- Anna Freud, and Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Researchers interested in the effects of early experiences of caregiving adversity have employed neuroscientific methods to illuminate whether and how such environmental input impacts on brain development, and whether and how such impacts underpin poor socioemotional outcomes in this population. Evidence is compelling in documenting negative effects on the individual’s neurodevelopment following exposure to adverse or disadvantaged environments such as institutionalization or maltreatment. Neuroimaging research focused specifically on attachment-relevant processing of socioemotional stimuli and attachment outcomes among children looked-after is scarcer, but largely consistent. This review begins by summarizing the key general brain structural and functional alterations associated with caregiving deprivation. Then, neuroscientific evidence that is more directly relevant for understanding these children’s attachment outcomes, both by employing social stimuli and by correlating children’s neural markers with their attachment profiles, is reviewed. Brief interpretations of findings are suggested, and key limitations and gaps in the literature identified.
Introduction
Literature on the neurodevelopment of children currently or previously looked-after aims to understand whether and how such a significant experience in early caregiving is associated with measurable effects on the brain. This typically includes children looked-after in institutions (or orphanages) and foster care. Beyond the experience of disruption in their care and separation from previous caregivers, children looked-after have frequently been exposed to other adverse experiences which led to the removal from the care of their families in the first place, such as abuse and neglect. Being looked-after is consistently associated with poorer developmental outcomes in the socioemotional domain, including attachment behavior (e.g., Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2011).
Evidence compellingly shows that early exposure to adverse or disadvantaged rearing environments, including institutionalization and maltreatment, is associated with negative effects on the individual’s neurodevelopment (e.g., Fox et al., 2010). A persuasive assumption to explain this link is that failure of neglectful caregiving settings to provide many elements of an “expectable” rearing environment—such as access to a consistent caregiver and appropriate cognitive and emotional stimulation—can lead to neural alterations, possibly in the process of specification of neural circuitry and the pruning of neurons and synapses as well as alterations in other neurobiological systems (e.g., neuroendocrine) that are dependent on experience (Greenough et al., 1987; Nelson, 2007; Nelson et al., 2011; Pechtel and Pizzagalli, 2011).
Understanding the precise neural changes associated with exposure to these forms of environmental deprivation is a critical scientific goal, not only for improving our understanding of brain development, but also in advancing our capacity to effectively prevent and treat their psychological sequelae. Of these negative effects following adverse early caregiving experiences, difficulties in the socioemotional domain seem to be particularly persistent even after children are placed in more positive environments (e.g., Hodges and Tizard, 1989; Rutter et al., 2007; Kreppner et al., 2010).
This review aims to briefly synthesize what neural changes have been associated to growing up in out-of-home care and the contribution of such developmental differences to explaining these children’s poorer attachment outcomes. Yet, neuroimaging work specifically focused on attachment in children looked-after is scarce. Accordingly, it will be important to first review general effects of being looked-after on brain development, as well as neural correlates of attachment-relevant socioemotional processing, to contextualize the findings from the fewer studies measuring attachment outcomes in this population.
Brain development and general effects
Neuroscientific studies with children placed in out of home care have documented differences in brain structure (i.e., anatomy) as well as function, when compared to peers continuously raised by their families.
Studies of brain anatomy, using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), show that institutionally reared children have smaller volumes of both white and gray matter (Mehta et al., 2009; Sheridan et al., 2012), and altered structural integrity of white matter (Bick et al., 2015), when compared to home-reared counterparts. One of these studies is from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project (BEIP) with children living in Romanian institutions (which at the time were especially impoverished) who were randomly assigned, between 7 and 33 months of age, to remain at the institution or move to high-quality foster homes. While both groups with institutionalization experience, i.e., children who remained institutionalized and those who got moved to foster care, showed a similar reduction in gray matter volume, those who moved to foster placements showed an encouraging recovery in white matter volume and integrity (Sheridan et al., 2012). Parallels can be established with research assessing children exposed to maltreatment (compared to non-maltreated), where a similar reduction in cortical gray and white matter volumes has been reported, with important differences remaining even after controlling for their overall smaller brain size (De Bellis et al., 1999, 2002).
