below, but close to normality (less than 10% difference), the BPM of the stimulus was set at normality values; (c) if a patient’s cadence was above the normality, the BPM of the stimulus was set at a value equal to one’s own cadence. The interval between one beat/step and the subsequent one was constant, in both conditions. Prototypical examples of artificial and ecological sounds are illustrated in Figure 2 and attached in Supplementary Material.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Prototypical examples of the artificial (A) and ecological sounds (B) used in this study. The upper graphs describe the amplitude of the sounds; the lower graphs describe their frequencies.

Assessment Protocol

Assessment was carried out when patients were in “ON” state 60–90 min after intake of the usual morning l-DOPA dose. The assessment was carried out at the Laboratory of Biomechanics and Industrial Ergonomics of the Department of Mechanical, Chemical and Materials Engineering, University of Cagliari (Italy), lasted about 90–100 min and included both biomechanical and clinical evaluations.

Biomechanical Evaluation

We measured the spatio-temporal parameters of gait. The acquisition of these parameters was performed using a motion capture system composed of 8 infrared cameras (Smart-D system, BTS Bioengineering, Italy) set at a frequency of 120 Hz. Before the tests, a number of anthropometric features (i.e., height, weight, anterior superior iliac spines distance, pelvis thickness, knee and ankle width, leg length) were collected. Then, 22 reflective passive markers (14 mm diameter) were placed on specific landmarks of individual’s lower limbs and trunk according to the protocol described by Davis et al. (50). Participants were asked to walk at a self-selected speed in the most natural manner possible on a 10 m walkway for at least six times, allowing suitable rest were needed in order to avoid fatiguing effects. The raw data were then processed using dedicated software (Smart Analyzer, BTS Bioengineering, Italy) to calculate the following spatio-temporal parameters, which were the primary outcome measures of the present study: gait speed, step length and width, stride length, cadence, stance, swing, and double support phase duration (the latter three parameters expressed as percentage of the gait cycle duration).

Clinical Evaluation

A clinical evaluation of the patients was performed by a team of clinicians’ expert in PD. Clinical measures were the secondary outcome measures of the present study. The patients were evaluated by using the following tests:

• Modified H&Y to assess the severity of symptoms of each patient on one of the seven levels of the scale (51, 52).

• Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (UPDRS) part 3 to specifically evaluate the motor symptoms (53).

• Functional independence measure (FIM) to measure the patients’ disability, revealed by their independence, by the amount of care needed and by their cognitive skills (54).

• Tinetti test to evaluate the static and dynamic balance (55).

• Short physical performance battery (SPPB) to evaluate the functionality of the lower limbs. This test is composed of three sections: balance (assessed in three different trials), walking speed (4 m test), and sit to stand (STS) transfer (56).

• Geriatric depression scale (GDS)—short version (15 items), a self-report scale to assess depression in elderly (57).

• Parkinson’s disease quality of life questionnaire (PDQ-8), a specific scale to evaluate the quality of life of patients affected by PD (58).

• Falls efficacy scale (FES) to evaluate the patients’ fear of falling (59).

• Activities specific balance confidence (ABC), a self-efficacy scale regarding activities which require specific balance confidence (60).

• Freezing of gait questionnaire (FOGQ) to evaluate freezing episodes reported by patients during walking (61).

Rehabilitation Protocol

Participants were engaged in a supervised rehabilitative treatment which lasted 5 weeks; during this period patients were engaged in two sessions per week, whose duration was 45 min each. Patients were individually assisted in the training by a physical therapist, under the supervision of a physical medicine specialist; the treatment sessions were held at “G. Brotzu” General Hospital.

The treatment sessions consisted of standard and personalized exercises aimed at enhancing mobility, balance, and posture. Twenty minutes of each session were dedicated to specific gait training with RAS, with participants engaged in walking while listening to their own personalized soundtrack (either ecological or artificial). Moreover, during the 5 weeks of treatment, participants were invited to train at least three times a week at their homes, performing a subset of the same exercises typically performed at the hospital and 30 min of gait training with RAS (they were provided with an MP3 player). Participants were asked to set the volume at a comfortable level and were allowed to modify it anytime.1 The rehabilitation protocol was similar to that used by Pau and colleagues (13); a detailed description of the exercises is reported in Appendix.

