- 1NMR Research Unit, Department of Neuroinflammation, Queen Square MS Centre, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
- 2Department of Electrical, Computer and Biomedical Engineering, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
- 3Brain Connectivity Center, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
- 4Blackheath Brain Injury Rehabilitation Centre, London, United Kingdom
- 5NMR Research Unit, Department of Brain Repair and Rehabilitation, Queen Square MS Centre, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
- 6Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
- 7Brain MRI 3T Center, Neuroradiology Unit, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
- 8Department of Physics, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
- 9Brain MRI 3T Center, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
While task-dependent responses of specific brain areas during cognitive tasks are well established, much less is known about the changes occurring in resting state networks (RSNs) in relation to continuous cognitive processing. In particular, the functional involvement of cerebro-cerebellar loops connecting the posterior cerebellum to associative cortices, remains unclear. In this study, 22 healthy volunteers underwent a multi-session functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) protocol composed of four consecutive 8-min resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) scans. After a first control scan, participants listened to a narrated story for the entire duration of the second rs-fMRI scan; two further rs-fMRI scans followed the end of story listening. The story plot was purposely designed to stimulate specific cognitive processes that are known to involve the cerebro-cerebellar loops. Almost all of the identified 15 RSNs showed changes in functional connectivity (FC) during and for several minutes after the story. The FC changes mainly occurred in the frontal and prefrontal cortices and in the posterior cerebellum, especially in Crus I-II and lobule VI. The FC changes occurred in cerebellar clusters belonging to different RSNs, including the cerebellar network (CBLN), sensory networks (lateral visual network, LVN; medial visual network, MVN) and cognitive networks (default mode network, DMN; executive control network, ECN; right and left ventral attention networks, RVAN and LVAN; salience network, SN; language network, LN; and working memory network, WMN). Interestingly, a k-means analysis of FC changes revealed clustering of FCN, ECN, and WMN, which are all involved in working memory functions, CBLN, DMN, and SN, which play a key-role in attention switching, and RSNs involved in visual imagery. These results show that the cerebellum is deeply entrained in well-structured network clusters, which reflect multiple aspects of cognitive processing, during and beyond the conclusion of auditory stimulation.
Introduction
The brain is known to operate through multiple loops forming networks consisting of spatially distributed, but functionally connected regions that continuously share information with each other (van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010). Functional connectivity (FC) can be defined as the temporal correlation of neural activity patterns between anatomically separated brain regions (Friston et al., 1993; Biswal et al., 1997; Friston, 2011). Even at rest, the brain is organized in networks, known as resting state networks (RSNs), in which distinct brain areas exhibit consistent synchronization (Beckmann et al., 2005; Buzsaki, 2006; Sadaghiani et al., 2010; Boubela et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013; Chen and Glover, 2015). The organization and integration of processing into RSNs can be effectively investigated using independent component analysis (ICA) of -weighted fMRI time series as proposed for resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) (Fox and Raichle, 2007). Traditionally, rs-fMRI is based on the assumption of temporal stationarity, in which linear correlation of BOLD signals has been used to assess FC across regions computed over the whole duration of a single-session scan (Fox and Raichle, 2007). However, there is growing evidence from MRI and animal electrophysiology that RSNs are affected by interfering physiological factors, like arousal and attention, or by performing naturalistic tasks, like watching a movie (Hasson et al., 2010; Kauppi et al., 2010), or listening to a narration (Hasson et al., 2009), so that their stationarity can be lost over time; in this situation, RSN changes can be used to investigate brain dynamics (Deco et al., 2011; Menon, 2012; Elton and Gao, 2015; Hansen et al., 2015; Spadone et al., 2015; Yuste and Fairhall, 2015). Some studies have shown that brain state-dependent FC changes may be related to a variety of different causes such as mental tasks, sleep, and learning (Hutchison et al., 2013). Despite this, how RSNs are recruited and operate during continuous cognitive processing under naturalistic stimulation in humans in vivo is still elusive and it is unclear whether and how long for the RSNs engagement persists after the conclusion of sensory stimulation (Hasson et al., 2009; Berns et al., 2013; Mackey et al., 2013).
Among subcortical structures, special interest has recently been raised by the cerebellum, since growing evidence indicates that, in humans, it plays a relevant role in high-level cognitive and behavioral processing (Schmahmann and Caplan, 2006; Ito, 2008; Buckner, 2013; D‘Angelo and Casali, 2013; Sokolov, 2018). Neuronal recordings in monkeys have demonstrated that the cerebellum does perform sophisticated internal computations essential for motor learning through prediction (Brooks et al., 2015). Moreover, the cerebellum has been shown to connect tightly to associative cerebro-cortical areas and to take part in networks involved in cognitive processing (Castellazzi et al., 2014; Palesi et al., 2015, 2017; Pezoulas et al., 2017) including attention switching, language, imagery and visuo-spatial processing, decision making and reasoning (Ito, 2008; Stoodley, 2012) as well as cognitive control (O’Connell and Basak, 2018). This complex set of operations is expected to involve multiple sub-networks and different cerebellar modules associated with various cortical regions.
In this work, we have developed a framework to estimate and classify FC changes of the RSNs during a multi-session protocol, designed to identify how RSNs FC changes before, during and after listening to a narrated story. The story was enriched with elements of movement and mental manipulation, error/novelty detection, working memory and planning to enhance the engagement of working memory and executive functions supported by the cerebro-cerebellar loops.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Magnetic resonance imaging acquisitions were performed on 22 healthy subjects (mean age 27.68 ± 3.53, 11 males, all Italians). All subjects had normal hearing and high educational level (mean years of education = 18.45 ± 1.89). The study was approved by the ethics committee of the IRCCS Mondino Foundation and all subjects provided written informed consent.
MRI Acquisitions
All subjects underwent MRI examination using a 3T Siemens Skyra scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. For each subject, an fMRI scan consisted in acquiring images using a gradient echo - echo planar imaging (GE-EPI) sequence with TR/TE = 3010/20 ms, flip angle = 89°, voxel size = 2.5 mm isotropic, FOV = 224 mm2, 60 slices, 160 volumes, total acquisition time 8.05 min. For anatomical reference a high-resolution 3D T1-weighted (3DT1) volume was collected using a MPRAGE sequence with TR/TE = 2300/2.95 ms, TI = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, voxel size = 1 mm × 1 mm × 1.2 mm, FOV = 270 mm, 144 sagittal slices, acquisition time 6.31 min. The overall acquisition protocol involved repeating the fMRI scan four times and the total acquisition time was approximately 40 min.
fMRI Experimental Design
The experiment was designed to study RSN changes in response to an evolving complex cognitive stimulation, resembling the ecological context of every-day life. This was achieved by delivering a narrated story (in Italian). The full text of the story is available as Supplementary Material in the original Italian language and in its English translated version. In order to investigate the intervention of the cerebellum in cognition, the story was purposely written following a categorization of functions based on extensive literature revisitation at the physiological, psychological, and neurological level (D‘Angelo and Casali, 2013). Elements that were embedded in the story are movement and mental manipulation of objects in space, error/novelty detection, working memory and mental planning of the consequences of events (prediction). In order to place the story in a context familiar to most listeners, and easy to engage with, the story plot was set in a “school of magic,” which is also the title. The story was recorded on an audio-CD by a female voice. The audio-stimulus had a length of 8.05 min and was presented to the subjects binaurally using a digital audio system.
