Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Comput. Neurosci., 07 September 2022
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain Function: From Experimental and Computational Neuroscience to Brain Engineering Vol II View all 4 articles

Compensatory mechanism of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder recovery in resting state alpha rhythms

\r\nChuanliang Han,&#x;Chuanliang Han1,2†Tian Wang,,Tian Wang1,2,3Yujie Wu,Yujie Wu1,2Hui Li,Hui Li4,5Encong Wang,Encong Wang4,5Xixi Zhao,Xixi Zhao4,5Qingjiu Cao,Qingjiu Cao4,5Qiujin Qian,Qiujin Qian4,5Yufeng Wang,Yufeng Wang4,5Fei Dou,,Fei Dou1,3,6Jian K. LiuJian K. Liu7Li Sun,*Li Sun4,5*Dajun Xing,*Dajun Xing1,2*
  • 1State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 3College of Life Sciences, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 4Peking University Sixth Hospital/Institute of Mental Health, Beijing, China
  • 5National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorder and Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
  • 6Beijing Key Laboratory of Genetic Engineering Drugs and Biotechnology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 7School of Computing, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom

Alpha rhythms in the human electroencephalogram (EEG), oscillating at 8-13 Hz, are located in parieto-occipital cortex and are strongest when awake people close their eyes. It has been suggested that alpha rhythms were related to attention-related functions and mental disorders (e.g., Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)). However, many studies have shown inconsistent results on the difference in alpha oscillation between ADHD and control groups. Hence it is essential to verify this difference. In this study, a dataset of EEG recording (128 channel EGI) from 87 healthy controls (HC) and 162 ADHD (141 persisters and 21 remitters) adults in a resting state with their eyes closed was used to address this question and a three-gauss model (summation of baseline and alpha components) was conducted to fit the data. To our surprise, the power of alpha components was not a significant difference among the three groups. Instead, the baseline power of remission and HC group in the alpha band is significantly stronger than that of persister groups. Our results suggest that ADHD recovery may have compensatory mechanisms and many abnormalities in EEG may be due to the influence of behavior rather than the difference in brain signals.

Introduction

Alpha rhythm (8-13 Hz) is prominent in the parieto-occipital electroencephalogram (EEG) of awake humans. Alpha power increases with the eyes closed but attenuates on eye-opening (Berger, 1929; Niedermeyer, 1999; Draguhn and Buzsáki, 2004). Alpha-band activity was modulated by visual attention (Hanslmayr et al., 2011; Klimesch, 2012; Weisz et al., 2014; Snyder et al., 2015; Capotosto et al., 2017) and memory load (Palva and Palva, 2007; Freunberger et al., 2009; Sauseng et al., 2010; Foster et al., 2017). Furthermore, alpha has been considered to provide feedback connections among visual areas in both macaques and humans (Wang, 2010; Van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Samaha et al., 2015; Mejias et al., 2016; Michalareas et al., 2016; Helfrich et al., 2017). The alpha-band activities were also thought to be related to attention-related mental deficits like attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Ter Huurne et al., 2013; Mazaheri et al., 2014).

Despite over 50% of children with ADHD continuing to show symptoms in adulthood (Lara et al., 2009), adult ADHD is less well understood. Hoping to use brain oscillations in different frequency bands (delta, theta, alpha, and beta bands) as biomarkers for brain disorders, many studies have been looking for oscillatory biomarkers for ADHD adults (Clarke et al., 2008; Giertuga et al., 2017). However, many previous studies showed inconsistent results. Taking alpha band in the resting state as an example, some studies showed reduced alpha power associated with ADHD (Woltering et al., 2012; Li et al., 2019), and some indicated that the ADHD patients showed an increase in alpha power (Koehler et al., 2009), and some others showed that alpha power is not significantly changed in ADHD patients (Clarke et al., 2008). There are two possibilities that might cause this inconsistency, one reason might be the sample size in these previous studies was relatively small, and another one might be the relatively rough measurement of the power in the alpha frequency band since these studies did not dissect narrowband and broadband power in the spectrum.

In this paper, we quantitatively studied the difference in alpha-band activity among the ADHD persister, remitter, and healthy control groups, using a three-gauss model to dissect narrowband (component) and broadband (baseline) power in the alpha-band in individuals. By using power spectrum analysis with fine frequency resolution, we found evidence that indicates the existence of more than two oscillators within the alpha band. We then demonstrated how these alpha components and baseline power contributed to differences between ADHD persisters, remitters, and healthy control adults.

