- 1School of Psychology, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
- 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Freiburg Medical Center, Freiburg, Germany
- 3Department of Neurology, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
The relationship between sleep disorders and neurological disorders is often reciprocal, such that sleep disorders are worsened by neurological symptoms and that neurological disorders are aggravated by poor sleep. Animal and human studies further suggest that sleep disruption not only worsens single neurological symptoms but may also lead to long-term negative outcomes. This suggests that sleep may play a fundamental role in neurorehabilitation and recovery. We further propose that sleep may not only alter the efficacy of behavioral treatments but also plasticity-enhancing adjunctive neurostimulation methods, such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). At present, sleep receives little attention in the fields of neurorehabilitation and neurostimulation. In this review, we draw together the strands of evidence from both fields of research to highlight the proposition that sleep is an important parameter to consider in the application of tDCS as a primary or adjunct rehabilitation intervention.
Introduction
Sleep disorders are often comorbid to neurological disorders, such as Parkinson’s disease (1), multiple sclerosis (2), traumatic brain injury (3, 4), and stroke (5). Their manifestation can be a direct consequence of the neuropathology, resulting from a dysfunction of neural networks that are implicated in sleep regulation. In addition, sleep disorders can emerge more indirectly mediated, for example, by medication, stress, depression, fatigue, or pain.
The onset and presentation of sleep disorders may vary widely between individuals and across different neurological disorders. For example, 74–98% of patients with Parkinson’s disease will develop sleep problems, such as a decrease in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and total sleep time, as late as 10 years after their diagnosis (6). In stroke, more than half of patients have sleep-disordered breathing (SDB), and around 20–40% experience sleep–wake disorders, such as insomnia or excessive daytime sleepiness, at some point after their vascular insult (5). Stroke patients in the chronic phase of recovery further have poorer sleep efficiency (7) and also show greater prominence of slow-wave EEG at wake than age-matched controls (8). Moreover, explorative qualitative data revealed that patients with chronic low-functioning hemiparesis feel their sleep has deteriorated gravely since suffering the stroke, and, critically, that their difficulty sleeping is caused by the stroke and the impact it had on their physical and mental health (unpublished data). Together, these data suggest that sleep disturbances in neurological conditions are a common yet diverse problem, which is multifactorial in origin.
The presence of sleep disorders can compromise or complicate the treatment of neurological disorders. For instance, sleep problems early after stroke might interfere with acute recovery, potentially leading to further medical complications and prolonged hospitalization, and also increase the risk for a second stroke (9). Besides, stroke patients with sleep disorders can experience persistent fatigue, mood changes, lack of motivation, and decline in cognitive functioning (9). When combined with the modulatory effects of sleep on neuroplasticity and learning mechanisms (10, 11), sleeping poorly might compromise motor recovery, long-term functional outcomes, and quality of life. Moreover, we argue that sleeping poorly is likely to promote adverse health behaviors, such as a sedentary lifestyle, which in turn may aggravate poor sleep and poor health, thereby creating a vicious circle. For example, it is entirely conceivable that limited physical activity experienced by patients in the chronic phase of stroke is a potential candidate responsible for sleep difficulties through autonomic dysregulation, metabolic changes, or altered sleep/arousal promoting activity. Understanding the modulatory effects of sleep on the efficacy of neurorehabilitative interventions, such as motor training and neuromodulation, is therefore intrinsically and reciprocally linked to daytime behavior.
In this article, we will discuss the impact of sleep and its disorders on the rehabilitation of neurological patients. Using the example of stroke, we will first review the theoretical rationale underlying the application of specific modulatory therapies that aim to augment motor recovery after stroke, such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). In the subsequent section, we make the case for a link between sleep and tDCS based on the argument that they exert their biological effects through a common mechanism, that is, the modulation of neuroplasticity processes and therefore might influence each other’s outcome. For instance, tDCS, when delivered as a stand-alone intervention, might alter specific aspects of sleep, such as enhancing slow-wave sleep (SWS), which is crucial for memory formation and consolidation (12). On the other hand, sleep characteristics might influence tDCS efficacy, whether it is delivered as a stand-alone or adjunctive intervention, for example, combined with training-based motor rehabilitation therapies. Therefore, we propose that a better appreciation of the interaction between sleep, tDCS mechanisms, and neurorehabilitation is needed to maximize therapy efficacy and improve rehabilitation outcomes in patients.
tDCS in Stroke Rehabilitation
Stroke is a major cause of disability worldwide. Rehabilitation after stroke is essential for alleviating the associated motor impairments and disabilities in patients, and currently represents an important aspect of the global stroke challenge (13). Basic science research has made an enormous contribution toward improving neurorehabilitation. This led to new training principles for motor deficits after stroke (14), which in turn contributed to a theoretical and empirical step change in the field and improved the prospect of long-term care for patients (15). Furthermore, a better understanding of the neural mechanisms of recovery and interventions included the exploration of neuromodulatory methods, such as peripheral sensory stimulation, pharmacological intervention, cell-based therapy, and brain stimulation, such as tDCS and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) (16–18). Out of these methods, tDCS has emerged as the most promising and practical approach.
In brief, tDCS is a safe, portable, and low-cost technique capable of altering the efficacy of brain plasticity and hence learning mechanisms. It is used to modify neuronal activity in target brain regions by delivering a weak electric current of typically 1.0–2.0 mA through two large and easily affixed surface electrodes mounted to the head (Figure 1) (19, 20). Two types of stimulation, which have opposite effects on the brain, are thereby discriminated: anodal tDCS, which causes a reduction in the excitability threshold of neurons (excitatory tDCS), and cathodal tDCS associated with a downregulation of cortical excitability (inhibitory tDCS) (21). Generally, tDCS is applied for 20–60 min, and its effect can be observed during and, importantly, for a prolonged period after the stimulation has ceased. For example, a randomized, double-blind cross-over study in stroke patients demonstrated that 20 min of 1-mA anodal stimulation over the primary motor cortex induced performance gains in the paretic hand that lasted for more than 30 min after the stimulation (22, 23). Other studies found therapeutic gains, sustained for weeks or even month, following repeated tDCS stimulation in combination with motor training over several days (24–26). Moreover, cathodal (inhibitory) tDCS has successfully been used to decrease interhemispheric inhibition from the unaffected hemisphere (27), providing an alternative pathway to recovery through increasing cortical excitability in the affected hemisphere.