After the 5 weeks in which participants were engaged in the supervised treatment, they were invited to daily perform their home-exercises for the subsequent 12 weeks. The activities performed by patients during these 12 weeks were unsupervised, however, participants were asked to keep a log of their home-exercises and such data were discussed with clinicians during regularly scheduled meetings.

Experimental Design

Participants were randomly assigned to the groups (ecological or artificial RAS) in a 1:1 fashion, using blocked randomization (62). Randomization was generated by MM, by means of an online sequence generator (www.random.org) inserting 1 and 38 as smallest and largest numbers and calculating random sequences in 2 columns of 19 numbers each. MM also assigned participants to interventions. Participants were evaluated in three moments: before the rehabilitative treatment (T0), at the end of the 5-week rehabilitative treatment (T5), and 3 months after the end of the treatment, namely 17 weeks after the first assessment (T17). Thus, in this study there were two independent variables: (1) RAS, between subjects, two levels (ecological RAS, artificial RAS); (2) Time, within subjects, three levels (T0, T5, T17). The dependent variables were all the biomechanical and clinical measurements described above. The researcher who assigned participants to the conditions was not involved in the enrollment and evaluation of patients, while the researchers involved in the evaluation of patients (MP, FC, RP, CC, GC) were not aware of the conditions under which the patients were treated (observer-blind trial). Data collection was concluded in April 2016.

Statistical Analyses

As regards the biomechanical variables, a preliminary t-test was run to test for possible differences between the left and right limbs (when separate data were available). As no significant difference was revealed by the analyses, we used the mean of the two limbs for each parameter, for each participant, in the subsequent analyses. We conducted a 3 × 2 mixed MANOVA, using all biomechanical variables as dependent measures. Post hoc comparisons were adjusted with LSD test; the alpha level was set at 0.05.

As regards the clinical variables, for each dependent variable, a 3 × 2 mixed ANOVA (Time × RAS) was applied. Post hoc analyses were calculated by using repeated measures ANOVAs and t-tests. The alpha level was set at 0.05 for the omnibus tests and was adjusted with the Bonferroni formula for post hoc analyses (p value = 0.05/n comparisons).

Moreover, independently of the outcomes of the previous analyses, we planned to conduct a set of additional exploratory analyses, to better examine the potential of the footstep sounds. To this purpose, we separately tested the two groups of participants, by conducting two repeated measures MANOVAs and a set of contrasts adjusted with LSD test on the primary outcome measures of this study (i.e., the biomechanical measures).

All the analyses were performed using the SPSS Statistics software.

Results

A preliminary set of analyses was run to compare the two groups at baseline for each variable, and no significant difference was found. The effects of the two treatments (rehabilitation with ecological vs. artificial RAS) across time (T0, T5, T17) were observed using both biomechanical and clinical measures.

Biomechanical Measures

Overall, the analyses run on biomechanical measures (Table 2) did not reveal any significant result for the interaction Time x RAS and for the main effect of RAS. Although the results did not reveal a statistical significance for interaction—which was the primary interest of our investigation—the majority of the considered parameters indicated a significant effect of the variable Time [Wilk’s λ = 0.098, F(16, 15) = 8.652, p < 0.001, ηp2=0.902]. In particular, this was found for cadence [F(2, 60) = 4.595; p = 0.01; ηp2=0.133], gait speed [F(2, 60) = 8.538; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.222], step width [F(2, 60) = 12.647; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.297], step length [F(2, 60) = 17.752; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.372], stride length [F(2, 60) = 3.681; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.109], percentage of double support phase [F(2, 60) = 4.911; p = 0.01; ηp2=0.141], and percentage of swing phase [F(2, 60) = 6.843; p < 0.005; ηp2=0.186]. Conversely, the effect of time on the percentage of stance phase was not significant.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison between spatio-temporal parameters assessed before and after rehabilitation for each group.