Materials and methods

All participants in EEG experiments gave informed consent to participate in this study. The experiments were conducted in accordance with the principles embodied in the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Peking University Institute of Mental Health, and Beijing Normal University Institutional Review Board.

Participants

A total of 261 adults (143 ADHD persisters, 24 remitters, and 94 controls) participated in the experiment, twelve participants were excluded because they did not follow the instructions. Analyzed data were collected from 162 ADHD (141 persisters and 21 remitters) adults (ages 18-39 years old) diagnosed at Peking University; 87 normal healthy controls recruited through campus advertisements also participated in the study (Table 1). ADHD participants fulfilled a diagnosis of adult ADHD through Conners’ Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. All ADHD participants were medication naive. Another current psychopathology was assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). Control participants were recruited from local universities and communities, and interviewed to ensure an absence of past or current ADHD. The ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS), Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale–Self-Report Screening Version, and SCID were applied for assessing all participants. All the control participants had no current or previous psychiatric disorders. All participants were of Chinese Han descent.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject information.

Electroencephalogram recordings in the resting state

Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a dimly lit, electrically shielded room with a low level of environmental noise. Scalp EEG data were recorded continuously with a 128-channel EEG net (Electrical Geodesic Inc., EGI). After they became familiar with the environment, participants were told to close their eyes and stay relaxed and still for 6-10 min (average of 8.84 min), and to do nothing else. Scalp EEG data were recorded at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. All electrode impedances were kept under 50 kΩ. Data were referenced to electrode CZ originally and then referenced to a frontal channel near FZ. The stereotypical artifacts, such as eye blinks, eye movements, and muscle tension, were separately removed using the artifact rejection method based on the blind source separation algorithm, Independent Component Analysis (ICA) (Delorme and Makeig, 2004; Li et al., 2019; Zhao et al., 2020). On average, there are four ICs were selected to remove.

Data analysis

Data processing was performed in MATLAB1 with custom scripts. The original continuous data were high-pass filtered at 0.5 Hz and low-pass at 40 Hz. Both the high-pass and low-pass filters were zero-phased FIR filters (third order Butterworth filter) which filter the data both forward and backward to ensure phase delays introduced by each filter are nullified. The dickey-Fuller test was used to test the stationary property of the data. Power spectra of the EEG signals were calculated using multi-taper methods with 5 tapers. Each epoch lasted 10 s, enabling a precise spectrum with a resolution of 0.1 Hz.

Three gaussian model

The Three Gaussian Model is the summation of a baseline and three gaussian functions which represent three component sources in the alpha band for all 128 channels (Figures 1, 2). It is described as follows:

Power(h,f)=Baseline(h,f)+Wh,1×S(1,h,f)
+Wh,2×S(2,h,f)
          +Wh,3×S(3,h,f)(1)
Baseline(h,f)=1ahf+bh+ch(2)
S(i,h,f)=exp(-(f-μhi)2σhi 2)(3)
FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Demo of EEG signal during eye-closed state. (A) shows the demo of the channel locations of the EEG, (B) shows the EEG signal in a demo electrode in the occipital region and (C) shows the corresponding power spectrums.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Emergence of multiple alpha rhythms could be well explained by a three-gaussian model. Two subjects’ EEG power spectrums from three example electrodes in three brain regions were shown in the first and second rows, respectively. The raw data was shown in black dots, the fitted curve was shown in a black curve, and the baseline fitted in the model was shown in a black dashed curve.

where S(i,h,f) is the i-th source function in h-th electrode of the signal depending on the frequency f, Wi is the weight of each source, μi and σi are peak frequency and bandwidth of the i-th source. To evaluate the fitting performance, we calculated the fitting index as follows, the value of which indicated the percentage variance that can be explained by the model. This method has been used in describing gamma-band activity (Han et al., 2020, 2021a,b,c; Wang et al., 2021).

FittingIndex=1-Σ(FittingData-RawData)2N*Σ(var(FittingData)+var(RawData))(4)

Statistical analysis

We used the Jarque-Bera test for the normality of the data. The non-parametric ANOVA test (Kruskal-Walis H test) was employed first to test whether the difference exists for the power (baseline and components) among three groups (ADHD persister, remitter, and control), and then the Mann–Whitney U test with Bonferroni correction was used as the post-hoc to check the difference between pairs of groups.

