- 1Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Biology (CIRB), College de France, CNRS, INSERM, Université PSL, Paris, France
- 2Neuro-Electronics Research Flanders, Leuven, Belgium
- 3System Emotional Science, University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan
- 4Genetics of Cognition Laboratory, Neuroscience Area, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
Introduction: Dopamine release in the forebrain by midbrain ventral tegmental nucleus (VTA) and substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) neurons is implicated in reward processing, goal-directed learning, and decision-making. Rhythmic oscillations of neural excitability underlie coordination of network processing, and have been reported in these dopaminergic nuclei at several frequency bands. This paper provides a comparative characterization of several frequencies of oscillations of local field potential and single unit activity, highlighting some behavioral correlates.
Methods: We recorded from optogenetically identified dopaminergic sites in four mice training in operant olfactory and visual discrimination tasks.
Results: Rayleigh and Pairwise Phase Consistency (PPC) analyses revealed some VTA/SNc neurons phase-locked to each frequency range, with fast spiking interneurons (FSIs) prevalent at 1–2.5 Hz (slow) and 4 Hz bands, and dopaminergic neurons predominant in the theta band. More FSIs than dopaminergic neurons were phase-locked in the slow and 4 Hz bands during many task events. The highest incidence of phase-locking in neurons was in the slow and 4 Hz bands, and occurred during the delay between the operant choice and trial outcome (reward or punishment) signals.
Discussion: These data provide a basis for further examination of rhythmic coordination of activity of dopaminergic nuclei with other brain structures, and its impact for adaptive behavior.
1. Introduction
Dopamine is implicated in reward signaling, goal-directed learning, and decision-making, and related dysfunctions are linked with psychiatric disorders (Grace, 2016; Schultz, 2016). The midbrain ventral tegmental nucleus (VTA) and substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) are principal sources of dopamine to the forebrain, and exert vital influences in the prefrontal-basal ganglia circuit via both neuromodulation and neurotransmission (Williams et al., 2002; Chau et al., 2018; Ott and Nieder, 2019; Cerpa et al., 2021). One proposed mechanism for dopaminergic function to contribute to reinforcement learning concerns the credit assignment problem: “which synaptic strengths to reinforce or diminish following reward (or missed expected reward)?” (Schultz, 2015; Sutton and Barto, 2018). But dopamine release is also associated with processing sensory stimuli (Nieoullon et al., 1977), motivation, and movement (Berke, 2018), which are also vital for learning and expressing goal-directed behaviors. One proposed mechanism to coordinate the precise timing of activity of the dopaminergic nuclei with the multiple brain structures involved in these processes could involve synchronous rhythmic oscillations (Womelsdorf et al., 2007).
Increased coherence of rhythmic oscillations of local field potentials, can modulate the timing of neuronal discharges, and lead to synchronous activation of neurons as assemblies (e.g., Benchenane et al., 2010). Furthermore, increased dopamine leads to changes in amplitude of oscillations in downstream structures (Berke, 2009). Such coordinated activity between different brain structures is associated with executive functions (Gray et al., 1989; Varela et al., 2001; Womelsdorf et al., 2007; Oberto et al., 2022).
To examine brain rhythms in experimental animal models, intracranial recordings of local field potentials (LFPs) can provide a greater degree of anatomical precision than surface EEG recordings. Rhythmic oscillations of field potentials and rhythmic activity of neurons have been reported in the VTA-prefrontal cortex-amygdala circuit, principally in the 4 Hz, theta and gamma bands. The 4 Hz rhythm there is associated with working memory (Fujisawa and Buzsáki, 2011; Duvarci et al., 2018) and modulates the 8 Hz theta (a hallmark of hippocampal processing), and 50 Hz gamma rhythms (often associated with precise synchronization of neuronal activity). However, there are only rare reports concerning other prominent frequency bands present in VTA LFP: during sleep there is infraslow (<1 Hz; Peters et al., 2004) and delta (0.5–1.5 Hz; Gao et al., 2007) rhythmicity while theta/alpha band VTA oscillations were reported in behaving rats (mean frequency = 10 Hz; Park and Moghaddam, 2017). However, none of this work examined animals performing instrumental tasks. There is some evidence for rhythms originating in the VTA (Peters et al., 2004; Lowes et al., 2021). Here, we identify and characterize a wide range of frequency bands operating in the brainstem dopaminergic nuclei through analyses of LFPs and neuronal phase-locking.
We find that multiple rhythms are prominent in the VTA/SNc of mice acquiring and performing a goal-directed task (specifically selected to elicit dopaminergic activity). These rhythms are observed in LFPs, and their role in local processing (as opposed to volume conduction) is evidenced by detection of phase-locking of spike trains of neurons at optogenetically confirmed recording sites. These rhythms could orchestrate interactions between VTA and an extended network of cortical and subcortical structures including striatum and prefrontal cortex (PFC).
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Animals
All experiments were performed in accordance with EU guidelines (directive 86/609/EEC). Subjects were four transgenic adult (2–9 months) male mice expressing a channelrhodopsin2- yellow fluorescent protein fusion protein (ChR2 (H134R)-eYFP). These mice were obtained by mating mice expressing the CRE recombinase under the control of the dopamine transporter (DAT-IRES-CRE mice, Stock 006660, Jackson Laboratory, Bar Harbor, ME, USA) with mice bearing a CRE-dependent ChR2(H134R)-eYFP gene (Ai32 mice, Stock 012569, Jackson Laboratory, Bar Harbor, ME, USA). Animals were housed in temperature-controlled rooms with standard 12-h light/dark cycles and food and water were available ad libitum. Each workday, animals were handled to habituate to human contact, and weighed. During pre-training and experimental periods, food intake, including pellets provided in the experimental apparatus, was restricted to a maximum of 3 g/day. Water access remained ad lib. Supplemental food was provided if weight fell below 85% of the normal weight.
2.2. Recording and stimulation
In order to compare activity along the mediolateral axis of the dopaminergic nuclei, an array of 4 recording probes was constructed attached to 2 optical stimulation probes, with each serving the medial or lateral pair of recording probes (see Supplementary Figure 1). Twisted bundles of 8 formvar coated 12 micron diameter nichrome wires (“octrodes”) were inserted in polyimide tubes. The wires were gold-plated to an impedance of about 350 kohm. The main body of the headstage is composed of a resin (MadeSolid, Oakland, CA, USA) molded in a 3D printer. The drives were adapted from the “lambda drive” of Headley et al. (2015). Two optic fibres angled rostrally by 15° in the headstage were positioned to be implanted at depths of 3.9 mm (VTA) and 3.5 mm (SNc). The medial two octrodes shared a microdrive, and were positioned for implantation at a depth of 3.9 mm, while the lateral two had independent drives, and their implanted depths were 3.7 and 3.5 mm. The entire assembly is shielded from electrical noise with a fine copper mesh spot-glued with dental cement. This was connected to ground, serving as a miniature Faraday cage. Recordings were made with an Ampliplex system (sampling rate: 20 kHz; cutoff frequencies of the analog low-pass and high-pass filters were 0.3 and 10 kHz, respectively). After surgical implantation, electrodes were advanced as often as twice daily until neurons appeared with responses to optical stimulation. Electrodes were further advanced when discriminable units were no longer present.
