Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 28 September 2017
Sec. Neurotrauma

Concussion As a Multi-Scale Complex System: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis of Current Knowledge

\r\nErin S. Kenzie*Erin S. Kenzie1*Elle L. ParksElle L. Parks1Erin D. BiglerErin D. Bigler2Miranda M. Lim,Miranda M. Lim3,4James C. ChesnuttJames C. Chesnutt5Wayne Wakeland\r\nWayne Wakeland1
  • 1Systems Science Program, Portland State University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, United States
  • 3Sleep Disorders Clinic, Division of Hospital and Specialty Medicine, Veterans Affairs Portland Health Care System, Portland, OR, United States
  • 4Departments of Neurology, Medicine, and Behavioral Neuroscience, and Oregon Institute of Occupational Health Sciences, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 5TBI/Concussion Program, Orthopedics & Rehabilitation and Family Medicine, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) has been called “the most complicated disease of the most complex organ of the body” and is an increasingly high-profile public health issue. Many patients report long-term impairments following even “mild” injuries, but reliable criteria for diagnosis and prognosis are lacking. Every clinical trial for TBI treatment to date has failed to demonstrate reliable and safe improvement in outcomes, and the existing body of literature is insufficient to support the creation of a new classification system. Concussion, or mild TBI, is a highly heterogeneous phenomenon, and numerous factors interact dynamically to influence an individual’s recovery trajectory. Many of the obstacles faced in research and clinical practice related to TBI and concussion, including observed heterogeneity, arguably stem from the complexity of the condition itself. To improve understanding of this complexity, we review the current state of research through the lens provided by the interdisciplinary field of systems science, which has been increasingly applied to biomedical issues. The review was conducted iteratively, through multiple phases of literature review, expert interviews, and systems diagramming and represents the first phase in an effort to develop systems models of concussion. The primary focus of this work was to examine concepts and ways of thinking about concussion that currently impede research design and block advancements in care of TBI. Results are presented in the form of a multi-scale conceptual framework intended to synthesize knowledge across disciplines, improve research design, and provide a broader, multi-scale model for understanding concussion pathophysiology, classification, and treatment.

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a significant public health concern. The United States alone sees an estimated 2.5–3.8 million cases per year (14), approximately 70–90% of which are mild TBI (mTBI), also called concussion (5, 6).1 A recent National Public Radio poll found that one in four Americans report having suffered a concussion at some point in their lives (8). Because many of those affected do not seek medical treatment, concussion is vastly underreported (4); one study estimated that at least 88% of cases might go unrecognized (9).

Despite increased awareness of TBI—and particularly concussion—in recent years, we still lack effective means of diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment (1012). Over 30 clinical trials of pharmaceutical products to treat TBI have failed, and the U.S. Food and Drug Administration has yet to approve a single diagnostic test or therapy for the condition (13). The total cost of these failed clinical trials is estimated at 1.1 billion dollars (14). Research is hampered by imprecise classification, methodological inconsistencies, measurement issues, and uncertainty about underlying pathophysiology.

It is estimated that somewhere between 5 and 43% of concussion patients experience prolonged somatic, emotional, or cognitive impairments lasting longer than 3 months, a state referred to as post-concussive syndrome (PCS) (1520). A recent longitudinal study found that only 27% of PCS sufferers meeting strict inclusion criteria at 3 months post-injury eventually made a full recovery (21). Accordingly, persistent post-concussive symptoms comprise a significant health burden in modern society (15). Moreover, the reasons why some patients recover quickly and others do not remain poorly understood (2124). Factors such as genetics, health status, biomechanics, and myriad premorbid and environmental factors all likely come into play (20). In addition, the mode of injury, clinical features, and patient experience are all highly heterogenous (18, 21, 23, 2527). These differing recovery trajectories form the primary motivation for the current project.

Traumatic brain injury has been called “the most complicated disease of the most complex organ of the body” (28). The field of systems science offers methods for understanding such complexity, as seen in the growing field of systems medicine (2931). In this review, we survey the concussion literature through a systems lens, highlighting the complex nature of injury and recovery, as well as limitations in the existing literature. As a first step toward building a comprehensive systems understanding of concussion, we present the current state of knowledge about concussion pathophysiology using a series of multi-scale systems diagrams and discuss how this initial effort might inform clinical practice, future research, and further development of systems models.

This project has been led by a team of systems scientists in collaboration with TBI experts from the fields of neurology, neurosurgery, psychiatry, sports medicine, rehabilitation, neuropsychology, neuroscience, and others. This non-traditional review was done iteratively through multiple phases of literature review, expert interviews,2 and systems diagramming. It is presented here as an example of how systems methods can enrich understanding of a complex medical issue and as the first phase of an effort to develop systems models of concussion (a future publication will present a more formalized model based on this research). It should be noted that this review does not include a systematic assessment of study quality [see Ref. (32) for such a review]. The small number of high-quality studies in this field is insufficient for understanding the big picture of concussion recovery. Systems science methods have the flexibility to incorporate various types of information, including expert opinion. This big-tent approach allows for the construction of a hypothesis model that reflects a simplified vision of how a system is thought to work. The value of such a working model is that it enables a holistic perspective and discussion at the level of whole systems and subsystems—a potentially transformative perspective for complex issues such as concussion.

State of the Research

A 2004 World Health Organization review notes the “variable quality” of mTBI studies (33), while Cassidy (34) argues that before 2002, study quality was “poor.” In an evidence-based systematic review of the literature on prevalent indicators of concussion, Carney et al. (32) found that only 26 of 5,437 studies met their criteria for analysis. Common shortcomings found in the review include lack of a comparison group, measurement and reporting inconsistencies, and potential for bias or confound. Bigler et al. (35) argue that some neuropsychological metrics used in TBI research are not suitable for detecting mild impairment, and that type II statistical error is common. Moreover, several recent systematic reviews focusing on mTBI specifically have noted a lack of a shared methodological framework within the research community (18, 36, 37).

While many of the shortcomings identified in these reviews could be addressed by more robust research methodology, progress is also hindered by more fundamental uncertainties regarding pathophysiology and measurement of TBI. These ontological and epistemological uncertainties are compounded by heterogeneity and non-linear interactions between variables—concerns common to complex problems.

Unknown Pathophysiology

The pathophysiology of concussion is largely unknown, although several hypotheses have emerged. Giza and Hovda (38, 39) have identified a neurometabolic cascade in the acute stage following injurious impact in animal models, and some of these neurometabolic alterations have been confirmed in humans with H1-MR spectroscopy techniques (40, 41). Diffuse axonal injury (DAI) has also been identified in animal studies and proposed as an explanatory model (36, 37, 42). Other researchers, however, suggest that DAI is more likely involved in moderate to severe TBI cases, while traumatic axonal injury, which is more capable of repair, is likely to be prevalent in concussion (43). Complicating matters further is a significant literature demonstrating that the biomechanics and neurophysiology of impact injuries differ significantly from those underlying blast or penetrating concussive injuries, leading many researchers and clinicians to treat them as separate conditions (4446). A growing body of literature examines alterations in functional connectivity networks in concussion, and how network changes over time might influence recovery trajectories and outcomes (4752).

Cellular metabolic dysfunction immediately following injury initiates a vulnerability window; if a second concussion occurs within this window, significant further damage can occur, a phenomenon described as second impact syndrome (38, 53, 54). Questions about prognosis and post-injury vulnerability are of particular concern in military and athletic contexts, where social pressure exists to return to combat or play (55, 56).

No single definition of concussion is accepted across disciplines, although several are available (7, 32, 5759). The consensus NIH definition of TBI as “an alteration in brain function, or other evidence of brain pathology, caused by an external force” (60) lacks specificity for concussion. Other definitions tend to focus on mode of injury or clinical parameters, and in sports are closely linked with guidelines for return to play (61, 62). Disagreement and uncertainty also exist about the threshold for diagnosis, or how to examine the outcome of multiple subconcussive blows to the head (6365). However, research does support that chronic cellular dysfunction and repeated head injury can cause chronic traumatic encephalopathy, a disease often present in athletes as well as combat military personnel (66, 67).

Heterogeneity

Many of the research shortcomings mentioned earlier can be traced to inadequate accounting for patient and injury heterogeneity and variability in clinical identification and diagnosis, which is found in several aspects of concussion (13, 23, 27). First, the mode of injury is highly heterogeneous. Traumatic biomechanical forces in the brain can occur from direct (to the head) or indirect (to the body) impact (e.g., sports, workplace accidents, and violent trauma), fast acceleration or deceleration forces (e.g., whiplash and motor vehicle accidents), or intense changes in pressure (e.g., blast exposure) (68), each inducing distinct parameters of parenchymal displacement (45, 46, 65). Biomechanics and other injury characteristics further interact with individual variation in physiology (particularly idiosyncrasies in brain topography and connectivity), along with other personal characteristics such as age (69), sex (7072), pre-injury diseases and medications (73), and genetics (74, 75).

Concussion patients suffer myriad complaints—headache, disorientation, language impairments, loss of consciousness, mood disruptions, cognitive deficits, sleep disorders, sensitivity to light and sound, and problems with balance or gait, among others—although not all symptoms may be present in every case (18, 32, 76). Indeed, no impairment is common across all cases or all modes of injury. Loss of consciousness, once widely thought of as characteristic and diagnostic of concussion, is now understood to be present in only 1–14% of cases [(32); see Ref. (77) for discussion]. The severity and duration of each of these impairments varies and is largely unpredictable. Function in school, work, and social relationships can also be compromised, ranging from stress to disability. In addition to heterogeneity in signs, symptoms, and deficits, the factors influencing recovery—such as adherence to treatment, amount of social support, behavioral adaptation, cognitive reserve, and psychological resilience—also vary widely between individuals (27, 78, 79). Due to the heterogeneity and complexity seen within concussion, it is likely that a wide variety of destructive and restorative processes are at work following injury, some at the cellular level and others at larger scales.

At all levels of severity, TBI is increasingly recognized to be a chronic condition or ongoing disability, rather than an isolated event or fixed injury, further complicating definition and classification (7, 18, 80). Identification of any specific fixed pathophysiology therefore needs to account for dynamic evolving changes occurring over days, weeks, months, or even years. Indeed, the question of how to identify proper time points for diagnosis, measurement, or recovery trajectory has been a recurring theme among reviewers of failed clinical trials (12, 13, 58).

The types and extent of injuries currently included under the umbrella of concussion are so different from one another that a diagnosis of “concussion” alone is not very useful for informing treatment (27). Patient and injury heterogeneity, along with the lack of a common known pathophysiological explanation, raises the question of whether multiple distinct etiologies are in fact present. Sequential neuroimaging or other metrics may eventually enable characterization of subgroups defined by disease etiology, although this remains to be seen (8183).