Results

To characterize alpha-band oscillations in the resting state, we recorded the Scalp EEG from ADHD persister, remitter, and healthy control adults. Participants were told to close their eyes and stay relaxed without doing anything for 6-10 min (Figure 1). For each EEG channel, the recorded data was divided into small segments 10 s in length, and then the power spectrum was estimated by a multi-taper method with a frequency resolution of 0.1 Hz (see Materials and methods).

Multiple oscillatory peaks found in alpha band

To see the EEG power in the alpha range more clearly on each electrode, with fine frequency resolution (0.1 Hz), two or even three frequency peaks were visible on many electrodes from temporal (Figure 2 first column), parietal (Figure 2 second column) to occipital (Figure 2 third column) lobe. Based on carefully scrutinizing our data (N = 249), we found that it was typical that there were three frequency components in the narrow range of the alpha band (low alpha (LA), medium alpha (MA), and high alpha (HA)). Their range was defined based on the value orders of their peak frequencies fitted by the descriptive model. Low alpha was defined as the alpha with the lowest peak frequency (LA: mean = 8.42 ± 0.94 Hz), and the high alpha was defined as the alpha with the highest peak frequency (HA: mean = 11.81 ± 0.84 Hz); the medium alpha was defined as the one between LA and HA (MA: mean = 10.15 ± 0.76 Hz).

Dissecting different components in the alpha band

Based on the observation in our database, we hypothesized that for each individual subject, the EEG power in the resting state in the alpha range on all 128 electrodes could be modeled by the sum of three frequency components. In more detail, the EEG power in the alpha range was a weighted sum of three frequency components and a baseline (Figure 1 dashed line). The frequency profiles of the three components were all modeled as Gaussian functions, and the frequency profile of the baseline was modeled as a function decreasing monotonically in frequency (see more details in the Materials and methods section for model and model fitting). The three oscillatory components were LA, MA, and HA, with different peaking frequencies (LA: mean = 8.42 ± 0.94 Hz; MA: mean = 10.15 ± 0.76 Hz; HA: mean = 11.81 ± 0.84 Hz) (Figure 3). We found that the EEG power could be reconstructed very well by this model. Across the whole dataset, the model explained 99.29% variance of the normalized data (99.27, 99.31, and 99.35% in groups of ADHD persister, control, and ADHD remitter, respectively).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. 3D Topographic map of three alpha components. The first row shows the spatial distribution of the power of three alpha components was shown, respectively. The second row shows the distribution of the peak frequency of three alpha components.

Difference of alpha components and baseline power among attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder persisters, remitters, and healthy controls

We further asked whether the powers of alpha components and baseline showed significant differences among ADHD persister, remitter, and normal group by the non-parametric one-way ANOVA test (Kruskal-Walis H test). We found that the power of alpha components was not significantly different among the three groups by one-way ANOVA test (Figure 4; in parietal region, LA: p = 0.11, MA: p = 0.98, HA: p = 0.18; in occipital region, LA: p = 0.18, MA: p = 0.62, HA: p = 0.51). Instead, the baseline power of the three groups showed strong significance among three groups by one-way ANOVA test (Figure 4; in parietal region, LA: p < 0.001, MA: p < 0.001, HA: p < 0.001; in occipital region, LA: p < 0.001, MA: p < 0.001, HA: p < 0.001). In specific multiple comparison, the ADHD persister group is significant smaller than that of remitter (Figure 4; in parietal region, LA: p = 0.0028, MA: p = 0.0034, HA: p = 0.0017; in occipital region, LA: p = 0.047, MA: p = 0.040, HA: p = 0.029) and health control group (Figure 4; in parietal region, LA: p < 0.001, MA: p = 0.0066, HA: p = 0.0074; in occipital region, LA: p = 0.0011, MA: p = 0.0024, HA: p = 0.0014).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparison of the components and baseline power in alpha band among three groups. The baseline power of three groups showed strong significance among three groups (One-way ANOVA) (in parietal region, LA: p < 0.001, MA: p < 0.001, HA: p < 0.001; in occipital region, LA: p < 0.001, MA: p < 0.001, HA: p < 0.001). The multiple comparison of the baseline power in three alpha bands after one-way ANOVA test was shown in the first (parietal region) and third (occipital region) row, where red is for ADHD group, blue is for HC group, and light blue is for remission group (ns: no significance, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001). The power of alpha components was not significantly (One-way ANOVA) different among three groups (in parietal region (second row), LA: p = 0.11, MA: p = 0.98, HA: p = 0.18; in occipital region (fourth row), LA: p = 0.18, MA: p = 0.62, HA: p = 0.51).