The overall acquisition protocol consisted in four repetitions of the rs-fMRI scan, labeled, respectively: pre, story, post1 and post2. The protocol started with the acquisition of the first rs-fMRI repetition (i.e., pre), which represented the baseline, i.e., the first control scan of the experiment. During the second rs-fMRI repetition (i.e., story), the recording of “School of magic” was played. A third rs-fMRI repetition (i.e., post1) was acquired with no stimulus straight after the story scan to capture the initial return to baseline. The post1 acquisition represented the second control scan of the experiment. Since we hypothesized that cognitive processing and working memory-related changes in FC might continue for several minutes (Barnes et al., 2009; Hasson et al., 2009; Gordon et al., 2014), we acquired a second post-stimulus rs-fMRI repetition (i.e., post2) as far away as possible in time from the story to verify the complete recovery of the resting state baseline status. The post2 acquisition represented, therefore, the third control scan of the study. In order to keep the overall experiment time feasible and avoid dead periods while the subjects were in the scanner, we performed a high resolution 3DT1 acquisition in between post1 and post2. The time between the end of story and the beginning of post2 was therefore maximized to 14.36 min. The experimental design is summarized in Figure 1, where timings between the beginning of each fMRI acquisition and the following one are also indicated.
FIGURE 1. Schematic illustration of the fMRI study design. The acquisition protocol included four consecutive fMRI scans (labeled, respectively, pre, story, post1, and post2). During the second scan (story) subjects were asked to listen to an 8.05 min-long narrated story. The MRI acquisition session had a total duration of 40 min. We imposed an interval of about 15 min between the end of the fMRI with the story (story) and the beginning of the last fMRI (post2) in order to have a temporal window suitable to detect the evolution of the FC magnitude and spatial extent of each RSN.
The participants remained inside the MRI scanner for the entire duration of the multi-session acquisition protocol, which was run without any break. The acquisition protocol was explained to each participant rigorously before the start of the MRI examination, asking them to pay attention to the story narration, which they heard in the scanner for the first time. Participants were also informed that they would be required to fill in a verification questionnaire at the end of the experiment in order to check their level of attention as well as their level of comprehension of the story content. No vocal inputs were delivered to the participants in the scanner after the starting of the MRI acquisition to avoid any sort of listening inputs, other than the naturalistic stimulus, that might interfere with the purpose of the study.
fMRI Analysis
In this study, rs-fMRI images were treated with the ICA followed by a dual regression technique to identify the RSNs and their changes in relation to the naturalistic stimulation. To investigate the networks’ trends of changes across the four scans of the rs-fMRI protocol, we introduced a relative total change (rcT) index, derived from specific dual regression maps; we then performed a k-means clustering to group the networks depending on their specific patterns of rcT changes. We used the k-means results to speculate about the similarity of the RSNs’ responses to the naturalistic task. The details of each analysis are reported below.
Data Pre-processing
All fMRI analysis was performed with FSL (FMRIB Software Library, version 5.0.91). Individual subject’s pre-processing consisted in motion correction, brain extraction, spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) of 5 mm, and high pass temporal filtering equivalent to 120 s (0.008 Hz). Individual fMRI volumes were registered to the corresponding structural 3DT1 scan using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT) and subsequently to standard space (MNI152) using FMRIB’s Non-linear Image Registration Tool (FNIRT) with default options.
For each subject, rs-fMRI images were analyzed using the ICA first during pre-processing at single-subject level (single-ICA) for denoizing, using the ICA-based X-noiseifier (FIX) tool (Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014) as implemented in FSL. ICA was then applied at group-level (group-ICA) on the pre-processed rs-fMRI data using the Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC) method in order to characterize the RSNs (Beckmann et al., 2005).
Identification of RSNs
For each recruited subject, pre-processed fMRI images underwent group-ICA analysis to characterize RSNs. Specifically, pre-processed functional data, containing 160 time points (volumes) for each subject, were temporally concatenated across subjects to create a single 4-dimensional data set. The dataset was decomposed into independent components (ICs), with an automatic estimation of the number of components, which resulted in spatial maps used subsequently for assessing parameters’ time course over the four fMRI scans. Model order was estimated using the Laplace approximation to the Bayesian evidence for a probabilistic principal component model. Some of the ICs were identified as noise while others as RSNs, based on their frequency spectra and spatial patterns (Beckmann et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2009). In other words, this processing is run on the entire dataset (i.e., the total 4 fMRI acquisitions × 26 subjects = 104 fMRI scans) and decomposes data into spatial maps that are the independent components (ICs) relative to the total processed dataset, or the multi-subject ICA components. This means that ICs are the same for each subject and represent the maps within which inference between scans (pre, story, post1, and post2) is then evaluated applying the dual regression processing step (see the section “Dual Regression Analysis”).
Dual Regression Analysis
A non-parametric permutation test, referred to as “dual regression” technique, was then applied to detect statistically significant differences in the ICs, including the RSNs, among the 4 consecutive repetitions of the fMRI protocol. The dual regression analysis was carried out on the total ICs using age, gender and score of questionnaire as additional covariates (Filippini et al., 2009). In detail, the spatial ICs were used in a linear model fit against each individual fMRI data set (spatial regression), to create matrices that described the temporal dynamics for each component and subject’s session (i.e., pre, story, post1, and post2 for each single subject) separately. Subsequently these matrices were used in another linear model fit against the associated subject’s session data set (temporal regression) to estimate subject’s session-specific spatial correlation map. Spatial maps of all subjects’ sessions were then collected into single dimensional files for each original IC and tested voxel-wise for group-comparison contrasts, where in this study group means scan session; we also assessed the group mean effect (ME) contrasts (labeled MEpre, MEstory, MEpost1 and MEpost2) running non-parametric permutation tests (i.e., FSL randomize algorithm) (Winkler et al., 2014) with 5,000 permutations. In detail, the group-comparison contrasts were first used to identify significant FC changes within the RSNs when looking at the story scan vs. all the other scans (pre, post1, and post2) taken together. This first comparisons (e.g., story scan > all other scans and story scan < all other scans) allowed us to investigate whether there were FC differences between the resting state signal in the presence of the story stimulus and all conditions without the stimulus. Direct comparisons of a single scan session vs. another one (e.g., story > pre) were then assessed for a more detailed analysis of the FC changes within the RSNs during different stages of the experiment. The group ME contrasts were instead calculated and fed into a clustering analysis (see the section “Clustering of RSNs Changes” for full details).