Even when promising avenues of research have been identified, translation to classification and treatment have fallen short, and disagreement continues over appropriate inclusion and exclusion criteria for concussion (13, 35, 84). For example, questions remain as to whether the mechanism of injury (e.g., blast or impact) or context of injury (e.g., football game, car accident, or fall) should be stratified in trials (22). Overly narrow inclusion criteria—such as only including individuals who have had a loss of consciousness, for example—artificially reduce heterogeneity (32), while overly broad criteria risk the inclusion of non-concussed or more severely injured persons. Highly variable subject groupings and lack of control groups also make studies incomparable, which significantly hinders meta-analysis and systematic review. Synthesis projects in turn are critical for creating an evidence-based definition of concussion and classification of TBI, creating a mutually dependent scenario that hinders further progress.

Measurement

Uncertainty about the nature of concussion is compounded by problems with measurement and classification. When a TBI is suspected, classification is determined by the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) (85), which categorizes all TBI—from concussion to coma—on a single spectrum of mild/moderate/severe (86). This ordinal scale is based solely on measures of arousal, while assessments used to measure recovery in the clinic are diverse and often continuous. Although the acute clinical care of moderate to severe TBI patients has benefited from use of this scale, the same is not true for concussion (87), for which arousal is less informative and the GCS fails to predict recoveries or outcomes. Hack and others have argued that categorizing TBI using the GCS is “the equivalent of describing cancer as mild, moderate, and severe and then expecting that one treatment will cure all cancer” (13).

Currently, clinically practical in vivo neuroimaging tests sensitive to the type of structural tissue damage present in concussion remain lacking. When TBI is suspected in acute clinical settings, computed tomography and conventional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) may be performed to identify trauma-related abnormalities such as skull fracture, brain edema, or intracranial hemorrhage. However, in concussion, such abnormalities are uncommon, and positive neuroimaging findings fail to predict long-term outcomes (43, 48, 88).

While diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) has been widely used as a measure of in vivo white matter integrity (66, 89), DTI metrics are also influenced by and reflect various neuropathological changes including neuroinflammation and dynamic variation, depending on time post-injury and potential reparative influences during recovery (90, 91). Two recent systematic reviews of DTI studies of concussion in humans, with inferences about in vivo axonal injury and integrity in human subjects, emphasized a lack of high-quality data, missing control groups, and discrepant analytic techniques in the existing literature (36, 37). Few studies met all inclusion criteria for either analysis. Asken et al. (36) concluded that while DTI is sensitive to a wide range of group differences, it currently lacks the specificity necessary for meaningful clinical application in concussion. To address these limitations, more recent studies have included larger and more diverse sample sizes and controls not meeting criteria for mTBI but with either neuropsychiatric diagnoses or other health/injury-related problems (9294). Accordingly, future DTI studies hold considerable promise in overcoming the limitations mentioned earlier and providing objective neuroimaging correlates of brain injury and outcome.

Other methods have been used in attempt to measure and classify concussion. Event-related potentials (ERP) and quantitative EEG (QEEG) provide a direct window into the neurophysiology of the injured brain. Advantages compared to neuroimaging include portability, and high temporal and reasonable spatial resolution. QEEG has long been proposed as a potential means to aid in the diagnosis and prognosis of TBI of all severity grades (9598). ERP, in particular somatosensory-evoked potentials, have also been investigated in both animal models and human subjects with TBI, but its clinical utility is still controversial (99, 100).

Considerable effort has been devoted to identifying serum biomarkers to identify methods for diagnosis, vulnerability, recovery, and outcome, but the clinical utility of these measures also remains limited at present (101). In particular, promising research into glial fibrillary acidic protein and ubiquitin C-terminal hydrolase-L1 is being conducted, but so far neither marker have been shown to reliably predict concussion recovery (102, 103).

Toward New Approaches

Rosenbaum and Lipton (27) suggest that “ultimately, outcome will probably be most reliably predicted based on a complex system of clinical, pathological, and imaging variables.” Some have thus turned toward “big data” approaches. New attempts are underway to assemble large TBI data sets, both through multisite studies (104) and the compilation of existing data into shared repositories (105). For example, a recent collaboration between the National Collegiate Athletic Association and the US Department of Defense uses the Federal Interagency Traumatic Brain Injury Research (FITBIR) database to compare clinical and neurobiological recovery after concussion in student athletes and military personnel (106). These efforts are still in the early stages and face obstacles—including inherited and even amplified problems with constituent data sets—and are further complicated by the diverse perspectives of the many stakeholders involved, which include athletic and military organizations, universities, and others. Progress has also been made in establishing common data elements (107) to increase compatibility of data sets.

Group collaboration is complicated by the fact that concussion crosses multiple domains and contexts and does not fall within one medical specialty. Disparate specialties offer alternative perspectives and hypotheses about pathophysiological mechanisms, what constitutes recovery, and how to measure progress.

These concerns indicate a lack of a shared explanatory model, or idea of how concussion “works.” At issue is how we understand the full spectrum of brain injury (if it is indeed a spectrum), and how uncertainty and heterogeneity interact at various points. Many of the obstacles faced in research and clinical practice related to concussion, and TBI more broadly, ultimately stem from the complexity and heterogeneity of the condition itself. A systems perspective therefore could facilitate the creation of a shared explanatory framework—known in the systems literature as a mental model (108).

Systems Approaches to Medicine

In the past decade, systems approaches to medicine have emerged to address the complexity seen in conditions such as diabetes, cancer, and heart disease (2931, 109). Although several approaches exist, they all distinguish themselves from reductionist methods. While reductionist science is critical for answering well-defined empirical questions, it is less equipped to address questions involving greater uncertainty, complexity, and heterogeneity. In contrast, systems approaches to medicine seek to incorporate—rather than control for—the dimensions of context, space, and time (29). They emphasize the inclusion of all relevant factors to understand the function of the whole and recognize that system behavior is strongly influenced by causal structure (110, 111).

The application of systems biology to medicine has resulted in a growing area of research based on the “biology as information science” paradigm (31). This research—referred to as P4 medicine, personalized medicine, precision medicine, systems medicine, or systems biology—uses large amounts of high-throughput data (often genomic, proteomic, and other “omic” data) about an individual to develop personalized diagnosis and treatment. This approach has been used successfully for cancer and other diseases (112) and works best for diseases with strong intrinsic causal factors or known pathophysiological mechanisms (113). Other data-driven approaches, such as machine learning (114) and reconstructability analysis (115), are currently being used to identify complex, non-linear, and multivariate correlations in concussion. However, all of these approaches depend on the availability of large high-quality data sets, which are currently lacking.

Methods from the field of systems science are also increasingly applied to medicine. Systems science methods “enable investigators to examine the dynamic interrelationships of system components which may span multiple levels of analysis (e.g., from cells to society), while simultaneously studying the behavior of the system as a whole over time” (116). Rather than creating models directly from clinical data, this approach identifies model structure and parameters based on primary sources (e.g., published literature and expert interviews). Models are constructed qualitatively and then validated against clinical data. Models produced in this way have the advantage of offering a coherent hypothesis for how a system functions. As such, this approach “opens the black box” of a system and introduces a common discussion platform for diverse disciplines or stakeholders. In a medical context, these models can offer hypotheses for the behavior of populations (useful in public health), or for pathophysiological processes. Consequently, systems models can provide an approach to address the lack of a shared mental model for concussion. These models are also well suited for identifying points of leverage or intervention in complex systems. Systems science has been used in various contexts in public health (117), such as cardiovascular disease (118), obesity (119), and drug diversion and abuse (120). Recently, system dynamics modeling was used to better understand the drivers and dynamics of depression (121).

A systems focus has also begun to emerge from within disciplines. Systems neuroscience, for example, examines how neural circuits form whole systems and subsystems within the brain that relate to function and behavior (122). A recent series of articles in this journal calls attention to ways in which TBI can be studied from this perspective (123). A review by Bigler (124) in this series discusses a systems approach for examining the TBI spectrum, with a particular focus on how neuroimaging may inform multiple levels of inquiry. Here, we apply systems science methods to better understand injury pathophysiology and recovery in concussion.

Taking a Systems View of Concussion

We assert that many of the difficulties in concussion prognosis ultimately stem from the complex nature of the condition. While definitions of complex system vary, the term is widely understood to refer to systems in which the behavior of the whole is not entirely explained by the behavior of parts or subsystems (116). Complex systems often have many interrelated components at multiple scales and demonstrate non-linearity, feedback, dynamic change over time, and emergent properties (125). Rather than reducing or controlling for aspects of complexity in an issue like concussion, systems methods seek to incorporate them as properties of the system.

To take a systems approach, one must identify all relevant factors or variables in a system, articulate the relationships between those variables, and acknowledge system boundaries. Variables, relationships, and boundaries together constitute system structure, which in turn determines system behavior. This determination is not always straightforward; indeed, in complex systems, certain causal structures result in counterintuitive non-linear feedback and emergent behavior. Examining how behavior changes dynamically over time in turn gives insight into system structure and provides utility to stakeholders—particularly researchers and clinicians in the case of concussion. Choices regarding indicator variables and system boundaries are determined by the problem or question that drives the modeling effort and are therefore to some degree value-laden and influenced by the perspective of the modeler. As such, it is good practice to include experts and stakeholders in the modeling process.

Building a model of a complex system often requires a considerable amount of information. The modeler needs to know which variables to include and how to measure them, how to quantify the relationships between those variables, and how best to describe system behavior over time. Also required is data against which to verify the model. For systems with known variables, undisputed structure, and good reference data, models can be built with solid empirical support. Missing data or uncertainty or disagreement about system structure, however, limits one’s ability to build such a model. In these cases, understanding the complexity of the system also entails acknowledging and incorporating this uncertainty through qualitative approaches. Being able to incorporate expert judgment into a model of hypothesized causal structure allows for the examination of system-scale behavior even if variable-level data is sparse. It also allows us to examine the extent of agreement and identify gaps in existing knowledge and inconsistencies in mental models.

Here, we provide a conceptual framework that identifies system boundaries and variables influential to concussion recovery. This is a preliminary step in the modeling process and is intended to guide further inquiry into the complex nature of concussion recovery, including the construction of formal models. The lack of a shared mental model for the pathophysiology of concussion, disagreement over definition, diagnosis and recovery, failed clinical trials, and unavoidable patient and injury heterogeneity all support the need for a common conceptual framework. Such a conceptual framework should be flexible enough to account for individual differences while providing enough structure to enhance understanding, and may serve as a useful decision support tool in clinical and research settings.