Discussion

With the increasing number of studies on alpha rhythm and its relationship to cognitive functions, it is important to understand the basic properties of alpha. In this study, we developed a model to dissect three oscillators in the alpha range (8-13 Hz) in individual humans. The model enabled us to characterize the properties of distinct alpha components among different groups of human subjects. Our results showed that no significant difference in alpha was found. However, the baseline of the spectrum is significantly different. Our results not only suggest a compensatory mechanism for ADHD recovery in brain oscillations but also demonstrate that dissecting distinct narrow-band oscillatory components is a necessary step for understanding their relation to cognitive functions and brain disorders.

Compensatory mechanism of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder recovery

There are some possible perspectives on the mechanisms of ADHD remission (Sudre et al., 2018). Some researchers found evidence of a convergence mechanism (Schneider et al., 2010; Shaw et al., 2013; Hoogman et al., 2017; Schulz et al., 2017) that views AD/HD as a neurodevelopment defect and the rectification of early anomalies in brain structure with age contributes to the relief of clinical symptoms (El-Sayed et al., 2003). Others proposed that the neural anomalies of AD/HD leave an indelible mark on the brain that will persist across the lifespan regardless of the clinical effect of AD/HD, and remitters recruit new brain systems that allow effective compensation for AD/HD symptoms (Proal et al., 2011; Francx et al., 2015) which is referred to as the “fixed trait” and compensation mechanism. Our results found that the power of alpha components was not significantly different among the three groups. Instead, the baseline power of the remission group in the high alpha band is significantly smaller than that of persisters and healthy controls. This suggests that ADHD recovery may have a compensatory mechanism.

Our work detected a decreased baseline power in the high alpha band in the ADHD remission group. The previous inconsistent results might be due to a blurring of multiple oscillatory components in the alpha band. We should also notice that for the amplitude of alpha components, there is no significant difference among ADHD persisters, remitters, and normal adults, which may indicate that these alpha components are modulated by some other tasks. Further, dissecting oscillatory components not only increases the sensitivity of specific functions related to alpha but also creates a more precise frequency target for neurofeedback, which has attracted more and more attention recently for the treatment of brain disorders including ADHD.

Multiple distinct alpha oscillation-band vs. sub-bands of alpha

With the increasing number of studies on alpha oscillation, it has been suggested that there existed two or even three sub-bands of alpha oscillation (Klimesch, 1999; Makeig et al., 2002), which might be related to different cognitive functions. Previous results have shown that the power in these different sub-bands (upper and lower alpha) also differed in tasks requiring visual attention (Ding et al., 2006; Sadaghiani et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2016) and memory (Gruber et al., 2005; Michels et al., 2008; Hanslmayr et al., 2012; Elmer et al., 2015; Barnes et al., 2016), and also in the brain’s network (Murias et al., 2007; Sadaghiani et al., 2012), mental disorders (Stoffers et al., 2007; Poil et al., 2014; Yu et al., 2017), neurofeedback training and resting state (Manshanden et al., 2002; Thorpe et al., 2016). Some animal and human studies also suggested that different cortical regions could generate their own alpha oscillations (Lopes Da Silva et al., 1977; Bollimunta et al., 2008, 2011; Scheering et al., 2016). More specifically, previous works have shown that alpha rhythms could be dissected into two components in scalp EEG (Chiang et al., 2011; Barzegaran et al., 2017; Knyazeva et al., 2018). Our results showed that three components are necessary to reconstruct the power spectrum around the alpha band for most individuals. This suggests that alpha contains at least three distinct and significant oscillatory components in the resting EEG, a result that is consistent with the three sub-bands concept. However, our results also suggest that the way to divide multiple oscillatory components based on fixed frequency bands/ranges with respect to the alpha peak frequency might not be precise; one precise way to divide these components should be based on their oscillatory properties, such as peak frequencies and bandwidths in the power spectrum. The theory of Alpha suppression suggests that many cortical regions can generate alpha rhythm when the main rhythm is inactivated (Palva and Palva, 2007) and electrophysiological studies on animals also showed that multiple cortical regions could generate their own alpha oscillation (Lopes Da Silva et al., 1977; Bollimunta et al., 2008, 2011). Therefore, in theory, we might be able to find multiple alphas in the scalp EEG, but practically, our data suggest that three components are enough to capture alpha at the resting state.