2.3. Surgery
Anesthesia was induced with a mixture of ketamine (66 mg/kg) and xylazine (13 mg/kg) and sustained with isoflurane (0.5–1.0%). Mice were placed in a stereotaxic device (David Kopf Instruments) and maintained at 37°C. The scalp was exposed and cleaned with hydrogen peroxide solution. Miniature jeweler’s screws were screwed into trephines, and attached with dental cement. The electrode assembly described above was implanted into the left VTA and SNc. The stainless-steel screws implanted on the left and right cerebellum as ground and reference electrodes, respectively.
2.4. Behavioral task
2.4.1. Experimental apparatus
The “Operon” system developed by Scheggia et al. (2014) was adapted as an experimental chamber permitting the mice to perform olfactory and visual discrimination tasks, (Figure 6A). Each of the short sides of the rectangular (160 L × 136 W × 160 H mm) plexiglass chamber has two arrays of LEDs and two ports used for both odor sampling and nose pokes. An olfactometer presented d- and l- stereoisomers of limonene at the two odor ports in a pseudo-random sequence. All six white LEDs were either lit or off above the respective odor ports, again in pseudo-random sequence. Between the ports and array is a central feeder port with an overhead lamp which is lit for 7 s during the delay after error trials, while the barrier remained closed. A reward pellet (5TUL TestDiet, Richmond, IN, USA, 14 mg) was delivered by a dispenser (ENV-203-14P, Med Associates, Fairfax, VT, USA). A plexiglass barrier divided the two sides of the chamber and was slid up from below via an automated motor assembly. Behavioral procedures followed those of the “Extradimensional shift” protocol of Scheggia and Papaleo (2016) but without textural cues, and further details can be found there and in Scheggia et al. (2014).
2.4.2. Pre-training
Mice were provided water ad libitum, but mildly food deprived to maintain them above 85% their baseline weight, as monitored with daily weighing. The behavioral apparatus was turned on, and the olfactometer outputs were confirmed to present odors discriminable by the experimenter. The headstage was then plugged in, and the mouse was placed into one compartment of the conditioning chamber. The room lights were dimmed and the recording system was turned on and the behavioral task was started. Animals were first pre-trained to shuttle between the two boxes for reward. In the first stage of training, a nose poke into either port triggered a pellet delivery. Then animals were pre-trained for simple visual discrimination task (light-on vs. light off). While the rewarded side was varied pseudo-randomly, in cases of spatial persistence, the other side was rewarded more. Once they could regularly perform sequential trials in the maze, mice were provided food ad lib prior to implantation with optrode assemblies, and then training was resumed along with recordings.
2.4.3. The behavioral task
At the beginning of each trial, the barrier was lowered. Once the mouse crossed a photodetector on its way to the other half of the chamber, one of the two LED arrays was lit and the two odors were released from the respective odor ports there. This photodetector crossing is called the “Cue” event. Once the mouse cleared these photodetectors, the barrier was slid shut. Nosepokes to the port on the side of the (preselected) odor or visual cue blocked a photodetector within the port, triggering food pellet release, or an error signal, at a variable latency ranging from 0.5 to 1.0 s. The position of the rewarded cue was pseudorandomly varied on successive trials so that the same odor or visual cue were not presented more than twice at the left or the right nosepoke ports. Furthermore, the same trajectory orientation across the chamber (diagonally crossing or running along a side wall) was not rewarded on more than 2 successive trials. The task control system permitted programming the cue contingencies and reward delivery, outputting time stamps. Erroneous choices triggered illumination of an overhead light during a timeout period of 7 s while the barrier remained up. Once mice attained criterion performance (8 rewarded choices out of 10 consecutive trials, or 6 consecutively rewarded trials) in the olfactory or visual discrimination tasks, the system automatically triggered a contingency switch to the other task. Overall, only 14.3% of trials were at criterion performance levels prior to a rule shift, and thus mice were training most of the time. As criterion performance was successive attained, the mice were cycled between the two tasks. In some sessions, it was necessary to pretrain the mice, starting with only the visual or olfactory cue discrimination before adding the other modality cues.
When the mouse stopped performing trials for more than 120 s, the behavioral session was ended. The headstage plug was changed, optic fibers were connected, and the animal was placed in a large plastic beaker. Using a 475 nm-laser light source (DPSSL BL473T3-100FL, Shanghai Laser and Optics Century, Shanghai, PRC), trains of 10 light pulses (pulse duration: 10 ms; light power: 10 mW max; frequency: 5 Hz) were given for 20 times with 10 s inter train intervals under control of an Arduino-based system. The weight of the rewards earned was calculated, and the required amount of chow for the daily feeding was calculated and provided.
2.5. Data analyses
2.5.1. Single unit discrimination
Offline spike sorting was carried out with KiloSort (Pachitariu et al., 2016) followed by manual curation using Klusters (Hazan et al., 2006)1.
To control for the risk that neurons stably recorded across days would be counted more than once, we compared electrophysiological features of pairs of neurons recorded from the same octrode on consecutive days. These features included spike waveform shape, spike auto-correlogram profiles, and mean firing rate with the method of Fraser and Schwartz (2012). To quantify the waveform similarity between a putative single unit recorded on different days, the peak value of the crosscorrelogram between the pairs of samples of average waveform shapes of the units from the 2 days was calculated for each channel of the octrode. The resulting correlation coefficients of different channels were then averaged. The waveform similarity score was calculated as Fisher transform of these averaged coefficients. To quantify the auto-correlogram similarity, the auto-correlograms were calculated from 0 to 100 ms with 5 ms binning. Pearson correlation coefficients between the auto-correlograms of a pair of units were calculated and Fisher transformed. The resulting value is the auto-correlogram similarity score. The mean firing rate similarity score was calculated as the difference between the logs of the mean firing rates in the two recordings. The three similarity scores were combined with a quadratic classifier that computes an optimal decision boundary between same and different neuron pairs under the assumption that the underlying data can be modeled as a mixture of multivariate Gaussians. Since the distribution of different neuron pairs can be obtained using the data obtained from different octrodes, we used partially supervised expectation-maximization to fit a mixture of Gaussian models utilizing this prior information. This was used to estimate the distribution of same-neuron pairs (red dots and curves in Supplementary Figure 2). Specifically, the decision boundary of the classifier is calibrated to produce a 1% error rate in the known different-neuron distribution, and pairs with values lying inside the boundary were identified as same-neuron pairs. Then, different-neuron pairs recorded from same octrodes were estimated. To be conservative, those different-neuron pairs were estimated after the decision boundary of the classifier was re-calibrated to produce a 1% error rate in the same-neuron pair distribution estimated above (Supplementary Figure 2). For the data analysis, we used only a single recording day from each series of recordings from a potentially repeatedly recorded neuron [series of recordings connected with pairs represented by magenta (unknown pair) and red (same-neuron pair) dots in Supplementary Figure 2]. Pairs not assigned as same or different were considered as “unclassified.”
