Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Comput. Neurosci., 25 May 2016

Dependence of Nociceptive Detection Thresholds on Physiological Parameters and Capsaicin-Induced Neuroplasticity: A Computational Study

  • 1Applied Analysis, MIRA Institute for Technical Medicine and Biomedical Technology, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
  • 2Biomedical Signals and Systems, MIRA Institute for Technical Medicine and Biomedical Technology, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands

Physiological properties of peripheral and central nociceptive subsystems can be altered over time due to medical interventions. The effective change for the whole nociceptive system can be reflected in changes of psychophysical characteristics, e.g., detection thresholds. However, it is challenging to separate contributions of distinct altered mechanisms with measurements of thresholds only. Here, we aim to understand how these alterations affect Aδ-fiber-mediated nociceptive detection of electrocutaneous stimuli. First, with a neurophysiology-based model, we study the effects of single-model parameters on detection thresholds. Second, we derive an expression of model parameters determining the functional relationship between detection thresholds and the interpulse interval for double-pulse stimuli. Third, in a case study with topical capsaicin treatment, we translate neuroplasticity into plausible changes of model parameters. Model simulations qualitatively agree with changes in experimental detection thresholds. The simulations with individual forms of neuroplasticity confirm that nerve degeneration is the dominant mechanism for capsaicin-induced increases in detection thresholds. In addition, our study suggests that capsaicin-induced central plasticity may last at least 1 month.

1. Introduction

The nociceptive system processes pain-related information and its function results from the delicate balance of a myriad of mechanisms (Coutaux et al., 2005; Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009; Sandkühler, 2009). Clinical interventions can perturb or recover this balance in multiple ways (Kyranou and Puntillo, 2012). Changes in the whole nociceptive system may be assessed with quantitative sensory testing (Wilder-Smith, 2002; Maier et al., 2010). However, separate contributions of distinct mechanisms are difficult to assess, which may be unraveled partially by using a modeling approach accounting for the relevant mechanisms.

The nociceptive system contains multiple pathways to detect noxious stimuli, i.e., actually or potentially tissue damaging events. The peripheral paths start with myelinated Aδ- and non-myelinated C-fibers, e.g., with nerve endings in the skin, responding to such stimuli. These fibers project to secondary neurons in the dorsal horn. Central terminals of both types of fibers contact secondary neurons extensively within the superficial dorsal horn, where nociceptive information is relayed to supra-spinal structures (Todd, 2010). Changes in the neurophysiological properties of both peripheral and central nociceptive subsystems can be induced by diseases, clinical interventions, or experimental conditioning stimuli, and can result in sensitization manifesting itself as hyperalgesia (Sandkühler, 2009). Sensitization is hypothesized to be one important factor leading to persistent pain (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009; Woolf, 2011). For an improved treatment of persistent pain, it is important to understand the individual and combined contributions of peripheral and central nociceptive processing.

To study neuroplasticity underlying sensitization processes, we need to preferentially measure activation of nociceptive subsystems. Electrocutaneous stimulation at low stimulus amplitudes achieves this preferential activation of Aδ-fibers (Inui et al., 2002; Mouraux et al., 2010; Mørch et al., 2011; Steenbergen et al., 2012). Furthermore, electrical stimulation can be applied as a square wave pulse train characterized by three temporal properties, namely the number of pulses (NoP), the interpulse interval (IPI), and the pulse width (PW). The relation between the detection probability and stimulus amplitudes can be studied with fixed temporal properties. This relation, referred to as the psychometric function, is different for various values of temporal properties. A short PW (i.e., < 1 ms) controls the activation of Aδ fibers, whereas the IPI is in the order of tens of milliseconds and contributes to temporal summation of neuronal activity of dorsal horn neurons. To study nociceptive processing underlying the detection task, we developed a computational model accounting for activation of Aδ-fibers and central processing by secondary dorsal horn neurons (Yang et al., 2015). This computational model is consistent with the principle of probability summation in psychophysical studies, and we refer to it as the hazard model (HM). The computational convenience of the HM may facilitate further model-based studies. In addition, the model replicated the experimentally observed dependence of detection thresholds on temporal stimulus properties for healthy subjects. However, it was not addressed how changes of parameters due to neuroplasticity affect detection thresholds.

As a tool in pain research and as a therapeutic agent, capsaicin, the pungent substance in chili peppers, is widely used due to its capability to induce multiple forms of plasticity (O'Neill et al., 2012). Topical application of capsaicin is used as an experimental human pain model, thereby altering normal nociceptive functioning (Schmelz and Kress, 1996; Petersen and Rowbotham, 1999) accompanied by changes in peripheral and central nociceptive properties. Topical application of capsaicin includes activation of afferent fibers via the transient receptor potential vanilloid 1 (TRPV1) by increasing calcium and sodium influx into peripheral fibers (O'Neill et al., 2012). A high level of intracellular calcium ions causes the degeneration of nerve endings. In addition to structural changes, activation of C-fibers triggers the release of neuropeptides leading to sensitization of peripheral nerve endings and dorsal horn neurons (Coutaux et al., 2005; Todd, 2010). A significant effect of 8%-topical capsaicin treatment on detection thresholds was observed over three months in a human subject study for healthy subjects (Doll et al., 2016b). In addition, different patterns of changes of detection thresholds for single- and double-pulse stimuli were observed, shown in Figure 1. However, a mechanistic explanation is not straightforward as changes in multiple subsystems may be involved.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Detection thresholds (mean ± SEM) using the four combinations of temporal properties on five study days before and after 1-h capsaicin treatment (reproduced from Doll et al., 2016b under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License). (A) (NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms), (B) (NoP = 1, PW = 0.525 ms), (C) (NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms), and (D) (NoP = 2, IPI = 50 ms, PW = 0.525 ms), respectively.

Here, we propose and demonstrate a model-based approach to understand the effects of neuroplasticity on nociceptive detection of electrocutaneous stimuli. With the hazard model, we study how variations in single-model parameters affect detection thresholds by parameter sweeping. Next, we find that the dependence of detection thresholds on the IPI for double-pulse stimuli can be non-monotone for which we derive an explicit condition. As a case study using 8%-topical capsaicin treatment, we translate neuroplasticity into plausible perturbations of model parameters over three months. We compare patterns of changes in model-based detection thresholds with experimentally obtained patterns qualitatively. We discuss how our findings could be used for future experiments or further exploration of capsaicin-induced effects relevant to pain management.