Potential sources for the multiple alpha components

Besides the alpha rhythm in the parieto-occipital lobe, two other rhythms, the mu rhythm and the sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) were also found oscillating in the alpha band (8-13 Hz). The mu rhythm is found in the sensorimotor, motor, and somatosensory cortex (Arroyo et al., 1993; Jones et al., 2010; Liao et al., 2015; Coll et al., 2017). The sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) appears over the sensorimotor cortex (Reichert et al., 2015). Some studies suggested that spectral or topographic properties of the functionally- identified mu rhythm strongly reflect those of upper alpha (Thorpe et al., 2016). Based on previous work on the brain regions that are sources of the EEG (Plattner et al., 2014), our results indicate that low and high alpha is unlikely to be mu and SMR because they are mostly peaking in the parietal lobe, more posterior to sensorimotor, motor and somatosensory cortex. Besides mu and SMR rhythms, alpha oscillation can also be generated by other cortical regions, including multiple areas in the visual cortex (Lopes Da Silva et al., 1977; Bollimunta et al., 2008, 2011).

Limitations of the study

In this study, there are some limitations should be mentioned. The subjects were Chinese, whose data was collected in Beijing, China. Future studies would collect the data in a wider spatial range. Another limitation is the small sample size of the ADHD remitter group, since the EEG data of the follow-up group is precious, and not easy to collect. Future work would consider more follow-up subjects.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Peking University Institute of Mental Health. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DX, CH, YW, and LS designed the research. LS, CH, and DX performed the research. CH, HL, EW, XZ, and DX analyzed the data. All authors wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China Grants 32171033 (DX), 81971284 (LS), National Natural Science Foundation of China Grant 32100831 (TW), The Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (DX), The 111 Project Grant (BP0719032) (DX). All the above funders had no role in study design, data collection, and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.mathworks.com

References

Arroyo, S., Lesser, R. P., Gordon, B., Uematsu, S., Jackson, D., and Webber, R. (1993). Functional significance of the mu rhythm of human cortex: an electrophysiologic study with subdural electrodes. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 87, 76–87. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(93)90114-B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, J. J., Nobre, A. C., Woolrich, M. W., Baker, K., and Astle, D. E. (2016). Training working memory in childhood enhances couplinbetween frontoparietal control network and task-related regions. J. Neurosci. 36, 9001–9011. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0101-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barzegaran, E., Vildavski, V. Y., and Knyazeva, M. G. (2017). Fine Structure of Posterior alpha rhythm in human EEG: frequency components, their cortical sources, and temporal behavior. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08421-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, H. (1929). Über das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 87, 527–570. doi: 10.1007/BF01797193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bollimunta, A., Chen, Y., Schroeder, C. E., and Ding, M. (2008). Neuronal mechanisms of cortical alpha oscillations in awake-behaving macaques. J. Neurosci. 28, 9976–9988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2699-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bollimunta, A., Mo, J., Schroeder, C. E., and Ding, M. (2011). Neuronal mechanisms and attentional modulation of corticothalamic alpha oscillations. J. Neurosci. 31, 4935–4943. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5580-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capotosto, P., Baldassarre, A., Sestieri, C., Spadone, S., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2017). Task and regions specific top-down modulation of alpha rhythms in parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 27, 4815–4822. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, A. K. I., Rennie, C. J., Robinson, P. A., van Albada, S. J., and Kerr, C. C. (2011). Age trends and sex differences of alpha rhythms including split alpha peaks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1505–1517. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.01.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Heaven, P. C. L., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., and Byrne, M. K. (2008). EEG in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 70, 176–183. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coll, M. P., Press, C., Hobson, H., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2017). Crossmodal classification of mu rhythm activity during action observation and execution suggests specificity to somatosensory features of actions. J. Neurosci. 37, 5936–5947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3393-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, J., Sperling, G., and Srinivasan, R. (2006). Attentional modulation of SSVEP power depends on the network tagged by the flicker frequency. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1016–1029. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Draguhn, A., and Buzsáki, G. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1930.