For each tested contrast, the resulting statistical maps were corrected for both family-wise error (FWE) and threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE). The FWE-TFCE-corrected maps’ voxels that survived a statistical threshold of p ≤ 0.05 were considered significant.
Clustering of RSNs Changes
For an objective assessment of the RSN changes across the four scans of the fMRI protocol, we performed a clustering analysis using indexes derived from the group ME maps we obtained from the randomize step (see the section “Dual Regression Analysis”).
For each RSN and for each scan, we used the corresponding ME map to evaluate its spatial extent (SpE, i.e., number of significant non-zero voxels). We used the individual subject’s mean FC maps to obtain values of FC magnitude for each voxel and for each RSN, averaged across the group (i.e., mean FC value of non-zero voxels per scan across subjects) over the areas identified by the corresponding ME maps. For each network, we calculated two parameters that we labeled “relative change in FC” (rcFC) and “relative change in spatial extent” (rcSpE) by applying a normalization of FC and SpE specific to each RSN. In detail, the values of FC magnitude and SpE for each RSN and for each scan were normalized, respectively, to the values of FC magnitude and SpE measured in pre, as well as at their peak values, as described in formulas (1) and (2):
where j represents the scans of interest for the evaluation of the rc indices (i.e., pre, story, post1, or post2). We visually inspected the overall RSNs behavioral changes by plotting rcFC and rcSpE for each fMRI run. Values of rcFC and rcSpE calculated from different fMRI scans were also statistically compared using the repeated measures ANOVA test with Bonferroni correction using SPSS (version 23.0, Chicago, IL, United States).
To have an overall view of the dynamical changes for each RSN between scans over time, we calculated a composite “relative total change” parameter, rcT, as:
Finally, we run a k-means clustering analysis (Duda et al., 2000) using as input argument rcT values for each RSN and each scan of interest, i.e., story, post1 and post2. The k-means algorithm was run initialized the cluster centroids applying the k-means++ method (Arthur and Vassilvitskii, 2007) and used Euclidean distance as dissimilarity metrics to iteratively (300 repetitions) assign inputs to the closest centroid (Duda et al., 2000). The Silhouette score was used to automatically determine the optimal number of clusters (k) in the data. Specifically, this method computes the average silhouette of observations for different values of k measures, with 2 ≤ k ≤ 10, determining at each run how well each object laid within its cluster. The optimal number of clusters k is the one that maximizes the average silhouette over the range of possible values for k (Kaufman and Rousseeuw, 1990).
The clustering analysis was carried out using the Orange software tool (version 3.9).
Results
In this study, 22 healthy subjects underwent rs-fMRI, during which they listened to a narrated story. At the end of the recording, the subjects were interrogated about the story content demonstrating their attentive engagement.
RSN Identification
The results of ICA processing on all fMRI scans resulted in 57 independent components, 15 of which were recognized as plausible networks based on their frequency spectra and spatial pattern (Smith et al., 2009; Castellazzi et al., 2014). The remaining 42 components, reflected artifacts like movement, physiological noise or cerebro-spinal fluid (CSF) partial volume effects.
The resulting 15 RSNs were (Figure 2): sensory motor network (SMN), lateral visual network (LVN), medial visual network (MVN), auditory network (AN), default mode network (DMN), executive control network (ECN), frontal cortex network (FCN), right (R) and left (L) ventral attention networks (VAN), task positive network (TPN), precuneus network (PN), salience network (SN), language network (LN), working memory network (WMN), and the cerebellar network (CBLN).
FIGURE 2. Illustration of the 15 RSNs identified in this study. From top left: sensory motor network (SMN), lateral visual network (LVN), medial visual network (MVN), auditory network (AN), default mode network (DMN), executive control network (ECN), frontal cortex network (FCN), right (R) and left (L) ventral attention networks (VAN), task positive network (TPN), precuneus network (PN), salience network (SN), language network (LN), working memory network (WMN) and the cerebellar network (CBLN). Each RSN is presented as a blue mask on a sagittal, coronal and axial view. For each RSN, the last column of each trio of views show the cerebellar areas (in red-yellow scale) of the network plotted on a flatmap of the cerebellar cortex. With the exception of CBLN which involved the almost the entire cerebellum, nine RSNs showed at least one cerebellar node.
SMN, LVN, MVN, and AN are directly implicated in sensory processing, while DMN, FCN, ECN, VAN (RVAN and LVAN), TPN, PN, SN, LN, and WMN are associated with higher cognitive functions (Papanicolaou, 2017). As such, we will refer to this latter group of RSNs as “cognitive” RSNs. The CBLN is usually considered a sensorimotor network, although recent studies highlight its involvement in cognitive circuitries too (Stoodley, 2012).
Furthermore, with the exception of CBLN, which involved almost the entire cerebellar cortex, 9 out of the 15 identified RSNs included clusters in the cerebellum (Figure 2). Hence cerebellar nodes were present in the majority of the sensory processing networks (LVN and MVN) and of the cognitive ones (DMN, ECN, RVAN, LVAN, SN, LN, and WMN).
Figure 3 shows the time course signal for each RSN as output by ICA.
FIGURE 3. Fluctuations of the ICA time courses associated to the 15 identified RSNs during the entire experiment. The time interval during which the naturalistic stimulus has been delivered (i.e., during story scan) is highlighted with a gray panel in the plot. Note that switching brain activity from a resting state (i.e., brain condition during the pre scan) into an active state (condition during the story scan) and then back again to resting state (post1 scan condition), over a time scale of several minutes, is associated to a marked change in the amplitude of the RSNs’ time course signals.
Changes in RSNs During Naturalistic Stimulation
Group-Comparisons: FC Changes in RSNs
For each RNS, dual regression analysis was firsts used to reveal the presence of significant FC differences between the story scan versus all the remaining scans (pre, post1, and post2) considered altogether as a single (“all”) group (i.e., story < all, story > all). This comparison yielded the following results:
- story < all: no significant changes were observed when looking for reduced FC areas in story versus the remaining scans of the fMRI protocol (i.e., all group).