2. Materials and Methods

We begin with a brief description of a previously built computational model of Aδ-fiber-mediated detection of electrocutaneous stimuli. Next, to understand possible neuroplasticity, we vary each model parameter to study its effects on detection thresholds. For double-pulse stimuli, we derive a quantity determining the monotonicity of detection thresholds with respect to the interpulse interval. Lastly, to investigate capsaicin-induced neuroplasticity, we simulate our model with plausible perturbations of model parameters.

2.1. Computational Modeling of Nociceptive Detection of Electrocutaneous Stimulation

In psychophysical experiments, electrocutaneous stimuli are delivered by an intra-epidermal needle electrode (Steenbergen et al., 2012; Doll et al., 2014). In this experiment, stimuli with multiple combinations of temporal properties (NoP, IPI, and PW) were applied to a human subject. The inter-stimulus interval was 3 s on average. The experimental details were reported in Doll et al. (2016a,b). For each stimulus, the binary response R was recorded, i.e., R = 1 if the stimulus was detected, and R = 0 if not. Fixing the three temporal properties, a psychometric function describes the conditional probability to detect a stimulus with the amplitude (A), i.e., Pr(R = 1|A). In our modeling study, we denote Ψ(A) as the model-based psychometric function. The binary response in a single trial can be simulated by drawing a random number ξ from a standard uniform distribution. The response is R = 1 when ξ < Ψ(A), indicating that the stimulus is detected, and R = 0 otherwise. We denote the model-based detection threshold by A50, and it is implicitly defined by Ψ(A50) = 0.5. As Ψ(A) is an increasing function of A, the threshold A50 is well defined if Ψ(0) < 0.5. Then, A50 can be determined numerically.

Here, we briefly describe the model that represents essential mechanisms of Aδ-fiber-mediated nociceptive processing (Yang et al., 2015). We start with peripheral mechanisms. Electrocutaneous stimulation at low amplitudes focally recruits nerve endings of Aδ fibers (Mouraux et al., 2010). According to the cutaneous innervation (Provitera et al., 2007; Granstein and Luger, 2009), one can treat nerve endings perpendicular to the skin surface, where the needle electrode was attached. Upon brief stimulation (i.e., PW < 1 ms), peripheral activation depends on both the geometry (e.g., the depth of nerve endings h) and neurophysiological properties of both endings (e.g., the time constant τ1 and the firing threshold Vth) and the skin (e.g., electrical conductivity). Hence, peripheral activation is described by the threshold-linear function [fA − α1]+: = π(fA − α1)H(fA − α1), where fA:=A(1-exp(-PWτ1)), H(·) is the Heaviside step function, and α1 is the activation threshold of afferent fibers. In the model, electrical conductivity and resistance of the skin are absorbed in the lumped parameter α1, see its expression in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of physical and lumped parameters in the computational model.

As Aδ-fibers are myelinated, the evoked spikes propagate robustly along the axons. Along the way to synapse onto a secondary neuron, the T-junction of a dorsal root ganglion can act as a low-pass filter if the frequency of the spike train is high, resulting in propagation failures (Stoney, 1990; Zhou and Chiu, 2001). As the time between consecutive stimuli is only a few seconds, the evoked spike train has a relatively low frequency. In addition, the experimentally used PW is relatively short (i.e., < 1 ms) (Doll et al., 2016a,b). Hence, it is unlikely to have multiple spikes evoked by a single pulse. Therefore, the model does not include propagation processes.

Next, through synaptic connections, an excitatory postsynaptic current (PSC) Ip*(t) is given as:

Ip*(t)=[fA-α1]+τsk = 0NoP-1exp(-t-kIPIτs)H(t-kIPI),    (1)

with time constant τs = 1.5 ms (Gabbiani et al., 1994). There are more mechanisms, such as short-term synaptic plasticity, that affect PSCs, but both facilitation and depression have been reported (Luo et al., 2014). As the overall effect is unclear, we do not incorporate this in the model. The PSC drives the postsynaptic potential (PSP) of a secondary neuron in the dorsal horn x(t), which we model as a leaky integrator,

τ2=-x+Ip*(t),x(0)=0.    (2)

The time constant τ2 has a value of about several tens of milliseconds (Prescott and Koninck, 2002; Weng et al., 2006). The spike generation of a secondary neuron depends on the PSP together with other factors, which act as noise. Therefore, we model its spiking behavior as a non-homogeneous Poisson process with an instantaneous firing rate (Plesser and Gerstner, 2000) given by:

λ(t)=λL(1+exp(αL-x(t)σL))-1.    (3)

Here, the lumped parameters αL, σL, and λL represent the threshold, the slope parameter, and the maximal firing rate, respectively. The expected value of the number of spikes during a trial with the interval of duration T is

λT=0Tλ(t)dt.    (4)

This model assumes that the binary response R equals one if at least one secondary neuron generates an action potential during the trial interval T (Yang et al., 2015). Hence, the model-based psychometric function is given by:

Ψ=1-exp(-λT).    (5)

Our model contains six lumped parameters, which depend on more than 10 physical quantities, characterizing peripheral, and central nociceptive subsystems. We summarize descriptions of physical quantities as well as expressions of lumped parameters with respect to physical quantities in Table 1, for more details we refer to Yang et al. (2015).