Figure 1. (A) The hand-held one-channel transcranial DC stimulator and (B) transcranial direct current stimulation set up to promote functional motor recovery after stroke. Following a stroke, motor deficit may occur as a result of interhemispheric inhibition from the contralesional (unaffected) M1 to the ipsilesional (affected) M1. Cathodal tDCS to the contralesional hemisphere can be used to decrease interhemispheric inhibition, while anodal tDCS to the ipsilesional M1 can be used to decrease motor deficit in the affected hemisphere. (C) Motor deficit after a stroke is associated with reduced participation from the ipsilesional M1. Anodal tDCS to the ipsilesional M1 can be used to enhance motor function, while cathodal tDCS to the contralateral supraorbital region is thought to be functionally ineffective.
The use of tDCS in stroke rehabilitation has been largely predicated on two complementary, evidence-based hypotheses. The first hypothesis assumes that motor recovery after stroke is mostly driven by neural mechanisms similar to those promoting motor learning in the intact brain, i.e., activity-dependent synaptic plasticity, characterized by both functional and structural changes in neuronal connections within spared sensorimotor circuits (28) and that such mechanisms, besides being essentially influenced by motor experience, can be further potentiated by neuromodulatory approaches (29–33). On the other hand, the second hypothesis assumes that motor deficits after stroke, such as hand paresis, often result from a complex interaction between the direct effects of the brain lesion itself, e.g., the focal disruption of motor cortex and/or its corticospinal fibres, and subsequent maladaptive plasticity processes occurring within structurally intact, residual brain circuits. The latter usually involves the combination of interacting phenomena such as depression/inactivity of perilesional motor cortex and/or corticospinal fibres and excessive transcallosal inhibition of the affected hemisphere by the opposite, unaffected hemisphere (18, 34, 35).
Based on these two key hypotheses, three types of tDCS protocols have emerged as adjunctive treatment strategies to be delivered in combination with motor training-based therapies. The first approach aims to facilitate synaptic activity and plasticity, and hence motor learning, in the ipsilesional motor cortex through excitatory anodal stimulation (17). The synaptic excitation/facilitation exerted by anodal tDCS in the affected hemisphere might contribute to improve motor function via at least two overlapping neural mechanisms: it may not only facilitate the recruitment of potentially spared corticospinal neurons innervating the paretic limb but also contribute to the normalization of interhemispheric imbalance by reactivating preserved, yet silent inhibitory influences from the affected motor cortex toward the contralateral motor area. The second approach consists in inhibiting/decreasing synaptic activity in the contralesional motor cortex with inhibitory cathodal stimulation (36). The inhibition of activity in the non-affected hemisphere contributes to a reduction of the increased inhibitory influence this hemisphere exerts upon the affected motor cortex. This, in turn, might contribute to the restoration of the interhemispheric equilibrium and facilitate corticospinal activation of the affected hemisphere. Finally, the third approach consists simply in combining the two in a bihemispheric stimulation protocol, with anodal stimulation of the ipsilesional and cathodal stimulation of the contralesional motor cortex provided at the same time. This combined protocol is thought to produce even greater improvements in motor function (26).
The results from studies combining tDCS with motor rehabilitation training are numerous and promising (20), highlighting the underlying potential of tDCS for improving motor outcomes beyond the levels obtained with physical practice alone. However, the evidence base required to firmly set tDCS as an effective adjunctive therapy is not yet robust (37). Many questions remain to be answered, such as the exact mechanisms by which tDCS affects neural processing that promotes motor improvements and the ideal timing between motor training and stimulation. However, the most critical question concerns the parameters needed to determine how patients can be optimally stratified according to specific markers that predict how effectively they will respond to the stimulation (20, 38). We argue that sleep is likely to be one of the parameters to be considered in this optimization.
Identifying modulators of treatment efficacy is a challenging yet important endeavor, which contributes not only to foster mechanistic understanding of the intervention itself and the targeted pathology but also, and critically, to improve methodological quality and thereby the impact of future studies testing that intervention. This is especially relevant for the process of creating the evidence base for a particular therapy. When not controlled for, those variables can critically affect the bivariate causal relationship of interest and generate misconceptions about the real efficacy of the intervention under investigation.
Within the context of motor rehabilitation, determining markers for rehabilitative therapies in patients has been a recognizable effort in the literature. For instance, Riley et al. (39) found that the degree of integrity of corticospinal fibers descending from the primary motor and dorsal premotor cortices in the affected hemisphere seems to be a better predictor of motor gains induced by robotic hand therapy when compared to baseline motor status and stroke volume measures (39). On the other hand, Sterr et al. (40, 41) recently suggested that corticospinal tract integrity may not have such an important role in mediating motor gains when considering motor therapies that address more gross upper limb functions, such as modified constraint-induced movement therapy (40, 41). Regarding neuromodulatory interventions, Ameli et al. (42) reported hand motor gains in subcortical but not in cortical stroke patients after a protocol of excitatory transcranial magnetic stimulation of the ipsilesional motor cortex (42). Similarly, Stagg et al. (43) found that anodal tDCS-induced behavioral gains in chronic stroke patients are associated with an increase in movement-related cortical activity within the stimulated ipsilesional motor cortex (43), a process that seems to be mediated by attenuation of γ-amino butyric acid (GABA) levels in that region (44). These stimulation studies suggest that both the structural – the degree of anatomical integrity – and functional – the amount of inhibitory activity within its internal circuitry – status of the targeted cortical region at baseline might be good predictors of motor outcomes after brain stimulation (43, 44).
Taken together, the studies outlined above provide insights into the mechanisms mediating treatment success in the case of specific intervention protocols, including adjuvant approaches such as tDCS. This harbors important implications for future studies investigating the involved therapies, as it may help to improve patient stratification, which in turn might contribute to the reduction of interindividual differences in outcomes and thereby maximize therapy impact (39, 44). As each stroke and the underlying recovery processes are different, the neural substrates underpinning treatment success might be preserved in some patients but not others (20, 38). Moreover, individual differences might further arise from psychological factors, such as mental health, motivation, and treatment compliance, as well as physical factors, such as fatigue, and their combined effect on sleep, all of which share a reciprocal relationship with each other as well as with the specific treatment mechanisms. These interactions can substantively influence outcome. For instance, exacerbated fatigue, particularly in cases of more severe hemiparesis, can decrease motivation and engagement with motor training, which in turn might adversely affect the neuroplasticity mechanisms driving (re)learning and recovery, and thereby limit motor gains (41, 45). Moreover, as discussed in detail below, sleep and plasticity are intrinsically linked, and poor sleep might hence have detrimental effects on stroke outcome. Because these variables are not stable across patients, i.e., individuals often present with varying degrees of decreased motivation, depression, fatigue, and sleep problems, and because they can interfere with neuroplasticity-driven recovery mechanisms, they not only might contribute to explain individual differences in rehabilitation outcomes but may also present important predictive markers for the efficacy of neurorehabilitation and neuromodulation as well as their combined application.