- story > all: significant areas (p < 0.05, FWE-TFCE-corrected) of increased FC in story compared to the other scans altogether were found in nine RSNs: MVN, AN, DMN, RVAN, LVAN, TPN, SN, LN, and CBLN (Figure 4). Large clusters (>50 voxels) of increased FC were located in the precuneus (involving the posterior DMN and MVN) and in the middle and superior frontal gyrus (LVAN, SN, TPN and the anterior part of DMN). Large clusters (30 < voxels < 50) of increased FC were found in lobule VI of the posterior cerebellum (CBLN, LVAN, and RVAN), culmen and lobule V of the anterior cerebellum (CBLN), posterior cingulum (LVAN, SN), thalamus (LN) and superior and middle temporal gyrus (LVAN, TPN, RVAN, and AN).
FIGURE 4. Global FC changes within the RSNs in story > all. On top (A): in red, brain areas showing significantly increased FC (p ≤ 0.05, FWE-TFCE-corrected) within the RSNs when comparing the story scan to the remaining scans (pre, post1, and post2) considered altogether as a unique (“all”), i.e., story > all. The red bar plot on the bottom (B) shows, for story > all, the ranking of the RSNs according to the percentage of their alteration calculated as the per cent ratio between the number of altered voxels in the specific RSN (Ntstat) and the number of voxels of the RSN mask (NRNS) as it output from ICA.
Given the significant changes observed when considering story > all, three further comparisons were tested in order to detect significant areas (p < 0.05, FWE-TFCE-corrected) of increased FC in story compared to either pre (i.e., story > pre), post1 (i.e., story > post1) or post2 (i.e., story > post2) scans. The following results were found (see also Figure 5):
- story > pre: significant areas of increased FC in story compared to the pre scan were found in six networks: DMN, RVAN, LVAN, TPN, SN, and CBLN. In detail, large clusters (>70 voxels) of increased FC were found in the cingulate gyrus [Brodmann area (BA)23 and BA3], involving LVAN and DMN, in the superior and middle frontal gyri (BA9-10, BA46) involving LVAN, SN, and RVAN and in the middle temporal gyrus (BA39) involving SN and RVAN. Smaller clusters (20 < voxels < 50) of increased FC were detected in the precentral gyrus (BA4) of the anterior DMN, in the parahippocampal gyrus involving SN, in the inferior parietal cortex (BA40) of LVAN and TPN, and in the cerebellar Crus II of the CBLN and LVAN networks.
- story > post1: compared to post1, in story significant areas of increased FC were found in four networks: LVN, DMN, LVAN, and SN. A large cluster (>50 voxels) of increased FC was centered in the lingual gyrus involving SN. Smaller clusters (20 < voxels < 50) were found in the middle frontal gyrus (BA9-10) involving DMN and SN, in the anterior cingulate gyrus of DMN, in the cuneus (BA19) involving SN and LVN, in the superior temporal gyrus (BA41-42) of SN and in the cerebellar areas of Crus I and lobule VI involving DMN and LVAN.
- story > post2: significant clusters of increased FC in story compared to the post2 scan were found in five networks: LVN, MVN, DMN, TPN, and SN. A large cluster (>1,000 voxels) of increased FC was located in the occipital lobe, mainly involving LVN and MVN and extended to the superior parietal lobule, cuneus and precuneus involving areas of DMN, TPN, and SN. Smaller clusters (50 < voxels < 150) were found in the middle (BA9-10) and inferior (BA45-46) frontal gyri involving TPN and DMN. Furthermore, a small cluster (50 < voxels < 100) of increased FC was also detected in the cerebellar Crus I and lobule VI involving LVN.
FIGURE 5. Global FC changes within the RSNs in story > pre, story > post1 and story > post2. The picture show in green the brain areas significantly increased in story > pre (A), story > post1 (B), and story > post2 (C). Below each 3D brain pictures are reported the bar plot which show, for each contrast (A–C) the ranking of the RSNs according to the percentage of their alteration calculated as described in Figure 3 legend.
Clustering of RSNs According to Their Dynamic Changes
In order to identify possible RSN clusters, a k-means analysis was performed. The Silhouette index resulted maximum for k = 3, which indicates the optimal number of clusters to be used for k-means (Figures 6A,B). When the k-means algorithm was instructed to group data into three clusters (k = 3), the RSNs turned out to be sorted as follows. The first cluster (C1) included eight networks: LVN, MVN, SMN, AN, RVAN, TPN, PN, and LN. The second cluster (C2) included the four RSNs: DMN, SN, LVAN, and CBLN. The third cluster (C3) included the remaining three networks: ECN, FCN, and WMN (Figure 7A). These three clusters were functionally related to sensory processing, cognitive processing and working memory (see “Discussion” section below).
FIGURE 6. Details of the Silhouette scoring. (A) Silhouette plot offering a graphical representation of consistency within the three clusters (C1 in blue, C2 in green and C3 in magenta) identified by k-means with k = 3. The Silhouette coefficient values, reported on each bar of the plot, represent a measure of how similar each data instance (i.e., each RSN) is to its own cluster in comparison to other clusters. Specifically, a silhouette coefficient close to 1 indicates that the RSN is close to the center of the cluster, while RNSs with silhouette coefficients close to 0 are on the border between two neighboring clusters. The average value of the Silhouette coefficients reported on the bars is 0.652 and represents the final Silhouette score for k = 3. (B) Plot showing the average Silhouette scores for different values of k (i.e., different number of k clusters), with 2 ≤ k ≤ 10. Note that, for our data, the maximum Silhouette score (0.652), which corresponds to the optimal number of k clusters to be used for the analysis, is obtained for k = 3 (Silhouette score = 0.652).
FIGURE 7. Results of the k-means clustering results analysis for k = 3. (A) 3D scatter plot showing the position of the elements of each identified cluster (C1, C2, and C3). C1 (blue) contains eight RSNs: LVN, MVN, SMN, AN, RVAN, TPN, PN, and LN. C2 (green) contains four RSNs: DMN, SN, CBLN, and LVAN. The third cluster, C3 (magenta) groups three RSNs: WMN, FCN, and ECN. The axes of the 3D plot represent the three dimensions of the k-means input data: rcTstory, rcTpost1, rcTpost2. (B) Plot of the behavioral trends associated to the three clusters. Each trend has been obtained by averaging the rcT values across scans of the RNSs belonging to the specific cluster. For each trend, the relative standard error has also been reported. C2 cluster has been associated to a “long-lasting” pattern of changes since the mean rcT trend peaks in post1 after the listening task which occurs during story. On the contrary, C3 has been associated to a “short lasting” rcT trend which peaks in story and recovers rapidly during post1, while C1 presents a mixed-trend between those of C2 and C3.