2.2. Effects of Single-Model Parameters on Detection Thresholds

Here, we study how the model-based detection threshold changes by sweeping values of single physical or lumped parameters. In the model, there are two kinds of parameter redundancies. One is among the physical quantities. There are three subgroups of such parameters: {Vth, c0, c1}, {ρ, ḡ, K}, and {l, λh}. The other kind of redundancy reflects the relation between physical quantities and six lumped parameters: h and α1; C1 and τ1; C2 and τ2; αh and αL; σh and σL; and {l, λh} and λL. Because of this redundancy, we only give reference values of lumped parameters (see Table 1) rather than physical quantities. To simulate detection thresholds, we set α1 = 0.125 mA, τ1 = 0.2 ms, τ2 = 45 ms, αL = 0.00417 A/s, σL=8.33×10-5 A/s, and λL = 0.01 kHz. The magnitudes of the time constants τ1 and τ2 are similar to those from Mogyoros et al. (1996), Prescott and Koninck (2002), and Weng et al. (2006). The values of the remaining four lumped parameters are chosen to produce model-based detection thresholds similar to the experimentally observed ones (Doll et al., 2016a,b). On the basis of the expressions in Table 1, it is sufficient to consider 10 physical quantities: (Vth, C1, G1, ρ, h, C2, G2, αh, σh, and λh) to cover all parameters. Monotonicity of the detection threshold with respect to parameters may be utilized as a qualitative measure in a model validation study when an experimental technique to tune values of a specific parameter is available. To study monotonicity in the HM, we vary the values of the parameters from 60 to 170% of the reference values. In model simulations, we use four combinations of temporal stimulus properties as used in Doll et al. (2016b): (NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms), (NoP = 1, PW = 0.525 ms), (NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms), and (NoP = 2, IPI = 50 ms, PW = 0.525 ms). To illustrate the possible non-monotone effects of σh and σL on detection thresholds, we use another combination of temporal stimulus properties: (NoP = 2, IPI = 30 ms, PW = 0.525 ms).

2.3. Effects of Temporal Stimulus Properties on Detection Thresholds

Here, we analyse the effects of temporal stimulus properties on the detection threshold, in particular the IPI. As we reported in Yang et al. (2015), the threshold decreases as PW increases because enhanced fiber activation gives more impulses to secondary neurons. For multiple pulses, the detection threshold is also lower compared to single-pulse stimuli. For double-pulse stimuli, our previous modeling study suggests that the dependence of the threshold A50 on IPI could be non-monotone. In the following, we derive a condition on the values of the parameters whether this relationship is monotone or not.

For simplicity, we will restrict ourselves to the case NoP = 2. The effect for more pulses is similar, but less pronounced, and the analysis is more involved. First, we assume τs → 0, as τs ≪ τ2 based on (Gabbiani et al., 1994; Prescott and Koninck, 2002). Hence, the lumped postsynaptic potential in (2) can be simplified to

x0(t)=Bk=0NoP-1exp(-t-kIPIτ2)H(t-kIPI),    (6)

where B=[fA-α1]+τ2. Second, we consider the limit σh → 0, resulting in σL → 0. This simplifies the analysis, as there is only a contribution to the integral λT if x0(t)>αL, t ∈ (0, T). Indeed, as (3) becomes a step function, the psychometric function in (5) has the simple form Ψ = 1 − exp(−λLΔT), where ΔT denotes the total time x0 is above the threshold αL, see Figure 2. Now, we have three distinct cases to reach the threshold αL. First, the activity would be above threshold after the second pulse, but not the first. Second, both pulses may lead to supra-threshold activity but during two separate time intervals. Third, if a single pulse leads to supra-threshold activity, we may have one interval, if x0(IPI-)>αL. The time ΔTi, i = 1, 2, 3 above αL can be calculated explicitly as:

ΔT1=τ2(log(1+exp(-IPIτ2))-log(αLB)),ΔT2=τ2(log(1+exp(-IPIτ2))-2log(αLB)),ΔT3=IPI+τ2(log(1+exp(-IPIτ2))-log(αLB)).    (7)

To illustrate the three cases, we use double-pulse stimuli with IPI = 20 ms and PW = 0.525 ms at amplitudes A = 0.19, 0.22, and 0.26 mA. We simulate the lumped postsynaptic potential of the HM using the reference values for the lumped parameters in Table 1.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Three ways the postsynaptic potential x0(t) (solid) can reach the threshold (dashed) when two pulses are applied. The above-threshold time ΔT may consist of one interval by crossing upon the second pulse only (A) or upon the first pulse staying above threshold until the second pulses (C). If the pulses are well separated in time ΔT consists of two intervals (B). Temporal properties are NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms and PW = 0.525 ms, and the amplitude are varied, i.e., not equal to A50.

We consider model-based detection thresholds in these three cases. From (5), it follows that the detection threshold A50 and IPI are related by the implicit equation λLΔT = log(2). If the IPI is large, then the time above threshold is always given by ΔT2. By decreasing IPI, the detection threshold A50 may change such that either ΔT1 or ΔT3 should be used. The value of IPI, where we switch from the second case to the first, is given by:

IPI21=-τ2log(21(τ2λL)-1),    (8)

and to the third case by:

IPI23=τ2log(21(τ2λL)+14-12).    (9)

Now, either λLτ2 > 1, and then IPI21 is negative and IPI23 is positive, or λLτ2 < 1 and then IPI23 is negative and IPI21 is positive. Hence, as we decrease IPI, we find either case 1 or case 3, and this depends on whether λLτ2 is smaller or larger than one.

It is easy to see that ΔT1 and ΔT2 are decreasing functions of IPI, and ΔT3 is an increasing function. It follows from the definition of the psychometric function (5) that ∂A50(IPI)∕∂IPI < 0, if both IPI < IPI23 and λLτ2 < 1; otherwise, the derivative is positive. Hence, if λLτ2 < 1, then the dependence of the detection threshold on the interpulse interval is non-monotone.

2.4. Neuroplasticity Induced by Topical Capsaicin Treatment and Relevant Modeling

We consider the observed patterns of changes in detection thresholds on five study days with 1-h capsaicin treatment over three months with 8% capsaicin treatment (Doll et al., 2016b), as illustrated in Figure 1. First, we briefly review neuroplasticity caused by topical application of capsaicin. Second, with plausible changes in related model parameters, we study the patterns of changes in model-based detection thresholds.