Sleep as a Potential Modulator of tDCS-Based Stroke Rehabilitation
The hypothesis put forward in this article is that sleep might modulate the efficacy of plasticity-based therapies after stroke, such as tDCS, and thereby influence functional recovery during neurorehabilitation. This idea seems reasonable and justifiable by the literature. Empirical evidences have consistently reported the detrimental effect of sleep deprivation on memory, learning, and plastic processes in the brain (46–49). In addition, our hypothesis follows a long line of research suggesting a link between organic sleep disorders and a number of metabolic and cardiovascular diseases [see Ref. (50, 51) for reviews]. Whether sleep disorders modulate therapeutic interventions in such patients is presently unclear (50, 51). However, the presence of organic sleep disorders, and in particular SDB, in stroke patients is well established in the literature (9, 52); therefore, we will not cover this in detail in this review. Individuals with stroke often experience changes in sleep continuity and architecture, characterized by a reduction in total sleep time, sleep efficiency, and SWS, as well as increased sleep fragmentation and high incident rates of SDB (53, 54). The emergence of these poor sleep traits are caused by intrinsic and extrinsic factors, such as pain, fatigue, poor mental health, and immobility (intrinsic factors), as well as direct or indirect damage to the sleep regulatory pathways or prolonged hospitalization affecting regular sleep routine (extrinsic factors) (54, 55). Experimental evidence further suggests that poor sleep after stroke may worsen the condition and lead to a second stroke (9). However, the knowledge of the effect of sleep on rehabilitation is limited and, to the best of our knowledge, no study so far has addressed the role of sleep specifically in tDCS, either as an adjuvant or a stand-alone neurorehabilitation intervention. Therefore, we discuss below the candidate mechanisms for a modulatory effect of sleep on tDCS-based stroke rehabilitation and the implications for clinical practice.
Neuroplasticity, Sleep, and tDCS
Below we discuss evidence showing an overlap between the neuroplasticity processes occurring during sleep and those occurring during motor training and tDCS stimulation. We first highlight the mechanism by which sleep and tDCS alter neural plasticity, which is critical for the formation of new neuronal connections in order to compensate for impairment after stroke. We then explore the link between tDCS and sleep mechanisms, which are relevant to motor learning and memory. Subsequently, the benefit of sleep on motor skill (re)learning, and how tDCS can be used to enhance this processes, will be put forward.
Principles of Neuroplasticity
Neuroplasticity involves functional and/or structural modification of neuronal circuits in response to conditions of altered afferent and/or efferent demands (56). Neuronal connections are continuously remodeled throughout life, which allows the brain not only to learn new skills under healthy conditions but also to relearn previously acquired ones, for instance, to compensate for the loss of function caused by brain disease (56). The basic tenet underlying the efficacy of most of the current stroke rehabilitation therapies, including tDCS as an adjunctive or stand-alone intervention, is the modulation and potentiation of neuroplasticity processes (57). However, neuroplasticity is also a key function of sleep. Thus, sleep research conducted over the past decade or so has clearly demonstrated a pivotal role of sleep in the neuroplasticity processes subserving learning and memory consolidation (10, 11, 58–62). The exact mechanisms explaining the relationship between sleep and neuroplasticity are still under debate. However, two hypotheses offer an attractive proposition, namely, the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis and sleep-dependent synaptic formation. The synaptic homeostasis hypothesis described by Tononi and Cerelli describes sleep as an adaptive state that helps to maintain synaptic homeostasis and renormalization in order to recover from increased net synaptic strength and density that occurs during wake (63–67). Prior to sleep, synapses that are strongly activated are stabilized and consolidated, making them less prone to decay (68). In the context of motor skill learning, sleep-dependent synaptic potentiation has been shown not only to decrease decay of motor skills acquired during wake but also improve performance after subsequent sleep (69–72). This improvement in performance is thought to be due to sleep-dependent memory consolidation (11), which is the active reorganization of memory during sleep, leading to a more efficient memory storage (72, 73). In a motor skill learning task, such as the serial reaction time task (SRTT), initial memories are stored in the primary motor cortex and later transferred to the premotor and parietal cortices, where they are consolidated and stabilized during subsequent sleep (74). Positron emission tomography (PET) and regional cerebral blood flow measurements during sleep show more activity in brain regions involved in the execution of SRTT during wake (75). This may be due to the ongoing consolidation and stabilization of acquired skills during sleep.
In addition to the synaptic homeostatic hypothesis mechanism, which we propose as a potential mechanism by which sleep promotes plasticity in the motor cortex after stroke, sleep-dependent synaptic strengthening and growth may also be an advocate. Initial data from animal studies further suggest that sleep can promote synaptic reorganization and growth in the motor cortex after motor training. For example, Yang et al. (76) demonstrated the effect of sleep on the remodeling of postsynaptic dendritic spines in the primary motor cortex after motor training in mice. Using transcranial two-photon microscopy, they showed that sleep promotes branch-specific dendritic spines formation in the primary motor cortex in response to motor training during wake (76). In mice that underwent motor training, the rate of dendritic spine formation in pyramidal neurons from motor cortex was significantly higher, compared to mice that did not undergo motor training. Moreover, in mice that slept after the motor training, the rate of formation and retention of new dendritic spines in the motor cortex was significantly higher, compared to the sleep-deprived mice. On the molecular level, it has been proposed that the expression of genes related to neuroplasticity processes is one of the mechanisms by which sleep consolidates memory (77). This idea is supported by several animal studies, showing increased expression of immediate early genes, essential for synaptic plasticity, during the first few hours of sleep (78–81). Although these findings have been obtained from rodent models and are not entirely conclusive, they support the notion that sleep-dependent learning and memory enhancement does not only involve synaptic downregulation but also promotes synaptic plasticity associated with learning and memory. Taken together, the studies summarized above corroborate our hypothesis on the link between sleep, tDCS-induced neuroplasticity, and stroke recovery.
Interestingly, sleep is not only beneficial for memory consolidation and stabilization but also promotes improvement in performance by reactivation of neuronal circuits (70). Insights into the role of sleep in memory enhancement dates as far back as to the 1920s. In 1924, Jenkins and Dallenbach first reported improved performance in a verbal learning task after a period of sleep compared to wake (82). Recently, these findings have been replicated using a procedural motor sequence learning task (70, 73, 83, 84). One study, in particular, reported a 33% increase in performance and 30% decrease in error rate in a sequential motor task (84). This discovery suggests that sleep is not only beneficial for memory consolidation but also enhances previously acquired skills.