Despite these findings of overall differences in cortical volume, such differences do not seem to be distributed evenly across the brain, nor consistently so throughout development. Researchers have looked at specific regions of interest, particularly those known to be highly sensitive to the influence of early stress. Many studies have documented regional differences between children exposed to early caregiving adversity (mainly maltreatment, but also institutional rearing) and controls, with effects predominantly reported in the emotion regulation circuitry. This circuitry crucially involves areas of the prefrontal cortex (which has an executive role in higher-order functions such as cognitive control and emotional regulation) and the amygdala (a limbic structure key in emotion processing), as well as connectivity between them.
Differences have been found in both regions, among children looked-after. Specifically, reduced gray matter volume, blood flow and cortical thickness in some prefrontal areas, including the orbitofrontal cortex, have been reported in institutionally reared children (Chugani et al., 2001; McLaughlin et al., 2014). Similar reductions have been reported in children exposed to maltreatment (Hanson et al., 2010; De Brito et al., 2013; Fujisawa et al., 2019).
A great number of studies has focused on the amygdala, because it is critically involved in emotional processing and threat detection and is also very susceptible to the early environment. Yet, findings are not straightforward to interpret. While increased amygdala volume has been documented in samples who were maltreated or institutionally reared earlier in life, usually assessed during childhood, the opposite finding of reduced volume has been reported in studies with adolescents and adults (Mehta et al., 2009; Tottenham and Sheridan, 2009; Tottenham et al., 2010). This pattern of results suggests effects of timing of exposure and of assessment, whereby an initial stress-induced hypertrophy and hyperactivity of amygdala neurons eventually lead to neuronal atrophy or cell death by adulthood (Tottenham and Sheridan, 2009; Teicher and Samson, 2016). This account of amygdala development trajectory in the context of early caregiving adversity received support from a longitudinal study with post-institutionalized youth internationally adopted into the United States – where larger volumes were seen before 6.5 years of age, but smaller volumes from 11 onwards (VanTieghem et al., 2021). Beyond differences in volume, there are also indications of alterations in network connectivity involving the amygdala, among children who experienced early adversity. Notably, evidence suggests that type of adversity, namely abuse or neglect, is associated with alterations in the connectivity with different clusters of regions (Cheng et al., 2021).
Focusing now on white matter, differences have been documented in several fiber tracts among children exposed to early caregiving adversity. Looking at the corpus callosum as an example (given its crucial role as the largest white matter tract in the brain and its involvement in interhemispheric communication) evidence with children looked-after consistently shows a reduced volume and integrity of this structure (Sheridan et al., 2012; Bick et al., 2015). Similar findings have been documented in children who experienced maltreatment, including neglect (De Bellis et al., 1999, 2002; Teicher et al., 2004). As seen earlier regarding recovery in white matter more generally, findings from the BEIP suggest some capacity for recovery in corpus callosum volume for children randomized to high-quality foster care, when compared to the group of children who remained institutionalized (Sheridan et al., 2012).
Thus far I have summarized (s/f)MRI findings emerging from the literature with children looked-after. Remarkably consistent with such evidence is that obtained from electrophysiological research (i.e., using electroencephalography, or EEG, which records electrical activity along the scalp, and the related technique of event-related potentials, or ERPs, which are derived from time-locked EEG responses to discrete presentations of stimuli). Indeed, institutionally reared children have been found to show an overall reduced EEG power and reduced brain activation during EEG recordings.
The term ‘reduced power’ is used to describe the finding that institutionalized children show higher power in low-frequency (theta) and lower power in mid- to high-frequency (alpha and beta) bands, compared to never-institutionalized controls. This result was obtained in two different samples (Marshall et al., 2004; Vanderwert et al., 2010; Tarullo et al., 2011; Debnath et al., 2020), spanning a long period of development from the toddler years up until 16 years of age. This atypical concentration of power in lower frequencies has been interpreted to signify neural hypoactivation in children raised in institutions, which has been hypothesized to result from lack of stimulation from responsive caregivers during a sensitive period of development (Marshall et al., 2004; Tarullo et al., 2011). Findings from the BEIP provide further insight into this issue. Specifically, the association between institutional care and reduced EEG alpha-power in that sample could be partly explained by the mediating role of reduced cortical white matter volume in the children who remained institutionalized (Sheridan et al., 2012). Encouragingly, benefits from placing children in high-quality foster care before age 2 saw these children’s alpha power at age 8 to be comparable to that of never-institutionalized children. These findings suggest that institutional rearing has an impact in shaping brain anatomy in ways that alter neural activity, but also that intervening early by placing children in high-quality foster-care may be able to mitigate, to some extent, the deleterious effects of deprivation. Consistent with this result is the effect of an attachment-based intervention for families classified as at risk for maltreatment, where children in those families who received the intervention presented increased high frequency power years later (Bick et al., 2019).