2.5.2. Neuron categories
To identify neurons as dopaminergic, we used the Stimulus-Associated spike Latency Test (SALT; Kvitsiani et al., 2013; Eshel et al., 2015). The test determines whether light pulses significantly changed a neuron’s spike timing by comparing the distribution of first spike latencies relative to the light pulse, assessed in a 10 ms window after light-stimulation, to 10 ms epochs in the baseline period (−150 to 0 ms from the onset of light-stimulation; see Kvitsiani et al., 2013 for details). A significance level of p < 0.01 was selected for this.
All neurons recorded from an octrode with at least one SALT+ response were considered to be in a dopaminergic nucleus. If the octrode was not advanced, neurons on the day before and the day after were also counted as in VTA or SNc, even if no SALT+ responses were recorded on those days. Similarly, if the octrode had not been advanced, and SALT+ responses had been recorded on non-consecutive days, intervening days with no SALT+ responses were still considered as in a dopaminergic nucleus. Also, when electrodes had been advanced, all recordings in days between those with SALT+ recordings were also considered to be in VTA/SNc. Neurons with SALT+ responses are considered dopaminergic. Neurons in a dopaminergic nucleus not demonstrating a significant SALT+ response are labeled SALT-, although this negative result does not conclusively show that the neuron is not dopaminergic. All neurons qualifying as in dopaminergic nuclei were categorized with clustering according to the following parameters: spike width, mean firing rate. The criterion for fast spiking interneurons (FSI) was firing rate >15 Hz and spike width <1.5 ms (cf., Ungless and Grace, 2012). No FSI’s were SALT+ . All other SALT- neurons in dopaminergic nuclei are referred to as “Other.”
2.5.3. LFP recordings and phase-locking in neurons
Local field potentials were derived from wideband signals that had been down-sampled to 1,250 Hz on all channels. Power spectra were calculated with wavelet methods. LFP was taken from the median voltage of all active channels of an octrode containing at least one channel with SALT+ responses. Spikes were removed from the LFP signal according to the method of https://www.fieldtriptoolbox.org/tutorial/spikefield/#analyzing-spikes-and-lfps-recorded-from-the-same-electrode. We quantified phase consistency of spikes relative to the LFP band with both the Rayleigh test of circular uniformity and the unbiased pairwise phase consistency (PPC, Vinck et al., 2012). With the Rayleigh test, a neuron was considered as phase-locked if p < 0.05. In the second method, PPC threshold was determined from a shuffling analysis: For each neuron and each peri-event window, inter-spike intervals were shuffled for each trial. The actual values were considered significant if they exceeded the 95th percentile of this distribution of PPCs calculated for the shuffled data. Response profiles were similar for Rayleigh and PPC tests, and most results are given for the latter, unless otherwise noted.
To control for potential influence of different numbers of spikes in samples on PPC results (e.g., due to differences in firing rates among neurons, or during the respective task events), PPC values and significance thresholds were calculated on equal sample sizes by down-sampling the data. PPC was calculated on 50 spikes randomly selected from the task event period or, for analyses of the entire session, 250 spikes. This procedure was repeated 1,000 times, and resultant PPC values are averaged to calculate the subsampled PPC value. The threshold for testing the significance of the PPC value (p < 0.05) was determined by bootstrapping the PPC results using shuffled spike data (Thorn and Graybiel, 2014). Median PPC’s were calculated for neurons tested that had 50 or more spikes for the period examined.
2.5.4. Behavioral analyses
Data were analyzed for [−500 ms, 500 ms] periods around three trial events. “Cue” is when the mouse first crossed the photobeam while entering the next chamber, triggering cue onset. “Choice” is the time of the nosepoke response, as detected by the photo-detectors in the ports. “Outcome” is either the instant the dispenser released the reward pellet (which made a salient sound), or the onset of the punishment period. For PPC and Rayleigh analyses, eight trial periods are examined and compared: pre-cue [cue onset −0.5 s, cue onset], post-cue [cue onset, cue onset + 0.5 s], and corresponding periods for the choice onset, reward onset and punishment onset events.
2.5.5. Histology
Once stable recordings were no longer possible, marking lesions were made with 10 s of 30 μA cathodal current. After waiting at least 90 min, mice were then killed with a lethal intraperitoneal injection of sodium pentobarbital, and perfused intra-ventricularly with phosphate buffered saline solution (PBS), followed by 10% phosphate buffered formalin. The brain was removed, post-fixed overnight, and placed in phosphate buffered 30% sucrose solution for 2–3 days. Frozen sections were cut at 80 μm, and permeabilized in 0.2% Triton in PBS for 1 h at room temperature. Antibody staining was performed to confirm localization of electrodes in dopaminergic nuclei. They were treated with 3% bovine serum albumen (BSA) and 0.2% Triton in PBS for 1 h with gentle agitation at room temperature to block non-specific binding. Sections were then rinsed for 5 min in PBS at room temperature with gentle agitation. Then the sections were left overnight with gentle agitation at 4°C in a solution of the first antibody, mouse monoclonal anti-TH MAB318 (1:500), 0.067% Triton, and 1% BSA in PBS. After three rinses for 5 min in PBS, the sections were treated with a second antibody (1/200, anti-mouse, green), Nissl-red (1:250), and 0.067% Triton in PBS for 2 h at room temperature. Then sections were rinsed 3 times for 5 min in PBS, and mounted with Fluoromount®. Sections were examined with fluorescence microscopy to verify electrode tips and trajectories in the immunofluorescent stained zones (Supplementary Figure 3D).
3. Results
3.1. Determining the principal frequency bands of LFP oscillations and neuronal phase-locking in VTA/SNc
In recordings from microelectrodes confirmed optogenetically to be in the midbrain dopaminergic nuclei (Supplementary Figure 3), several rhythms were readily apparent even in raw unfiltered LFP traces (Figure 1). To verify this, spectra were analyzed to determine the principal frequency bands most prominent in these nuclei, without making any a priori assumptions of their ranges (Figure 2). Peak frequencies were identified as inflection points in this spectrum, visualized as local minima in the second derivatives of these traces. Their frequencies were 0.7, 4, 10, 22, and 40 Hz (Figure 2). To verify this, a FOOOF analysis (Donoghue et al., 2020) was performed, and curve-fitting yielded bands centered at 4, 8, 41, and 50 Hz (not shown). These results derive exclusively from LFP traces, which carry the risk of contamination with volume-conducted signals. Thus, single unit recordings were analyzed for evidence of rhythmic activity intrinsic to the dopaminergic nuclei.