Then, we investigated more deeply how these parameters changed across time. We found that the cadence significantly increased between T0 and T5 (p = 0.021) and between T0 and T17 (p = 0.029), while it remained constant between T5 and T17. Analogous results were revealed by the analysis on gait speed, indeed it significantly increased between T0 and T5 (p = 0.006) and between T0 and T17 (p = 0.001) and remained constant between T5 and T17. Similarly, the percentage of swing phase significantly increased between T0 and T5 (p = 0.001) and between T0 and T17 (p = 0.033), while it remained constant between T5 and T17. Consistently, the percentage of double support phase significantly decreased between T0 and T5 (p = 0.009) and between T0 and T17 (p = 0.013) and remained constant between T5 and T17. As concerns step length, we found an increase between T0 and T5 (p = 0.004), between T0 and T17 (p < 0.001), and also between T5 and T17 (p = 0.01). Surprisingly, we did not find a difference between T0 and T5 regarding step width (p = 0.568), but the values observed in T17 were significantly higher than those observed in T0 (p < 0.001) and T5 (p < 0.001). Moreover, as concerns stride length, there was no difference between T0 and T5 (p = 0.108), while it significantly increased between T0 and T17 (p = 0.011); no difference was found between T5 and T17.

Clinical Measures

The analyses run on clinical measures (Table 3) did not reveal any significant result for the time × RAS interaction and for the main effect of RAS. As regards time, similarly to the analyses on biomechanical measures, we found that the main effect was significant for almost all clinical measures. In particular, a significant main effect of time was observed for the UPDRS—part 3 [F(2, 52) = 32.749; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.557], ABC [F(2, 56) = 5.418; p < 0.01; ηp2=0.162], FES [F(2, 58) = 4.819; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.143], FOGQ [F(2, 60) = 3.926; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.116], GDS [F(2, 58) = 3.663; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.112], PDQ8 [F(2, 58) = 7.343; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.202], Tinetti test [F(2, 58) = 3.945; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.12], SPPB [F(2, 58) = 5.330; p < 0.01; ηp2=0.155] and its subcomponents STS [F(2, 42) = 15.390; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.423], and 4-m test [F(2, 40) = 7.382; p < 0.01; ηp2=0.270]. Conversely, no significant effect was found for the FIM scale.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison between clinical scores assessed before and after rehabilitation for each group.

On the basis of the ANOVAs results, we decided to further explore how the clinical measures changed across time. We found that the FES scores decreased between T0 and T5 [t(30) = 2.375; p < 0.05; d = 0.377] and between T0 and T17 [t(30) = 2.367; p < 0.05; d = 0.406], while they remained constant between T5 and T17. Analogous results were observed for SPPB, with an increase between T0 and T5 [t(30) = 2.456; p = 0.01; d = 0.304] and between T0 and T17 [t(30) = 2.367; p < 0.05; d = 0.253], and no difference between T5 and T17. This trend was also observed in two of the subcomponents of SPPB, namely STS and 4-m test. There was a decrease of time necessary to complete the test between T0 and T5 for both the STS [t(22) = 4.082; p < 0.001; d = 1.016] and the 4-m test [t(22) = 2.331; p < 0.05; d = 0.647], and between T0 and T17 for both the STS [t(22) = 4.651; p < 0.001; d = 1.061] and the 4-m test [t(22) = 2.666; p < 0.01; d = 0.569]; in no case a difference between T5 and T17 was observed.

The FOGQ scores also decreased between T0 and T5 [t(31) = 2.459; p = 0.01; d = 0.217], while the difference between T0 and T17 was no longer significant after the Bonferroni correction [t(31) = 1.797; p < 0.05; d = 0.173]; no difference was observed between T5 and T17. A similar trend was observed for the Tinetti test, with marginally significant increases between T0 and T5 [t(30) = 2.129; p < 0.05; d = 0.376] and between T0 and T17 [t(30) = 2.037; p < 0.05; d = 0.438], which were no longer significant after the Bonferroni adjustment; again no difference was observed between T5 and T17.

The UPDRS—part 3 scores decreased between T0 and T5 [t(27) = 7.701; p < 0.001; d = 0.548], between T0 and T17 [t(27) = 6.261; p < 0.001; d = 0.781], and also between T5 and T17 [t(31) = 2.598; p < 0.01; d = 0.269]. Analogously, the ABC scores increased between T0 and T5 [t(29) = 1.997; p < 0.05; d = 0.302], between T0 and T17 [t(29) = 2.556; p < 0.01; d = 0.609] and between T5 and T17 [t(30) = 2.240; p < 0.05; d = 0.385], however, the first and the last comparisons were no longer significant after Bonferroni correction.

The PDQ8 revealed the best results at T17, with higher scores compared to both T0 [t(30) = 2.950; p < 0.01; d = 0.490] and T5 [t(31) = 2.966; p < 0.01; d = 0.285], while no difference was observed between T0 and T5. As concerns the GDS, a marginal decrease was observed between T0 and T17 [t(30) = 2.171; p < 0.05; d = 0.476], which disappeared after the Bonferroni correction, while the other comparisons did not reach any significant value.