In addition to the association between adverse early caregiving and an overall reduced EEG power, atypical patterns of hemispheric asymmetry have been documented in this population. Hemispheric asymmetry is a relevant index of brain function to test in this context, because there is literature associating right-hemisphere dominance with withdrawal behavior and negative emotionality (Davidson, 1992). The most compelling evidence from children exposed to caregiving adversity comes from the BEIP. In that sample, institutionalized children developed an atypical trajectory of hemispheric asymmetry, with a prolonged period of increased right hemisphere activation (until 42 months of age) and a blunted rebound in left frontal activation, meaning that by 8 years of age they had greater activity in the right than the left hemisphere, when compared to never-institutionalized children (and those who were placed in foster-care at earlier ages). Consistent with this result among the Romanian children, a right alpha power asymmetry has also been reported in two American samples, one of toddlers in foster care and the other of maltreated school-aged children living with their families (Curtis and Cicchetti, 2007; Blaisdell et al., 2020).
In summary, neuroimaging findings from children exposed to institutional rearing or maltreatment present a picture of reduced volume in stress-sensitive brain structures and reduced brain electrical activity, as well as an altered trajectory of hemispheric asymmetry, which seem to result from their negative early caregiving experiences. It has been hypothesized that these effects develop from the lack of expected environmental input which the nervous system requires for typical development to unfold. Nevertheless, evidence points to the crucial intervention potential of improving caregiving to alter children’s developmental trajectories. What these studies cannot tell us is whether such differences in brain structure and function translate to children’s social phenotypes, and why some children with these adverse experiences show attachment difficulties while others do not. To examine this issue, research on socioemotional processing and on attachment behavior correlates is required, and it is reviewed next.
Neuroimaging during processing of socioemotional stimuli
Both fMRI and ERP methods have been used to explore whether there are differences in how children with experience of adverse early care process attachment-relevant social cues, specifically faces as highly salient and relevant stimuli.
fMRI work with this population has substantially focused on regions and circuits critically involved in two types of processing: reward and threat. In general, a reduced activation in circuits related to reward (Mehta et al., 2010; Goff et al., 2013) and increased for threat processing have been found. Here we focus on the latter, as the paradigm of choice has been to present participants with face stimuli.
Research on threat detection has found increased amygdala activation in response to emotional faces in children adopted from care (Maheu et al., 2010; Tottenham et al., 2011), in line with the more abundant evidence from individuals exposed to child maltreatment (McCrory et al., 2011; Jenness et al., 2021). Unsurprisingly, this increased amygdala activation is accompanied by decreased activation in regions supporting cognitive control (Tottenham et al., 2011; Jenness et al., 2021). Increased amygdala reactivity has been hypothesized to serve an adaptive function in maltreated individuals, in allowing enhanced capacity for detecting threatening stimuli (McLaughlin et al., 2016), even if it may lead to later difficulties, including psychopathology (Jenness et al., 2021).
ERP studies of children looked-after while they view faces have investigated processing of face familiarity and processing of emotional expressions. To investigate the former, researchers have presented to children pictures of their caregiver and pictures of a stranger while recording their neural responses. Consistently across different samples exposed to varying levels of caregiving deprivation (including the Romanian children from the BEIP, children in institutional care in Portugal, and children in foster care in Germany), looked-after toddlers and pre-schoolers showed reduced ERP amplitudes in components involved in face processing, when compared to home-reared controls (Parker et al., 2005a; Moulson et al., 2009b; Kungl et al., 2017; Oliveira et al., 2023). These components include the P1, which indexes early and low-level feature processing of visual stimuli, and the N170, which occurs after the P1 and is a marker of more elaborate face-sensitive perceptual processes. Nevertheless, all groups of children were able to discriminate, at the neural level, the caregiver from the stranger’s face, indicating a preservation of this ability even in those exposed to multiple rotating caregivers (Parker et al., 2005a; Moulson et al., 2009b; Oliveira et al., 2023).