Long-term retention of motor skills after training is dependent on the neuroplastic processes, as described above. A motor skill acquired through training or practice is susceptible to interference and performance deterioration over a period of wake (10). Therefore, it is perhaps no surprise that motor skill learning studies have consistently reported performance improvements between training trials filled with sleep compared to wake (73, 84). Assuming that sleep is an active ingredient of memory consolidation, rather than a passive one simply allowing for reduced interference, we presume that good quality sleep is critical for consolidation, stabilization, and offline learning. It follows that good sleep is likely to enhance effective stroke recovery and rehabilitation, while poor sleep is likely to reduce it.
In this context, tDCS is particularly interesting since the main mechanism for augmenting neuroplasticity with tDCS, as well as the functional role of sleep in neuroplasticity, is the enhancement of synaptic potentiation processes such as long-term potentiation (LTP). During wake, active synapses undergo LTP as a result of learning and experience, while during sleep, a negative feedback response known as long-term depression (LTD) occurs, which prevents those synapses from saturation (85). Moreover, it is proposed that LTP is a relevant mechanism for stroke recovery (57). For example, anodal tDCS to the primary motor cortex, in combination with motor training, can elicit long-lasting potentiation (19), and repeated tDCS over the motor cortex is able to induce LTP-like plasticity (86). LTP takes place at glutamatergic neurones (87). Glutamate binds to its receptors in the postsynaptic terminal, that is, α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor and N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor. NMDA receptors are ionotropic receptors that are involved in the maintenance of neuronal plasticity and memory function. Motor skill learning and consolidation are suggested to be NMDA dependent (88). Similarly, tDCS LTP-like modulation is thought to be dependent on NMDA in order to enhance learning and memory (86). Interestingly, blocking NMDA receptors suppresses the effect of anodal and cathodal tDCS in humans and also impairs sleep in rats (86, 89, 90). These finding are in line with the idea that tDCS, sleep, memory, and learning all share a common physiological substrate, which involves NMDA-dependent LTP modulation. One might further speculate that this overlap in NMDA-dependent processes may lead to a trade-off in tDCS-based neurorehabilitation if good sleep quality is compromised.
The most beneficial stage of sleep to learning and memory occurs during SWS (12), which is often used as an index of sleep quality because of the recovery role it plays (64, 91, 92). The modulatory effect of sleep on tDCS-based neurorehabilitation, which we emphasize in this review, most likely occurs during SWS. Studies in healthy controls have shown that consolidation of motor skills and memory formation after practice occurs during SWS (93–95). Consequently, night sleep and daytime naps rich in SWS can actively prevent memory deterioration by synaptic downscaling (73, 96). In contrast, lack of SWS has been shown to increase cognitive and memory deficit (97, 98), probably due to lack of synaptic downscaling and/or aberrant memory consolidation. Moreover, EEG studies have demonstrated that brain regions most active during wakefulness show more SWS during subsequent sleep (99–101). Studies on local sleep further demonstrate a regional increase in SWS after motor practice, specifically in those areas involved in motor control (72, 99, 102, 103). Conversely, we postulate that tDCS-enhanced motor training during wake will lead to more SWS in the brain regions involved in the execution of the task during subsequent sleep. Lastly, SWS is characterized by slow-wave oscillations (SWO), which decrease during the course of the night (67). tDCS applied during SWS reduces the decay of SWO (65, 67, 104), while anodal tDCS over frontocortical areas is beneficial to declarative memory when applied during SWS, an effect mediated by decreasing the rate of decrease of SWO (65, 105–108). Taken together, these studies identify tDCS as a stand-alone intervention to enhance SWS and boost declarative cognitive and motor memory acquired during the day over subsequent sleep. Presuming that the respective mechanisms underpinning these effects are preserved into older age, tDCS-induced enhancement of SWS might also be an effective pathway to enhance stroke rehabilitation (Figure 2).
Figure 2. A schematic illustration of the possible application of tDCS in stroke recovery in the sleep/wake context. During wake (light-blue background), motor training leads to initial skill acquisition and motor memory formation. This is a result of synaptic potential and increase in synaptic strength and density. These processes can be enhanced by the combination of tDCS and motor training, which reduces the action potential threshold of neurones. During sleep (dark-blue background), synaptic downscaling occurs to restore neuronal homeostasis, and initially acquired memories are consolidated and stabilized during slow wave sleep (SWS). tDCS can be used to reduce the decay of SWS, thus further maintaining and strengthening memory consolidation and stabilization. In addition, sleep-dependent synaptic formation may also enhance tDCS-induced neuroplasticity and stroke recovery.
Beyond the systems level, SWS further contributes to brain function at the cellular and local neuronal network level. In the cellular level, energy levels in terms of glycogen and ATP are restored during SWS (109, 110), whereas synaptic weight and connectivity are regulated in the neuronal level (67). This suggests that SWS may be a critical period for neuronal restoration and gaining functional recovery after stroke. Interestingly, ischemic stroke decreases SWS (111), whereas administration of SWS enhancement agent, such as γ-hydroxybutyrate (GHB), improves sensorimotor recovery following ischemic stroke (112). Therefore, this hints to two possible applications of tDCS to promote functional recovery after stroke. First, tDCS can be applied as a cognitive enhancer during motor skill learning during wake. Second, in the sleep context, tDCS can be applied during sleep to enhance SWS, thus increasing reactivation of memories and restoration of neuronal and cellular homeostasis. This proposed application takes advantage of the overlapping mechanisms of tDCS and sleep when combined in order to enhance motor recovery.
Taken together, good sleep habits after stroke in combination with conventional rehabilitation techniques and tDCS may promote function recovery as illustrated in Figure 3. In contrast, the evidence described in the review indicates that poor sleep after stroke may potentially lead to the occurrence of another stroke and slow down neuroplasticity processes, and hence recovery (9). Lastly, sleep my potentially modulate plasticity-based interventions, such as tDCS, by altering neuronal plasticity and other underlying mechanisms, which have not been assessed yet.
Figure 3. Schematic diagram illustrating the potential interaction between tDCS-enhanced stroke rehabilitation and the modulatory effect of sleep. tDCS in combination with conventional neurorehabilitation techniques and good sleep quality may promote functional recovery. Contrarily, bad sleep as a result of direct consequence of stroke or indirect outcome from complications associated with stroke may reduce the efficacy of tDCS-based intervention, aggravate the disease condition, and lead to long-term negative outcomes.