Additional Exploratory Analyses

We conducted a set of exploratory analyses by separately examining the two groups of participants on the primary outcome measures (i.e., biomechanical measures). The repeated measures MANOVAs revealed significant results for the ecological RAS group [Wilk’s λ = 0.23, F(16, 46) = 3.149, p = 0.001, ηp2=0.523], but not for the artificial RAS group. The MANOVA conducted on the ecological RAS group data revealed significant values for the following measures: cadence [F(2, 30) = 4.367; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.225], gait speed [F(2, 30) = 5.914; p < 0.01; ηp2=0.283], percentage of swing phase [F(2, 30) = 5.533; p < 0.01; ηp2=0.269], step length [F(2, 30) = 10.23; p < 0.001; ηp2=0.405], and step width [F(2, 30) = 5.322; p < 0.05; ηp2=0.262]. In our opinion, of particular interest are the results concerning cadence and gait speed (see Figure 3), since these are the two measures more directly related to the auditory stimuli. The contrasts for these variables indicate that cadence significantly increased from T0 to T5 (p = 0.011), but this advantage was not maintained at T17 (p = 0.072), while gait speed increased from T0 to T5 (p = 0.017) and this advantage was maintained at T17 (p = 0.13).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean cadence (A) and gait speed (B) for both ecological rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) participants and artificial RAS participants. Error bars represent SD.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate whether a PD rehabilitation program integrated with ecological RAS (i.e., footstep sounds) can be more effective than the same program integrated with artificial RAS (i.e., metronome sounds). We hypothesized that both groups would benefit from the rhythmic information of the stimuli, but the group exposed to ecological RAS would also take advantage of the priming effect elicited by the ecological information of footstep sounds. The results observed on the primary outcome measures (biomechanical measures) suggest that the treatments are equally effective as no significant interaction was revealed by analyses. The same results were given by the secondary outcome measures (clinical measures).

As a whole, our results indicate that—independently of the type of sound— the rehabilitation programs integrated with RAS are effective. Indeed, comparing the biomechanical and clinical data collected before and after the treatment, we observed noticeable improvements in the majority of the variables. This evidence is in line with previous literature, which clearly proved the efficacy of rehabilitation with RAS, in terms of both spatio-temporal parameters and clinical variables (2, 911, 17). Moreover, we observed that these improvements were largely maintained at the follow-up, 3 months after the end of the supervised period, which represents a longer term compared to the majority of previous RAS studies. However, given that the RAS efficacy is well-documented in literature, our main aim was not to further confirm it, but to examine whether the type of sound (i.e., ecological or artificial) can actually influence the efficacy of rehabilitation.

The novelty of this study is that, for the first time, we used biological motion sounds as RAS in a rehabilitation program with PD patients. The same rehabilitation program was integrated either with footstep sounds (ecological RAS condition) or with metronome sounds (artificial RAS condition) and the overall results indicated that the effects of the two sounds—in a rehabilitation context—are equivalent. However, to better examine the potential of the footstep sounds and obtain additional information that could be used as starting point for future investigations, we ran a set of exploratory analyses on biomechanical measures for each group, separately. These analyses revealed that only the patients assigned to the ecological RAS condition significantly improved. Among the various measures showing significant improvements, in our opinion a particular attention should be dedicated to cadence and gait speed, since these are the two parameters more directly linked to the auditory stimuli. However, we acknowledge that our exploratory analyses are only informative but not conclusive; indeed these analyses do not compare the two groups and cannot prove a superior effect of ecological sounds over artificial sounds. Future studies should better clarify the potential of footstep sounds in the rehabilitation context.

The effects of ecological and artificial sounds on motor tasks in PD patients were examined in previous research (37). In their study, Young and colleagues found that in some spatio-temporal parameters patients performed better in the ecological sound condition than in the artificial sound condition. However, the methods used in the previous and in this study are quite different: the patients tested by Young and colleagues were engaged in a real-time imitation task, while in our case the patients were engaged in a 5-week rehabilitation program. To the best of our knowledge this is the first time that a rehabilitation program is integrated with ecological RAS, since previous RAS experiments generally used music or metronome sounds (46). Therefore, our study can be considered as a first attempt to investigate in this direction and further research is needed to understand whether this line of investigation can be fruitful.