In order to quantitatively inspect the kinetics of FC alterations typical of the three clusters identified by k-means analysis, the mean rcT signal was computed by averaging the rcT values across scans in each cluster (Figure 7B). In C1, the mean rcT trend peaked in story and remained almost at the same level even in post1 before recovering toward the initial value (as in pre) in post2. In C2, the mean rcT trend peaked in post1 and partially recovered its initial value in post2. We called the C2 trend as “long-lasting” pattern of changes. In C3, the mean rcT trend reached the maximal alteration in story and recovered rapidly already during post1, defining therefore what was called a “short-lasting” pattern of changes.
Discussion
This paper shows that the cerebellum is deeply entrained in well-structured RSN clusters, which reflect multiple aspects of cognitive processing during and beyond the conclusion of auditory stimulation. While it is well known that RSNs (Beckmann et al., 2005; Buzsaki, 2006; Sadaghiani et al., 2010; Sporns, 2013) can change their FC in some physiological and pathological conditions (Hasson et al., 2010; Kauppi et al., 2010), this paper further demonstrates that appropriate paradigms can capture RSN changes occurring when brain activity switches from the actual resting state to a continuous cognitive processing (two states that we will call quiescent and engaged) during a naturalistic stimulation, i.e., while listening to a narrated story.
In this study, 15 RSNs were identified (Figure 2), comprising: SMN, LVN, MVN, AN, which are directly implicated in sensory processing; DMN, FCN, ECN, RVAN, LVAN, TPN, PN, SN, LN, WMN, which are associated with higher cognitive functions (Papanicolaou, 2017); and CBLN, which is usually considered a sensorimotor network, but has recently been correlated to cognitive processing too (Stoodley, 2012). In addition to CBLN, 9 out of the 15 RSNs showed clusters in the cerebellum, including both sensory networks (LVN and MVN) and cognitive networks (DMN, ECN, RVAN, LVAN, SN, LN, and WMN). Interestingly, in addition to frontal and occipital cortex, the posterior cerebellum showed amongst the most marked FC changes inside these networks. This observation supports the role of the cerebellum in processing movement and mental manipulation, error/novelty detection, working memory and planning through extended cerebro-cerebellar loops (Palesi et al., 2017).
The switching of brain state from quiescent to engaged and then back to quiescent over a time scale of several minutes, was associated with a marked change in RSNs activity (Figures 3, 7B). Local FC was changed during the story in all the RSNs, consistent with the knowledge that attentive brain activity is associated with BOLD signal changes (Buzsaki, 2006; Huettel et al., 2009; Hasson et al., 2010; Deco et al., 2011; Golkowski et al., 2017). Consistently, several brain areas showed significantly higher FC during the story than before or after it. The visual networks (LVN and MVN) and the cognitive networks (TPN, LVAN, SN, and DMN) were those presenting the largest areas of increased FC (Figures 4, 5). This is not surprising given that MVN (which involves the primary visual cortex, V1), and DMN are involved in mental imagery (Zvyagintsev et al., 2013; Pearson et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2018), while DMN and SN are known to play a key role in attention switching (Seeley et al., 2007; Menon, 2011). There are only far and few articles considering changes during naturalistic stimuli. Some papers reported an FC reduction during naturalistic stimulation (Fransson and Marrelec, 2008; Hasson et al., 2009; Arbabshirani et al., 2013), but a direct comparison is difficult since they used acquisition and analysis protocols very different from ours (e.g., seed-based approaches and connectomics). Other studies comparing FC at rest versus speech listening report indeed strengthened connectivity among the language related brain regions during the naturalistic stimulation (Hampson et al., 2002). Moreover, Shirer et al. (2012), using whole brain connectivity, reports increased FC during memory and subtraction tasks among task-related regions compared to rest condition. A strengthening of connectivity may indeed subtend the requirement for high-level functional integration among distant brain areas during cognitive processing.
This study shows a strong cerebellar involvement in cognitive processing of a narrated story as demonstrated by the characteristic localization of FC changes (Figure 8). None of these areas are localized in the anterior cerebellum, which is related to motor control. The cerebellar changes are all localized in the posterior lateral cerebellum, primarily involving Crus-I, Crus-II, and lobule VI, that have previously been related to cognitive processing (Stoodley, 2012). Looking at Figure 2 it is evident that, these cerebellar areas are part not only of the CBLN, but they are also nodes shared with high-order cognitive networks, such as those processing working memory, attention, internal versus external state switching and language (WMN, VANs, DMN, SN, LN). Specifically, activation in lobule VI could be related to mental rotation or spatial transformations of objects, as observed during fMRI tasks (Vingerhoets et al., 2002; Zacks et al., 2002; Weiss et al., 2009; Stoodley et al., 2010). Activation in Lobule VI and Crus-I could be related to language processing, as demonstrated during reading and lexical decision making tasks (Booth et al., 2007; Carreiras et al., 2007), also in connection with basal ganglia (Booth et al., 2007), and during emotional processing elicited by actions observed in others (Singer et al., 2004; Schulte-Rüther et al., 2007).
FIGURE 8. Bar plots showing for each discussed contrast: (B) story > all, (C) story > pre, (D) story > post1, and (E) story > post2, the localization of FC changes within the 28 areas of the cerebellum identified with the SUIT atlas (A) (Diedrichsen et al., 2009). For each contrast, each bar height corresponds to the percentage of FC alteration observed in the specific cerebellar area (i.e., number of altered voxels divided for the number of voxels of the area mask in the SUIT atlas). Note that for each contrast, the most altered cerebellar areas are those located in the posterior cerebellum.