Concussion at Multiple Scales

Systems approaches conceptualize complex biological systems as consisting of elements at multiple nested scales (126, 127). Figure 1 shows key aspects of concussion injury and recovery mapped across four scales: cellular, network, experiential, and social. Factors endogenous to the system—those that affect and are affected by other factors in the system—are included inside the scale boxes. Exogenous factors that drive the system from the outside are indicated at the margins, including those present at the time of injury (injury phenomena and biomechanics, personal characteristics, and injury context), as well as interventions that take place during recovery. Aspects of the ongoing environment influence factors at every scale and can themselves be affected. It should be noted that Figure 1 is a static representation of a dynamic, interconnected system and as such does not capture the full complexity of the system over time. It also does not capture the full extent of variables relevant to concussion recovery, nor the individual heterogeneity seen for these variables.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Multi-scale framework for concussion. Factors influencing concussion pathophysiology and recovery are shown across four nested emergent scales: cellular, network, experiential, and social. Endogenous factors—those that affect and are affected by other factors in the system—are included inside the scale boxes. Exogenous system drivers that act upon the system are shown at the perimeter. On the left are exogenous factors present at the time of injury (e.g., injury phenomena and biomechanics, personal characteristics, and injury context), while interventions on the right and top margins impact the system dynamically during the recovery process. Aspects of the ongoing environment influence factors at all scales. Feedback exists within and also between scales. Medium gray arrows indicate cross-scale interactions. Factors show emergence, increasing size, and longer time-scale moving up from the cellular to social levels. A team of systems scientists produced this diagram based on literature review, interviews with researchers and clinicians, and iterative review by subject matter experts.

Cellular Scale

This scale includes cellular processes (along with molecular subprocesses) most critical to concussion. Ionic flux leads to metabolic dysfunction and energy imbalances, which in turn contribute to additional neuroinflammatory responses to injury (38, 39). This cascade impairs neurotransmission in individual neurons. An additional cause of impaired neurotransmission is axonal injury caused by mechanical shearing and stretching forces (128, 129). Also impacted at the cellular scale are vascular integrity and glial cell function, which exacerbate existing damage and further increase neuroinflammation (68). A recently described gliovascular pathway for clearance of toxic proteins and metabolic wastes, the glymphatic system, may also be impaired in TBI and be exacerbated by sleep disturbances (130, 131).

Multiple feedback loops exist within the cellular level. For example, axonal injury increases further neuroinflammation via microglial activation (43, 132, 133). These variables and their interrelationships may be caught in cycles of regeneration and repair, or degeneration and dysfunction (128). Note that effects at this scale are not limited to the acute phase of injury; rather, biochemical and cellular changes can occur days and even years after a traumatic event, even after clinical symptoms resolve (134).

Figure 1 shows that injury characteristics—particularly injury biomechanics—directly impact the cellular level. Injury biomechanics include the objective measures of force, direction, and physics of impact as they are transferred through the skull into the brain. Personal characteristics such as genetics, age, and sex can also influence cellular-scale processes. For example, sex differences in neck stability render female athletes significantly more likely to experience a concussion compared to male athletes in the same sport (71, 72, 135). Pharmacological interventions (e.g., medications to address headache or comorbid muscle pain and tension) can also modulate the cellular state on an ongoing basis during the recovery process.

Overall, well-functioning processes at the cellular scale allow for communication between individual neurons. The net effect of the cellular level constitutes effective neurotransmission—the unified orchestration of cells in a particular cellular microenvironment, including not only individual neurons but also their associated microvasculature, glial cells, and various cytostructural matrix proteins in the intracellular and extracellular space. At the cellular level, successful neurotransmission is what allows for a neuron to communicate with other neurons and function in modular neuronal assemblies (136138).

Network Scale

Neurotransmission leads to the emergence of neuronal networks from the synchronized activity of many neurons either simultaneously or in patterned sequences. These neurons may send information via direct electrical or molecular signaling (structural connectivity networks) or through temporally synchronized activations or deactivations (functional connectivity networks). Since there is often no relation between the location or extent of visible focal injuries and symptoms, performance, or outcome in concussion (139), and individual cellular processes cannot yet be easily examined non-invasively in humans in vivo, an examination of networks may provide the missing link between overt behavior and individual molecular or cellular trauma (47, 48).

By merging structural connectivity analyses (e.g., DTI) with functional measures (e.g., fMRI), researchers have been able to reliably identify intrinsic connectivity networks—specific groups of brain areas that temporally synchronize to support given functions that may be close or far apart and may be activated or deactivated as individual nuclei or entire regions (137, 138, 140142).

An emerging literature demonstrates structural and functional reorganization in network connectivity within and between networks in concussion, particularly in the acute to early timeframe (36, 37, 48, 140, 143150). Evidence suggests that network alterations are particularly prominent in frontoparietal regions following concussion (4850, 151153), which may directly slow information processing (154). Electrophysiological findings demonstrate slowed information processing and changes in neuronal encoding and function over time as a result of brain injury (155). Notably, several studies have now identified alterations in resting state networks, a global interhemispheric and intrahemispheric network of brain regions that are consistently turned off during task-related activities while remaining more active during rest (142, 156). In concussed patients, these resting state networks are disrupted and unable to completely turn off, thereby affecting self-awareness, working memory, attention and performance (26, 49, 146, 147, 157159). For a more thorough review of neuroimaging and concussion see Bigler et al. (48) and Hayes et al. (144).

The neurobehavioral and neurocognitive sequelae of concussion relate to how and at what levels networks become affected (see Figure 2). In the concussed patient, damage to a major neuronal hub—which plays a central role in connectivity and information processing—can have a significant impact. But if the injury impacts a peripheral small node there may potentially be only a minimal disruption in function, especially if functional connectivity is restored by rerouting information through another intact pathway. The brain’s ability to reroute will depend on neuroplasticity, neural reserve, and other network properties of structural and functional connectivity [see Ref. (136, 138) for a description of cortical communication dynamics].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Concussion at the network scale. When investigating how information passes through a network (A,B), a node may represent neuronal activations, or network properties of gray or white matter in a given location. Connections between nodes may represent structural white matter tracts (i.e., groups of axons), temporal synchronization, or other functional relationships. Major hubs [shown in panel (B)] refer to regions that play a central role in connectivity and information processing. For any given structural or functional connectivity network, major hubs may be contrasted with less important hubs and peripheral nodes and edges. Comparison of magnetic resonance imaging scans from mild and severe TBI patients is shown in panel (C). The smaller the site of injury, the less likely the damage is to significantly interrupt information processing within a given network. In the concussed patient, if the injury impacts a peripheral small node, there might only be a minimal disruption in function, especially if the functional connectivity needed for recovery is restored via rerouting. In the severe TBI case, such workarounds will not be possible as the damage is too extensive. A network approach illustrates the possibility that severity or prognosis may be based on extent of network damage. Panels (A,B) were borrowed from Ref. (138), and panel (C) was borrowed from Ref. (124). Used with permission.

Neural reserve reflects the amount of brain damage that can be sustained before reaching a threshold for clinical expression; it is a passive measure correlating with early childhood education and socioeconomic status and may be represented by brain size or overall synapse count (78). If a person has high neural reserve, information signals may bypass areas of tissue damage easily to find another route to their intended destination, thereby reducing or even preventing any noticeable functional impairment. The smaller the site of injury, the less likely it is that the damage will significantly interrupt information processing within a given network. MRI scans from mild and severe TBI patients (Figure 2C) illustrate the possibility when taking this approach that severity or prognosis may be based on extent of network damage.

Significant alterations in timing or neuronal synchronization at the neuronal population level may impede activation or deactivation of entire brain regions, or disrupt global processing. Even the slightest change in timing can significantly disrupt network coordination of cognition (160), attention (152), and even the sleep/wake cycle (161, 162). It is important to note that the effects of brain injury are not solely determined by the area of damage; rather, subtle changes in timing or processing speed can have large effects downstream (160). For example, networks underlying postural control and gait require constant updating and integration of current sensorimotor and vestibular information for normal balance and walking to occur (163). In the concussed patient, these networks might not be damaged per se but instead are simply functioning at slower processing speeds, leading to inaccurate updating and therefore functional impairments in gait. Indeed, mapping and understanding the complexity of temporal synchronization in the brain is critical to defining damage and predicting recovery.

Further study of temporal network dynamics will be critical for understanding the heterogenous symptoms and impairments that arise in response to concussion [see Ref. (160) for a review]. Since networks provide the link between cellular insult and the felt experience of concussion, focus on specific types of network damage may help clinicians guide treatment. For example, Ghajar and Ivry (152, 164) have argued that TBI selectively impairs attentional networks in the cerebellum, frontal lobe, and parietal lobes involved in the generation, maintenance, and precise timing of predictive eye movements. These distributed networks are hypothesized to allow the brain to be predictive (as opposed to reactive) with regards to external visual stimuli. In the injured brain, disrupted timing in this network may shift the brain into a reactive mode, causing impairments in visual smooth pursuit (or predictive eye tracking), which one day may be used as a diagnostic marker for concussion. Indeed, examination of networks underlying automatic eye movements is a promising area of TBI research (165167).

However, it should be noted that despite widespread optimism that network science will identify neuroimaging biomarkers for concussion (47, 48, 50), this research still faces many of the obstacles of uncertainty and heterogeneity outlined earlier, as well as its own methodological challenges. Networks are dynamic and constantly changing, which complicates determinations of baseline. Moreover, researchers are only beginning to understand the relative contributions of particular networks to individual conscious experiences. Even seemingly singular experiential events (e.g., pain) involve the coordination of multiple networks (168). Delineating how to categorize and compare networks (e.g., unimodal versus crossmodal and inter- versus intrahemispheric), as well as connectivity between networks (i.e., hyperconnectivity versus hypoconnectivity) will be critical to understanding network changes associated with concussion.

Experiential Scale

Networks working in concert with one another enable the emergence of consciousness, which introduces variables at the experiential scale. This is the level at which an individual experiences the concussion as it plays out in time and awareness, through symptoms such as headache or disorientation. Dysfunction of networks in concussion influences psychological, emotional, and cognitive states, causing such problems as memory and language impairments, mood disruptions, and gait/balance issues (37, 47, 124). For example, changes in networks subserving the sleep/wake cycle manifest in felt experience as noticeable disturbances in attention, cognition or mood, as reflected in the hypothetical case examples shown later in Figure 3. Temporal asynchronies in neuronal communication lead to disruptions within and between brain networks that manifest experientially as cognitive dysfunction (136, 160). For example, changes in neuronal timing brought on by concussion have been shown to disrupt networks underlying smooth pursuit eye movements, disturbing perception and leading to self-reported feelings of being “out of sync” (152).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Multi-scale map of factors influencing recovery from concussion in two cases. In this comparative hypothetical example, two individuals present with sleep disruption of the same magnitude at the same time point after injury. Additional variables differentiate the cases. Factors are mapped across four nested scales and are linked with arrows indicating causal influence. The minus sign in the diagram for person A signifies a reduction in depression. While both persons show the same symptom (i.e., sleep disturbance), outcomes differ between the two cases according to distinct profiles of feedback relationships with other variables. Disrupted sleep will result in clinical depression for person A if left untreated, placing her at increased risk of dementia, given her medical history and age. Person B might develop persistent post-concussive symptoms if the compounding effects of stress and headache are not addressed. Default mode network is abbreviated DMN. This example illustrates the utility of considering scale and feedback in the clinical care of concussion.