2.4.1. Capsaicin-Induced Neuroplasticity

Capsaicin activates the TRPV1-expressing C fibers and some Aδ-fibers (O'Neill et al., 2012). The application of a high-dose capsaicin patch results in high levels of intracellular calcium in nerve endings leading to mitochondrial dysfunction and retraction of nerve endings (Anand and Bley, 2011). In the periphery, neuropeptides, such as substance P (SP) and calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP), are released from the activated peptidergic C fibers, resulting in neurogenic inflammation (Coutaux et al., 2005; Voscopoulos and Lema, 2010). This inflammation is accompanied by the release of inflammatory agents such as prostaglandin E2 and histamine. In turn, these agents may trigger intracellular signaling in nociceptors through protein kinase A and C (PKA and PKC) (Khasar et al., 1999; Malmberg, 2000). These protein kinases can further modulate the properties of tetrodotoxin-resistant (TTXr) persistent sodium channels in nerve endings, thereby shifting the activation curve of these channels to more hyperpolarized values (Gilchrist and Bosmans, 2012). Hence, the excitability of Aδ-fibers may increase due to the presence of inflammatory agents. After a 60-min application of an 8% capsaicin patch, it is common to observe application-site erythema. The erythema was reported to resolve within 1–3 days after the capsaicin patch treatment (FDA Center for Drug Evaluation and Research, 2009). This suggests that capsaicin-induced neurogenic inflammation could last up to 3 days. In the superficial dorsal horn, the central terminals of peptidergic C fibers modulate the secondary neurons via release of SP (Todd, 2010). SP can further activate intracellular signaling, e.g., PKA, PKC, and extracellular-signal-regulated kinases (ERK) in superficial dorsal horn neurons, thereby resulting in shorter and longer-lasting neuroplasticity. The increased density of AMPA receptors enhances synaptic efficacy, and the reduction of A-type potassium currents increases membrane excitability (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009). These effects can last several hours in human experimental pain models (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009; Sandkühler, 2009) and are considered to be transcription-independent processes. The longer-lasting central neuroplasticity was suggested to be transcription-dependent, thereby possibly explaining the persistent pain state in pathological settings (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009).

2.4.2. Simulated Detection Thresholds Over Three Months

We model the capsaicin-induced effects by different perturbations of physical quantities for each study day. First, for degeneration of nerve endings and their regrowth, the intra-epidermal nerve fiber density ρ is expected to decrease initially and then to return to baseline, as also observed by Kennedy et al. (2010). In addition, as the nerve endings retract from the epidermis, their depth h increases (O'Neill et al., 2012). Second, we increase the membrane excitability of nerve endings by lowering the firing threshold Vth on Day 2. This models the capsaicin-induced neurogenic inflammation, which in normal circumstances is resolved within a couple of days. Third, increases in membrane excitability and synaptic efficacy of the superficial dorsal horn neurons can be captured by decreasing G2 and increasing ḡ, respectively. Summarizing, it is plausible that changes in the physical quantities ρ, h, Vth, G2, and ḡ can account for the experimental pattern in detection thresholds.

We consider the values of the model parameters on Day 0 as baseline. We use the same values for the lumped parameters as in the parameter sweeping: α1 = 0.125 mA, τ1 = 0.2 ms, τ2 = 45 ms, αL = 0.00417 A/s, σL=8.33×10-5 A/s, and λL = 0.01 kHz. The perturbation patterns are presented as multiplication factors (rρ, rh, rVth, rG2, and r) to the baseline values, see Table 2. Hence, the actual lumped parameter values are tuned according to the expressions given in Table 1. A previous human study with the same administration of the capsaicin patch to thighs reported decreased fiber densities on Day 7 and 84 to 21 and 80% of the baseline, respectively (Kennedy et al., 2010). Here, we set these two ratios for our model simulations. In addition, as the regeneration of fiber densities approximately obeyed a linear relationship with respect to time (Polydefkis et al., 2004), we determined the ratios of fiber densities on Day 2 and 28 by linear extrapolation and interpolation based on values for densities on Day 7 and 84 from literature. Values of other parameters are changed to capture the experimentally obtained changes in detection thresholds in a systematic way, which also agrees qualitatively with fiber retraction (O'Neill et al., 2012), changes of peripheral excitability (Coutaux et al., 2005), and plasticity of the central subsystem (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009). We compute the detection thresholds for each combination of temporal properties on each study day. To understand how changes in all five physical quantities act together, we also compute the thresholds with either peripheral or central functional plasticity omitted, or both.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Perturbation patterns of the ratios of five physical quantities.

3. Results

We explore effects of physiological parameters on detection thresholds by sweeping parameters. Next, we discuss the effect of the IPI on the detection threshold by both mathematical analysis and model simulations. Lastly, we show simulated detection thresholds on the five study days in the case study of topical capsaicin treatment. These simulations show qualitative agreement with patterns of changes of experimental thresholds.

3.1. Effects of Single Model Parameters on Detection Thresholds

Each panel in Figure 3 illustrates the effect of a single model parameter on the detection threshold. Big markers show detection thresholds for the reference values in Table 1. The steeper the curve around the reference value is, the stronger the threshold depends on this parameter locally. These results show that detection thresholds can depend non-monotonically on physical quantities C2 and σh or lumped parameters τ2 or σL. For secondary neurons, morphological properties, i.e., the thickness and the surface of the membrane, determine C2. It is not likely that C2 changes much over time, and if it would, e.g., due to cell growth, then also the membrane conductance G2 is expected to change accordingly. As τ2=C2G2-1, changes in both C2 and G2-1 compensate each other, resulting in an invariant τ2. However, a change of the thickness of the membrane can lead to a change solely in the physical quantity C2 or the lumped parameter τ2. For double-pulse stimuli, we notice that two combinations for double-pulse stimuli have very similar thresholds as shown in Figure 3. We comment on this in the next subsection. For IPI = 30 ms and PW = 0.525 ms, we find a non-monotone dependence of simulated detection thresholds on σh or σL, see Figure 3J. This is a small effect and may not be experimentally observable.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Simulated detection thresholds with perturbation of single parameters. The parameter changed in a particular panel is displayed above panels (A–J). Black: NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms; Red: NoP = 1, PW = 0.525 ms; Cyan: NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms; Blue: NoP = 2, IPI = 50 ms, PW = 0.525 ms and Gray cross: NoP = 2, IPI = 30 ms, PW = 0.525 ms (only in J). The titles in panels indicate perturbed parameters. In (I), the y-axis has a log-scale for better visualization of the non-monotonicity of detection thresholds with respect to C2 or τ2. In (J), the y-axis has different scales for better visualization of the non-monotone effects of σh and σL for two combinations of temporal properties, respectively.