Our hypothesis, based on the audiovisual mirror neurons (4144) and on the common coding perceptual-motor theories (45, 46), was that the use of ecological RAS would constitute an advantage. In particular, we hypothesized that footstep sounds would evoke a mental representation of walking, directly activating the motor systems and, consequently, facilitating patients’ walking. The overall results we observed are not consistent with this hypothesis. Only the additional exploratory analyses are in line with this hypothesis, however, further research stressing the role of RAS is necessary to confirm our exploratory observations. Indeed, we examined the effects of ecological RAS integrated with a standard rehabilitation protocol performed at a hospital. On the one hand, this has a strong ecological validity, since it represents a typical context in which RAS can be employed; on the other hand, the effects of RAS can be somehow masked by the effects of the other exercises performed by the patients. Thus, in our opinion, future studies should isolate the effects of gait training with RAS from the effects of other exercises, making more salient the possible differences between ecological and artificial RAS.

Like every study, also our work does have some limitations. The severity of the disease of our participants was mild–moderate, so we cannot extend our results to patients with more severe impairments. Moreover, by experimentally testing a sample of individuals with a higher level of motor impairment it could be possible to observe stronger effects. This might be helpful to better understand the possible different effects between the two types of sound. Another limitation is that we did not control/manipulate important parameters of sounds (i.e., volume, frequency). In our opinion, specific research of how these features of the stimuli affect gait training should be performed in more controlled laboratory experiments, rather than in ecological settings as in our study. Moreover, the subjective pleasantness of the two types of sound (ecological vs. artificial) should be investigated.

From an applied perspective, our results suggest that ecological RAS is equally effective compared to artificial RAS, therefore, it would be possible to let patients decide what type of sound they prefer for gait training. This is particularly important for their compliance with the treatment, because the administration of sounds which are perceived as annoying by patients might lead to a low adherence to the training. Moreover, it is noteworthy that several participants reported that the footstep sounds were meaningful and reminded them some physical activities they used to do in the past (e.g., military march), thus evoking motor images directly linked with walking. For future research, the implementation of ecological RAS training in the standard rehabilitation protocols at the hospitals could represent an important source of data, allowing researchers to examine the efficacy of ecological RAS on larger samples of participants.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Independent Ethics Committee of the A.O.U. Cagliari with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Independent Ethics Committee of the A.O.U. Cagliari.

Author Contributions

MM, RP, FC, FS, TA, PB, CC, GC, MG, and MP designed the study. CC and RP performed the physical capacity assessment and prepared the rehabilitation protocol. GC performed the neurological evaluations. MP and FC collected and processed the biomechanical data. MM, TA, FS, and PB prepared the stimuli. MM and MG performed the statistical analyses. MM, FS, and MP wrote the manuscript. RP, FC, TA, PB, CC, GC, and MG revised the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was partly supported by the Autonomous Region of Sardinia (grant CRP-78543 L.R. 7/2007). The author MM was supported by the Autonomous Region of Sardinia, Master and Back Programme 2013 (PRR-MAB-A2013-19330). The authors wish to express their gratitude to the physical therapists Marilena Fara, Giovanna Ghiani, Alessandra Pani, Elsa Sau, Gino Sedda, and Mauro Usala for their valuable support during the training program.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2018.00348/full#supplementary-material.

Footnote

  1. ^It is noteworthy that participants were asked to use the auditory stimulation for 30 min almost every day, thus it was important that they feel comfortable with it. Moreover, they needed to adapt the volume during home-training depending on the environmental noise.

References

1. Blin O, Ferrandez AM, Serratrice G. Quantitative analysis of gait in Parkinson patients: increased variability of stride length. J Neurol Sci (1990) 98:91–7. doi:10.1016/0022-510X(90)90184-O