Neurogenesis
There is much debate on the role of sleep in adult neurogenesis, although modest evidence suggests a negative effect of sleep deprivation on cell proliferation and survival (113–115). Neurogenesis is important for spatial navigation learning, long-term spatial memory retention (116, 117), as well as trace and fear conditioning (118). Available evidence suggests that sleep alone may not promote adult neurogenesis, but lack of sleep may be detrimental to the process (113, 114). For instance, glucocorticoid elevation as a result of stress arising from prolonged sleep deprivation may alter adult neurogenesis by inducing dendritic atrophy in the hippocampus (113). In relation to tDCS, Rueger et al. (119) reported that 10 days cathodal tDCS in rays increased neural stem cells proliferation. Furthermore, compelling evidence for the interaction of sleep and tDCS on neurogenesis is further coming from studies on brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) encoded by the BDNF gene. BDNF is expressed in the hippocampus and the cortex, which are the brain regions involved in learning and memory (120, 121). It is also expressed in motor neurones, where it is involved in consolidation of motor skills (120). BDNF is a neurotrophin, which promotes survival of existing neurones, as well as the growth and differentiation of new neurones and synapses. It regulates learning and memory by activating NMDA receptors through phosphorylation of one of its subunits (122). Interestingly, an association between BDNF Val(66) polymorphism with functional recovery in subcortical stroke has recently been reported (123, 124). At the same time, initial evidence also suggests that the tDCS mechanism of action, that is, NMDA-dependent LTP modulation, is driven through BDNF-dependent plasticity (120). Finally, Giese et al. (125) reported that BDNF levels are influenced by sleep, such that sleep deprivation causes a decrease in serum BDNF levels. Although this modest evidence suggests that sleep and tDCS may be beneficial to neuronal recovery by modulating neurogenesis, and possibly with involvement of BDNF, further research is warranted to investigate the molecular mechanism underlying this process.
Conclusion
In this review, we have presented the current application of tDCS in neurorehabilitation with a focus on stroke, and also, we highlighted potential variables that can modulate tDCS-based neurorehabilitation with a focus on sleep. Furthermore, background on the concept of tDCS in brain injury treatment and the beneficial effect of good sleep and detrimental effect of poor sleep on cognitive function were provided. At present, research on the variables that can influence tDCS outcomes is relatively sparse; therefore, proposing a recommendation at this point will be rather speculative. Moreover, with the available evidence of sleep disorders prevalence and persistence in the lives of people living with neurological disorders, such as stroke, and the potential modulatory effect of sleep on tDCS efficacy, finding ways to prevent, manage, and treat sleep disorders is imperative. Furthermore, a wealth of research already suggest poor sleep is detrimental to stroke pathophysiology; therefore, it is important to establish if this relationship really exits by abolishing sleep disturbances and avoiding sleep deficit to assess if this will enhance tDCS effect in stroke patients. With this knowledge, neuropsychologists and clinicians can effectively design and implement tDCS interventions that take sleep into consideration. Lastly, a comprehensive understanding of modulators that influence the efficacy and safety of tDCS will be profitable to health professions, caregivers, citizens, and patients.
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Funding
LF is funded by CAPES Foundation, Ministry of Education, Brazil (BEX 0996/14-9).
References
1. Kumar S, Bhatia M, Behari M. Sleep disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord (2002) 17(4):775–81. doi:10.1002/mds.10167
2. Veauthier C, Radbruch H, Gaede G, Pfueller CF, Dorr J, Bellmann-Strobl J, et al. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis is closely related to sleep disorders: a polysomnographic cross-sectional study. Mult Scler (2011) 17(5):613–22. doi:10.1177/1352458510393772
3. Baumann CR, Werth E, Stocker R, Ludwig S, Bassetti CL. Sleep-wake disturbances 6 months after traumatic brain injury: a prospective study. Brain (2007) 130(Pt 7):1873–83. doi:10.1093/brain/awm109
4. Ouellet MC, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Morin CM. Sleep-wake disturbances after traumatic brain injury. Lancet Neurol (2015) 14(7):746–57. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00068-X
5. Hermann DM, Bassetti CL. Sleep apnea and other sleep-wake disorders in stroke. Curr Treat Options Neurol (2003) 5(3):241–9. doi:10.1007/s11940-003-0015-y
6. Goulart FO, Godke BA, Borges V, Azevedo-Silva SM, Mendes MF, Cendoroglo MS, et al. Fatigue in a cohort of geriatric patients with and without Parkinson’s disease. Braz J Med Biol Res (2009) 42(8):771–5. doi:10.1590/S0100-879X2009000800014
7. Sterr A, Herron K, Dijk DJ, Ellis J. Time to wake-up: sleep problems and daytime sleepiness in long-term stroke survivors. Brain Inj (2008) 22(7–8):575–9. doi:10.1080/02699050802189727
8. Herron K, Dijk DJ, Dean P, Seiss E, Sterr A. Quantitative electroencephalography and behavioural correlates of daytime sleepiness in chronic stroke. Biomed Res Int (2014) 2014:794086. doi:10.1155/2014/794086
9. Bassetti CL, Hermann DM. Sleep and stroke. Handb Clin Neurol (2011) 99:1051–72. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52007-4.00021-7
10. Censor N, Sagi D, Cohen LG. Common mechanisms of human perceptual and motor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci (2012) 13(9):658–64. doi:10.1038/nrn3315
11. Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature (2005) 437(7063):1272–8. doi:10.1038/nature04286
12. Landmann N, Kuhn M, Piosczyk H, Feige B, Baglioni C, Spiegelhalder K, et al. The reorganisation of memory during sleep. Sleep Med Rev (2014) 18(6):531–41. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2014.03.005
13. Furlan L. Potential barriers and promising opportunities for stroke rehabilitation in Brazil. Int J Stroke (2014) 9(Suppl A100):144. doi:10.1111/ijs.12338
14. Nudo RJ. Recovery after brain injury: mechanisms and principles. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:887. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00887
15. Langhorne P, Bernhardt J, Kwakkel G. Stroke rehabilitation. Lancet (2011) 377(9778):1693–702. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60325-5
16. Dimyan MA, Cohen LG. Neuroplasticity in the context of motor rehabilitation after stroke. Nat Rev Neurol (2011) 7(2):76–85. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2010.200
17. Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Non-invasive brain stimulation: a new strategy to improve neurorehabilitation after stroke? Lancet Neurol (2006) 5(8):708–12. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70525-7
18. Ward NS, Cohen LG. Mechanisms underlying recovery of motor function after stroke. Arch Neurol (2004) 61(12):1844–8. doi:10.1001/archneur.61.12.1844
19. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology (2001) 57(10):1899–901. doi:10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899
20. Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation in neurorehabilitation. Handb Clin Neurol (2013) 116:499–524. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53497-2.00040-1
21. Dayan E, Censor N, Buch ER, Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nat Neurosci (2013) 16(7):838–44. doi:10.1038/nn.3422
22. Hummel F, Celnik P, Giraux P, Floel A, Wu WH, Gerloff C, et al. Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain (2005) 128(Pt 3):490–9. doi:10.1093/brain/awh369
23. Hummel F, Cohen LG. Improvement of motor function with noninvasive cortical stimulation in a patient with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2005) 19(1):14–9. doi:10.1177/1545968304272698
24. Boggio PS, Nunes A, Rigonatti SP, Nitsche MA, Pascual-Leone A, Fregni F. Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulation is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients. Restor Neurol Neurosci (2007) 25(2):123–9.