Figure 1. Identification of principal bands in recordings from the same octrode in VTA in a behaving mouse. Top row is an unfiltered trace. Cue, choice and reward refer to the three task events (see section “2.4.3. The behavioral task”). Examples of occurrences of oscillations in respective frequency bands are circled in the top trace, and also in their respective bandpass filtered traces below. All y-axes are voltage in arbitrary units (a.u.).
Figure 2. Detection of the dominant LFP frequencies in the midbrain dopaminergic nuclei. (Top) Mean (black trace) of recordings from all octrode recordings in optogenetically identified dopaminergic nuclei. Gray traces represent SEM’s. 1/f correction was made. (Middle and bottom) First and second derivatives of the top trace. Ranges of the respective frequency bands are indicated on the band at the top of the Figure, and their centers correspond to local minima in the (bottom) plot.
We recorded in mice training and performing sensory discrimination tasks (described further below). The neurons reported here were recorded from octrodes with at least one positive dopaminergic response (SALT+), or on days between days with dopaminergic responses, and when the electrodes had not been advanced. There were 171 dopaminergic neurons (DA), 113 FSI neurons, and 375 Other neurons (a total of 659 neurons). Neurons were excluded when there was evidence that they had been repeatedly recorded on successive days (see section “2.5.1 Single unit discrimination”). After this, there remained 85 dopaminergic neurons (DA, SALT+), 41 fast spiking inter neurons (FSI), and 254 SALT- non-FSI (Other). Note that the latter group could include dopaminergic neurons whose optogenetic responses were not significant (because of possible technical issues such as insufficient illumination or expression of opsins).
Rayleigh and pairwise phase consistency (PPC; Vinck et al., 2012) analyses assessed neuronal activity phase-locked to LFP oscillations. Frequency bands with narrow windows were examined, and the sliding windows passed successively through the range from 0.1 to 100 Hz. The incidences of significant results were tallied, and several frequencies appeared repeatedly in both tests (Figure 3). These were primarily centered around bands similar to those previously reported elsewhere in the brain: 1–2.5 (slow), 2.5–6.5 (4 Hz), 6.5–13 (theta), 13–30 (alpha), 25–55 (low gamma), and 40–75 Hz (high gamma). These bands are also largely consistent with the LFP spectrum analyses, and thus are selected for further analyses below.
Figure 3. Determining principal frequency bands of oscillatory entrainment in neurons (all cell types pooled together) in the brainstem dopaminergic nuclei. (Top row) Significant responses in the Rayleigh test, PPC or both for each neuron (rows) at various frequencies (tested with a sliding window). The yellow bars signify significant results in the respective tests (p < 0.05) while blue bars indicate absence of significance. (Middle row) Cumulative summary of counts of significant results from neurons from Top row. (Bottom) Superimposition of results from the middle row for comparison.
3.2. Characterization of neuron phase-locking to rhythms
Figure 4 shows examples of neurons phase-locked to the respective frequency bands. For the example in the slow band, the spike-triggered LFP averages (STA) completed a full cycle 0.6–0.8 s after the spike time, corresponding to frequencies of 1.2–1.7 Hz. STA cycle lengths for the other frequency bands are highlighted by arrows above the plots, and these correspond to the respective frequency bands. For 4 Hz, the auto-correlogram of the example on the left in Figure 4 shows a prominent peak corresponding to this frequency band. For the theta band, both examples have auto-correlograms and spike-triggered average with delays on the order of 100 ms, corresponding to 10 Hz.
Figure 4. Examples of neurons in the midbrain dopaminergic nuclei with phase-locking to the principal frequency bands. Data were recorded during performance of the olfactory and visual discrimination tasks, and are from periods from 5 s prior to cue onset until 5 s after the trial outcome (reward or punishment) over all session trials. Two examples (left and right columns) are shown for each frequency band. (Top and 4th row) Averaged waveform from one octrode channel (scale bar is 1 ms). (2nd and 5th rows) Spike auto-correlograms show peaks, bumps or shoulders at delays corresponding to the respective frequency bands (highlighted with vertical bars and arrows). (3rd and 6th rows) Phase-locking of spike activity to the respective frequency bands. Cosine wave above is schematic. (4th and 7th rows) Spike-triggered averages (STA) of the LFPs filtered to the respective bands (arbitrary units). Arrows and vertical bars above STA’s show intervals corresponding to the respective frequency bands. For 1.0–2.5 Hz, both neurons were recorded from octrodes with at least one optically identified dopaminergic neuron, but did not qualify as dopaminergic or FSI (thus referred to as unclassified); 2.5–6.5 Hz are unclassified (left) and DA neurons (right); 6.5 –13 Hz are unclassified (left) and FSI (right); 13–25 Hz and 25–55 Hz are both DA; and 40–75 Hz are DA (left) and unclassified (right).
Pairwise Phase Consistency analyses of whole session data examined the incidence of phase-locking of DA and FSI to the respective bands. In FSIs, the incidences of phase-locked neurons exceeded 25% in the slow and 4 Hz bands, but these were not significantly greater than the other bands for FSI’s (Figure 5A). However, in DA neurons, there was a greater incidence of phase-locking in the theta band than slow or beta bands (binomial test, p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction).
Figure 5. (A) Incidence of PPC phase-locking in DA neurons and FSI in recordings from entire sessions. Percentages are over all DA neurons or FSI recorded. (B) Polar plots of circular distribution of preferred phases of DA (black) and FSI (white). Circular means of distributions that are statistically significant (Rayleigh test, p < 0.05) are indicated by radial bars, and their values in radians for [DA, FSI] are: slow [n.s., 3.1], 4 Hz [0.8, 2.2], theta [1.0, 2.2], beta (not shown) [n.s., n.s.], low gamma [2.4, 2.5], high gamma [2.9, 1.8]. Overlapping zones are in gray. (*–p < 0.05; binomial test with Bonferroni correction).
We next tested whether neurons were phase-locked to multiple bands in PPC analyses (e.g., two adjacent frequency intervals). Supplementary Figure 4 shows that low proportions of neurons were phase-locked to more than one band, indicating that the selected frequency bands are discrete and independent.
Pairwise Phase Consistency analyses of whole session data examined the incidence of phase-locking of DA and FSI to the respective bands. This was significantly greater in FSI than DA neurons in the slow and 4 Hz bands (Figure 5A; binomial test p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction).
We then examined the preferred phases of FSI and DA for the respective bands for all neurons (excluding repeat recordings; Figure 5B). Rayleigh tests revealed significant (p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction) preferred phases for both neuron types for all bands except for the beta band, and DA in the slow band. The preferred phases of FSI and DA were significantly different at 4 Hz, theta and high gamma (Kuiper test, p < 0.05) but not low gamma (which had a relatively low sample).