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Thaut MH, McIntosh GC, Rice RR, Miller RA, Rathbun J, Brault JM. Rhythmic auditory stimulation in gait training for Parkinson’s disease patients. Mov Disord (1996) 11:193–200. doi:10.1002/mds.870110213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lim I, van Wegen E, de Goede C, Deutekom M, Nieuwboer A, Willems A, et al. Effects of external rhythmical cueing on gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. Clin Rehabil (2005) 19:695–713. doi:10.1191/0269215505cr906oa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Murgia M, Corona F, Pili R, Sors F, Agostini T, Casula C, et al. Rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) and motor rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease: new frontiers in assessment and intervention protocols. Open Psychol J (2015) 8:220–9. doi:10.2174/1874350101508010220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Nombela C, Hughes LE, Owen AM, Grahn JA. Into the groove: can rhythm influence Parkinson’s disease? Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:2564–70. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Thaut MH, Abiru M. Rhythmic auditory stimulation in rehabilitation of movement disorders: a review of current research. Music Percept Interdiscip J (2010) 27:263–9. doi:10.1525/mp.2010.27.4.263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fernandez del Olmo M, Cudeiro J. Temporal variability of gait in Parkinson disease: effects of a rehabilitation programme based on rhythmic sound cues. Parkinsonism Relat Disord (2005) 11:25–33. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2004.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Fernandez del Olmo M, Arias P, Furio MC, Pozo MA, Cudeiro J. Evaluation of the effect of training using auditory stimulation on rhythmic movement in Parkinsonian patients – a combined motor and [18F]-FDG PET study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord (2006) 12:155–64. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2005.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ford MP, Malone LA, Nyikos I, Yelisetty R, Bickel CS. Gait training with progressive external auditory cueing in persons with Parkinson’s disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2010) 91:1255–61. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2010.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Frazzitta G, Maestri R, Uccellini D, Bertotti G, Abelli P. Rehabilitation treatment of gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease with freezing: a comparison between two physical therapy protocols using visual and auditory cues with or without treadmill training. Mov Disord (2009) 24:1139–43. doi:10.1002/mds.22491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kadivar Z, Corcos DM, Foto J, Hondzinski JM. Effect of step training and rhythmic auditory stimulation on functional performance in Parkinson patients. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2011) 25:626–35. doi:10.1177/1545968311401627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Miller RA, Thaut MH, McIntosh GC, Rice RR. Components of EMG symmetry and variability in parkinsonian and healthy elderly gait. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1996) 101:1–7. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(95)00209-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pau M, Corona F, Pili R, Casula C, Sors F, Agostini T, et al. Effects of physical rehabilitation integrated with rhythmic auditory stimulation on spatio-temporal and kinematic parameters of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurol (2016) 7:126. doi:10.3389/fneur.2016.00126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Avanzino L, Pelosin E, Vicario CM, Lagravinese G, Abbruzzese G, Martino D. Time processing and motor control in movement disorders. Front Hum Neurosci (2016) 10:631. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bieńkiewicz MMN, Craig CM. Parkinson’s is time on your side? Evidence for difficulties with sensorimotor synchronization. Front Neurol (2015) 6:249. doi:10.3389/fneur.2015.00249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Dalla Bella S, Benoit C-E, Farrugia N, Keller PE, Obrig H, Mainka S, et al. Gait improvement via rhythmic stimulation in Parkinson’s disease is linked to rhythmic skills. Sci Rep (2017) 7:42005. doi:10.1038/srep42005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. McIntosh GC, Brown SH, Rice RR, Thaut MH. Rhythmic auditory-motor facilitation of gait patterns in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1997) 62:22–6. doi:10.1136/jnnp.62.1.22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Arias P, Cudeiro J. Effect of rhythmic auditory stimulation on gait in parkinsonian patients with and without freezing of gait. PLoS One (2010) 5:e9675. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Baker K, Rochester L, Nieuwboer A. The immediate effect of attentional, auditory, and a combined cue strategy on gait during single and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2007) 88:1593–600. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2007.07.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Baker K, Rochester L, Nieuwboer A. The effect of cues on gait variability – reducing the attentional cost of walking in people with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord (2008) 14:314–20. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2007.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fernandez del Olmo M, Cudeiro J. A simple procedure using auditory stimuli to improve movement in Parkinson’s disease: a pilot study. Neurol Clin Neurophysiol NCN (2003) 2003:1–7.

Google Scholar

22. Freedland RL, Festa C, Sealy M, McBean A, Elghazaly P, Capan A, et al. The effects of pulsed auditory stimulation on various gait measurements in persons with Parkinson’s disease. NeuroRehabilitation (2002) 17:81–7.