Effects of variation in the level of caregiver deprivation or lack of individualized care were seen in both the BEIP and the Portuguese samples. Specifically, for children in the BEIP who were randomized to foster care following institutionalization, amplitudes in the P1 were intermediate between those who remained institutionalized and those who were continuously raised by their birth families, suggesting positive effects of the improvement in caregiving. In the Portuguese institutionalized group, smaller amplitudes in the P400 component were observed in those living in placements with a poorer ratio of children-to-caregivers (i.e., more children per adult), highlighting the importance of consistent and individualized caregiving.
Children from the BEIP also completed ERP tasks of discrimination and recognition of faces posing different emotional expressions (Parker et al., 2005b; Moulson et al., 2009a; Nelson et al., 2013). Those who were institutionalized showed differences in patterns of processing of facial emotion when they were infants/toddlers, and again at age eight, but not during the preschool years. In the preschool assessment the task required simple emotion discrimination, whereas at age eight the task involved emotion recognition and was more demanding, perhaps justifying the discrepant results. Nevertheless, it was not clear which emotions were more affected by institutional rearing, as in the early years the main differences were in regard to sad and fearful emotions, while at age eight they were seen mostly for anger. Lastly, deficits in emotion recognition appeared to be remediable, given that subtle improvements in processing facial emotion were observed among the group moving from the institution to high-quality foster care.
In summary, blunted neural responses have been observed in children exposed to different levels of caregiver deprivation, with both institutional and foster care associated with some alterations in face familiarity processing. This indicates that the signs of reduced brain electrical activity described earlier apply specifically to the processing of stimuli that are highly relevant to attachment, that is the faces of caregivers (vs. other adults). The impact of care experience on the processing of emotional faces has been demonstrated in terms of increased amygdala activation to threatening stimuli, but electrophysiological responses to emotional faces requires further investigation.
Neuroimaging and attachment phenotypes
Neuroimaging methods have the potential to contribute to our understanding of the increased rates of socioemotional and attachment difficulties observed in children who have been exposed to caregiving adversity. In this section we summarize neuroimaging findings on attachment security and the more extreme forms of attachment problems, namely those captured by the labels of Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD) and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder (DSED) in children looked-after.
First focusing on attachment quality, two studies have linked neural correlates with attachment security among children in care. The first was another piece of work from the BEIP (Almas et al., 2012), which reported that alpha power at 8 years of age moderated the relation between children’s attachment security to the primary caregiver (at 42 months) and later social skills, among all children who experienced institutionalization. Specifically, only for children with higher alpha power, did a greater attachment security significantly predict better social skills. The second study, also cited already, assessed children in foster care in Germany (Kungl et al., 2017). In addition to the group differences we have seen for ERP amplitudes, researchers also found that variation in attachment security played a role in children’s face-familiarity processing, with insecurely attached children showing a reduced N170 component compared to secure ones. Even if preliminary due to the small sample size, these results may be interpreted as increased face expertise in secure children, beyond the experience of foster care, which we may speculate has been allowed by contingent social interactions within an adequately stimulating social environment provided by their caregiver (Kungl et al., 2017).
Another line of work has focused on more extreme forms of attachment difficulties which are intimately associated with experiences of early caregiving neglect, namely symptoms of RAD and of DSED.
A few different teams have employed neuroimaging tools to study this topic. An EEG study with post-institutionalized adopted toddlers found that their pattern of EEG power distribution (described earlier), namely a concentration in lower frequencies, predicted their display of socially disinhibited behavior later on at 36 months of age (Tarullo et al., 2011). Another team used fMRI with post-institutionalized adopted youth, who showed reduced differentiation in amygdala activation between the mother’s and a stranger’s face (compared to controls), which correlated with their disinhibited behavior; also, these effects were particularly prominent in children who were adopted later in life, suggesting effects of timing of removal from institutional care (Olsavsky et al., 2013). Consistent with results from both these studies were obtained from ERPs with a relatively large sample of Portuguese institutionalized pre-schoolers (Oliveira et al., 2023). Among these children who were currently living in institutional care, higher levels of DSED symptoms were associated with: (a) smaller P1 amplitudes (for both caregiver and stranger’s faces) and (b) smaller P400 differences in amplitude to each face, as well as a smaller P400 amplitude in response to the stranger’s face specifically—even after controlling for chronological and mental age.