Impairment in self-awareness is common after TBI and may occur due to decreased functional connectivity within frontoparietal control networks that are also associated with deficits in attention and performance monitoring (49, 164). This disruption in self-awareness can impact a person’s ability to successfully report on her own symptoms and can influence how she perceives and interacts with her environment. These disturbances are influenced by the individual’s premorbid emotional and psychological functioning (169, 170). In patients with comorbid injuries or a history of chronic pain, comorbid pain or muscle tension can disturb perception further, increasing psychological distress and interfering with daily function. Several experts interviewed in the course of our research emphasized how perception of one’s injury and expectations for recovery can profoundly shape experience. Also influential is how a person copes with or adapts to their symptoms and deficits. Coping and adaptation skills may be learned but can also be facilitated at the neuronal level by cognitive reserve. Influenced by neural reserve, cognitive reserve impacts one’s ability to adjust to brain injury through various compensatory mechanisms or adaptations [see Ref. (78) for a review].

Personal characteristics such as psychiatric history (e.g., mood disorders or PTSD) and personal resilience also influence how people experience concussion (79). These factors are also thought to influence the brain and body’s response to injury at the cellular and network levels (171). The injury context—the setting in which the injury took place—may impact one’s perception of the injury, as well as determine how quickly they are evaluated, receive treatment, and return to play or work. In addition, the demands of the environment shape recovery in an ongoing way, particularly in the presentation of symptoms and efficacy of interventions. For example, a person living in a combat zone or other high-stress situation might not be able to take as much time for cognitive rest as a person in a low-stress home environment. Tasks required to obtain medical care or pursue litigation can also be stressors. Physical or sensory aspects of the immediate physical environment greatly influence certain symptoms, such as headache and cognitive fatigue. Individuals with light sensitivity may only experience their symptoms in lighted environments, but not when they are sitting in a dark room. Thus, to understand the heterogeneity, uncertainty, and complexity in the conscious, felt experience of concussion, we must understand how these variables appear, disappear, and are modulated by personal characteristics, history, and context.

Social Scale

A comprehensive map of concussion necessarily includes the social level of the system. This scale encompasses the manner in which relationships and interactions with other people impact an individual’s injury or recovery, and acknowledges that many aspects of concussive symptoms are determined intersubjectively. While these factors exist externally to the individual, they are included here as endogenous to the system of injury because they substantively influence—and are influenced by—properties of the individual. The robustness of social relationships with family and friends, as well as the support provided by these relationships, can play a significant role in recovery from TBI (172, 173). Support is needed to meet logistical needs (e.g., transportation and household tasks), as well as emotional needs, which may be elevated due to the psychological and emotional toll of migraine, sleep disruption, and cognitive or language impairments.

Quality of social relationships has been shown to strongly influence prognosis and recovery in cancer (174) and is likely to have similar effects in TBI. Similarly, research in cancer populations has repeatedly demonstrated that social factors, particularly physician communication styles, are powerful determinants of outcome and symptom expression (175178). Given the complexity of concussion as an oftentimes “invisible” injury (179), it stands to reason that social norms, expressions, and communication styles surrounding the condition likely contribute to its observed heterogeneity and should not be ignored. Indeed, patient-directed educational materials about concussion, including pamphlets and information sheets received at medical discharge, may profoundly impact symptoms and deficits (181).

A person’s engagement in group settings such as work or school can also play a role (positively or negatively), as can broad social factors such as community and norms. Lack of support and pressure to perform or meet social demands can negatively influence recovery. In parallel with neural and cognitive reserve, we use the term social reserve to indicate the extent to which an individual’s social relationships serve to buffer the negative effects of the injury and promote coping and healing. The larger social context of the injury also shapes social dynamics. For example, a playoff game and a motor vehicle accident are contexts with different social demands. During recovery, the ongoing environment continually shapes social contexts and social expression.

The feedback from the experiential scale to the social scale is significant; a person experiencing post-concussive impairments in cognitive functioning, mood, and working memory may be compromised in his ability to function successfully in social environments, which can weaken social relationships and reduce engagement in group settings (181). Headache and the need to avoid overstimulating environments provide further obstacles to maintenance of social relationships. When a concussion is sustained in sports or military contexts, dynamics of inclusion and duty may alter when and how symptoms are expressed.

Emergence across Scales

These scales show emergence in the sense that the interaction between elements at one scale gives rise to entities at a larger scale (182). Definitions of emergence vary widely. In a commonly cited definition, Goldstein refers to emergence as “the arising of novel and coherent structures, patterns and properties during the process of self-organization in complex systems” (183). Networks have distinct emergent properties of their own, but cannot exist without functioning individual neurons and circuits. Likewise, a person’s conscious experience depends on the functioning of underlying brain networks. When damage to networks results in loss of consciousness, for example, all phenomena above the network scale are temporarily suspended.

When functionality is compromised but not completely suspended, the effects might only be evident at higher levels. While slight disruptions in network timing would not be sufficient for networks to go offline, they could, for example, cause a person to feel “out of sync” or experience balance problems. In a similar vein, certain psychological impairments might not be noticed by the concussed individual but are evident to others close to him. The scales are nested in the sense that smaller-scale phenomena take place within the context of phenomena at larger scales. From bottom to top, the size of the elements at each scale increases, as does the relevant time frame at which action is observed.

Feedback between Scales

Importantly, these scales do not exist in isolation, nor are their interactions limited to upward linear emergence. Factors influencing concussion recovery are highly interconnected and can show feedback, which is evident when the output or outcomes influenced by a variable ultimately return to and influence that variable. For example, headaches can cause increased stress, which can cause additional headaches (184). This kind of feedback results in non-linear system dynamics. Such system behavior can be counterintuitive and difficult to predict. Most statistical analysis methods cannot easily account for non-linearity, which may partly explain why TBI research has yet to discover measures that accurately predict outcome (124).

Figure 3 shows how one symptom—disrupted sleep—exists within a constellation of factors influencing the recovery of two hypothetical individuals. Both individuals suffered concussions and experienced post-concussive symptoms at 3 months post-injury. In both cases, disrupted sleep is at the center of two feedback loops: one in which sleep disruption causes fatigue and mood disturbance, which further impacts sleep, and another in which decreased glymphatic clearance results in toxic cellular buildup, exacerbating fatigue. The other factors at play in each case are different: for person A, an elderly woman who suffered a concussion from a fall in her home, a history of depression as well as additional network damage to the default mode network and limbic systems put her at risk for acute mood disruption, and lead to clinical depression. Tendency toward depression in this case is reduced, however, by strong social support and social reserve. Person A’s advanced age also reduces the efficiency of glymphatic clearing, which puts her at increased risk for dementia via amyloid plaque deposition. Person B, a young man injured during football practice, experiences stress and headaches, which compound one another and worsen in environments with loud noises, bright lights, or prolonged screen time. These symptoms combined with vestibular–ocular network dysfunction prevent him from fully returning to play and school. A recent prior concussion caused residual hypometabolism, which increased the amount of axonal damage he suffered during the second injury. Because he is away from home and may not have many supportive relationships outside of football, he is considered to have low social reserve which, combined with social pressure to return to play, creates a negative social environment that increases his stress. Outcomes for both patients are potentially negative without early intervention, but the unique constellation of factors in both cases produces differing profiles of symptoms and deficits.

The feedback relationships formed by these interlinkages exert influences that persist over time. Articulating these dynamics can aid in understanding an individual’s trajectory of recovery and help to identify possible leverage points and interventions in a clinical setting. By considering system drivers, an astute clinician might determine that for person A, sleep disturbance and a history of depression puts her at greater risk for acute mood disruption to turn into full-blown depression and should therefore be more aggressively treated. A clinician treating person B, whose vestibulo-ocular and headache symptoms might typically be treated with medication, could be inspired by a systems perspective to also look into improving social support for his patient, or provide education about how to deal with overstimulating environments.

Measurement and Limitations to Knowledge

Ontological differences at each scale lead to epistemological differences. Because dysfunction can happen at any scale, a wide variety of measures are used in research and clinical practice to assess injury and impairment (see Table 1). Measures at each scale often have common challenges, which are shaped by the types of knowledge obtainable at that level and methodological constraints.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Assessment of concussion across scales.

Uncertainty in measurement is compounded by a more fundamental uncertainty about exactly what is being measured in the first place. Without a shared understanding of the etiology (or etiologies) of concussion, decisions about what to measure—and when and how to measure it—vary widely. This diversity in assessment presents obstacles for secondary analysis.

Mapping the Landscape of Concussion

A multi-scale systems model of concussion can serve as a framework for synthesizing data and knowledge about correlations between variables to visualize a “landscape” of factors, within which patterns or subgroups might be identified (Figure 4). These subgroups might differ in etiology or presentation and ideally would be distinguished using one or more biomarkers. But because it is presently unknown whether heterogeneity in signs, symptoms, and deficits is intrinsic to concussion or simply an artifact of data collection or improper patient groupings, a multi-scale framework can be a first step to navigating what is observed. Organizing the “black box” of concussion into scales can allow for the identification of methodological constraints and knowledge limitations at each scale, and then aid in the refinement of research questions and implementation.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Identifying patterns of variables across multiple scales in concussion. Three subgroups are shown using dashed, solid, and dotted lines to indicate relationships between variables in concussion, mapped across four nested scales. This framework can be utilized for synthesizing existing data and knowledge in concussion, based on systematic review or big data analytics. Relationships between variables can be organized to visualize a “landscape” of factors, within which patterns or subgroups might be identified. This subgrouping would allow better phenotyping for the design, recruitment, and analysis of clinical trials and might enable reanalysis of failed drug trials to distinguish responders from non-responders. The ultimate goal of such a model is better clinical prognostication of outcomes following concussion and therefore more personalized treatments.

While emerging research on imaging and serum biomarkers is promising, we cannot presuppose that a single “silver bullet” biomarker will be identified. Indeed, in systems medicine, disease heterogeneity is seen as necessitating multiple markers (185). An expanded conceptual framework for concussion provides a way to contextualize possible biomarkers and see how the variables they measure are interconnected with other relevant factors.

Informed by data-driven efforts, existing clinical studies, and expert opinion, the TBI community could use this initial framework to help develop a shared hypothesis of the pathophysiology and factors influencing concussion recovery. Big data approaches can contribute to this mapping and use the framework to help interpret results. Indeed, iteration between predictive and explanatory models can be beneficial to both endeavors (186). Given the present failure of multisite clinical trials, this framework may also be used to compare data sets from multiple research sites, or contrast data from distinct modes of injury (e.g., military, athletics, and auto accidents) or time periods within recovery. Similarly, one may compare cases of better and worse prognosis to help identify variables most critical to recovery outcomes, and tailor treatments or interventions at that level.

In addition to increasing our overall understanding of this complex, traumatic syndrome, the framework may also contribute to clarifying the definition of concussion and to help develop a more nuanced classification of concussion to better facilitate personalization of treatment and to sharpen clinical trials. A systems model cannot replace a classification system, as the identification of subtypes or patterns with which to classify patient populations is necessary for clinical and research applications. But by providing a way to articulate hypothesized causal relationships in concussion, it can serve as a decision support tool for the TBI community. Because a systems model reflects the ontological structure of the phenomenon, it can serve as a foundation for future inquiry, including more sophisticated modeling efforts.