3.2. Effect of IPI on Detection Thresholds

Using different amplitudes of double-pulse stimuli, we illustrate the three ways how the modeled postsynaptic potential x can cross the activation threshold αL of central neurons, see Figure 2. Next, as we analyzed the transition between the above different scenarios in Section 2.3, the quantity λLτ2 determines the monotonicity of the detection threshold with respect to the IPI. Although this result is obtained in the limit when σL → 0 and τs ≪ τ2, we verify this also for non-zero σL (i.e., 8.33 × 10−5 A/s) and τs = 1.5 ms, see Figure 4. Black and blue curves depict the relationship between the detection threshold and the IPI with λL = 0.01 and 0.05 kHz, respectively, and the values of the other five lumped parameters are set to the reference values. We find that the simulated detection thresholds (dashed curves) follow the analytical expressions (plotted as solid curves) very closely. This demonstrates the validity of our analysis. As τ2=C2G2-1 and λL = h, changes in these four physical quantities could affect the monotonicity of detection thresholds.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Relationship between the detection threshold and the IPI. With τs, σL → 0, solid curves are determined with analytically expression ΔTi, i = 1, 2, 3. The simulated detection thresholds for nonzero σL:=8.33×10-5 A/s, τs = 1.5 ms are indicated by dashed curves. For the black curves, we use τ2 = 45 ms, λL = 0.01 kHz with product smaller than one. For the blue curves, we use τ2 = 45 ms, λL = 0.05 kHz with product larger than one.

3.3. Simulated Detection Thresholds and Psychometric Curves after Topical Capsaicin Treatment

We have computed the model-based detection thresholds for the perturbations of five physical quantities in Table 2. We observe that there is a qualitative agreement between the simulated thresholds (red cross) and experimental data (blue) for all four combinations of temporal properties. For single-pulse stimuli, detection thresholds increase on Days 2 and 7, and for double-pulse stimuli, thresholds increase on Days 7 and 28. Moreover, for double-pulse stimuli on Day 84, the simulated detection thresholds are still larger than their baselines, which also agrees with the elevated thresholds compared to those on Day 0 (although not significant).

To study different combinations of neuroplasticity with the model, we omit either peripheral or central functional plasticity, or both. We present the corresponding simulated detection thresholds for these three cases in Figure 5 with gray markers. In these three cases, we find much higher thresholds. With only nerve degeneration (circles), the thresholds would increase on Days 2, 7, and 28 similarly for both single- and double-pulse stimuli. Adding only peripheral sensitization (diamonds) results in different patterns, but these simulations do not agree with detection thresholds for single-pulse stimuli observed on Day 28. With central functional plasticity and nerve degeneration, the simulation (squares) shows a qualitative disagreement for double-pulse stimuli on Day 2. We find that only all parameter changes combined lead to the same qualitative pattern as experimentally observed. The differences between thresholds for one- and two-pulse stimuli are caused by a simultaneous effect on the shape of the psychometric curve. To show, this we have computed the psychometric curves given by Equation (5) for the model on five study days for two stimulus properties (NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms) and (NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms) using the parameter values in in Tables 1, 2, see Figure 6.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Experiental and simulated detection thresholds on five study days using four combinations of the temporal properties. For simulations, we consider the changes in parameters ρ, h, Vth, G2, and ḡ. Row label (A–D) represent the four combinations of temporal properties: (A) (NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms), (B) (NoP = 1, PW = 0.525 ms) (C) (NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms), and (D) (NoP = 2, IPI = 50 ms, PW = 0.525 ms). The first column on the left shows simulations (red cross) with all forms of plasticity and the experimental data (blue). Other columns show simulations of detection thresholds with different combinations of forms of plasticity: circles for simulations without functional plasticity, diamonds for simulations when central functional plasticity omitted, and squares for simulations when peripheral functional plasticity omitted.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Model-based psychometric functions on five study days. The parameter values of the hazard model are set according to Tables 1, 2. Two combinations of temporal stimulus properties are used in the simulations: (A) (NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms) and (B) (NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms). The inset plots show two groups of psychometric functions with amplitudes near detection thresholds. The intersection between each psychometric curve and the gray horizontal line is the detection threshold. Psychometric functions for the other two combinations (NoP = 1, PW = 0.525 ms) and (NoP = 2, IPI = 50 ms, PW = 0.525 ms) are not shown, as they are qualitatively similar to those in (A,B), respectively.

4. Discussion

We have investigated the effects of model parameters on detection thresholds with our computational model in terms of neuroplasticity. First, we demonstrated that the detection threshold varies monotonically for most single parameters except for C2, σh, τ2, and σL involved in central nociceptive function. Second, we developed an analytical argument why detection thresholds may depend non-monotonically on the IPI. Third, we translated biologically plausible capsaicin-induced neuroplasticity into perturbations in values of model parameters. Agreement between simulations and observations of detection thresholds suggests the prospective usage of this computational model for a malfunctioning nociceptive system.

4.1. Effects of Parameters on Detection Thresholds

Detection thresholds depend on most of single parameters in a monotone manner, see Figure 3. In addition to obvious parameter redundancy, the effects of different parameters on detection thresholds could be similar. For example, this occurs for the pair of physical quantities C1 and αh (equivalently for lumped parameters τ1 and αL) when (NoP = 1, PW = 0.21 ms), (NoP = 2, IPI = 20 ms, PW = 0.525 ms), or (NoP = 2, IPI = 50 ms, PW = 0.525 ms), see Figures 3B,G. This similarity challenges the identifiability of such model parameters, as they can compensate each other, resulting in equal detection thresholds. Hence, in future work like estimation of model parameters, we recommend to design more appropriate combinations of temporal properties to enrich the information in the data.

In a previous work (Doll et al., 2016a), varying the interpulse interval from 10 to 50 ms for double-pulse stimuli, only a small change in the detection thresholds was found. This small change could be attributed to a non-monotonic relation between IPI and detection thresholds (Yang et al., 2015). Our present analysis shows that the product C2G2-1lλh determines the monotonicity. In turn, suppose that the monotonicity can be tested in (future) experimental work, one might treat this monotonicity as an indication to infer changes of central properties C2, G2, l, and λh. We acknowledge that it may be challenging to detect the (non-)monotonicity of detection thresholds with respect to the IPI, as statistical power may be limited for the IPI from 10 to 50 ms (Yang et al., 2015). For future work to verify the existence of non-monotonicity, first, one may use more or other values for the IPI and then study the relation between detection thresholds about these IPIs. Second, one could study the relation between the detection probability and the IPI for double-pulse stimuli by varying IPI but fixing A.