25. Dockery CA, Hueckel-Weng R, Birbaumer N, Plewnia C. Enhancement of planning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. J Neurosci (2009) 29(22):7271–7. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0065-09.2009
26. Lindenberg R, Renga V, Zhu LL, Nair D, Schlaug G. Bihemispheric brain stimulation facilitates motor recovery in chronic stroke patients. Neurology (2010) 75(24):2176–84. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e318202013a
27. Fregni F, Boggio PS, Valle AC, Rocha RR, Duarte J, Ferreira MJ, et al. A sham-controlled trial of a 5-day course of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Stroke (2006) 37(8):2115–22. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000231390.58967.6b
28. Murphy TH, Corbett D. Plasticity during stroke recovery: from synapse to behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10(12):861–72. doi:10.1038/nrn2735
29. Conforto AB, Anjos SM, Saposnik G, Mello EA, Nagaya EM, Santos W, Jr, et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in mild to severe hemiparesis early after stroke: a proof of principle and novel approach to improve motor function. J Neurol (2012) 259(7):1399–1405. doi:10.1007/s00415-011-6364-7
30. Kleim JA, Schwerin S. Motor map plasticity: a neural substrate for improving motor function after stroke. In: Cramer SC, Nudo RJ, editors. Brain Repair after Stroke. New York: Cambridge University Press (2010). p. 1–10.
31. Monfils MH, Plautz EJ, Kleim JA. In search of the motor engram: motor map plasticity as a mechanism for encoding motor experience. Neuroscientist (2005) 11(5):471–83. doi:10.1177/1073858405278015
32. Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji YY, Cohen LG, et al. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron (2010) 66(2):198–204. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035
33. Dos Santos-Fontes RL, de Andrade KN, Sterr A, Conforto AB. Home-based nerve stimulation to enhance effects of motor training in patients in the chronic phase after stroke: a proof-of-principle study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2013) 27(6):483–90. doi:10.1177/1545968313478488
34. Nowak DA, Grefkes C, Ameli M, Fink GR. Interhemispheric competition after stroke: brain stimulation to enhance recovery of function of the affected hand. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2009) 23(7):641–56. doi:10.1177/1545968309336661
35. Schlaug G, Cohen L. Electromagnetic approaches to stroke recovery. In: Cramer SC, Nudo RJ, editors. Brain Repair after Stroke. New York: Cambridge University Press (2010). p. 207–17.
36. Nair DG, Renga V, Lindenberg R, Zhu L, Schlaug G. Optimizing recovery potential through simultaneous occupational therapy and non-invasive brain-stimulation using tDCS. Restor Neurol Neurosci (2011) 29(6):411–20. doi:10.3233/Rnn-2011-0612
37. Elsner B, Kugler J, Pohl M, Mehrholz J. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving function and activities of daily living in patients after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2013) 11:CD009645. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD009645.pub2
38. Raffin E, Siebner HR. Transcranial brain stimulation to promote functional recovery after stroke. Curr Opin Neurol (2014) 27(1):54–60. doi:10.1097/WCO.0000000000000059
39. Riley JD, Le V, Der-Yeghiaian L, See J, Newton JM, Ward NS, et al. Anatomy of stroke injury predicts gains from therapy. Stroke (2011) 42(2):421–6. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.599340
40. Sterr A, Shen S, Szameitat AJ, Herron KA. The role of corticospinal tract damage in chronic motor recovery and neurorehabilitation: a pilot study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2010) 24(5):413–9. doi:10.1177/1545968309348310
41. Sterr A, Dean PJA, Szameitat AJ, Conforto AB, Shen S. Corticospinal tract integrity and lesion volume play different roles in chronic hemiparesis and its improvement through motor practice. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2014) 28(4):335–43. doi:10.1177/1545968313510972
42. Ameli M, Grefkes C, Kemper F, Riegg FP, Rehme AK, Karbe H, et al. Differential effects of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over ipsilesional primary motor cortex in cortical and subcortical middle cerebral artery stroke. Ann Neurol (2009) 66(3):298–309. doi:10.1002/ana.21725
43. Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, O’Shea J, Allman C, Bosnell RA, Kischka U, et al. Cortical activation changes underlying stimulation-induced behavioural gains in chronic stroke. Brain (2012) 135(Pt 1):276–84. doi:10.1093/brain/awr313
44. O’Shea J, Boudrias MH, Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Kischka U, Blicher JU, et al. Predicting behavioural response to tDCS in chronic motor stroke. Neuroimage (2014) 85(Pt 3):924–33. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.096
45. Sterr A, Furlan L. A case to be made: theoretical and empirical arguments for the need to consider fatigue in post-stroke motor rehabilitation. Neural Regen Res (2015) 10(8):1195–7. doi:10.4103/1673-5374.162689
46. Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, Saletin JM, Ancoli-Israel S, Jagust WJ, et al. Impaired prefrontal sleep spindle regulation of hippocampal-dependent learning in older adults. Cereb Cortex (2013) 24(12):3301–9. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht188
47. McCoy JG, Strecker RE. The cognitive cost of sleep lost. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2011) 96(4):564–82. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.004
48. Walker MP. Cognitive consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Med (2008) 9(Suppl 1):S29–34. doi:10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70014-5
49. Zunzunegui C, Gao B, Cam E, Hodor A, Bassetti CL. Sleep disturbance impairs stroke recovery in the rat. Sleep (2011) 34(9):1261–9. doi:10.5665/SLEEP.1252
50. Sharma S, Kavuru M. Sleep and metabolism: an overview. Int J Endocrinol (2010) 2010:270832. doi:10.1155/2010/270832