Google Scholar

23. Hove MJ, Suzuki K, Uchitomi H, Orimo S, Miyake Y. Interactive rhythmic auditory stimulation reinstates natural 1/f timing in gait of Parkinson’s patients. PLoS One (2012) 7:e32600. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Howe TE, Lövgreen B, Cody FWJ, Ashton VJ, Oldham JA. Auditory cues can modify the gait of persons with early-stage Parkinson’s disease: a method for enhancing parkinsonian walking performance? Clin Rehabil (2003) 17:363–7. doi:10.1191/0269215503cr621oa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Nieuwboer A, Baker K, Willems A-M, Jones D, Spildooren J, Lim I, et al. The short-term effects of different cueing modalities on turn speed in people with Parkinson’s disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2009) 23:831–6. doi:10.1177/1545968309337136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Rochester L, Burn DJ, Woods G, Godwin J, Nieuwboer A. Does auditory rhythmical cueing improve gait in people with Parkinson’s disease and cognitive impairment? A feasibility study. Mov Disord (2009) 24:839–45. doi:10.1002/mds.22400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Suteerawattananon M, Morris GS, Etnyre BR, Jankovic J, Protas EJ. Effects of visual and auditory cues on gait in individuals with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci (2004) 219:63–9. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2003.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Willems AM, Nieuwboer A, Chavret F, Desloovere K, Dom R, Rochester L, et al. The use of rhythmic auditory cues to influence gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease, the differential effect for freezers and non-freezers, an explorative study. Disabil Rehabil (2006) 28:721–8. doi:10.1080/09638280500386569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Willems A-M, Nieuwboer A, Chavret F, Desloovere K, Dom R, Rochester L, et al. Turning in Parkinson’s disease patients and controls: the effect of auditory cues. Mov Disord (2007) 22:1871–8. doi:10.1002/mds.21445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Marchese R, Diverio M, Zucchi F, Lentino C, Abbruzzese G. The role of sensory cues in the rehabilitation of parkinsonian patients: a comparison of two physical therapy protocols. Mov Disord (2000) 15:879–83. doi:10.1002/1531-8257(200009)15:5<879::AID-MDS1018>3.0.CO;2-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Rochester L, Nieuwboer A, Baker K, Hetherington V, Willems A-M, Chavret F, et al. The attentional cost of external rhythmical cues and their impact on gait in Parkinson’s disease: effect of cue modality and task complexity. J Neural Transm (Vienna) (2007) 114:1243–8. doi:10.1007/s00702-007-0756-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ellis T, de Goede CJ, Feldman RG, Wolters EC, Kwakkel G, Wagenaar RC. Efficacy of a physical therapy program in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2005) 86:626–32. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2004.08.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Corona F, Pau M, Guicciardi M, Murgia M, Pili R, Casula C. Quantitative assessment of gait in elderly people affected by Parkinson’s disease. IEEE International Symposium on Medical Measurements and Applications (MeMeA). Benevento (2016). p. 1–6.

Google Scholar

34. Murgia M, Santoro I, Tamburini G, Prpic V, Sors F, Galmonte A, et al. Ecological sounds affect breath duration more than artificial sounds. Psychol Res (2016) 80:76–81. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0647-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Effenberg AO, Fehse U, Schmitz G, Krueger B, Mechling H. Movement sonification: effects on motor learning beyond rhythmic adjustments. Front Neurosci (2016) 10:219. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Rodger MWM, Young WR, Craig CM. Synthesis of walking sounds for alleviating gait disturbances in Parkinson’s disease. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng (2014) 22:543–8. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2013.2285410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Young WR, Rodger MWM, Craig CM. Auditory observation of stepping actions can cue both spatial and temporal components of gait in Parkinson’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia (2014) 57:140–53. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rodger MWM, Craig CM. Beyond the metronome: auditory events and music may afford more than just interval durations as gait cues in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurosci (2016) 10:272. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res (1996) 3:131–41. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cook R, Bird G, Catmur C, Press C, Heyes C. Mirror neurons: from origin to function. Behav Brain Sci (2014) 37:177–92. doi:10.1017/S0140525X13000903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Galati G, Committeri G, Spitoni G, Aprile T, Di Russo F, Pitzalis S, et al. A selective representation of the meaning of actions in the auditory mirror system. Neuroimage (2008) 40:1274–86. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C. Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr Biol (2006) 16:1824–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lewis JW, Brefczynski JA, Phinney RE, Janik JJ, DeYoe EA. Distinct cortical pathways for processing tool versus animal sounds. J Neurosci (2005) 25:5148–58. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0419-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Tettamanti M, Buccino G, Saccuman MC, Gallese V, Danna M, Scifo P, et al. Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J Cogn Neurosci (2005) 17:273–81. doi:10.1162/0898929053124965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W. The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci (2001) 24:849–878; discussion 878–937. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Prinz W. Perception and action planning. Eur J Cogn Psychol (1997) 9:129–54. doi:10.1080/713752551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1988) 51:745–52. doi:10.1136/jnnp.51.6.745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hollman JH, McDade EM, Petersen RC. Normative spatiotemporal gait parameters in older adults. Gait Posture (2011) 34:111–8. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Oberg T, Karsznia A, Oberg K. Basic gait parameters: reference data for normal subjects, 10-79 years of age. J Rehabil Res Dev (1993) 30:210–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