4.2. Modeling Capsaicin-Induced Plasticity and Its Interpretation

We studied plausible neuroplasticity for the topical capsaicin treatment with the computational model. The changes of the fiber densities on study days were set partly based on literature data and also nerve regeneration with a constant regeneration rate. Other changes of physical quantities were based qualitatively on physiological mechanisms (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009; Sandkühler, 2009; O'Neill et al., 2012). In addition, values of time constants of afferent fibers and secondary neurons were set to similar values obtained in previous experimental studies (Mogyoros et al., 1996; Prescott and Koninck, 2002; Weng et al., 2006). With these physiological restrictions on the computational model, we demonstrated qualitative agreement of simulated patterns of changes of detection thresholds to experimental patterns, see Figure 5. For a future, more quantitative study, we suggest to estimate the lumped parameters in the model using the stimulus-response pairs of single subjects. The changes in estimates of parameters might unravel the possible individual-specific nociceptive characteristics of capsaicin-induced plasticity.

Capsaicin-induced nerve degeneration is thought to underlie the therapeutic basis in pain management (Anand and Bley, 2011; O'Neill et al., 2012). Our model simulations together with observed elevations of detection thresholds on Days 2, 7, and 28 (Doll et al., 2016b) offer a theoretical support in terms of nociceptive processing. In addition, topical application of capsaicin possibly triggers two forms of neuroplasticity, thereby pushing single nociceptors or secondary neurons into sensitized states (Coutaux et al., 2005; Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009; Sandkühler, 2009). The two forms of neuroplasticity in peripheral and central nociceptive subsystems compensate the elevations in detection thresholds. Each form of plasticity results in different patterns of changes in detection thresholds for stimuli with either single- or double-pulses on the five study days. Our model simulations dissect different longitudinal effects caused by peripheral and central plasticity, as illustrated in Figure 5. Such difference provides valuable information to identify the existence of underlying plasticity.

For our model simulations on Day 2, both forms of peripheral and central plasticity are required for the small change of detection thresholds with NoP = 2, as well as for clear elevations of detection thresholds with NoP = 1. Then, the small change in detection threshold with NoP = 2 is less straightforwardly interpretable. Here, we provide an explanation based on how physical quantities affect psychometric curves. Nerve degeneration with a clearly decreased value of ρ and increased h tends to shallow Ψ and to shift Ψ to the right. However, peripheral plasticity with a decreased value of Vth overcomes this tendency, resulting in an effectively smaller shift of Ψ to the left. In addition, central plasticity with a decreased G2 and an increased ḡ compensates the dominant tendency with a shallower Ψ caused by the dramatic loss of nerves. In addition, it is expected that the psychometric function for double-pulse stimuli is steeper in comparison to single-pulse stimuli. Taken together, on Day 2, detection thresholds are elevated for single-pulse stimuli but not for double-pulse stimuli. The simulations of the psychometric functions are illustrated in Figure 6.

On Day 28, the modeled central neuroplasticity, especially the enhanced synaptic efficiency, compensates the loss of nerve endings, resulting in a less increased detection threshold for single-pulse stimuli but not for double-pulse stimuli. One can also explain this observation based on the distortion of psychometric functions due to changes in model parameters. As the peripheral plasticity is resolved within 3 days after capsaicin application, Ψ is expected to shift to the right due to an increased value of h. In addition, the central plasticity leads to an effectively steeper Ψ than the baseline as nerve endings regenerate with an increased ρ compared to that on Day 2. Hence, all these nociceptive changes result in increases of detection thresholds for double-pulse stimuli but not for single-pulse stimuli on Day 28. The simulations of the psychometric functions are illustrated in Figure 6.

4.3. Limitations of the Model and Possible Extensions

Our present study uses a computational model (Yang et al., 2015) to study the effects of neuroplasticity on detection thresholds. This model is relatively simple as it assumes that an evoked spike leads to a single synaptic event at a secondary neuron. The model is mainly concerned with nerve endings in the skin and secondary neurons. There exist many models of axonal transmission, e.g., incorporating the geometry of branch points in dorsal root ganglion and distribution of myelin along the axon, leading to propagation failure and ectopic spiking behavior (Zhou and Chiu, 2001; Coggan et al., 2010, 2011; Volman and Ng, 2013, 2014), thereby possibly leading to neuropathic pain (Coggan et al., 2015). It would be interesting to incorporate the effects of axonal transmission into the hazard model to study the effect of demyelination on nociceptive detection thresholds. For the current study, however, we remark that the topical application of capsaicin is thought to focally affect free nerve endings (Anand and Bley, 2011). There has been no report, to the best of our knowledge, that this application would induce axonal injury or degeneration in myelinated axons of Aδ-fibers, which are located within deeper tissue than the dermis (Provitera et al., 2007). As we noted before, see also (Yang et al., 2015), another model extension would be to include short-term synaptic plasticity. The temporal summation of postsynaptic potentials may be affected by facilitation or depression. In turn, this would lower or increase the detection threshold, respectively. In Doll et al. (2016a), it was noted that the detection threshold for two-pulse stimuli was close to as expected based on temporal summation of postsynaptic potentials. As we model the detection as the occurrence of the first spike, supra-threshold mechanisms seem to be less relevant. Sub-threshold mechanisms could also play a role, e.g., if that changes the secondary neuron into a resonator instead of an integrator. It might be possible to study that more precisely by measuring the detection threshold for more values of IPI. Still, being aware of these limitations, our simple model and possible refinements might help to investigate relevant neuroplasticity induced by other medical interventions or diseases.