3.3. Behavioral correlates of LFP amplitude and phase-locking of neurons in VTA/SNc
Next, we examined the evolution LFP amplitude and incidence of phase-locking during acquisition of visual and odor discrimination tasks (described in section “2.4. Behavioral task”; see Figure 6A). Since the respective phases of task execution would call upon different cognitive processes, requiring synchronization of various circuits, analyses compare event-related oscillations and phase-locking of neurons at different task phases. As the mice performed a trial, they first entered the opposite compartment, crossing a photo-detector, which triggered onset of visual or olfactory cues, or both. This is the “cue” event, and the 500 ms periods prior and after this involved approach behavior (“pre-cue”) and cue detection and analysis (“post-cue”). The next event was the nose-poke response to the left or right odor ports, again crossing a photo-detector. The 500 ms events prior and after corresponded to decision (“pre-choice”) and anticipation of reward or punishment (“post-choice”). There was a variable delay between the nose-poke and either the click of the food dispenser for rewarded trials, or the onset of the punishment signals. These delimited the 500 ms “pre-outcome” and “post-outcome” periods.
Figure 6. (A) Schematic of the behavioral chamber [adapted from Scheggia et al. (2014)]. (B) LFP during task periods for rewarded trials only (corrected for 1/f). There are discontinuities because the time between successive events varied among trials. (Left) Mean spectrograms from all data, synchronized with the 3 principal task events. (Right) Summary histograms for the respective bands. (C) Incidence of PPC significant neurons in the respective task periods (DA and FSI pooled together). Detailed phase-locking results for this and other bands are in Supplementary Figure 5. (*–p < 0.05; **–p < 0.01; ***–p < 0.005; binomial test with Bonferroni correction).
3.3.1. The slow band
In the slow band, LFP amplitude was maximal during the cue presentation period and lowest during the outcome period (Figure 6B). A one-way ANOVA of PPC values of FSI’s across trial periods was significant (Kruskal–Wallis, p = 0.029), with the values in the pre-reward period greater than the pre-cue period (binomial test; p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction, reward and punishment outcomes were analyzed separately here). Thus, even though LFP amplitude is relatively lower during this trial period, cells still can maintain regular phase relationships then. In all neurons pooled together, in the slow band there were significantly more neurons phase-locked in the pre-outcome period than the pre-cue period (Figure 6C). Detailed phase-locking results for this and other bands are in Supplementary Figure 5.
3.3.2. The 4 Hz band
In the 4 Hz band, LFP amplitude was maximal during the cue period and lowest during the outcome period (Figure 6B). PPC values of FSIs are significantly different among task event periods (Kruskal–Wallis, p < 0.001), with the post-choice period significantly greater than the pre-cue period (binomial test; p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction). A greater proportion of neurons phase-locked during the post-choice period than several other periods (Figure 6C; both neuron types pooled together). This tendency is apparent, but not significant, when DA and FSI are examined separately (Supplementary Figure 5).
3.3.3. The theta band
Theta LFP amplitude peaks prior to the nose-poke choice and has local minima pre-cue and post-choice. Pairwise phase consistency (PPC) is significantly different between task periods for FSIs (Kruskal–Wallis, p = 0.004; not shown).
3.3.4. The beta and gamma bands
Beta LFP amplitude has local maxima at the nose-poke choice and again 300 ms after, and has local minima pre-cue and post-choice. Low and high gamma remain relatively stable, with a peak in low gamma 400 ms after the choice, figuring prominently in the spectrogram (Figure 6B). There were no significant differences between incidence of significant phase-locking across the task events or between DA vs. FSI for beta or gamma bands.
Note that the samples reported here (e.g., FSI vs. DA, frequency band comparisons) are conservative, since neurons determined to be repeatedly recorded in successive sessions were excluded. Also, differences in firing rates are taken into account for PPC, where we subsampled spikes (see section “2.5.3. LFP recordings and phase-locking in neurons”). While Rayleigh results are not reported above, the patterns of distributions of incidences of neurons significant for the Rayleigh test appear to resemble the PPC results (Supplementary Figure 5).
3.4. Phase-locking dependence upon task performance level
Since the task involved acquisition of new cue-reward associations, for which the dopaminergic nuclei are strongly implicated, we assessed if phase-locking changed as a function of performance level of the animals. We computed the proportion of phase-locked neurons in sessions where the animal reached criterion performance in at least one of the tasks, triggering the rule to be changed (n = 96). These were compared to other sessions where mice did not achieve criterion performance (n = 33). Significant differences were rare, and were not concentrated on any particular frequency band or trial event.
Other tests showed no differences in incidences of phase-locking of cells or of PPC values in comparisons of rewarded vs. punished trials or between cues presented (visual vs. odor vs. both; p < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction; data not shown).
4. Discussion
Local field potentials in the mouse dopaminergic nuclei showed prominent oscillations in several frequency bands. These were not simply volume conducted signals originating elsewhere since neurons recorded in optogenetically identified dopaminergic sites were also modulated by these rhythms. While delta, 4 Hz, theta and low gamma have been documented, this is the first report, to our knowledge, of beta oscillations in the dopaminergic nuclei of behaving animals. Furthermore, behavioral correlates are reported concerning the amplitude of these oscillations, and phase-locking in neurons. Below these are discussed in relation to the published literature for each frequency band.
The distinct nature of these bands is supported by the minor incidence of neurons with phase-locking to more than one band. This tendency for exclusivity is consistent with the low incidences of neurons with phase-locking in the respective bands. If the respective frequency bands correspond to communication channels for different functional networks, this would suggest that these neurons have functional specificities.
Results were similar between Rayleigh tests and PPC, although more cells had significant responses with the former, especially when spike counts were high. This is likely because the Rayleigh test determines if there is a deviation from equal firing at all phases of the oscillation, while PPC determines whether there are common phases among spikes. Since we were interested in phase-locking during brief intervals related to the respective task events, and this reduced the number of spikes that could be analyzed, we selected PPC (which can yield results for as few as 30 spikes; Womelsdorf et al., 2010) for most of these analyses. The Rayleigh test requires a minimal number of spikes for each bin of phase ranges to avoid false positives.
4.1. The slow rhythm
The frequency bands here were determined with several approaches based upon the actual data. This led to distinguishing separate bands for slow and 4 Hz. In some work, the delta band is considered to range from 1 to 5 Hz, and this could lead to confounds. For this reason, we did not use the term “delta” to describe the 1 to 2.5 Hz band here.
Overall, reports in the literature of slow oscillations during waking behavior are relatively sparse. However, Orzeł-Gryglewska et al. (2014) observed prominent 1.5 Hz oscillations in VTA and hippocampal LFP in awake rats, and their power was cross-correlated. Here, the incidence of phase-locking of neurons to the slow rhythm here tended to be greater than the other bands, although this difference was not significant.
There are reports of cortical delta rhythms in awake rodents during stationary immobility in the absence of experimental sensory stimulation (Crochet and Petersen, 2006; Schultheiss et al., 2020). However, these results are not directly comparable here since the range of their delta frequency band included 4 Hz. With intracranial recordings, Halgren et al. (2018) found 2 Hz oscillations that were generated in the superficial layers of the cerebral cortex of awake epilepsy patients. These were coupled to higher frequency oscillations in other layers. We found slow rhythms in hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of rats performing in a T-maze, and noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons were phase-locked to these slow oscillations (Xiang et al., 2023).