In contrast to what was found for DSED, RAD symptoms did not predict any neural responses in the ERP components assessed in the Portuguese institutionalized pre-schoolers (Oliveira et al., 2023). However, a diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study of previously maltreated youth with a RAD diagnosis living in a child welfare facility in Japan did show alterations to white matter microstructural integrity (Makita et al., 2020). Specifically, these youth showed alterations in the structure of the corpus callosum and pathways that are important for emotion regulation—alterations that, as we have seen, are associated with being looked-after more generally. The same team also reported reduced striatum activity in youth with RAD, during a monetary reward task (Takiguchi et al., 2015), therefore extending previous literature of the general impact of maltreatment and institutional rearing on the reward circuit.
To conclude, brain alterations observed in institutionally reared children, such as reduced EEG power and blunted ERP components, as well as alterations in white matter integrity, are associated with poor attachment outcomes, contributing to our understanding of difficulties that many children display following caregiving adversity. Such findings also provide preliminary insight into the underlying contributors explaining the heterogeneity in socioemotional functioning in this population, with a clear implication of the generic brain differences reviewed earlier for this specific phenotypic domain. Finally, the establishment of secure attachment relationships shows a preliminary buffering effect against the negative impact of adversity on these neural systems.
Conclusion
This review provides a brief, and necessarily limited overview of the neuroimaging literature that is relevant for our understanding of the socioemotional development and, specifically, attachment outcomes in children looked-after. Evidence indicates widespread neural alterations in both brain structure and function among children who have experienced early caregiving adversity. Such alterations may be speculated to result from a protective adaptation to their adverse circumstances and/or damage to developing systems that are very sensitive to insults from the environment. These alterations are likely to partly underlie these children’s increased rates of socioemotional difficulties, including those captured by the DSED label, which tend to persist in some cases even after improvement in care (Zeanah et al., 2003). There are, nevertheless, encouraging examples of children’s capacity for recovery and resilience, and the positive impact of improved caregiving.
Our ability to generalize from these findings, particularly regarding longitudinal trajectories of brain structure and function, is limited by the fact that the studies included here span across different time points and developmental stages, and that many findings come from one single extreme setting (the BEIP). Another significant limitation of this work is the confounding overlap and lack of differentiation between different experiences of adversity. We know that often these children are exposed to different combinations of maltreatment, relationship disruptions and suboptimal caregiving, but most studies were not able to account for this.
Despite this limitation, all children in the studies included herein have been exposed to major adversity in the relationships with those that are supposed to nurture and protect them at a key period in their development. Indeed, the different types of caregiving adversity have in common well documented socioemotional and neurodevelopmental effects (Smith and Pollak, 2021). However, research linking early caregiving adversity, brain mediators and attachment outcomes is still scarce. While existing evidence supports the view that it is social neglect from being reared in unstable and unresponsive caregiving contexts what seems to be the key mechanism underlying both brain and attachment effects (beyond global deprivation or lack of cognitive stimulation; Tarullo et al., 2011; Bick et al., 2019; Blaisdell et al., 2020; Oliveira et al., 2023), more research is needed to verify this assumption, as well as to better understand the environment-brain-attachment links. Presently, any mechanistic interpretations are tentative.
Across development, and critically in the first few years of life, both experience-expectant and experience-dependent processes drive the growth and organization of the nervous system, with cascading effects of these dynamic processes on behavior (Greenough et al., 1987; Marshall and Kenney, 2009; Rutter, 2012). Environmental insults from neglectful or otherwise adverse caregiving experiences might contribute to alterations in neural pruning, axonal organization and myelination that explain the effects summarized in this review (e.g., Nelson, 2007; Pechtel and Pizzagalli, 2011; Bick et al., 2019). Specifically, it is plausible that lack of contingent, responsive stimulation from nurturing caregivers leads to over-pruning of cortical gray matter and reductions in white matter or myelination (e.g., Sheridan et al., 2012; Bick et al., 2015, 2019). In addition, such brain alterations are likely to have a complex impact on behavior, with effects on basic skills such as inhibitory control and attention regulation likely contributing to the resulting socioemotional phenotypes, including DSED (Tarullo et al., 2011).