51. Wolk R, Gami AS, Garcia-Touchard A, Somers VK. Sleep and cardiovascular disease. Curr Probl Cardiol (2005) 30(12):625–62. doi:10.1016/j.cpcardiol.2005.07.002
53. Pasic Z, Smajlovic D, Dostovic Z, Kojic B, Selmanovic S. Incidence and types of sleep disorders in patients with stroke. Med Arh (2011) 65(4):225–7. doi:10.5455/medarh.2011.65.225-227
54. Terzoudi A, Vorvolakos T, Heliopoulos I, Livaditis M, Vadikolias K, Piperidou H. Sleep architecture in stroke and relation to outcome. Eur Neurol (2009) 61(1):16–22. doi:10.1159/000165344
55. da Costa SV, Ceolim MF. Factors that affect inpatients’ quality of sleep. Rev Esc Enferm USP (2013) 47(1):46–52. doi:10.1590/S0080-62342013000100006
56. Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci (2005) 28:377–401. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144216
57. Pekna M, Pekny M, Nilsson M. Modulation of neural plasticity as a basis for stroke rehabilitation. Stroke (2012) 43(10):2819–28. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.654228
58. Gais S, Born J. Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learn Mem (2004) 11(6):679–85. doi:10.1101/lm.80504
59. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J Cogn Neurosci (1997) 9(4):534–47. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.534
60. Walker MP, Stickgold R. It’s practice, with sleep, that makes perfect: implications of sleep-dependent learning and plasticity for skill performance. Clin Sports Med (2005) 24(2):301–17. doi:10.1016/j.csm.2004.11.002
61. Walker MP, Stickgold R. Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annu Rev Psychol (2006) 57:139–66. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070307
62. Walker MP, Stickgold R, Alsop D, Gaab N, Schlaug G. Sleep-dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain. Neuroscience (2005) 133(4):911–7. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.007
63. Dijk DJ. EEG slow waves and sleep spindles: windows on the sleeping brain. Behav Brain Res (1995) 69(1–2):109–16. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00007-G
64. Gorgoni M, D’Atri A, Lauri G, Rossini PM, Ferlazzo F, De Gennaro L. Is sleep essential for neural plasticity in humans, and how does it affect motor and cognitive recovery? Neural Plast (2013) 2013:103949. doi:10.1155/2013/103949
65. Reato D, Gasca F, Datta A, Bikson M, Marshall L, Parra LC. Transcranial electrical stimulation accelerates human sleep homeostasis. PLoS Comput Biol (2013) 9(2):e1002898. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002898
66. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev (2006) 10(1):49–62. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002
67. Vyazovskiy VV, Faraguna U. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2015) 25:91–121. doi:10.1007/7854_2014_301
68. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron (2014) 81(1):12–34. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025
69. Fischer S, Nitschke MF, Melchert UH, Erdmann C, Born J. Motor memory consolidation in sleep shapes more effective neuronal representations. J Neurosci (2005) 25(49):11248–55. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1743-05.2005
70. Korman M, Doyon J, Doljansky J, Carrier J, Dagan Y, Karni A. Daytime sleep condenses the time course of motor memory consolidation. Nat Neurosci (2007) 10(9):1206–13. doi:10.1038/nn1959
71. Nishida M, Walker MP. Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles. PLoS One (2007) 2(4):e341. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000341
72. Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron (2002) 35(1):205–11. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00746-8
73. Walker MP, Brakefield T, Seidman J, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Sleep and the time course of motor skill learning. Learn Mem (2003) 10(4):275–84. doi:10.1101/lm.58503
74. Nitsche MA, Jakoubkova M, Thirugnanasambandam N, Schmalfuss L, Hullemann S, Sonka K, et al. Contribution of the premotor cortex to consolidation of motor sequence learning in humans during sleep. J Neurophysiol (2010) 104(5):2603–14. doi:10.1152/jn.00611.2010
75. Maquet P, Laureys S, Peigneux P, Fuchs S, Petiau C, Phillips C, et al. Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep. Nat Neurosci (2000) 3(8):831–6. doi:10.1038/77744
76. Yang G, Lai CS, Cichon J, Ma L, Li W, Gan WB. Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science (2014) 344(6188):1173–8. doi:10.1126/science.1249098
77. Rasch B, Born J. Maintaining memories by reactivation. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2007) 17(6):698–703. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.007
78. Aton SJ, Seibt J, Dumoulin M, Jha SK, Steinmetz N, Coleman T, et al. Mechanisms of sleep-dependent consolidation of cortical plasticity. Neuron (2009) 61(3):454–66. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.007
79. Hanlon EC, Faraguna U, Vyazovskiy VV, Tononi G, Cirelli C. Effects of skilled training on sleep slow wave activity and cortical gene expression in the rat. Sleep (2009) 32(6):719–29.
80. Ribeiro S, Goyal V, Mello CV, Pavlides C. Brain gene expression during REM sleep depends on prior waking experience. Learn Mem (1999) 6(5):500–8. doi:10.1101/lm.6.5.500
81. Seibt KM, Wenke T, Wollrab C, Junghans H, Muders K, Dehmer KJ, et al. Development and application of SINE-based markers for genotyping of potato varieties. Theor Appl Genet (2012) 125(1):185–96. doi:10.1007/s00122-012-1825-7
82. Jenkins JG, Dallenbach KM. Obliviscence during sleep and waking. Am J Psychol (1924) 35:605–12. doi:10.2307/1414040
83. Cohen DA, Pascual-Leone A, Press DZ, Robertson EM. Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102(50):18237–41. doi:10.1073/pnas.0506072102
84. Fischer S, Hallschmid M, Elsner AL, Born J. Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2002) 99(18):11987–91. doi:10.1073/pnas.182178199
85. Dale Purves, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, Hall WC, LaMantia A-S. Neuroscience. 2nd ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates (2001).