In chloral hydrate anesthetized rats, Peters et al. (2004) and Gao et al. (2007) observed infraslow (0.65 and 0.86 Hz, respectively) oscillations in DAergic neurons. They considered this as an experimental model for delta rhythms during sleep. Intracellular PFC UP states (Peters et al., 2004) and PFC LFP negative deflections (Gao et al., 2007) were synchronous with negative deflections of the VTA LFP at this frequency range. The VTA neurons fired in phase with high, but not low amplitude prefrontal cortical oscillations at this frequency. Non-DA neurons were also modulated, but were almost pi radians (180°) out of phase relative to the DA neurons (Gao et al., 2007). In contrast, in our awake behaving mice, the FSI and SALT- Other neurons had a phase difference of only 0.8 and 1.2 rad, respectively from DAergic neurons. TTX infusion in PFC suppressed the slow oscillations in VTA neurons (Gao et al., 2007). Interestingly, lidocaine infusion in VTA (but not substantia nigra) suppressed the UP-DOWN state oscillations in PFC.
Here, more FSIs than DA were phase-locked to the slow and 4 Hz rhythms. Interneurons are known to be instrumental in the generation of theta and gamma rhythms. Our experimental design was not appropriate for testing whether the rhythms are generated locally. While the higher firing rates of interneurons relative to principal neurons could render the PPC and Rayleigh tests more sensitive, this result remained robust since we down-sampled all spikes for PPC.
More neurons were phase-locked to the slow rhythm during the pre-outcome period than the pre-cue period. This could be related to synchronizing dopaminergic structures with associated structure during the period when reward is being anticipated.
van der Velden et al. (2020) optogenetically stimulated lateral VTA in mouse brain slices. Varying the frequency of stimulation, they found that individual neurons had optimal responses (“resonance”) at several values between 0.5 and 5 Hz. This corresponds to the slow band observed here, extending to the 4 Hz band as well. The authors suggest these variations in preferred infraslow and slow frequencies among individual neurons could support temporal processing of inputs and coding for outputs.
4.2. The 4 Hz rhythm
Fujisawa and Buzsáki (2011) showed that the power of 4 Hz oscillation (filtered from 2 to 5 Hz) in rat PFC and VTA, and its coherence between these two structures, are stronger during the choice phase of a working memory task. They found that 46% of putative dopaminergic and 38% of putative GABAergic VTA neurons were significantly phase-locked to the 4 Hz oscillation. Similarly, 4 Hz rhythmicity (with 3–6 Hz filters) occurs in mice during choice periods in another working memory task (Duvarci et al., 2018). 75% of DAergic, and 78% of GABAergic VTA cells were modulated by VTA 4 Hz. (In these papers, neuron types were identified according to responses to systemic apomorphine administration, baseline firing rates and spike width). In contrast, here, the incidence of neurons with significant 4 Hz PPC was significantly elevated during the post-choice period (during the delay prior to reward or punishment). Discrepancies with the cited results could be related to our task lacking an explicit trial-by-trial working memory component. Another difference is that here, twice as many FSI’s than DAergic neurons were modulated by 4 Hz oscillations. This difference could be related to different methods used to identify the two types of neurons, the different filter range used, or sampling properties of the different types of electrodes used. Also, these other papers used the Rayleigh test to detect phase-locking, while we reported PPC results, which tended to have fewer significant FSI’s in this band (Supplementary Figure 5).
In Duvarci et al. (2018), the median preferred phase of DAergic neurons was about −2 rad, while GABAergic neurons were about −0.75 rad. In Fujisawa and Buzsáki (2011), relative to PFC LFP, DAergic neurons had a median phase of about −0.5 rad and GABAergic neurons had a median phase of about −2.6 rad. Here the mean preferred phase of DA neurons in the corresponding frequency band was 0.8 rad, but for FSI’s the value was 2.2 rad.
Lowes et al. (2021) found increased amplitude of 4 Hz amplitude in VTA and nucleus accumbens (NAc) LFPs during stressful restraint in mice (which also reduces reward seeking). Furthermore, multi-unit activity in VTA was phase-locked to NAc 4 Hz. Interestingly, silencing VTA GABAergic neurons with muscimol suppressed NAc LFP 4 Hz in stressed animals.
One potential confound for 4 Hz LFP in mice is the correlation with breathing signals sent from olfactory bulb through piriform cortex and PFC (e.g., Bagur et al., 2021). Rhythmic firing in the DAergic neurons here provides evidence that this is not simply a volume-conducted signal. Thus, the DA nuclei are part of the widespread network of brain structures coordinated by the 4 Hz rhythm (Tort et al., 2018).
4.3. The theta rhythm
In behaving rodents, hippocampal theta is typically reported at 7–8 Hz. Here, the spectral data revealed an inflection point at 10 Hz, and thus the range studied was 8 to 13 Hz, and this helped to avoid overlap with the band centered on 4 Hz. Fujisawa and Buzsáki (2011) filtered for theta in their hippocampal LFP recordings at 7–11 Hz, but did not report VTA LFPs in this frequency range. However, they did observe VTA neurons modulated both by VTA 4 Hz (as replicated here) and by hippocampal theta. Future work should test for potential relations between hippocampal theta and VTA 10 Hz oscillations. Orzeł-Gryglewska et al. (2014) observed VTA LFP at 6–9 Hz, and its power was cross-correlated with that of hippocampal theta. Note that Park and Moghaddam (2017) filtered rat VTA LFPs for theta between 5 and 15 Hz, and found peaks at 10–12 Hz, consistent with the present work in mice. Fujisawa and Buzsáki (2011) found significant phase locking to the hippocampal theta rhythm in 44% of putative dopaminergic neurons and 39% of putative GABAergic cells. In the Park and Moghaddam (2017) study, 45% of DAergic neurons were modulated by VTA 5–15 Hz, while only 23% of non-DAergic neurons were. Here 26% of DA neurons, and 24% of FSI’s were phase-locked to 8–13 Hz, comparable to the latter work. In conditioned rats, Kim et al. (2012) found increased theta (4–8 Hz) power in VTA LFP for appetitive conditioned stimuli (CS) but not aversive CS. Following learning, more putative GABA neurons were phase-locked to the theta rhythm than putative dopamine neurons.
Here, there were no significant differences between incidence of theta phase-locked neurons during the respective trial periods. However, the failure of results to be significant cannot prove the absence of an effect, in particular, in light of our conservative selection criteria. Park and Moghaddam (2017) found coherence between LFPs in VTA and PFC at 5–15 Hz, and this decreased with greater risk of punishment. Thus, both the latter study and Lowes et al. (2021) support a role of these rhythmic oscillations in gating of communication between dopaminergic nuclei and striatum (4 Hz) and with neocortex (10 Hz). However, both Fujisawa and Buzsáki (2011) and Duvarci et al. (2018) found that VTA activity at hippocampal theta frequency was not associated with working memory performance, in contrast with the 4 Hz band. This is consistent with the proposal that the different frequency bands could coordinate communication of complementary types of information (Akam and Kullmann, 2014).