The dynamic cognitive processing required for elaborating the story is thought to involve multiple operations (Eysenck, 2012) that can be summarized as follows. Semantic content is analyzed to extract the information required to generate, in association with previous memory, an internal representation of objects and scenes (visual mental imagery). This representation has to account for relative movement and involves mental manipulation (spatial transformations and rotation) of the objects in time and space. Working memory is required to bind temporally distant elements, while error/novelty-detection allows the identification of violations of expectation elaborated on the basis of the ongoing information flux. The occurrence of unexpected elements (either “wrong” or novel) determines attention switching and stimulates planning of new internal schemes. At the same time, some learning processes are expected to take place in order to memorize the story and follow its content. How are our results fitting with this overall processing scheme? The network changes that we observed in our study automatically identified, indeed, three clusters of RSNs that we could speculate are associated with three main mental functions: mental imagery, attention switching and working memory (C1, C2, C3 in Figure 7A). Mental imagery can be associated to the cluster (C1) that includes the visual networks MVN and LVN. Both these networks are mainly located in the occipital lobe and include the primary visual cortex (V1) that has been shown to be involved in visual mental imagery (Knauff et al., 2000; Pearson et al., 2015). The C2 cluster including DMN, SN, CBLN, and LVAN can be associated to attention switching, as this action is known to involve SN and DMN (Menon, 2011), as well as to the error/novelty detection task for which the cerebellum, entirely included in CBLN, has been proposed to play a key role (Anderson et al., 2012; Picerni et al., 2013). Finally, FCN, ECN, and WMN, which grouped in cluster C3, can be associated to working memory involvement in the story listening. Indeed, these networks involve primarily the frontal and prefrontal cortices including BA9-10 and BA44-47 areas which are known to be active areas during working memory tasks (Ranganath et al., 2003). Thus, the three main functions that are supposed to involve the cerebellum along with corresponding cerebro-cortical areas during cognitive processing (D‘Angelo and Casali, 2013) are identified in the three RSN clusters.
It is interesting to note that cluster C3, which includes all the RNSs primarily supporting working memory (WMN and FCN) and executive functioning (ECN), showed a short-lasting change overall (Figure 7B). This finding may be interpreted as a further indication that these three RSNs may be actively engaged in the sensorial perception of the story as well as in the cognitive processing of its content through an immediate increase of attentional processing. Conversely, cluster C2, which includes the attentive network LVAN, CBLN as well as DMN and SN, and is involved in the external-internal switching loop of attention, showed a long-lasting RSN change (Figure 7B). We speculate that this long-lasting change supports an emotional and attentional involvement related to the story content that persists after the naturalistic stimulation. Although some indication that this might happen a few seconds after the stimulus was reported in Hasson et al. (2009), our results indicate that this persistence of RSNs FC alterations can last for up to 15 min and involve large scale networks including the cerebellum.
Considering the specific cerebellar areas, visual imagery probably involved lobule VI and Crus-I in MVN as well as in LVN, providing the basis for spatial transformations and mental rotation of objects as well as movement perception and planning (Cattaneo et al., 2014; Ferrari et al., 2018). Language processing in LN was associated to FC change in lobule VI, Crus-I and Crus-II. While the role of cerebellum in visual and language processing is rather well established, somehow more surprising were the remarkable changes occurring in the attentive networks LVAN and RVAN involving lobule VI, Crus-I and Crus-II, implying a fundamental role of these cerebellar areas in attention. These findings allow us to speculate that the cerebellum was actively taking part to attention switching in relation to violations of expectations emerging from visual and semantic representations of the story content, as much as reported for sensorimotor control (Brooks et al., 2015). The active role of cerebellum in switching from the internal to external reference framework during the story can be supported when looking at clustering results of the network behaviors where the CBLN is grouped with DMN and SN, known to play a key role in performing this operation (Menon, 2012).
Methodological Considerations
Since in this study data were acquired using a sequence with relatively long TR (>2 s), the analysis was limited to FC below 0.1 Hz (Damoiseaux et al., 2006). Recent studies have shown that spontaneous BOLD activity may also persist in higher frequency bands (up to 0.8 Hz) (Boubela et al., 2013; Chen and Glover, 2015). It would be interesting to extend our study acquiring rs-fMRI with a multi-band EPI sequence, which would enable the investigation of changes of spontaneous BOLD activity above 0.1 Hz. This would also allow to adopt different analysis approaches, such as sliding window (SW) analysis (Shakil et al., 2016), inter-subject correlation (ISC) analysis (Hasson et al., 2010) and dynamic time warping (DTW) analysis (Meszlényi et al., 2017), that might reveal further time-varying aspects of RSN FC dynamics.
In this study, the naturalistic stimulation was represented by a story enriched with elements that are known to engage working memory and executive functions supported by the cerebro-cerebellar circuits. Here, we aimed to explore overall changes caused by this naturalistic stimulus through the analyses of the RSNs. The study of the contribution of each single element of the story to the FC changes was beyond the purposes of our work as would require a different acquisition strategy for example to allow a dynamical FC analysis. Future studies may be designed to assess individual elements contributions to RSNs FC changes or to assess RSNs FC alterations using different naturalistic stimuli, e.g., visual scenes or less engaging story contents.
Conclusion
The RSNs FC changes detected during continuous cognitive processing, which involved working memory along with attention switching and object mental manipulation, deeply involved the posterior cerebellum. Beyond demonstrating the existence of structural connections between the cerebellum and several cortical structures, this can be considered as clear evidence of the functional engagement of the cerebellum during cognitive processing. The specific role of cerebellum within these networks can be further analyzed considering brain theories that place the cerebro-cerebellar circuits at the core of processes of error detection and sensory prediction (D‘Angelo and Casali, 2013; Ito, 2013). Similarly, one should construct theories to understand how activity in these circuits is perpetuated beyond the story listening potentially to promote interpretation and memorization. Future studies could optimize recovery timings further in the experimental protocol in order to analyze these novel concepts in greater detail. Given the emerging involvement of the cerebellum in several neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders, one could envisage the use of this simple protocol to assess mechanisms of cognitive alterations in pathologies such as Alzheimer’s disease, Multiple Sclerosis and autism, just to name a few (Castellazzi et al., 2014; d’Ambrosio et al., 2017; Olivito et al., 2018).
Author Contributions
GC, CW-K, and ED’A conceptualized the study. GC designed and performed the rs-fMRI analysis. SB designed and wrote the narrated story that was used as naturalistic task in the study. GS and FP helped with the setting of the rs-fMRI sequence on the MRI scanner. CW-K and ED’A provided the support and guidance with data interpretation with the contribution of AT, LC, and GS. GC, CW-K and ED’A wrote the manuscript, with comments from all other authors.