86. Liebetanz D, Nitsche MA, Tergau F, Paulus W. Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain (2002) 125(Pt 10):2238–47. doi:10.1093/brain/awf238
87. Bachtiar V, Stagg CJ. The role of inhibition in human motor cortical plasticity. Neuroscience (2014) 278:93–104. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.059
88. Rioult-Pedotti MS, Friedman D, Donoghue JP. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science (2000) 290(5491):533–6. doi:10.1126/science.290.5491.533
89. Nitsche MA, Fricke K, Henschke U, Schlitterlau A, Liebetanz D, Lang N, et al. Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol (2003) 553(Pt 1):293–301. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049916
90. Stone WS, Walker DL, Gold PE. Sleep deficits in rats after NMDA receptor blockade. Physiol Behav (1992) 52(3):609–12. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(92)90355-6
91. Krystal AD, Edinger JD. Measuring sleep quality. Sleep Med (2008) 9(Suppl 1):S10–7. doi:10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70011-X
92. Riedel BW, Lichstein KL. Objective sleep measures and subjective sleep satisfaction: how do older adults with insomnia define a good night’s sleep? Psychol Aging (1998) 13(1):159–63. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.159
93. Rasch B, Born J. About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev (2013) 93(2):681–766. doi:10.1152/physrev.00032.2012
94. Rasch B, Buchel C, Gais S, Born J. Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science (2007) 315(5817):1426–9. doi:10.1126/science.1138581
95. Rudoy JD, Voss JL, Westerberg CE, Paller KA. Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep. Science (2009) 326(5956):1079. doi:10.1126/science.1179013
96. Mednick SC, Nakayama K, Cantero JL, Atienza M, Levin AA, Pathak N, et al. The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration. Nat Neurosci (2002) 5(7):677–81. doi:10.1038/nn864
97. Nissen C, Kloepfer C, Nofzinger EA, Feige B, Voderholzer U, Riemann D. Impaired sleep-related memory consolidation in primary insomnia – a pilot study. Sleep (2006) 29(8):1068–73. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00872.x
98. Westerberg CE, Mander BA, Florczak SM, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM, Zee PC, et al. Concurrent impairments in sleep and memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2012) 18(3):490–500. doi:10.1017/S135561771200001X
99. Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, Tononi G. Local sleep and learning. Nature (2004) 430(6995):78–81. doi:10.1038/nature02663
100. Kattler H, Dijk DJ, Borbely AA. Effect of unilateral somatosensory stimulation prior to sleep on the sleep EEG in humans. J Sleep Res (1994) 3(3):159–64. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00123.x
101. Vyazovskiy V, Borbely AA, Tobler I. Unilateral vibrissae stimulation during waking induces interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during subsequent sleep in the rat. J Sleep Res (2000) 9(4):367–71. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00230.x
102. Gulati T, Ramanathan DS, Wong CC, Ganguly K. Reactivation of emergent task-related ensembles during slow-wave sleep after neuroprosthetic learning. Nat Neurosci (2014) 17(8):1107–13. doi:10.1038/nn.3759
103. Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Riedner BA, Peterson MJ, et al. Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nat Neurosci (2006) 9(9):1169–76. doi:10.1038/nn1758
104. Saebipour MR, Joghataei MT, Yoonessi A, Sadeghniiat-Haghighi K, Khalighinejad N, Khademi S. Slow oscillating transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep has a sleep-stabilizing effect in chronic insomnia: a pilot study. J Sleep Res (2015) 24(5):518–25. doi:10.1111/jsr.12301
105. Eggert T, Dorn H, Sauter C, Nitsche MA, Bajbouj M, Danker-Hopfe H. No effects of slow oscillatory transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on sleep-dependent memory consolidation in healthy elderly subjects. Brain Stimul (2013) 6(6):938–45. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2013.05.006
106. Keeser D, Padberg F, Reisinger E, Pogarell O, Kirsch V, Palm U, et al. Prefrontal direct current stimulation modulates resting EEG and event-related potentials in healthy subjects: a standardized low resolution tomography (sLORETA) study. Neuroimage (2011) 55(2):644–57. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.004
107. Lo JC, Dijk DJ, Groeger JA. Comparing the effects of nocturnal sleep and daytime napping on declarative memory consolidation. PLoS One (2014) 9(9):e108100. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0108100
108. Marshall L, Molle M, Hallschmid M, Born J. Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J Neurosci (2004) 24(44):9985–92. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-04.2004
109. Benington JH, Heller HC. Restoration of brain energy metabolism as the function of sleep. Prog Neurobiol (1995) 45(4):347–60. doi:10.1016/0301-0082(94)00057-O
110. Dworak M, McCarley RW, Kim T, Kalinchuk AV, Basheer R. Sleep and brain energy levels: ATP changes during sleep. J Neurosci (2010) 30(26):9007–16. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1423-10.2010
111. Baumann CR, Kilic E, Petit B, Werth E, Hermann DM, Tafti M, et al. Sleep EEG changes after middle cerebral artery infarcts in mice: different effects of striatal and cortical lesions. Sleep (2006) 29(10):1339–44.
112. Gao B, Kilic E, Baumann CR, Hermann DM, Bassetti CL. Gamma-hydroxybutyrate accelerates functional recovery after focal cerebral ischemia. Cerebrovasc Dis (2008) 26(4):413–9. doi:10.1159/000151683
113. Mirescu C, Peters JD, Noiman L, Gould E. Sleep deprivation inhibits adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus by elevating glucocorticoids. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2006) 103(50):19170–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.0608644103
114. Mueller AD, Meerlo P, McGinty D, Mistlberger RE. Sleep and adult neurogenesis: implications for cognition and mood. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2013) 25:151–81. doi:10.1007/7854_2013_251
115. Zucconi GG, Cipriani S, Balgkouranidou I, Scattoni R. ‘One night’ sleep deprivation stimulates hippocampal neurogenesis. Brain Res Bull (2006) 69(4):375–81. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.01.009
116. Burgess N, Maguire EA, O’Keefe J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron (2002) 35(4):625–41. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9
117. King JA, Burgess N, Hartley T, Vargha-Khadem F, O’Keefe J. Human hippocampus and viewpoint dependence in spatial memory. Hippocampus (2002) 12(6):811–20. doi:10.1002/hipo.10070
118. Bangasser DA, Waxler DE, Santollo J, Shors TJ. Trace conditioning and the hippocampus: the importance of contiguity. J Neurosci (2006) 26(34):8702–6. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-06.2006
119. Rueger MA, Keuters MH, Walberer M, Braun R, Klein R, Sparing R, et al. Multi-session transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) elicits inflammatory and regenerative processes in the rat brain. PLoS One (2012) 7(8):e43776. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043776
120. Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji YY, Cohen LG, et al. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron (2010) 66(2):198–204. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035
121. Mizuno M, Yamada K, He J, Nakajima A, Nabeshima T. Involvement of BDNF receptor TrkB in spatial memory formation. Learn Mem (2003) 10(2):108–15. doi:10.1101/lm.56003
122. Slack SE, Pezet S, McMahon SB, Thompson SW, Malcangio M. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor induces NMDA receptor subunit one phosphorylation via ERK and PKC in the rat spinal cord. Eur J Neurosci (2004) 20(7):1769–78. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03656.x
123. Di Lazzaro V, Pellegrino G, Di Pino G, Corbetto M, Ranieri F, Brunelli N, et al. Val66Met BDNF gene polymorphism influences human motor cortex plasticity in acute stroke. Brain Stimul (2015) 8(1):92–6. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2014.08.006
124. Kim DY, Quinlan EB, Gramer R, Cramer SC. BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is related to motor system function after stroke. Phys Ther (2015). doi:10.2522/ptj.20150135
Keywords: stroke, neuromodulation, memory, motor learning, recovery
Citation: Ebajemito JK, Furlan L, Nissen C and Sterr A (2016) Application of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Neurorehabilitation: The Modulatory Effect of Sleep. Front. Neurol. 7:54. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00054
Received: 11 August 2015; Accepted: 24 March 2016;
Published: 06 April 2016
Edited by:
Mehmet Y. Agargün, Yuzuncu Yil University School of Medicine, TurkeyReviewed by:
Sara J. Aton, University of Michigan, USATimo Partonen, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Finland
Copyright: © 2016 Ebajemito, Furlan, Nissen and Sterr. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Annette Sterr, YS5zdGVyciYjeDAwMDQwO3N1cnJleS5hYy51aw==