4.4. Beta and gamma rhythms
Relatively few neurons were phase-locked to these bands of DA nucleus LFP’s, and this likely contributed to the absence of significant differences between incidences between cell types and task events. In calcium imaging of awake mice, Eban-Rothschild et al. (2020) found population activity of VTA-GABAergic neurons, but not VTA-dopaminergic neurons, was positively correlated with EEG gamma power, and negatively correlated with theta power. Fujisawa and Buzsáki (2011) found high gamma (30–80 Hz) coherence between PFC and the VTA. The narrow 50 Hz band in the choice arm persisted in both their working memory and control tasks.
5. Conclusion and perspectives
In summary, there is oscillatory activity at several different frequency bands in the dopaminergic VTA and SNc. Of particular interest are the slow (low delta) and 4 Hz bands since their time scale bridges behavioral and neural events. One mechanism for this would be by amplitude modulation of higher frequency oscillations by the phase of slow oscillations, as observed in other systems (Jensen and Colgin, 2007). Further work should investigate possible changes in rhythmic activity in the dopaminergic learning over the course of learning. Another issue to resolve is to determine the origin of slow oscillation in the dopaminergic nuclei. It would also be interesting to investigate coherence of VTA/SNc oscillations with prefrontal cortex, striatum, and hippocampus as well as other closely associated structures such as lateral habenula for this, and other, frequency bands. Indeed, the VTA and lateral habenula have reciprocal connections, and lateral habenula neuronal activity is phase-locked to the hippocampal theta rhythm (Goutagny et al., 2013). Overall, these oscillatory activities would provide a mechanism of potential utility for dopaminergic coordination of brain activity for goal-directed decision-making, learning, and motor control (Williams et al., 2002).
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Comité d’Éthique en Matière d’Expérimentation Animale no. 59 and French Ministère de l’Enseignement Supérieur et de la Recherché.
Author contributions
JM and SIW designed and performed the experiments. MNP performed experiments. JM constructed the experimental apparatus, the optrodes and optogenetic stimulation apparatus, informatics control, and data acquisition systems with support from HN. MV and LV maintained and genotyped the mouse line and guided immunohistochemical processing. FP guided adaptation of the maze and behavioral protocols for training and recording. JM, VJO, and SIW analyzed the data with support from HN, and wrote the manuscript. All authors approved of the manuscript.
Funding
This study was supported by grants from Uehara Memorial Foundation (to JM) and the Takeda Science Foundation (to JM and HN). Thanks to the Labex Memolife, Fondation Bettencourt Schueller, and International Research Project DALoops (Toyama University and CNRS) for the funding support.
Acknowledgments
We thank to France Maloumian for invaluable help with figures, to Yves Dupraz for constructing the experimental chamber, Estelle Anceaume for microscopy training, Michaël Zugaro for helpful suggestions and informatics support, Dr. Guillaume Dugué and NeuroFabLab for facilitating with 3D printing of headstages, Drs. Karim Benchenane and Liyang Xiang for help setting up the optical stimulation, and Dr. Philippe Faure for advice on recording dopaminergic neurons with chronically implanted octrodes.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fncel.2023.1131313/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Akam, T., and Kullmann, D. M. (2014). Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 111–122. doi: 10.1038/nrn3668
Bagur, S., Lefort, J. M., Lacroix, M. M., de Lavilléon, G., Herry, C., Chouvaeff, M., et al. (2021). Breathing-driven prefrontal oscillations regulate maintenance of conditioned-fear evoked freezing independently of initiation. Nat. Comm. 12:2605. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-22798-6
Benchenane, K., Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Tierney, P. L., Gioanni, Y., Battaglia, F. P., et al. (2010). Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal-prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron 66, 921–936. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.013
Berke, J. D. (2009). Fast oscillations in cortical-striatal networks switch frequency following rewarding events and stimulant drugs. Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 848–859. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06843.x
Berke, J. D. (2018). What does dopamine mean? Nat. Neurosci. 21, 787–793. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0152-y
Cerpa, J. C., Coutureau, E., and Parkes, S. L. (2021). Dopamine and noradrenaline modulation of goal-directed behavior in orbital and medial prefrontal cortex: Toward a division of labor? Behav. Neurosci. 135, 138–153. doi: 10.1037/bne0000426
Chau, B. K. H., Jarvis, H., Law, C. K., and Chong, T. T. (2018). Dopamine and reward: A view from the prefrontal cortex. Behav. Pharmacol. 29, 569–583. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000424
Crochet, S., and Petersen, C. C. (2006). Correlating whisker behavior with membrane potential in barrel cortex of awake mice. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 608–610. doi: 10.1038/nn1690
Donoghue, T., Haller, M., Peterson, E. J., Varma, P., Sebastian, P., Gao, R., et al. (2020). Parameterizing neural power spectra into periodic and aperiodic components. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1655–1665. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00744-x
Duvarci, S., Simpson, E. H., Schneider, G., Kandel, E. R., Roeper, J., and Sigurdsson, T. (2018). Impaired recruitment of dopamine neurons during working memory in mice with striatal D2 receptor overexpression. Nat. Commun. 9:2822. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05214-4
Eban-Rothschild, A., Borniger, J. C., Rothschild, G., Giardino, W. J., Morrow, J. G., and de Lecea, L. (2020). Arousal state-dependent alterations in VTA-GABAergic neuronal activity. eNeuro 7, ENEURO.0356–19.2020, doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0356-19.2020
Eshel, N., Bukwich, M., Rao, V., Hemmelder, V., Tian, J., and Uchida, N. (2015). Arithmetic and local circuitry underlying dopamine prediction errors. Nature 525, 243–246. doi: 10.1038/nature14855
Fraser, G. W., and Schwartz, A. B. (2012). Recording from the same neurons chronically in motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1970–1978. doi: 10.1152/jn.01012.2010
Fujisawa, S., and Buzsáki, G. (2011). A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron 72, 153–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.018
Gao, M., Liu, C. L., Yang, S., Jin, G. Z., Bunney, B. S., and Shi, W. X. (2007). Functional coupling between the prefrontal cortex and dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area. J. Neurosci. 27, 5414–5421. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5347-06.2007
Goutagny, R., Loureiro, M., Jackson, J., Chaumont, J., Williams, S., Isope, P., et al. (2013). Interactions between the lateral habenula and the hippocampus: Implication for spatial memory processes. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 2418–2426. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.142