However, clear differences also exist in the adverse outcomes associated with abuse or neglect, and important differences in effects from institutional or foster care rearing as well as variation in quality within these, beyond their social address (Soares et al., 2014; Cassiers et al., 2018). Considering these nuances, agglomeration of findings and direct comparisons between samples will always be tentative until further rigorous research is available. Notwithstanding such limitations, current understanding allows us to highlight the importance of contingent, responsive caregiving for children’s neurodevelopment, and the urgency of providing timely intervention to this vulnerable group.
Author contributions
PO: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Almas, A. N., Degnan, K. A., Radulescu, A., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Fox, N. A. (2012). Effects of early intervention and the moderating effects of brain activity on institutionalized children’s social skills at age 8. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 17228–17231. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121256109
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Steele, H., Zeanah, C. H., Muhamedrahimov, R. J., Vorria, P., Dobrova-Krol, N. A., et al. (2011). Attachment and emotional development in institutional care: characteristics and catch up. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 76, 62–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00628.x
Bick, J., Palmwood, E. N., Zajac, L., Simons, R., and Dozier, M. (2019). Early parenting intervention and adverse family environments affect neural function in middle childhood. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 326–335. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.020
Bick, J., Zhu, T., Stamoulis, C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C., and Nelson, C. A. (2015). Effect of early institutionalization and foster care on long-term white matter development: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 169, 211–219. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3212
Blaisdell, K. N., Barker, T. V., Giuliano, R. J., and Fisher, P. A. (2020). Alpha electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry among toddlers in foster care. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 1743–1753. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420001212
Cassiers, L. L. M., Sabbe, B. G. C., Schmaal, L., Veltman, D. J., Penninx, B. W. J. H., and Van Den Eede, F. (2018). Structural and functional brain abnormalities associated with exposure to different childhood trauma subtypes: a systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Front. Psych. 9:329. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00329
Cheng, T. W., Mills, K. L., Miranda Dominguez, O., Zeithamova, D., Perrone, A., Sturgeon, D., et al. (2021). Characterizing the impact of adversity, abuse, and neglect on adolescent amygdala resting-state functional connectivity. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 47:100894. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100894
Chugani, H. T., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Juhász, C., Nagy, F., and Chugani, D. C. (2001). Local brain functional activity following early deprivation: a study of Postinstitutionalized Romanian orphans. Neuroimage 14, 1290–1301. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0917
Curtis, W. J., and Cicchetti, D. (2007). Emotion and resilience: a multilevel investigation of hemispheric electroencephalogram asymmetry and emotion regulation in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 811–840. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000405
Davidson, R. J. (1992). Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain Cogn. 20, 125–151. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90065-t
De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Clark, D. B., Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., Boring, A. M., et al. (1999). Developmental traumatology part II: brain development. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 1271–1284. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00045-1
De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Shifflett, H., Iyengar, S., Beers, S. R., Hall, J., et al. (2002). Brain structures in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a sociodemographically matched study. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 1066–1078. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01459-2
De Brito, S. A., Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Kelly, P. A., Mechelli, A., Maris, H., et al. (2013). Reduced orbitofrontal and temporal grey matter in a community sample of maltreated children: reduced in maltreated children grey matter. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 105–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02597.x
Debnath, R., Tang, A., Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., and Fox, N. A. (2020). The long-term effects of institutional rearing, foster care intervention and disruptions in care on brain electrical activity in adolescence. Dev. Sci. 23:e12872. doi: 10.1111/desc.12872
Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., and Nelson, C. A. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Dev. 81, 28–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x
Fujisawa, T. X., Nishitani, S., Takiguchi, S., Shimada, K., Smith, A. K., and Tomoda, A. (2019). Oxytocin receptor DNA methylation and alterations of brain volumes in maltreated children. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 2045–2053. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0414-8
Goff, B., Gee, D. G., Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Gabard-Durnam, L., Flannery, J., et al. (2013). Reduced nucleus accumbens reactivity and adolescent depression following early-life stress. Neuroscience 249, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.010