Funding
This work was supported by the United Kingdom MS Society and UCL-UCLH Biomedical Research Centre and the National Institute for Health Research, University College London, United Kingdom. ECTRIMS and the Multiple Sclerosis International Federation (MSIF) supported the work of GC with funding (ECTRIMS Postdoctoral Research Fellowship Program, MSIF Du Pré grant). Further support came from grants of European Union (Human Brain Project; HBP-604102), the Italian Ministry of Health (RF-INM-2008-114341, RF-2009-1475845, and RC2014-2017) to ED’A (NET-2013-02355313) to FP and CW-K (RC2014-2017, MS society 77, WoF, ISRT, CHNF for the INSPIRED Project, H2020-EU.3.1-634541), and of Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) to CW-K (EP/I027084/1); “Ricerca Corrente 2015” to ED’A and CW-K; the University of Pavia to GC and FP.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer GO and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Marios Yiannakas, Dr. Moreno Pasin, Dr. Adnan Alahmadi, Dr. Paolo Vitali, and Mr. Giancarlo Germani for collaboration in data acquisition. We also thank Mrs. Mary Brightman for editing the translation of the story in English and all the participants who accepted to take part in this study.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fncel.2018.00331/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Anderson, S. R., Porrill, J., Pearson, M. J., Pipe, A. G., Prescott, T. J., and Dean, P. (2012). An internal model architecture for novelty detection: implications for cerebellar and collicular roles in sensory processing. PLoS One 7:e44560. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044560
Arbabshirani, M. R., Havlicek, M., Kiehl, K. A., Pearlson, G. D., and Calhoun, V. D. (2013). Functional network connectivity during rest and task conditions: a comparative study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2959–2971. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22118
Arthur, D., and Vassilvitskii, S. (2007). “k-means + + : the advantages of careful seeding,” in Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual ACM-SIAM Symposium on Discrete Algorithms, (New Orleans, LA: Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics), 1027–1035.
Barnes, A., Bullmore, E. T., and Suckling, J. (2009). Endogenous human brain dynamics recover slowly following cognitive effort. PLoS One 4:e6626. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006626
Beckmann, C. F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J. T., and Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1001–1013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634
Berns, G. S., Blaine, K., Prietula, M. J., and Pye, B. E. (2013). Short- and long-term effects of a novel on connectivity in the brain. Brain Connect. 3, 590–600. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0166
Biswal, B. B., Van Kylen, J., and Hyde, J. S. (1997). Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps. NMR Biomed. 10, 165–170. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1492(199706/08)10:4/5<165::AID-NBM454>3.0.CO;2-7
Booth, J. R., Wood, L., Lu, D., Houk, J. C., and Bitan, T. (2007). The role of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in language processing. Brain Res. 1133, 136–144. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.074
Boubela, R. N., Kalcher, K., Huf, W., Kronnerwetter, C., Filzmoser, P., and Moser, E. (2013). Beyond Noise: using Temporal ICA to extract meaningful information from high-frequency fMRI signal fluctuations during rest. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:168. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00168
Brooks, J. X., Carriot, J., and Cullen, K. E. (2015). Learning to expect the unexpected: rapid updating in primate cerebellum during voluntary self-motion. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1310–1317. doi: 10.1038/nn.4077
Buckner, R. L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80, 807–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044
Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301069.001.0001
Carreiras, M., Mechelli, A., Estévez, A., and Price, C. J. (2007). Brain activation for lexical decision and reading aloud: two sides of the same coin? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 433–444. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.433
Castellazzi, G., Palesi, F., Casali, S., Vitali, P., Sinforiani, E., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., et al. (2014). A comprehensive assessment of resting state networks: bidirectional modification of functional integrity in cerebro-cerebellar networks in dementia. Front. Neurosci. 8:223. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00223
Cattaneo, Z., Renzi, C., Casali, S., Silvanto, J., Vecchi, T., Papagno, C., et al. (2014). Cerebellar vermis plays a causal role in visual motion discrimination. Cortex 58, 272–280. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.012
Chen, J. E., and Glover, G. H. (2015). BOLD fractional contribution to resting-state functional connectivity above 0.1 Hz. Neuroimage 107, 207–218. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.012
d’Ambrosio, A., Hidalgo de la Cruz, M., Valsasina, P., Pagani, E., Colombo, B., Rodegher, M., et al. (2017). Structural connectivity-defined thalamic subregions have different functional connectivity abnormalities in multiple sclerosis patients: implications for clinical correlations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 6005–6018. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23805
Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103
Deco, G., Jirsa, V. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2011). Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961
Diedrichsen, J., Balsters, J. H., Flavell, J., Cussans, E., and Ramnani, N. (2009). A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 46, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045
Duda, R. Q., Hart, P. E., and Stork, D. G. (2000). Pattern Classification. New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience, 680.
D‘Angelo, E., and Casali, S. (2013). Seeking a unified framework for cerebellar function and dysfunction: from circuit operations to cognition. Front. Neural Circuits 6:116. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00116
Elton, A., and Gao, W. (2015). Task-related modulation of functional connectivity variability and its behavioral correlations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3260–3272. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22847
Ferrari, C., Oldrati, V., Gallucci, M., Vecchi, T., and Cattaneo, Z. (2018). The role of the cerebellum in explicit and incidental processing of facial emotional expressions: a study with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 169, 256–264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.026
Filippini, N., MacIntosh, B. J., Hough, M. G., Goodwin, G. M., Frisoni, G. B., Smith, S. M., et al. (2009). Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 7209–7214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106
Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201
Fransson, P., and Marrelec, G. (2008). The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. Neuroimage 42, 1178–1184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.059
Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 1, 13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008
Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Liddle, P. F., and Frackowiak, R. S. (1993). Functional connectivity: the principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 13, 5–14. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1993.4
Golkowski, D., Ranft, A., Kiel, T., Riedl, V., Kohl, P., Rohrer, G., et al. (2017). Coherence of BOLD signal and electrical activity in the human brain during deep sevoflurane anesthesia. Brain Behav. 7:e00679. doi: 10.1002/brb3.679
Gordon, E. M., Breeden, A. L., Bean, S. E., and Vaidya, C. J. (2014). Working memory-related changes in functional connectivity persist beyond task disengagement. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1004–1017. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22230
Hampson, M., Peterson, B. S., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J. C., and Gore, J. C. (2002). Detection of functional connectivity using temporal correlations in MR images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 247–262. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10022
Hansen, E. C., Battaglia, D., Spiegler, A., Deco, G., and Jirsa, V. K. (2015). Functional connectivity dynamics: modeling the switching behavior of the resting state. Neuroimage 105, 525–535. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.001
Hasson, U., Malach, R., and Heeger, D. J. (2010). Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011
Hasson, U., Nusbaum, H. C., and Small, S. L. (2009). Task-dependent organization of brain regions active during rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 10841–10846. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903253106
Huettel, S., Song, A., and McCarthy, G. (2009). Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Sunderland, MA: FMRI.