Grace, A. A. (2016). Dysregulation of the dopamine system in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 524–532. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.57
Gray, C. M., König, P., Engel, A. K., and Singer, W. (1989). Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338, 334–337. doi: 10.1038/338334a0
Halgren, M., Fabó, D., Ulbert, I., Madsen, J. R., Erőss, L., Doyle, W. K., et al. (2018). Superficial slow rhythms integrate cortical processing in humans. Sci. Rep. 8:2055. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20662-0
Hazan, L., Zugaro, M., and Buzsáki, G. (2006). Klusters, NeuroScope, NDManager: A free software suite for neurophysiological data processing and visualization. J. Neurosci. Meth. 155, 207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.01.017
Headley, D. B., Delucca, M. V., Haufler, D., and Pare, D. (2015). Incorporating 3D-printing technology in the design of head-caps and electrode drives for recording neurons in multiple brain regions. J. Neurophysiol. 113:00955.02014. doi: 10.1152/jn.00955.2014
Jensen, O., and Colgin, L. L. (2007). Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 267–269. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.003
Kim, Y., Wood, J., and Moghaddam, B. (2012). Coordinated activity of ventral tegmental neurons adapts to appetitive and aversive learning. PLoS One 7:e29766. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029766
Kvitsiani, D., Ranade, S., Hangya, B., Taniguchi, H., Huang, J. Z., and Kepecs, A. (2013). Distinct behavioural and network correlates of two interneuron types in prefrontal cortex. Nature 498, 363–366. doi: 10.1038/nature12176
Lowes, D. C., Chamberlin, L. A., Kretsge, L. N., Holt, E. S., Abbas, A. I., Park, A. J., et al. (2021). Ventral tegmental area GABA neurons mediate stress-induced blunted reward-seeking in mice. Nat. Commun. 12:3539. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-23906-2
Nieoullon, A., Cheramy, A., and Glowinski, J. (1977). Nigral and striatal dopamine release under sensory stimuli. Nature 269, 340–342. doi: 10.1038/269340a0
Oberto, V. J., Boucly, C. J., Gao, H. Y., Todorova, R., Zugaro, M. B., and Wiener, S. I. (2022). Distributed cell assemblies spanning prefrontal cortex and striatum. Curr. Biol. 32, 1.e6-13.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.10.007
Orzeł-Gryglewska, J., Matulewicz, P., and Jurkowlaniec, E. (2014). Theta activity in local field potential of the ventral tegmental area in sleeping and waking rats. Behav. Brain Res. 265, 84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.023
Ott, T., and Nieder, A. (2019). Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 213–234. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.006
Pachitariu, M., Steinmetz, N., Kadir, S., Carandini, M., and Harris, K. (2016). Kilosort: Realtime spike-sorting for extracellular electrophysiology with hundreds of channels. bioRxiv [Preprint] doi: 10.1101/061481
Park, J., and Moghaddam, B. (2017). Risk of punishment influences discrete and coordinated encoding of reward-guided actions by prefrontal cortex and VTA neurons. ELife 6:e30056. doi: 10.7554/eLife.30056
Peters, Y., Barnhardt, N. E., and O’Donnell, P. (2004). Prefrontal cortical up states are synchronized with ventral tegmental area activity. Synapse 52, 143–152.
Scheggia, D., and Papaleo, F. (2016). An operant intra-/extra-dimensional set-shift task for mice. J. Vis. Exp. 107:e53503. doi: 10.3791/53503
Scheggia, D., Bebensee, A., Weinberger, D. R., and Papaleo, F. (2014). The ultimate intra-/extra-dimensional attentional set-shifting task for mice. Biol. Psychiat. 75, 660–670. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.021
Schultheiss, N. W., Schlecht, M., Jayachandran, M., Brooks, D. R., McGlothan, J. L., Guilarte, T. R., et al. (2020). Awake delta and theta-rhythmic hippocampal network modes during intermittent locomotor behaviors in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 134, 529–546. doi: 10.1037/bne0000409
Schultz, W. (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: From theories to data. Physiol. Rev. 95, 853–951. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00023.2014
Schultz, W. (2016). Dopamine reward prediction error coding. Dialog. Clin. Neurosci. 18, 23–32. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.1/wschultz
Sutton, R. S., and Barto, A. G. (2018). Reinforcement learning. An introduction, 2nd Edn. Cambridge: A Bradford Book.
Thorn, C. A., and Graybiel, A. M. (2014). Differential entrainment and learning-related dynamics of spike and local field potential activity in the sensorimotor and associative striatum. J. Neurosci. 34, 2845–2859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1782-13.2014
Tort, A. B. L., Ponsel, S., Jessberger, J., Yanovsky, Y., Brankačk, J., and Draguhn, A. (2018). Parallel detection of theta and respiration-coupled oscillations throughout the mouse brain. Sci. Rep. 8:6432. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24629-z
Ungless, M. A., and Grace, A. A. (2012). Are you or aren’t you? Challenges associated with physiologically identifying dopamine neurons. Trends Neurosci. 35, 422–430. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.02.003
van der Velden, L., Vinck, M. A., and Wadman, W. J. (2020). Resonance in the mouse ventral tegmental area dopaminergic network induced by regular and Poisson distributed optogenetic stimulation in-vitro. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 14:11. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2020.00011
Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550
Vinck, M., Battaglia, F. P., Womelsdorf, T., and Pennartz, C. (2012). Improved measures of phase-coupling between spikes and the local field potential. J. Comput. Neurosci. 33, 53–75. doi: 10.1007/s10827-011-0374-4
Williams, D., Tijssen, M., Van Bruggen, G., Bosch, A., Insola, A., Di Lazzaro, V., et al. (2002). Dopamine-dependent changes in the functional connectivity between basal ganglia and cerebral cortex in humans. Brain 125(Pt 7), 1558–1569. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf156
Womelsdorf, T., Fries, P., and Pennartz, C. M. (2010). The pairwise phase consistency: A bias-free measure of rhythmic neuronal synchronization. Neuroimage. 51, 112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.073
Womelsdorf, T., Schoffelen, J. M., Oostenveld, R., Singer, W., Desimone, R., Engel, A. K., et al. (2007). Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science 316, 1609–1612.
Keywords: awake delta rhythm, substantia nigra pars compacta, ventral tegmental area, slow rhythm, 4 Hz
Citation: Oberto VJ, Matsumoto J, Pompili MN, Todorova R, Papaleo F, Nishijo H, Venance L, Vandecasteele M and Wiener SI (2023) Rhythmic oscillations in the midbrain dopaminergic nuclei in mice. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 17:1131313. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2023.1131313
Received: 24 December 2022; Accepted: 29 May 2023;
Published: 23 June 2023.
Edited by:
George C. McConnell, Stevens Institute of Technology, United StatesReviewed by:
Martin Darvas, University of Washington, United StatesJolanta Orzeł-Gryglewska, University of Gdańsk, Poland
Copyright © 2023 Oberto, Matsumoto, Pompili, Todorova, Papaleo, Nishijo, Venance, Vandecasteele and Wiener. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sidney I. Wiener, c2lkbmV5LndpZW5lckBjb2xsZWdlLWRlLWZyYW5jZS5mcg==
†These authors share first authorship