Decision Support for Research and Practice

Systems models can serve as powerful tools for synthesizing information, which can provide decision support to researchers and clinicians by encouraging whole-systems thinking, facilitating communication across specialties, and supporting the development of shared hypotheses to identify potential treatment interventions. A student with sound sensitivity, for example, could be offered a solitary work space that would be quieter but more socially isolated, or she could be given earplugs and remain in the regular classroom. Both situations allow for more focus and fewer headaches, but the setting with more social interaction may be better for recovery. For an athlete with post-concussive concentration or attention deficits, the clinician might probe deeper about aspects of the patient’s ongoing environment to see if the deficits are due to cognitive deficits, altered sleep, distracting pain, or stress. Attempts to improve sleep hygiene, reduce pressure for return to play, manage pain, or increase cognitive rest might be helpful before or conjunction with prescribing attention-related medication.

A systems approach can also help identify unintended consequences. Suppose a patient has difficulty concentrating, headache pain, and a preexisting sleep disorder that is worsened after suffering a concussion. The physician has prescribed cognitive rest to help with concentration and exposure to stimulating environments but has not considered that reduced movement and increased downtime can exacerbate sleep problems and disrupt nocturnal rhythms. According to the above framework, the clinician has responded to symptoms at the experiential level without considering underlying processes related to sleep disruption and glymphatic clearing at the network and cellular levels. A more comprehensive approach incorporating physical exercise, cognitive rest, and behavioral sleep hygiene may be needed.

An awareness of the multi-scale nature of concussion and the key variables at each scale may help researchers identify gaps in the literature and better appreciate how individual investigations fit within the larger picture. Considering effects between levels, and drawing attention to understudied relationships, may help to sharpen inclusion and exclusion criteria for clinical trials.

Systems models—particularly diagrams—can facilitate discussion between health-care providers from different medical disciplines. Neurosurgeons and psychologists have different lexicons and research practices, for example, but both may be able communicate their knowledge using a diagram format organized according to causal or ontological structure. This kind of object—known as a boundary object in the science studies literature—can be used to identify research gaps and questions, synthesize a body of knowledge, and communicate with stakeholders (187). Systems models also encourage the identification of constructs that would be important to understand the system but might not be well defined in the literature. For example, in our research, the concept of bandwidth was repeatedly identified by clinicians as important to their patients but is not well defined in the literature. Further analysis could investigate the mechanisms behind this concept.

Future Directions

The multi-scale framework presented in this article is the first part of a larger project applying systems science approaches to concussion. The next step will be to construct a causal-loop diagram, a more precise conceptual model in which relationships between specific system variables and feedback dynamics are made explicit (manuscript in preparation). This diagram will serve as a map of the current knowledge about concussion pathophysiology and recovery. Future work could involve developing a computational system dynamics model, which would introduce the dimension of time and allow for a closer examination of recovery trajectories.

Conclusion

Accurate diagnosis, prognosis, and effective treatment of TBI—particularly concussion—are hindered by an imprecise classification system, a dearth of high-quality clinical trials, and lack of a shared, evidence-based working model of concussion pathophysiology and recovery within the medical community. A shared conceptual framework for concussion is needed to facilitate interdisciplinary communication and understanding of concussion, identify patterns and gaps in existing knowledge, and contribute to ongoing efforts to develop a new classification system for TBI that is more suitable for concussion diagnosis and treatment. Ultimately, our understanding of concussion will depend on the ability to account for patient and injury heterogeneity, dynamic non-linear feedback, and emergent properties intrinsic to consciousness. Systems science approaches can provide novel and useful contributions to the study of TBI and may provide a starting point for a paradigm shift in our conceptual grasp of concussion in all its complexity.

Author Contributions

EK was the lead author for the article (text and diagrams), co-lead for conception and design, and conducted expert interviews and literature review. EP was the lead for literature review, co-lead for conception and design, contributed substantially to the article, and conducted expert interviews. EB, ML, JC, and WW provided critical review and revision of the article and contributed to conception and design. WW also conducted expert interviews and review of the literature.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Brain Trauma Foundation, the Brain Trauma Evidence-based Consortium, and the associated team of expert clinicians and researchers for iterative feedback and support during this multiyear collaboration. The authors would also like to thank the TBI experts they interviewed, as well as Nancy Carney for initiating this project and for leading a paradigm shift in the study of TBI.

Funding

This material is based on work supported by the US Army Contracting Command, Aberdeen Proving Ground, Natick Contracting Division, through a contract awarded to Stanford University (W911QY-14-C-0086), and a subcontract awarded to the Brain Trauma Foundation. This material is in part the result of work supported with resources and the use of facilities at the VA Portland Health Care System, the VA Career Development Award #IK2 BX002712, and Portland VA Research Foundation (ML). In part, the Brain Imaging and Behavior Lab that EB directs was supported by the Army Medical Research and Material Command under Award No. W81XWH-13-2-0095 (Chronic Effects of Neurotrauma Consortium). Also, EB co-directs the Neuropsychological Assessment and Research Laboratory at Brigham Young University, which includes forensic consultation. Any opinions, findings and conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the US Army Contracting Command, Aberdeen Proving Ground, Natick Contracting Division, Stanford University, the Brain Trauma Foundation, U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs, or the United States Government.

Footnotes

  1. ^The extent to which mTBI and concussion can be used synonymously is debated in the literature (7). Here, we use them interchangeably. We primarily use the term concussion because although uncertainty exists regarding its definition, it is flexible enough to allow for a new classification system [whereas mTBI is tied to the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS)].
  2. ^The authors conducted 26 semi-structured interviews, each lasting 1.5–2 h, with researchers and clinicians in the field of TBI. Nearly all of the interviews were conducted by two interviewers, recorded, transcribed, and analyzed to identify claims useful to the development of the systems diagrams. Data from these interviews provided background knowledge for the current publication and will be used in the development of future systems models.

References

1. Arbogast KB, Curry AE, Pfeiffer MR, Zonfrillo MR, Haarbauer-Krupa J, Breiding MJ, et al. Point of health care entry for youth with concussion within a large pediatric care network. JAMA Pediatr (2016) 170:e160294. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.0294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bazarian JJ, McClung J, Shah MN, Cheng YT, Flesher W, Kraus J. Mild traumatic brain injury in the United States, 1998–2000. Brain Inj (2005) 19:85–91. doi:10.1080/02699050410001720158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. CDC. Injury Prevention & Control: Traumatic Brain Injury & Concussion. (2016). Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/traumaticbraininjury/basics.html

Google Scholar

4. Faul M, Xu L, Wald MM, Coronado VG. Traumatic Brain Injury in the United States: Emergency Department Visits, Hospitalizations and Deaths 2002-2006. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2010). Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/traumaticbraininjury/pdf/blue_book.pdf

Google Scholar

5. Cassidy JD, Carroll L, Peloso P, Borg J, von Holst H, Holm L, et al. Incidence, risk factors and prevention of mild traumatic brain injury: results of the WHO collaborating centre task force on mild traumatic brain injury. J Rehabil Med (2004) 36:28–60. doi:10.1080/16501960410023732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Numminen HJ. The incidence of traumatic brain injury in an adult population – how to classify mild cases? Eur J Neurol (2011) 18:460–4. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2010.03179.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. McCrory P, Meeuwisse WH, Aubry M, Cantu B, Dvorak J, Echemendia RJ, et al. Consensus statement on concussion in sport: the 4th International Conference on Concussion in Sport held in Zurich, November 2012. Br J Sports Med (2013) 47:250–8. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2013-092313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. NPR. Poll: Nearly 1 in 4 Americans Reports Having Had a Concussion. (2016). Available from: http://www.npr.org/sections/health-shots/2016/05/31/479750268/poll-nearly-1-in-4-americans-report-having-had-a-concussion

Google Scholar

9. Delaney JS, Abuzeyad F, Correa JA, Foxford R. Recognition and characteristics of concussions in the emergency department population. J Emerg Med (2005) 29:189–97. doi:10.1016/j.jemermed.2005.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Margulies S, Hicks R. Combination therapies for traumatic brain injury: prospective considerations. J Neurotrauma (2009) 26:925–39. doi:10.1089/neu.2008-0794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mayer AR, Quinn DK, Master CL. The spectrum of mild traumatic brain injury: a review. Neurology (2017) 89:1–10. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000004214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Stein DG. Embracing failure: what the phase III progesterone studies can teach about TBI clinical trials. Brain Inj (2015) 29:1259–72. doi:10.3109/02699052.2015.1065344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hack D. TBI clinical trials: past, present, and future. Bridge Natl Acad Eng (2016) 46:34–8.

Google Scholar

14. ASPE. Examination of Clinical Trial Costs and Barriers for Drug Development. (2014). Available from: https://aspe.hhs.gov/report/examination-clinical-trial-costs-and-barriers-drug-development

Google Scholar

15. Bigler ED. Neuropsychology and clinical neuroscience of persistent post-concussive syndrome. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2008) 14:1–22. doi:10.1017/S135561770808017X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Hall RCW, Hall RCW, Chapman MJ. Definition, diagnosis, and forensic implications of postconcussional syndrome. Psychosomatics (2005) 46:195–202. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.46.3.195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Iverson GL. Outcome from mild traumatic brain injury. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2005) 18:301–17. doi:10.1097/01.yco.0000165601.29047.ae