4.4. Future Studies on Capsaicin-Induced Neuroplasticity

In our case study, capsaicin-induced central plasticity appears to be present for a much longer time compared to what is usually mentioned for experimental pain models (Sandkühler, 2009). The time course of such plasticity is similar to estimated time courses found in human subject study using high frequency electrical stimulation as the conditioning stimulus (Pfau et al., 2011). Our results suggest that late-phase synaptic plasticity could have occurred in human subjects. The underlying physiological mechanisms would involve transcription-dependent processes (Kawasaki et al., 2004; Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009). Application of an 8% capsaicin patch might become a human experimental model to test possible medication to prevent or reverse the underlying hypothetical processes of this longer-lasting central plasticity. In addition, our modeling study also sheds light on the design of experiments to test the effects of certain medicine on alterations in the nociceptive system. Exploiting the capsaicin-induced plasticity in a human model, one may utilize other medical interventions together with the capsaicin treatment. One recommendation is to apply the lidocaine derivative (Binshtok et al., 2007) as well to prevent the activation of TTXr-sodium channels over a couple of days. Moreover, one could apply ketamine, an NMDA-antagonist, to effectively prevent the enhanced central plasticity (Woolf, 2011). The combination of ketamine and capsaicin would allow to further investigate contributions of peripheral and central plasticity. Accordingly, measurements with such perturbation could help to verify the presence of aforementioned abnormal spiking in axonal injuries. We recommend to compare prospective observations of detection thresholds with our model predictions of detection thresholds in Figure 5. Similar results in such a comparison would further validate our computational model. On the other hand, the possible difference from experimental observations would be useful for further refinement of the model toward more mechanistic insights of capsaicin-induced plasticity.

Author Contributions

HY, HM, RD, JB, and SV designed and conceived the study. HY and HM conducted the mathematical analysis. HY drafted the manuscript. HM, RD, JB, and SV commented and edited the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research is supported by the Dutch Technology Foundation STW (project number: 10740), which is part of the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) and partly funded by the Ministry of Economic Affairs, Agriculture and Innovation. The authors would like to thank the Centre for Human Drug Research in Leiden (CHDR) for collaboration in the capsaicin study. In particular, the authors thank Justin L. Hay and Geert J. Groeneveld from the CHDR for interesting discussions and useful remarks.

References

Anand, P., and Bley, K. (2011). Topical capsaicin for pain management: therapeutic potential and mechanisms of action of the new high-concentration capsaicin 8% patch. Br. J. Anaesth. 107, 490–502. doi: 10.1093/bja/aer260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binshtok, A. M., Bean, B. P., and Woolf, C. J. (2007). Inhibition of nociceptors by TRPV1-mediated entry of impermeant sodium channel blockers. Nature 449, 607–610. doi: 10.1038/nature06191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coggan, J. S., Ocker, G. K., Sejnowski, T. J., and Prescott, S. A. (2011). Explaining pathological changes in axonal excitability through dynamical analysis of conductance-based models. J. Neural Eng. 8:065002. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/6/065002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coggan, J. S., Prescott, S. A., Bartol, T. M., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2010). Imbalance of ionic conductances contributes to diverse symptoms of demyelination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 20602–20609. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013798107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coggan, J. S., Sejnowski, T. J., and Prescott, S. A. (2015). Cooperativity between remote sites of ectopic spiking allows afterdischarge to be initiated and maintained at different locations. J. Comput. Neurosci. 39, 17–28. doi: 10.1007/s10827-015-0562-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coutaux, A., Adam, F., Willer, J.-C., and Le Bars, D. (2005). Hyperalgesia and allodynia: peripheral mechanisms. Joint Bone Spine 72, 359–371. doi: 10.1016/j.jbspin.2004.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doll, R. J., Buitenweg, J. R., Meijer, H. G. E., and Veltink, P. H. (2014). Tracking of nociceptive thresholds using adaptive psychophysical methods. Behav. Res. Methods 46, 55–66. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0368-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doll, R. J., Maten, A. C. A., Spaan, P. G., Veltink, P. H., and Buitenweg, J. R. (2016a). Effect of temporal stimulus properties on the nociceptive detection probability using intra-epidermal electrical stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 219–227. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4451-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doll, R. J., van Amerongen, G., Hay, J. L., Groeneveld, G. J., Veltink, P. H., and Buitenweg, J. R. (2016b). Responsiveness of electrical nociceptive detection thresholds to capsaicin (8%) induced changes in nociceptive processing. Exp. Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4655-z. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

FDA Center for Drug Evaluation and Research (2009). Medical Review: Qutenza. Available online at: http://www.accessdata.fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/nda/2009/022395s000Medr.pdf

Gabbiani, F., Midtgaard, J., and Knopfel, T. (1994). Synaptic integration in a model of cerebellar granule cells. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 999–1009.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gilchrist, J., and Bosmans, F. (2012). Animal toxins can alter the function of Nav1. 8 and Nav1. 9. Toxins 4, 620–632. doi: 10.3390/toxins4080620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granstein, R. D., and Luger, T. A. (2009). Neuroimmunology of the Skin. Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer.

Google Scholar

Inui, K., Tran, T. D., Hoshiyama, M., and Kakigi, R. (2002). Preferential stimulation of Aδ fibers by intra-epidermal needle electrode in humans. Pain 96, 247–252. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00453-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawasaki, Y., Kohno, T., Zhuang, Z.-Y., Brenner, G. J., Wang, H., Van Der Meer, C., et al. (2004). Ionotropic and metabotropic receptors, protein kinase a, protein kinase c, and src contribute to C-fiber-induced erk activation and camp response element-binding protein phosphorylation in dorsal horn neurons, leading to central sensitization. J. Neurosci. 24, 8310–8321. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2396-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, W. R., Vanhove, G. F., Lu, S.-P., Tobias, J., Bley, K. R., Walk, D., et al. (2010). A randomized, controlled, open-label study of the long-term effects of NGX-4010, a high-concentration capsaicin patch, on epidermal nerve fiber density and sensory function in healthy volunteers. J. Pain 11, 579–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khasar, S. G., McCarter, G., and Levine, J. D. (1999). Epinephrine produces a β-adrenergic receptor-mediated mechanical hyperalgesia and in vitro sensitization of rat nociceptors. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 1104–1112.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kyranou, M., and Puntillo, K. (2012). The transition from acute to chronic pain: might intensive care unit patients be at risk? Annals Intens. Care 2, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/2110-5820-2-36

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latremoliere, A., and Woolf, C. J. (2009). Central sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticity. J. Pain 10, 895–926. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, C., Kuner, T., and Kuner, R. (2014). Synaptic plasticity in pathological pain. Trends Neurosci. 37, 343–355. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, C., Baron, R., Tölle, T. R., Binder, A., Birbaumer, N., Birklein, F., et al. (2010). Quantitative sensory testing in the german research network on neuropathic pain (DFNS): somatosensory abnormalities in 1236 patients with different neuropathic pain syndromes. Pain 150, 439–450. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malmberg, A. B. (2000). “Protein kinase subtypes involved in injury-induced nociception,” in Nervous System Plasticity and Chronic Pain, Vol. 129 Progress in Brain Research, eds J. Sandkuhler and B. G. G. Bromm (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 51–59.