Google Scholar

El-Sayed, E., Larsson, J. O., Persson, H. E., Santosh, P. J., and Rydelius, P. A. (2003). Maturational lag” hypothesis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an update. Acta Paediatr. 92, 776–784. doi: 10.1080/08035250310002777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elmer, S., Rogenmoser, L., Kuhnis, J., and Jäncke, L. (2015). Bridging the gap between perceptual and cognitive perspectives on absolute pitch. J. Neurosci. 35, 366–371. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3009-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, J. J., Bsales, E. M., Jaffe, R. J., and Awh, E. (2017). Alpha-Band activity reveals spontaneous representations of spatial position in visual working memory. Curr. Biol. 27, 3216–3223. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francx, W., Oldehinkel, M., Oosterlaan, J., Heslenfeld, D., Hartman, C. A., Hoekstra, P. J., et al. (2015). The executive control network and symptomatic improvement in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cortex 73, 62–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freunberger, R., Fellinger, R., Sauseng, P., Gruber, W., and Klimesch, W. (2009). Dissociation between phase-locked and nonphase-locked alpha oscillations in a working memory task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3417–3425. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giertuga, K., Zakrzewska, M. Z., Bielecki, M., Racicka-Pawlukiewicz, E., Kossut, M., and Cybulska-Klosowicz, A. (2017). Age-related changes in resting-state eeg activity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a cross-sectional study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:285. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruber, W. R., Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Doppelmayr, M. (2005). Alpha phase synchronization predicts P1 end N1 latency and amplitude size. Cereb. Cortex 15, 371–377. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Wang, B., Yang, G., Dai, W., Yang, Y., Wang, T., et al. (2020). Neural mechanism of orientation selectivity for distinct gamma oscillations in cat V1. J. Vis. 20:1116. doi: 10.1167/jov.20.11.1116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Wang, T., Wu, Y., Li, Y., Yang, Y., Li, L., et al. (2021a). The generation and modulation of distinct gamma oscillations with local, horizontal, and feedback connections in the primary visual cortex: A model study on large-scale networks. Neural Plast. 2021:8874516. doi: 10.1155/2021/8874516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Wang, T., Yang, Y., Wu, Y., Li, Y., Dai, W., et al. (2021b). Multiple gamma rhythms carry distinct spatial frequency information in primary visual cortex. PLoS Biol. 19:e3001466. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C. L., Shapley, R., and Xing, D. J. (2021c). Gamma rhythms in the visual cortex: Functions and mechanisms. Cogn. Neurodyn. 16, 745–756. doi: 10.1007/s11571-021-09767-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., Gross, J., Klimesch, W., and Shapiro, K. L. (2011). The role of alpha oscillations in temporal attention. Brain Res. Rev. 67, 331–343. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2011.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., Volberg, G., Wimber, M., Oehler, N., Staudigl, T., Hartmann, T., et al. (2012). Prefrontally driven downregulation of neural synchrony mediates goal-directed forgetting. J. Neurosci. 32, 14742–14751. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1777-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., Huang, M., Wilson, G., and Knight, R. T. (2017). Prefrontal cortex modulates posterior alpha oscillations during top-down guided visual perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 9457–9462. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705965114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogman, M., Bralten, J., Hibar, D. P., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., Schweren, L. S. J., et al. (2017). Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 4, 310–319. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. R., Kerr, C. E., Wan, Q., Pritchett, D. L., Hämäläinen, M., and Moore, C. I. (2010). Cued spatial attention drives functionally relevant modulation of the mu rhythm in primary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 13760–13765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2969-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knyazeva, M. G., Barzegaran, E., Vildavski, V. Y., and Demonet, J. F. (2018). Aging of human alpha rhythm. Neurobiol. Aging 69, 261–273. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehler, S., Lauer, P., Schreppel, T., Jacob, C., Heine, M., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., et al. (2009). Increased EEG power density in alpha and theta bands in adult ADHD patients. J. Neural Transm. 116, 97–104. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0157-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lara, C., Fayyad, J., de Graaf, R., Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Angermeyer, M., et al. (2009). Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results from the world health organization world mental health survey initiative. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Zhao, Q., Huang, F., Cao, Q., Qian, Q., Johnstone, S. J., et al. (2019). Increased beta activity links to impaired emotional control in ADHD Adults With High IQ. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 754–764. doi: 10.1177/1087054717739120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, Y., Acar, Z. A., Makeig, S., and Deak, G. (2015). EEG imaging of toddlers during dyadic turn-taking: mu-rhythm modulation while producing or observing social actions. Neuroimage 112, 52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Bengson, J., Huang, H., Mangun, G. R., and Ding, M. (2016). Top-down modulation of neural activity in anticipatory visual attention: control mechanisms revealed by simultaneous EEG-fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 26, 517–529. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes Da Silva, F. H., Storm Van, and Leeuwen, W. (1977). The cortical source of the alpha rhythm. Neurosci. Lett. 6, 237–241. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(77)90024-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Jung, T. P., Enghoff, S., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., et al. (2002). Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science. 295, 690–694. doi: 10.1126/science.1066168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manshanden, I., De Munck, J. C., Simon, N. R., Lopes, and da Silva, F. H. (2002). Source localization of MEG sleep spindles and the relation to sources of alpha band rhythms. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1937–1947. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00304-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazaheri, A., Fassbender, C., Coffey-Corina, S., Hartanto, T. A., Schweitzer, J. B., and Mangun, G. R. (2014). Differential oscillatory electroencephalogram between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes and typically developing adolescents. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 422–429. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mejias, J. F., Murray, J. D., Kennedy, H., and Wang, X. J. (2016). Feedforward and feedback frequency-dependent interactions in a large-scale laminar network of the primate cortex. Sci. Adv. 2:e1601335. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1601335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalareas, G., Vezoli, J., van Pelt, S., Schoffelen, J. M., Kennedy, H., and Fries, P. (2016). Alpha-Beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron 89, 384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michels, L., Moazami-Goudarzi, M., Jeanmonod, D., and Sarnthein, J. (2008). EEG alpha distinguishes between cuneal and precuneal activation in working memory. Neuroimage 40, 1296–1310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murias, M., Webb, S. J., Greenson, J., and Dawson, G. (2007). Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in eeg coherence in individuals with autism. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 270–273. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niedermeyer, E. (1999). “The normal eeg of the waking adult,” in Electroencephalography: Basic principles, Clinical Applications and Related Fields, 4th Edn, eds E. Niedermeyer and F. Lopes da Silva (Philadelphia, PA: Williams and Wilkins), 149–173.