Hutchison, R. M., Womelsdorf, T., Allen, E. A., Bandettini, P. A., Calhoun, V. D., Corbetta, M., et al. (2013). Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage 80, 360–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079
Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 304–313. doi: 10.1038/nrn2332
Ito, M. (2013). Error detection and representation in the olivo-cerebellar system. Front. Neural Circuits 7:1. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00001
Kaufman, L., and Rousseeuw, P. J. (1990). Finding Groups in Data: An Introduction to Cluster Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi: 10.1002/9780470316801
Kauppi, J. P., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Sams, M., and Tohka, J. (2010). Inter-subject correlation of brain hemodynamic responses during watching a movie: localization in space and frequency. Front. Neuroinform. 4:5. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2010.00005
Knauff, M., Kassubek, J., Mulack, T., and Greenlee, M. W. (2000). Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery as measured by fMRI. Neuroreport 11, 3957–3962. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200012180-00011
Lee, H. L., Zahneisen, B., Hugger, T., LeVan, P., and Hennig, J. (2013). Tracking dynamic resting-state networks at higher frequencies using MR-encephalography. Neuroimage 65, 216–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.015
Mackey, A. P., Miller Singley, A. T., and Bunge, S. A. (2013). Intensive reasoning training alters patterns of brain connectivity at rest. J. Neurosci. 33, 4796–4803. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4141-12.2013
Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
Menon, V. (2012). Functional Connectivity, Neurocognitive Networks and Brain Dynamics Principles of Brain Dynamics: Global State Interactions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Meszlényi, R. J., Hermann, P., Buza, K., Gál, V., and Vidnyánszky, Z. (2017). Resting State fMRI functional connectivity analysis using dynamic time warping. Front. Neurosci. 11:75. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00075
O’Connell, M. A., and Basak, C. (2018). Effects of task complexity and age-differences on task-related functional connectivity of attentional networks. Neuropsychologia 114, 50–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.013
Olivito, G., Lupo, M., Laghi, F., Clausi, S., Baiocco, R., Cercignani, M., et al. (2018). Lobular patterns of cerebellar resting-state connectivity in adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Eur. J. Neurosci. 47, 729–735. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13752
Palesi, F., De Rinaldis, A., Castellazzi, G., Calamante, F., Muhlert, N., and Chard, D. (2017). Contralateral cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathways reconstruction in humans in vivo: implications for reciprocal cerebro-cerebellar structural connectivity in motor and non-motor areas. Sci. Rep. 7:12841. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13079-8
Palesi, F., Tournier, J. D., Calamante, F., Muhlert, N., Castellazzi, G., and Chard, D. (2015). Contralateral cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathways with prominent involvement of associative areas in humans in vivo. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 3369–3384. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0861-2
Papanicolaou, A. C. (2017). The Oxford Handbook of Functional Brain Imaging in Neuropsychology and Cognitive Neurosciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental imagery: functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 590–602. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.003
Pezoulas, V. C., Zervakis, M., Michelogiannis, S., and Klados, M. A. (2017). Resting-State functional connectivity and network analysis of cerebellum with respect to crystallized IQ and gender. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:189. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00189
Picerni, E., Petrosini, L., Piras, F., Laricchiuta, D., Cutuli, D., Chiapponi, C., et al. (2013). New evidence for the cerebellar involvement in personality traits. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:133. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00133
Ranganath, C., Johnson, M. K., and D’Esposito, M. (2003). Prefrontal activity associated with working memory and episodic long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 41, 378–389. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00169-0
Sadaghiani, S., Hesselmann, G., Friston, K. J., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2010). The relation of ongoing brain activity, evoked neural responses, and cognition. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:20. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00020
Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Douaud, G., Beckmann, C. F., Glasser, M. F., Griffanti, L., and Smith, S. M. (2014). Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. Neuroimage 90, 449–468. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046
Schmahmann, J. D., and Caplan, D. (2006). Cognition, emotion and the cerebellum. Brain 129, 290–292. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh729
Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., and Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging approach to empathy. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1354–1372. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1354
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007
Shakil, S., Lee, C. H., and Keilholz, S. D. (2016). Evaluation of sliding window correlation performance for characterizing dynamic functional connectivity and brain states. Neuroimage 133, 111–128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.074
Shirer, W. R., Ryali, S., Rykhlevskaia, E., Menon, V., and Greicius, M. D. (2012). Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb. Cortex 22, 158–165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr099
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535
Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106
Sokolov, A. (2018). The cerebellum in social cognition. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:145. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00145
Spadone, S., Della Penna, S., Sestieri, C., Betti, V., Tosoni, A., Perrucci, M. G., et al. (2015). Dynamic reorganization of human resting-state networks during visuospatial attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 8112–8117. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1415439112
Sporns, O. (2013). Structure and function of complex brain networks. Dialog. Clin. Neurosci. 15, 247–262.
Stoodley, C. J. (2012). The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum 11, 352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7
Stoodley, C. J., Valera, E. M., and Schmahmann, J. D. (2010). An fMRI study of intra-individual functional topography in the human cerebellum. Behav. Neurol. 23, 65–79. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2010-0268
van den Heuvel, M. P., and Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 519–534. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008
Vingerhoets, G., de Lange, F. P., Vandemaele, P., Deblaere, K., and Achten, E. (2002). Motor imagery in mental rotation: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 17, 1623–1633. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1290
Weiss, M. M., Wolbers, T., Peller, M., Witt, K., Marshall, L., Buchel, C., et al. (2009). Rotated alphanumeric characters do not automatically activate frontoparietal areas subserving mental rotation. Neuroimage 44, 1063–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.042
Winkler, A. M., Ridgway, G. R., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2014). Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92, 381–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060
Yuste, R., and Fairhall, A. L. (2015). Temporal dynamics in fMRI resting-state activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 5263–5264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1505898112
Zacks, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., Sheridan, M. A., and Tversky, B. (2002). A parametric study of mental spatial transformations of bodies. Neuroimage 16, 857–872. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1129
Zhang, Z., Zhang, D., Wang, Z., Li, J., Lin, Y., Chang, S., et al. (2018). Intrinsic neural linkage between primary visual area and default mode network in human brain: evidence from visual mental imagery. Neuroscience 379, 13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.02.033
Keywords: cerebellum, resting state fMRI, functional connectivity, resting state networks, cognition
Citation: Castellazzi G, Bruno SD, Toosy AT, Casiraghi L, Palesi F, Savini G, D’Angelo E and Wheeler-Kingshott CAMG (2018) Prominent Changes in Cerebro-Cerebellar Functional Connectivity During Continuous Cognitive Processing. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:331. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00331
Received: 06 June 2018; Accepted: 10 September 2018;
Published: 01 October 2018.
Edited by:
Marco Bozzali, Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS), ItalyReviewed by:
Giusy Olivito, Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS), ItalyAlberto Benussi, Università degli Studi di Brescia, Italy
Copyright © 2018 Castellazzi, Bruno, Toosy, Casiraghi, Palesi, Savini, D’Angelo and Wheeler-Kingshott. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Gloria Castellazzi, gloria.castellazzi@unipv.it
†These authors have contributed equally to this work