50. Davis RB, Õunpuu S, Tyburski D, Gage JR. A gait analysis data collection and reduction technique. Hum Mov Sci (1991) 10:575–87. doi:10.1016/0167-9457(91)90046-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology (1967) 17:427–42. doi:10.1212/WNL.17.5.427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Goetz CG, Poewe W, Rascol O, Sampaio C, Stebbins GT, Counsell C, et al. Movement disorder society task force report on the Hoehn and Yahr staging scale: status and recommendations. Mov Disord (2004) 19:1020–8. doi:10.1002/mds.20213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Goetz CG, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, Poewe W, Sampaio C, Stebbins GT, et al. Movement disorder society-sponsored revision of the unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (MDS-UPDRS): process, format, and clinimetric testing plan. Mov Disord (2007) 22:41–7. doi:10.1002/mds.21198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Granger C, Hamilton B, Keith RA, Zielezny MA, Sherwin F, Keith RA, et al. Advances in functional assessment for medical rehabilitation. Top Geriatr Rehabil (1986) 1(3):59–74. doi:10.1097/00013614-198604000-00007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Tinetti ME, Williams TF, Mayewski R. Fall risk index for elderly patients based on number of chronic disabilities. Am J Med (1986) 80:429–34. doi:10.1016/0002-9343(86)90717-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Guralnik JM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Glynn RJ, Berkman LF, Blazer DG, et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol (1994) 49:M85–94. doi:10.1093/geronj/49.2.M85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Marc LG, Raue PJ, Bruce ML. Screening performance of the geriatric depression scale (GDS-15) in a diverse elderly home care population. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2008) 16:914–21. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e318186bd67

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Franchignoni F, Giordano A, Ferriero G. Rasch analysis of the short form 8-item Parkinson’s disease questionnaire (PDQ-8). Qual Life Res (2008) 17:541–8. doi:10.1007/s11136-008-9341-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tinetti ME, Richman D, Powell L. Falls efficacy as a measure of fear of falling. J Gerontol (1990) 45:239–43. doi:10.1093/geronj/45.6.P239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Powell LE, Myers AM. The activities-specific balance confidence (ABC) scale. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci (1995) 50A:M28–34. doi:10.1093/gerona/50A.1.M28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Giladi N, Shabtai H, Simon ES, Biran S, Tal J, Korczyn AD. Construction of freezing of gait questionnaire for patients with parkinsonism. Parkinsonism Relat Disord (2000) 6:165–70. doi:10.1016/S1353-8020(99)00062-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Schulz KF, Grimes DA. Generation of allocation sequences in randomised trials: chance, not choice. Lancet (2002) 359:515–9. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07683-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix: Rehabilitation Protocol

Keywords: rhythm, ecological sounds, auditory stimuli, rhythmic auditory stimulation, Parkinson disease, gait, spatio-temporal parameters, gait analysis

Citation: Murgia M, Pili R, Corona F, Sors F, Agostini TA, Bernardis P, Casula C, Cossu G, Guicciardi M and Pau M (2018) The Use of Footstep Sounds as Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation for Gait Rehabilitation in Parkinson’s Disease: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Front. Neurol. 9:348. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00348

Received: 01 July 2017; Accepted: 30 April 2018;
Published: 24 May 2018

Edited by:

Diego Minciacchi, Università degli Studi di Firenze, Italy

Reviewed by:

George C. McConnell, Stevens Institute of Technology, United States
Erwin Van Wegen, VU University Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright: © 2018 Murgia, Pili, Corona, Sors, Agostini, Bernardis, Casula, Cossu, Guicciardi and Pau. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mauro Murgia, mmurgia@units.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.