PubMed Abstract

Mogyoros, I., Kiernan, M. C., and Burke, D. (1996). Strength-duration properties of human peripheral nerve. Brain 119:439. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mørch, C. D., Hennings, K., and Andersen, O. K. (2011). Estimating nerve excitation thresholds to cutaneous electrical stimulation by finite element modeling combined with a stochastic branching nerve fiber model. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 49, 385–395. doi: 10.1007/s11517-010-0725-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouraux, A., Iannetti, G. D., and Plaghki, L. (2010). Low intensity intra-epidermal electrical stimulation can activate Aδ-nociceptors selectively. Pain 150, 199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Neill, J., Brock, C., Olesen, A. E., Andresen, T., Nilsson, M., and Dickenson, A. H. (2012). Unravelling the mystery of capsaicin: a tool to understand and treat pain. Pharmacol. Rev. 64, 939–971. doi: 10.1124/pr.112.006163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, K. L., and Rowbotham, M. C. (1999). A new human experimental pain model: the heat/capsaicin sensitization model. Neuroreport 10, 1511–1516. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199905140-00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfau, D. B., Klein, T., Putzer, D., Pogatzki-Zahn, E. M., Treede, R.-D., and Magerl, W. (2011). Analysis of hyperalgesia time courses in humans after painful electrical high-frequency stimulation identifies a possible transition from early to late LTP-like pain plasticity. Pain 152, 1532–1539. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plesser, H. E., and Gerstner, W. (2000). Noise in integrate-and-fire neurons: from stochastic input to escape rates. Neural Comput. 12, 367–384. doi: 10.1162/089976600300015835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polydefkis, M., Hauer, P., Sheth, S., Sirdofsky, M., Griffin, J. W., and McArthur, J. C. (2004). The time course of epidermal nerve fibre regeneration: studies in normal controls and in people with diabetes, with and without neuropathy. Brain 127, 1606–1615. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prescott, S. A., and Koninck, Y. D. (2002). Four cell types with distinctive membrane properties and morphologies in lamina I of the spinal dorsal horn of the adult rat. J. Physiol. 539, 817–836. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provitera, V., Nolano, M., Pagano, A., Caporaso, G., Stancanelli, A., and Santoro, L. (2007). Myelinated nerve endings in human skin. Mus. Nerve 35, 767–775. doi: 10.1002/mus.20771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandkühler, J. (2009). Models and mechanisms of hyperalgesia and allodynia. Physiol. Rev. 89, 707–758. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00025.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmelz, M., and Kress, M. (1996). Topical acetylsalicylate attenuates capsaicin induced pain, flare and allodynia but not thermal hyperalgesia. Neurosci. Lett. 214, 72–74. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12868-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenbergen, P., Buitenweg, J. R., Trojan, J., van der Heide, E. M., van den Heuvel, T., Flor, H., et al. (2012). A system for inducing concurrent tactile and nociceptive sensations at the same site using electrocutaneous stimulation. Behav. Res. Methods 44, 924–933. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0216-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoney, S. D. Jr. (1990). Limitations on impulse conduction at the branch point of afferent axons in frog dorsal root ganglion. Exp. Brain Res. 80, 512–524. doi: 10.1007/BF00227992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, A. J. (2010). Neuronal circuitry for pain processing in the dorsal horn. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 823–836. doi: 10.1038/nrn2947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volman, V., and Ng, L. J. (2013). Computer modeling of mild axonal injury: implications for axonal signal transmission. Neural Comput. 25, 2646–2681. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volman, V., and Ng, L. J. (2014). Primary paranode demyelination modulates slowly developing axonal depolarization in a model of axonal injury. J. Comput. Neurosci. 37, 439–457. doi: 10.1007/s10827-014-0515-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voscopoulos, C., and Lema, M. (2010). When does acute pain become chronic? Br. J. Anaesth. 105(Suppl. 1), i69–i85. doi: 10.1093/bja/aeq323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weng, H. R., Chen, J. H., and Cata, J. P. (2006). Inhibition of glutamate uptake in the spinal cord induces hyperalgesia and increased responses of spinal dorsal horn neurons to peripheral afferent stimulation. Neuroscience 138, 1351–1360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.11.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilder-Smith, O. H. G. (2002). Monitoring Nociceptive Neuroplasticity: Quantitative Sensory Testing: A Better Therapeutic Endpoint for Managing the Pain of Surgery? PhD thesis, Radboud University Nijmegen.

Woolf, C. J. (2011). Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain 152, S2–S15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., Meijer, H. G. E., Doll, R. J., Buitenweg, J. R., and van Gils, S. A. (2015). Computational modeling of Adelta-fiber-mediated nociceptive detection of electrocutaneous stimulation. Biol. Cybernet. 109, 479–491. doi: 10.1007/s00422-015-0656-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, L., and Chiu, S. Y. (2001). Computer model for action potential propagation through branch point in myelinated nerves. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 197–210. Available online at: http://jn.physiology.org/content/85/1/197

PubMed Abstract

Keywords: nociceptive detection, detection threshold, capsaicin, neuroplasticity, computational modeling

Citation: Yang H, Meijer HGE, Doll RJ, Buitenweg JR and van Gils SA (2016) Dependence of Nociceptive Detection Thresholds on Physiological Parameters and Capsaicin-Induced Neuroplasticity: A Computational Study. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 10:49. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2016.00049

Received: 29 October 2015; Accepted: 06 May 2016;
Published: 25 May 2016.

Edited by:

Si Wu, Beijing Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Vladislav Volman, L-3 Communications, USA
Yuwei Cui, Numenta, Inc., USA

Copyright © 2016 Yang, Meijer, Doll, Buitenweg and van Gils. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Huan Yang, aC55YW5nLTFAYWx1bW51cy51dHdlbnRlLm5s

Present Address: Huan Yang, Division of Toxicology, Leiden Academic Center for Drug Research, Leiden, Netherlands

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.