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. McInnes K, Friesen CL, MacKenzie DE, Westwood DA, Boe SG. Mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) and chronic cognitive impairment: a scoping review. PLoS One (2017) 12:e0174847. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0174847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. McMahon P, Hricik A, Yue JK, Puccio AM, Inoue T, Lingsma HF, et al. Symptomatology and functional outcome in mild traumatic brain injury: results from the prospective TRACK-TBI study. J Neurotrauma (2014) 31:26–33. doi:10.1089/neu.2013.2984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Tator C, Davis H, Dufort P, Tartaglia M, Davis K, Abraheem A, et al. Postconcussion syndrome: demographics and predictors in 221 patients. J Neurosurg (2016) 125:1206–16. doi:10.3171/2015.6.JNS15664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hiploylee C, Dufort PA, Davis HS, Wennberg RA, Tartaglia MC, Mikulis D, et al. Longitudinal study of postconcussion syndrome: not everyone recovers. J Neurotrauma (2017) 34:1511–23. doi:10.1089/neu.2016.4677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hawryluk GWJ, Bullock MR. Design of acute neuroprotection studies. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 128:761–78. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-63521-1.00047-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lingsma HF, Roozenbeek B, Steyerberg EW, Murray GD, Maas AI. Early prognosis in traumatic brain injury: from prophecies to predictions. Lancet Neurol (2010) 9:543–54. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70065-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Maas AIR, Lingsma HF, Roozenbeek B. Predicting outcome after traumatic brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 128:455–74. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-63521-1.00029-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dikmen S, Machamer J, Temkin N. Mild traumatic brain injury: longitudinal study of cognition, functional status, and post-traumatic symptoms. J Neurotrauma (2016) 34:1524–30. doi:10.1089/neu.2016.4618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Palacios EM, Yuh EL, Chang Y-S, Yue JK, Schnyer DM, Okonkwo DO, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity alterations associated with six-month outcomes in mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2017) 34:1546–57. doi:10.1089/neu.2016.4752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Rosenbaum SB, Lipton ML. Embracing chaos: the scope and importance of clinical and pathological heterogeneity in mTBI. Brain Imaging Behav (2012) 6:255–82. doi:10.1007/s11682-012-9162-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Marklund N, Hillered L. Animal modelling of traumatic brain injury in preclinical drug development: where do we go from here? Br J Pharmacol (2011) 164:1207–29. doi:10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.01163.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ahn AC, Tewari M, Poon C-S, Phillips RS. The clinical applications of a systems approach. PLoS Med (2006) 3:e209. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0030209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ahn AC, Tewari M, Poon C-S, Phillips RS. The limits of reductionism in medicine: could systems biology offer an alternative? PLoS Med (2006) 3:e208. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0030208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hood L. Systems biology and P4 medicine: past, present, and future. Rambam Maimonides Med J (2013) 4:e0012. doi:10.5041/RMMJ.10112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Carney N, Ghajar J, Jagoda A, Bedrick S, Davis-O’Reilly C, duCoudray H, et al. Executive summary of concussion guidelines step 1: systematic review of prevalent indicators. Neurosurgery (2014) 75:S1–2. doi:10.1227/NEU.0000000000000434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Carroll LJ, Cassidy JD, Holm L, Kraus J, Coronado VG; WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Methodological issues and research recommendations for mild traumatic brain injury: the WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. J Rehabil Med (2004) 36(43 Suppl):113–25. doi:10.1080/16501960410023877

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cassidy JD. Epidemiology of mild brain injury: quality of the scientific evidence. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2010) 81:e12. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2010.217554.28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bigler ED, Farrer TJ, Pertab JL, James K, Petrie JA, Hedges DW. Reaffirmed limitations of meta-analytic methods in the study of mild traumatic brain injury: a response to Rohling et al. Clin Neuropsychol (2013) 27:176–214. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.693950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Asken BM, DeKosky ST, Clugston JR, Jaffee MS, Bauer RM. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) findings in adult civilian, military, and sport-related mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI): a systematic critical review. Brain Imaging Behav (2017) 1–28. doi:10.1007/s11682-017-9708-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Oehr L, Anderson J. Diffusion-tensor imaging findings and cognitive function following hospitalised mixed-mechanism mild traumatic brain injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2017). doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2017.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Giza CC, Hovda DA. The neurometabolic cascade of concussion. J Athl Train (2001) 36(3):228–35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

39. Giza CC, Hovda DA. The new neurometabolic cascade of concussion. Neurosurgery (2014) 75:S24–33. doi:10.1227/NEU.0000000000000505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gardner A, Iverson GL, Stanwell P. A systematic review of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy findings in sport-related concussion. J Neurotrauma (2014) 31:1–18. doi:10.1089/neu.2013.3079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Henry LC, Tremblay S, Boulanger Y, Ellemberg D, Lassonde M. Neurometabolic changes in the acute phase after sports concussions correlate with symptom severity. J Neurotrauma (2010) 27:65–76. doi:10.1089/neu.2009.0962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Smith DH, Johnson VE, Stewart W. Chronic neuropathologies of single and repetitive TBI: substrates of dementia? Nat Rev Neurol (2013) 9:211–21. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2013.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bigler ED, Maxwell WL. Neuropathology of mild traumatic brain injury: relationship to neuroimaging findings. Brain Imaging Behav (2012) 6:108–36. doi:10.1007/s11682-011-9145-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bandak FA, Ling G, Bandak A, De Lanerolle NC. Injury biomechanics, neuropathology, and simplified physics of explosive blast and impact mild traumatic brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:89–104. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00006-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Chen Y, Huang W, Constantini S. The differences between blast-induced and sports-related brain injuries. Front Neurol (2013) 4:119. doi:10.3389/fneur.2013.00119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ling G, Ecklund JM, Bandak FA. Brain injury from explosive blast: description and clinical management. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:173–80. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00011-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Barbey AK, Belli A, Logan A, Rubin R, Zamroziewicz M, Operskalski JT. Network topology and dynamics in traumatic brain injury. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2015) 4:92–102. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Bigler ED, Abildskov TJ, Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Black G, Christensen ZP, Huff BS, et al. Structural neuroimaging findings in mild traumatic brain injury. Sports Med Arthrosc (2016) 24:e42–52. doi:10.1097/JSA.0000000000000119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Han K, Mac Donald CL, Johnson AM, Barnes Y, Wierzechowski L, Zonies D, et al. Disrupted modular organization of resting-state cortical functional connectivity in U.S. military personnel following concussive “mild” blast-related traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage (2014) 84:76–96. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mondello S, Hayes RL. Biomarkers. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:245–65. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00016-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Robinson ME, Lindemer ER, Fonda JR, Milberg WP, McGlinchey RE, Salat DH. Close-range blast exposure is associated with altered functional connectivity in Veterans independent of concussion symptoms at time of exposure. Hum Brain Mapp (2015) 36:911–22. doi:10.1002/hbm.22675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Shin SS, Bales JW, Dixon CE, Hwang M. Structural imaging of mild traumatic brain injury may not be enough: overview of functional and metabolic imaging of mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging Behav (2017) 11:591–610. doi:10.1007/s11682-017-9684-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Collins MW, Lovell MR, Iverson GL, Cantu RC, Maroon JC, Field MM. Cumulative effects of concussion in high school athletes. Neurosurgery (2002) 51:1175–81. doi:10.1097/00006123-200211000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Guskiewicz KM, McCrea M, Marshall SW, Cantu RC, Randolph C, Barr W, et al. Cumulative effects associated with recurrent concussion in collegiate football players: the NCAA concussion study. JAMA (2003) 290:2549–55. doi:10.1001/jama.290.19.2549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. LaRoche AA, Nelson LD, Connelly PK, Walter KD, McCrea MA. Sport-related concussion reporting and state legislative effects. Clin J Sport Med (2016) 26:33–9. doi:10.1097/JSM.0000000000000192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. McCrea M, Prichep L, Powell MR, Chabot R, Barr WB. Acute effects and recovery after sport-related concussion: a neurocognitive and quantitative brain electrical activity study. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2010) 25:283–92. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181e67923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Comper P, Bisschop SM, Carnide N, Tricco A. A systematic review of treatments for mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj (2005) 19:863–80. doi:10.1080/02699050400025042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Hawryluk GWJ, Manley GT. Classification of traumatic brain injury: past, present, and future. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:15–21. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00002-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Slobounov S, Bazarian J, Bigler E, Cantu R, Hallett M, Harbaugh R, et al. Sports-related concussion: ongoing debate. Br J Sports Med (2014) 48:75–6. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2013-092362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci (2011) 15:483–506. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cantu RC. Posttraumatic retrograde and anterograde amnesia: pathophysiology and implications in grading and safe return to play. J Athl Train (2001) 36:244–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

62. Harmon KG. Assessment and management of concussion in sports. Am Fam Physician (1999) 60:887–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

63. Abbas K, Shenk TE, Poole VN, Breedlove EL, Leverenz LJ, Nauman EA, et al. Alteration of default mode network in high school football athletes due to repetitive subconcussive mild traumatic brain injury: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Connect (2014) 5:91–101. doi:10.1089/brain.2014.0279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Blennow K, Hardy J, Zetterberg H. The neuropathology and neurobiology of traumatic brain injury. Neuron (2012) 76:886–99. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Budinger TF. An overview of concussion: history and needed research. Bridge Natl Acad Eng (2016) 46:5–22.

Google Scholar

66. Koerte IK, Lin AP, Willems A, Muehlmann M, Hufschmidt J, Coleman MJ, et al. A review of neuroimaging findings in repetitive brain trauma. Brain Pathol (2015) 25:318–49. doi:10.1111/bpa.12249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Omalu B. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy. Prog Neurol Surg (2014) 28:38–49. doi:10.1159/000358761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Patterson ZR, Holahan MR. Understanding the neuroinflammatory response following concussion to develop treatment strategies. Front Cell Neurosci (2012) 6:58. doi:10.3389/fncel.2012.00058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Duhaime A-C, Rindler RS. Special considerations in infants and children. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:219–42. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00015-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Cancelliere C, Donovan J, Cassidy JD. Is sex an indicator of prognosis after mild traumatic brain injury: a systematic analysis of the findings of the World Health Organization Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and the International Collaboration on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Prognosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2016) 97:S5–18. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2014.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Dick RW. Is there a gender difference in concussion incidence and outcomes? Br J Sports Med (2009) 43(Suppl 1):i46–50. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2009.058172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Munivenkatappa A, Agrawal A, Shukla DP, Kumaraswamy D, Devi BI. Traumatic brain injury: does gender influence outcomes? Int J Crit Illn Inj Sci (2016) 6:70. doi:10.4103/2229-5151.183024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Isokuortti H, Iverson GL, Kataja A, Brander A, Öhman J, Luoto TM. Who gets head trauma or recruited in mild traumatic brain injury research? J Neurotrauma (2015) 33:232–41. doi:10.1089/neu.2015.3888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Davidson J, Cusimano MD, Bendena WG. Post-traumatic brain injury genetic susceptibility to outcome. Neuroscientist (2015) 21:424–41. doi:10.1177/1073858414543150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Lipsky RH, Lin M. Genetic predictors of outcome following traumatic brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:23–41. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00003-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Katz DI, Cohen SI, Alexander MP. Mild traumatic brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:131–56. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00009-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Kelly JP. Loss of consciousness: pathophysiology and implications in grading and safe return to play. J Athl Train (2001) 36:249–52.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

78. Bigler ED, Stern Y. Traumatic brain injury and reserve. In: Salazar AM, editor. Handbook of Clinical Neurology Traumatic Brain Injury, Part II. Waltham, MA: Elsevier (2015). p. 691–710.