Google Scholar

Palva, S., and Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 150–158. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plattner, K., Lambert, M. I., Tam, N., Lamberts, R. P., and Baumeister, J. (2014). Changes in cortical beta activity related to a biceps brachii movement task while experiencing exercise induced muscle damage. Physiol. Behav. 123, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poil, S. S., Bollmann, S., Ghisleni, C., O’Gorman, R. L., Klaver, P., Ball, J., et al. (2014). Age dependent electroencephalographic changes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1626–1638. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proal, E., Reiss, P. T., Klein, R. G., Mannuzza, S., Gotimer, K., Ramos-Olazagasti, M. A., et al. (2011). Brain gray matter deficits at 33-year follow-up in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder established in childhood. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 1122–1134. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichert, J. L., Kober, S. E., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2015). Resting-state sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) power predicts the ability to up-regulate SMR in an EEG-instrumental conditioning paradigm. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 2068–2077. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadaghiani, S., Scheeringa, R., Lehongre, K., Morillon, B., Giraud, A. L., D’Esposito, M., et al. (2012). Alpha-band phase synchrony is related to activity in the fronto-parietal adaptive control network. J. Neurosci. 32, 14305–14310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1358-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadaghiani, S., Scheeringa, R., Lehongre, K., Morillon, B., Giraud, A. L., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2010). Intrinsic connectivity networks, alpha oscillations, and tonic alertness: a simultaneous electroencephalography/functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 30, 10243–10250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1004-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samaha, J., Bauer, P., Cimaroli, S., and Postle, B. R. (2015). Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 8439–8444. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1503686112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger, R., and Klimesch, W. (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1015–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheering, R., Koopmans, P. J., Van Mourik, T., Jensen, O., and Norris, D. G. (2016). The relationship between oscillatory EEG activity and the laminar-specific BOLD signal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 6761–6766. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1522577113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M. F., Krick, C. M., Retz, W., Hengesch, G., Retz-Junginger, P., Reith, W., et al. (2010). Impairment of fronto-striatal and parietal cerebral networks correlates with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) psychopathology in adults – A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 183, 75–84. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. P., Li, X., Clerkin, S. M., Fan, J., Berwid, O. G., Newcorn, J. H., et al. (2017). Prefrontal and parietal correlates of cognitive control related to the adult outcome of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder diagnosed in childhood. Cortex 90, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Greenstein, D., De Rossi, P., and Sharp, W. (2013). Trajectories of cerebral cortical development in childhood and adolescence and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, A. C., Morais, M. J., Willis, C. M., and Smith, M. A. (2015). Global network influences on local functional connectivity. Nat. Neurosci 18, 736–743. doi: 10.1038/nn.3979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoffers, D., Bosboom, J. L. W., Deijen, J. B., Wolters, E. C., Berendse, H. W., and Stam, C. J. (2007). Slowing of oscillatory brain activity is a stable characteristic of Parkinson’s disease without dementia. Brain 130, 1847–1860. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudre, G., Mangalmurti, A., and Shaw, P. (2018). Growing out of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Insights from the ‘remitted’ brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 94, 198–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ter Huurne, N., Onnink, M., Kan, C., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J., and Jensen, O. (2013). Behavioral consequences of aberrant alpha lateralization in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 227–233. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorpe, S. G., Cannon, E. N., and Fox, N. A. (2016). Rhythms from infancy through adulthood. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 254–269. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Kerkoerle, T., Self, M. W., Dagnino, B., Gariel-Mathis, M. A., Poort, J., Van Der Togt, C., et al. (2014). Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 14332–14341.

Google Scholar

Wang, B., Han, C., Wang, T., Dai, W., Li, Y., Yang, Y., et al. (2021). Superimposed gratings induce diverse response patterns of gamma oscillations in primary visual cortex. Sci. Rep. 11:4941. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83923-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. J. (2010). Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol. Rev. 90, 1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, N., Müller, N., Jatzev, S., and Bertrand, O. (2014). Oscillatory alpha modulations in right auditory regions reflect the validity of acoustic cues in an auditory spatial attention task. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2579–2590. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woltering, S., Jung, J., Liu, Z., and Tannock, R. (2012). Resting state EEG oscillatory power differences in ADHD college students and their peers. Behav. Brain Funct. 8, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Engels, M. M. A., Hillebrand, A., Van Straaten, E. C. W., Gouw, A. A., Teunissen, C., et al. (2017). Selective impairment of hippocampus and posterior hub areas in Alzheimer’s disease: an MEG-based multiplex network study. Brain 140, 1466–1485. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X., Li, H., Wang, E., Luo, X., Han, C., Cao, Q., et al. (2020). Neural correlates of working memory deficits in different adult outcomes of ADHD: an event-related potential study. Front. Psychiatry 11:348. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder, alpha oscillation, multiple components, resting state, model fitting

Citation: Han C, Wang T, Wu Y, Li H, Wang E, Zhao X, Cao Q, Qian Q, Wang Y, Dou F, Liu JK, Sun L and Xing D (2022) Compensatory mechanism of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder recovery in resting state alpha rhythms. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 16:883065. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2022.883065

Received: 24 February 2022; Accepted: 17 June 2022;
Published: 07 September 2022.

Edited by:

Hamid Reza Marateb, Universitat Politecnica de Catalunya, Spain

Reviewed by:

Daqing Guo, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China
Marjan Mansourian, Universitat Politecnica de Catalunya, Spain

Copyright © 2022 Han, Wang, Wu, Li, Wang, Zhao, Cao, Qian, Wang, Dou, Liu, Sun and Xing. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dajun Xing, ZGFqdW5feGluZ0BibnUuZWR1LmNu; Li Sun, c3VubGlvaEBiam11LmVkdS5jbg==

Present address: Chuanliang Han, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Neuropsychiatric Modulation and Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Behavior, Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Shenzhen–Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions, Shenzhen, China

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.