Google Scholar

79. McCauley SR, Wilde EA, Miller ER, Frisby ML, Garza HM, Varghese R, et al. Preinjury resilience and mood as predictors of early outcome following mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2012) 30:642–52. doi:10.1089/neu.2012.2393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Masel BE, DeWitt DS. Traumatic brain injury: a disease process, not an event. J Neurotrauma (2010) 27:1529–40. doi:10.1089/neu.2010.1358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Henry LC, Tremblay S, De Beaumont L. Long-term effects of sports concussions: bridging the neurocognitive repercussions of the injury with the newest neuroimaging data. Neuroscientist (2016) 23:567–78. doi:10.1177/1073858416651034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Honce J, Nyberg E, Jones I, Nagae L. Neuroimaging of concussion. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am (2016) 27:411–28. doi:10.1016/j.pmr.2016.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Lancaster M, Olson D, McCrea M, Nelson L, LaRoach A, Muftuler L. Acute white matter changes following sport-related concussion: a serial diffusion tensor and diffusion kurtosis tensor imaging study. Hum Brain Mapp (2016) 37:3821–34. doi:10.1002/hbm.23278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Wright DW, Yeatts SD, Silbergleit R. Progesterone in traumatic brain injury. N Engl J Med (2015) 372:1766–7. doi:10.1056/NEJMc1503138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. The Lancet (1974) 304:81–4. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Gaddis GM, Gaddis ML. Non-normality of distribution of Glasgow Coma Scores and Revised Trauma Scores. Ann Emerg Med (1994) 23:75–80. doi:10.1016/S0196-0644(94)70012-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Chung P, Khan F. Traumatic brain injury (TBI): overview of diagnosis and treatment. J Neurol Neurophysiol (2013) 5:1–10. doi:10.4172/2155-9562.1000182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Koerte IK, Hufschmidt J, Muehlmann M, Lin AP, Shenton ME. Advanced neuroimaging of mild traumatic brain injury. In: Laskowitz D, Grant G, editors. Translational Research in Traumatic Brain Injury. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor and Francis Group (2016).

Google Scholar

89. Hulkower MB, Poliak DB, Rosenbaum SB, Zimmerman ME, Lipton ML. A decade of DTI in traumatic brain injury: 10 years and 100 articles later. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol (2013) 34:2064–74. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A3395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Skinner NP, Kurpad SN, Schmit BD, Budde MD. Detection of acute nervous system injury with advanced diffusion-weighted MRI: a simulation and sensitivity analysis. NMR Biomed (2015) 28:1489–506. doi:10.1002/nbm.3405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Tu T-W, Williams RA, Lescher JD, Jikaria N, Turtzo LC, Frank JA. Radiological-pathological correlation of diffusion tensor and magnetization transfer imaging in a closed head traumatic brain injury model. Ann Neurol (2016) 79:907–20. doi:10.1002/ana.24641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Churchill NW, Caverzasi E, Graham SJ, Hutchison MG, Schweizer TA. White matter microstructure in athletes with a history of concussion: comparing diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging (NODDI). Hum Brain Mapp (2017) 38:4201–11. doi:10.1002/hbm.23658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Lepage C, de Pierrefeu A, Koerte IK, Coleman MJ, Pasternak O, Grant G, et al. White matter abnormalities in mild traumatic brain injury with and without post-traumatic stress disorder: a subject-specific diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain Imaging Behav (2017) 1–12. doi:10.1007/s11682-017-9744-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Main KL, Soman S, Pestilli F, Furst A, Noda A, Hernandez B, et al. DTI measures identify mild and moderate TBI cases among patients with complex health problems: a receiver operating characteristic analysis of U.S. veterans. Neuroimage Clin (2017) 16:1–16. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2017.06.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Modarres M, Kuzma NN, Kretzmer T, Pack AI, Lim MM. EEG slow waves in traumatic brain injury: convergent findings in mouse and man. Neurobiol Sleep Circadian Rhythms (2016) 2:59–70. doi:10.1016/j.nbscr.2016.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Sandsmark DK, Kumar MA, Woodward CS, Schmitt SE, Park S, Lim MM. Sleep features on continuous electroencephalography predict rehabilitation outcomes after severe traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2016) 31:101–7. doi:10.1097/HTR.0000000000000217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Sandsmark DK, Elliott JE, Lim MM. Sleep-wake disturbances after traumatic brain injury: synthesis of human and animal studies. Sleep (2017) 40:zsx044. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsx044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Schmitt S, Dichter MA. Electrophysiologic recordings in traumatic brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:319–39. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00021-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Fisher JAN, Huang S, Ye M, Nabili M, Wilent WB, Krauthamer V, et al. Real-time detection and monitoring of acute brain injury utilizing evoked electroencephalographic potentials. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng (2016) 24:1003–12. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2016.2529663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Morgalla MH, Tatagiba M. Long-term outcome prediction after a traumatic brain injury using early somatosensory and acoustic evoked potentials: analysis of the predictive value of the different single components of the potentials. Neurodiagn J (2014) 54:338–52. doi:10.1080/21646821.2014.11106818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Kulbe JR, Geddes JW. Current status of fluid biomarkers in mild traumatic brain injury. Exp Neurol (2016) 275(Pt 3):334–52. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Lewis LM, Schloemann D, Papa L, Fucetola R, Bazarian J, Lindburg M, et al. Utility of serum biomarkers in the diagnosis and stratification of mild traumatic brain injury. Acad Emerg Med (2017) 24:710–20. doi:10.1111/acem.13174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Posti JP, Hossain I, Takala RSK, Liedes H, Newcombe V, Outtrim J, et al. Glial fibrillary acidic protein and ubiquitin C-terminal hydrolase-L1 are not specific biomarkers for mild CT-negative traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2017) 34(7):1427–38. doi:10.1089/neu.2016.4442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. TRACK-TBI. TRACK-TBI Website. (2016). Available from: https://tracktbi.ucsf.edu/.

Google Scholar

105. FITBIR. FITBIR Website. (2016). Available from: https://fitbir.nih.gov/.

Google Scholar

106. Broglio SP, McCrea M, McAllister T, Harezlak J, Katz B, Hack D, et al. A national study on the effects of concussion in collegiate athletes and US military service academy members: the NCAA–DoD concussion assessment, research and education (CARE) consortium structure and methods. Sports Med (2017) 47:1437–51. doi:10.1007/s40279-017-0707-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. CDE. Common Data Elements Website. (2016). Available from: https://commondataelements.ninds.nih.gov.

Google Scholar

108. Jones NA, Ross H, Lynam T, Perez P, Leitch A. Mental models: an interdisciplinary synthesis of theory and methods. Ecol Soc (2011) 16(1):46. doi:10.5751/ES-03802-160146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Laubenbacher R, Hower V, Jarrah A, Torti SV, Shulaev V, Mendes P, et al. A systems biology view of cancer. Biochim Biophys Acta (2009) 1796:129–39. doi:10.1016/j.bbcan.2009.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Meadows DH. Thinking in Systems: A Primer. White River Junction, VT: Chelsea Green Publishing (2007).

Google Scholar

111. Sterman JD. System dynamics modeling: tools for learning in a complex world. Calif Manage Rev (2001) 43:8–25. doi:10.2307/41166098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Collins FS, Varmus H. A new initiative on precision medicine. N Engl J Med (2015) 372:793–5. doi:10.1056/NEJMp1500523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Jameson JL, Longo DL. Precision medicine—personalized, problematic, and promising. N Engl J Med (2015) 372:2229–34. doi:10.1056/NEJMsb1503104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Hellstrøm T, Kaufmann T, Andelic N, Soberg HL, Sigurdardottir S, Helseth E, et al. Predicting outcome 12 months after mild traumatic brain injury in patients admitted to a neurosurgery service. Front Neurol (2017) 8:125. doi:10.3389/fneur.2017.00125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Zwick M, Kolakowsky-Hayner S, Carney N, Balamane M, Nettleton T, Wright DW. Exploratory Modeling of TBI Data. (2016). Available from: https://www.pdx.edu/sysc/sites/www.pdx.edu.sysc/files/2016%20TBI%20OHSU%20presentation%2016Sept.pdf

Google Scholar

116. Lich KH, Ginexi EM, Osgood ND, Mabry PL. A call to address complexity in prevention science research. Prev Sci (2013) 14:279–89. doi:10.1007/s11121-012-0285-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Carey G, Malbon E, Carey N, Joyce A, Crammond B, Carey A. Systems science and systems thinking for public health: a systematic review of the field. BMJ Open (2015) 5:e009002. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Hirsch G, Homer J, Evans E, Zielinski A. A system dynamics model for planning cardiovascular disease interventions. Am J Public Health (2010) 100:616–22. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2009.159434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Johnston LM, Matteson CL, Finegood DT. Systems science and obesity policy: a novel framework for analyzing and rethinking population-level planning. Am J Public Health (2014) 104:1270–8. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2014.301884

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Wakeland W, Nielsen A, Geissert P. Dynamic model of nonmedical opioid use trajectories and potential policy interventions. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse (2015) 41:508–18. doi:10.3109/00952990.2015.1043435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Wittenborn AK, Rahmandad H, Rick J, Hosseinichimeh N. Depression as a systemic syndrome: mapping the feedback loops of major depressive disorder. Psychol Med (2016) 46:551–62. doi:10.1017/S0033291715002044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Hemmen JL, Sejnowski TJ. 23 Problems in Systems Neuroscience. USA: Oxford University Press (2006).

Google Scholar

123. Chen H-CI, Burke JF, Cohen AS. Editorial: traumatic brain injury as a systems neuroscience problem. Front Syst Neurosci (2016) 10. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2016.00100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Bigler ED. Systems biology, neuroimaging, neuropsychology, neuroconnectivity and traumatic brain injury. Front Syst Neurosci (2016) 10:55. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2016.00055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Mabry PL, Kaplan RM. Systems science: a good investment for the public’s health. Health Educ Behav (2013) 40:9S–12S. doi:10.1177/1090198113503469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Cohen IR, Harel D. Explaining a complex living system: dynamics, multi-scaling and emergence. J R Soc Interface (2007) 4:175–82. doi:10.1098/rsif.2006.0173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Vogt H, Hofmann B, Getz L. The new holism: P4 systems medicine and the medicalization of health and life itself. Med Health Care Philos (2016) 19:307–23. doi:10.1007/s11019-016-9683-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. McGinn MJ, Povlishock JT. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of injury and spontaneous recovery. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:67–87. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00005-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Meaney DF, Smith DH. Biomechanics of concussion. Clin Sports Med (2011) 30:19–31. doi:10.1016/j.csm.2010.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Iliff JJ, Chen MJ, Plog BA, Zeppenfeld DM, Soltero M, Yang L, et al. Impairment of glymphatic pathway function promotes tau pathology after traumatic brain injury. J Neurosci (2014) 34:16180–93. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3020-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Xie L, Kang H, Xu Q, Chen MJ, Liao Y, Thiyagarajan M, et al. Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science (2013) 342:373–7. doi:10.1126/science.1241224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Sharp DJ, Scott G, Leech R. Network dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Nat Rev Neurol (2014) 10:156–66. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2014.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Ziebell JM, Adelson PD, Lifshitz J. Microglia: dismantling and rebuilding circuits after acute neurological injury. Metab Brain Dis (2015) 30:393–400. doi:10.1007/s11011-014-9539-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Kovacks KS. Research to understand explosion-related injuries in military personnel. Bridge Natl Acad Eng (2016) 46:44–8.

Google Scholar

135. Collins CL, Fletcher EN, Fields SK, Kluchurosky L, Rohrkemper MK, Comstock RD, et al. Neck strength: a protective factor reducing risk for concussion in high school sports. J Prim Prev (2014) 35:309–19. doi:10.1007/s10935-014-0355-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Roland PE, Hilgetag CC, Deco G. Cortico-cortical communication dynamics. Front Syst Neurosci (2014) 8:19. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2014.00019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Sporns O, Betzel RF. Modular brain networks. Annu Rev Psychol (2016) 67:613–40. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci (2011) 31:15775–86. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Ham TE, Bonnelle V, Hellyer P, Jilka S, Robertson IH, Leech R, et al. The neural basis of impaired self-awareness after traumatic brain injury. Brain (2014) 137:586–97. doi:10.1093/brain/awt350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Aerts H, Fias W, Caeyenberghs K, Marinazzo D. Brain networks under attack: robustness properties and the impact of lesions. J Neurol (2016) 139:3063–83. doi:10.1093/brain/aww194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Laird AR, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB, Turner JA, Ray KL, McKay DR, et al. Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cogn Neurosci (2011) 23:4022–37. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci (2007) 27:2349–56. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Dean PJ, Sato JR, Vieira G, McNamara A, Sterr A. Multimodal imaging of mild traumatic brain injury and persistent postconcussion syndrome. Brain Behav (2015) 5:45–61. doi:10.1002/brb3.292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Hayes J, Bigler ED, Verfaellie M. Traumatic brain injury as a disorder of brain connectivity. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2016) 22:120–37. doi:10.1017/S1355617715000740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Mayer AR, Ling JM, Allen EA, Klimaj SD, Yeo RA, Hanlon FM. Static and dynamic intrinsic connectivity following mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2014) 32:1046–55. doi:10.1089/neu.2014.3542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Mayer AR, Toulouse T, Klimaj S, Ling JM, Pena A, Bellgowan PSF. Investigating the properties of the hemodynamic response function after mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2014) 31:189–97. doi:10.1089/neu.2013.3069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Slobounov SM, Gay M, Zhang K, Johnson B, Pennell D, Sebastianelli W, et al. Alteration of brain functional network at rest and in response to YMCA physical stress test in concussed athletes: RsFMRI study. Neuroimage (2011) 55:1716–27. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Venkatesan UM, Dennis NA, Hillary FG. Chronology and chronicity of altered resting-state functional connectivity after traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2014) 32:252–64. doi:10.1089/neu.2013.3318

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Zhou Y, Milham MP, Lui YW, Miles L, Reaume J, Sodickson DK, et al. Default-mode network disruption in mild traumatic brain injury. Radiology (2012) 265:882–92. doi:10.1148/radiol.12120748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Chamard E, Lassonde M, Théoret H. Neurometabolic, electrophysiological, and imaging abnormalities. Concussion (2014) 28:75–85. doi:10.1159/000358766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Bigler ED. Neuroimaging biomarkers in mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). Neuropsychol Rev (2013) 23:169–209. doi:10.1007/s11065-013-9237-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Ghajar J, Ivry RB. The predictive brain state: timing deficiency in traumatic brain injury? J Neurol Rehabil (2008) 22:217–27. doi:10.1177/1545968308315600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Harris NG, Verley DR, Gutman BA, Thompson PM, Yeh HJ, Brown JA. Disconnection and hyper-connectivity underlie reorganization after TBI: a rodent functional connectomic analysis. Exp Neurol (2016) 277:124–38. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Shumskaya E, Andriessen TMJC, Norris DG, Vos PE. Abnormal whole-brain functional networks in homogeneous acute mild traumatic brain injury. Neurology (2012) 79:175–82. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31825f04fb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Carron SF, Alwis DS, Rajan R. Traumatic brain injury and neuronal functionality changes in sensory cortex. Front Syst Neurosci (2016) 10:47. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2016.00047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:676–82. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Johnson B, Zhang K, Gay M, Horovitz S, Hallett M, Sebastianelli W, et al. Alteration of brain default network in subacute phase of injury in concussed individuals: resting-state fMRI study. Neuroimage (2012) 59:511–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Sours C, Zhuo J, Janowich J, Aarabi B, Shanmuganathan K, Gullapalli RP. Default mode network interference in mild traumatic brain injury – A pilot resting state study. Brain Res (2013) 1537:201–15. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Messé A, Caplain S, Pélégrini-Issac M, Blancho S, Lévy R, Aghakhani N, et al. Specific and evolving resting-state network alterations in post-concussion syndrome following mild traumatic brain injury. PLoS One (2013) 8:e65470. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Wolf JA, Koch PF. Disruption of network synchrony and cognitive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Front Syst Neurosci (2016) 10:43. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2016.00043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Krueger JM, Rector DM, Roy S, Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G, Panksepp J. Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nat Rev Neurosci (2008) 9:910–9. doi:10.1038/nrn2521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Krueger JM, Huang YH, Rector DM, Buysse DJ. Sleep: a synchrony of cell activity-driven small network states. Eur J Neurosci (2013) 38:2199–209. doi:10.1111/ejn.12238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. King LA, Horak FB, Mancini M, Pierce D, Priest KC, Chesnutt J, et al. Instrumenting the balance error scoring system for use with patients reporting persistent balance problems after mild traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2014) 95:353–9. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2013.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Ghajar J, Ivry RB. The predictive brain state: asynchrony in disorders of attention? Neuroscientist (2009) 15:232–42. doi:10.1177/1073858408326429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Diwakar M, Harrington DL, Maruta J, Ghajar J, El-Gabalawy F, Muzzatti L, et al. Filling in the gaps: anticipatory control of eye movements in chronic mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage Clin (2015) 8:210–23. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Maruta J, Heaton KJ, Maule AL, Ghajar J. Predictive visual tracking: specificity in mild traumatic brain injury and sleep deprivation. Mil Med (2014) 179:619–25. doi:10.7205/MILMED-D-13-00420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Samadani U, Ritlop R, Reyes M, Nehrbass E, Li M, Lamm E, et al. Eye tracking detects disconjugate eye movements associated with structural traumatic brain injury and concussion. J Neurotrauma (2015) 32:548–56. doi:10.1089/neu.2014.3687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Parks EL, Geha PY, Baliki MN, Katz J, Schnitzer TJ, Apkarian AV. Brain activity for chronic knee osteoarthritis: dissociating evoked pain from spontaneous pain. Eur J Pain Lond Engl (2011) 15:843.e1–14. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Cassidy JD, Cancelliere C, Carroll LJ, Côté P, Hincapié CA, Holm LW, et al. Systematic review of self-reported prognosis in adults after mild traumatic brain injury: results of the International Collaboration on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Prognosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2014) 95:S132–51. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2013.08.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Zemek RL, Farion KJ, Sampson M, McGahern C. Prognosticators of persistent symptoms following pediatric concussion: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatr (2013) 167:259–65. doi:10.1001/2013.jamapediatrics.216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Miyamoto A, Wake H, Moorhouse AJ, Nabekura J. Microglia and synapse interactions: fine tuning neural circuits and candidate molecules. Front Cell Neurosci (2013) 7:70. doi:10.3389/fncel.2013.00070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Eum RS, Seel RT, Goldstein R, Brown AW, Watanabe TK, Zasler ND, et al. Predicting institutionalization after traumatic brain injury inpatient rehabilitation. J Neurotrauma (2015) 32:280–6. doi:10.1089/neu.2014.3351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Yeates GN, Luckie M, De Beer Z, Khela P. Elucidating the psychosocial context of “post-concussion syndrome” (PCS): a case study from post-Milan systemic family therapy. J Fam Ther (2010) 32:186–202. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6427.2010.00493.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Hinzey A, Gaudier-Diaz MM, Lustberg MB, DeVries AC. Breast cancer and social environment: getting by with a little help from our friends. Breast Cancer Res (2016) 18:54. doi:10.1186/s13058-016-0700-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Arora NK. Interacting with cancer patients: the significance of physicians’ communication behavior. Soc Sci Med (2003) 1982(57):791–806. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00449-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Stewart MA. Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: a review. Can Med Assoc J (1995) 152:1423–33.

Google Scholar

177. Stewart M, Brown JB, Donner A, McWhinney IR, Oates J, Weston WW, et al. The impact of patient-centered care on outcomes. J Fam Pract (2000) 49:796–804.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

178. Walling AM, Keating NL, Kahn KL, Dy S, Mack JW, Malin J, et al. Lower patient ratings of physician communication are associated with unmet need for symptom management in patients with lung and colorectal cancer. J Oncol Pract (2016) 12:e654–69. doi:10.1200/JOP.2015.005538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Lindsley T. Legitimizing the wound: mapping the military’s diagnostic discourse of traumatic brain injury. Tech Commun Q (2015) 24:235–57. doi:10.1080/10572252.2015.1044120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Gordon WA, Cantor J, Kristen D, Tsaousides T. Long-term social integration and community support. Handb Clin Neurol (2015) 127:423–31. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52892-6.00027-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Kempe CB, Sullivan KA, Edmed SL. CE the effect of varying diagnostic terminology within patient discharge information on expected mild traumatic brain injury outcome. Clin Neuropsychol (2013) 27:762–78. doi:10.1080/13854046.2013.795245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Corning PA. The re-emergence of “emergence”: a venerable concept in search of a theory. Complexity (2002) 7:18–30. doi:10.1002/cplx.10043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Goldstein J. Emergence as a construct: history and issues. Emergence (1999) 1:49–72. doi:10.1207/s15327000em0101_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Conidi FX. Interventional treatment for post-traumatic headache. Curr Pain Headache Rep (2016) 20:40. doi:10.1007/s11916-016-0570-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Gustafsson M, Nestor CE, Zhang H, Barabási A-L, Baranzini S, Brunak S, et al. Modules, networks and systems medicine for understanding disease and aiding diagnosis. Genome Med (2014) 6:82. doi:10.1186/s13073-014-0082-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Agarwal R, Dhar V. Big data, data science, and analytics: the opportunity and challenge for IS research. Inf Syst Res (2014) 25:443–8. doi:10.1287/isre.2014.0546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Star SL, Griesemer JR. Institutional ecology, ‘translations’ and boundary objects: amateurs and professionals in Berkeley’s Museum of Vertebrate Zoology, 1907-39. Soc Stud Sci (1989) 19:387–420. doi:10.1177/030631289019003001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: concussion, traumatic brain injury, systems science, complex, multi-scale, networks, models of injury

Citation: Kenzie ES, Parks EL, Bigler ED, Lim MM, Chesnutt JC and Wakeland W (2017) Concussion As a Multi-Scale Complex System: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis of Current Knowledge. Front. Neurol. 8:513. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00513

Received: 21 June 2017; Accepted: 13 September 2017;
Published: 28 September 2017

Edited by:

Kenneth Curley, Iatrikos Research and Development Solutions, LLC, United States

Reviewed by:

Paul Rapp, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, United States
David F. Tate, University of Missouri–St. Louis, United States

Copyright: © 2017 Kenzie, Parks, Bigler, Lim, Chesnutt and Wakeland. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Erin S. Kenzie, erin.kenzie@pdx.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.