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., and Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Dev. 58, 539–559. doi: 10.2307/1130197
Hanson, J. L., Chung, M. K., Avants, B. B., Shirtcliff, E. A., Gee, J. C., Davidson, R. J., et al. (2010). Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: a tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. J. Neurosci. 30, 7466–7472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0859-10.2010
Hodges, J., and Tizard, B. (1989). Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 30, 77–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00770.x
Jenness, J. L., Peverill, M., Miller, A. B., Heleniak, C., Robertson, M. M., Sambrook, K. A., et al. (2021). Alterations in neural circuits underlying emotion regulation following child maltreatment: a mechanism underlying trauma-related psychopathology. Psychol. Med. 51, 1880–1889. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000641
Kreppner, J., Kumsta, R., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Stevens, S., et al. (2010). IV. Developmental course of deprivation-specific psychological patterns: early manifestations, persistence to age 15, and clinical features. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 75, 79–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2010.00551.x
Kungl, M. T., Bovenschen, I., and Spangler, G. (2017). Early adverse caregiving experiences and preschoolers’ current attachment affect brain responses during facial familiarity processing: an ERP study. Front. Psychol. 8:2047. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02047
Maheu, F. S., Dozier, M., Guyer, A. E., Mandell, D., Peloso, E., Poeth, K., et al. (2010). A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 34–49. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.1.34
Makita, K., Takiguchi, S., Naruse, H., Shimada, K., Morioka, S., Fujisawa, T. X., et al. (2020). White matter changes in children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 303:111129. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111129
Marshall, P. J., and Fox, N. A.Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core Group (2004). A comparison of the electroencephalogram between institutionalized and community children in Romania. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1327–1338. doi: 10.1162/0898929042304723
Marshall, P. J., and Kenney, J. W. (2009). Biological perspectives on the effects of early psychosocial experience. Dev. Rev. 29, 96–119. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.001
McCrory, E. J., De Brito, S. A., Sebastian, C. L., Mechelli, A., Bird, G., Kelly, P. A., et al. (2011). Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Curr. Biol. 21, R947–R948. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.015
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Gold, A. L., Duys, A., Lambert, H. K., Peverill, M., et al. (2016). Maltreatment exposure, brain structure, and fear conditioning in children and adolescents. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 1956–1964. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.365
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Winter, W., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Nelson, C. A. (2014). Widespread reductions in cortical thickness following severe early-life deprivation: a neurodevelopmental pathway to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 629–638. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.016
Mehta, M. A., Golembo, N. I., Nosarti, C., Colvert, E., Mota, A., Williams, S. C. R., et al. (2009). Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: the English and Romanian adoptees study pilot. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 943–951. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02084.x
Mehta, M. A., Gore-Langton, E., Golembo, N., Colvert, E., Williams, S. C. R., and Sonuga-Barke, E. (2010). Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in life. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2316–2325. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21394
Moulson, M. C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Nelson, C. A. (2009a). Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Dev. Psychol. 45, 17–30. doi: 10.1037/a0014035
Moulson, M. C., Westerlund, A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Nelson, C. A. (2009b). The effects of early experience on face recognition: an event-related potential study of institutionalized children in Romania. Child Dev. 80, 1039–1056. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01315.x
Nelson, C. A. (2007). A neurobiological perspective on early human deprivation. Child Dev. Perspect. 1, 13–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00004.x
Nelson, C. A., Bos, K., Gunnar, M. R., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2011). V. The neurobiological toll of early human deprivation. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 76, 127–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00630.x
Nelson, C. A., Westerlund, A., McDermott, J. M., Zeanah, C. H., and Fox, N. A. (2013). Emotion recognition following early psychosocial deprivation. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 517–525. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001216
Oliveira, P. S., Fearon, P., Belsky, J., Mesquita, A. R., Sampaio, A., Pinal, D., et al. (2023). Neural correlates of face familiarity in institutionalised children and links to attachment disordered behaviour. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 64, 736–746. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13728
Olsavsky, A. K., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., Humphreys, K. L., Flannery, J., Goff, B., et al. (2013). Indiscriminate amygdala response to mothers and strangers after early maternal deprivation. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 853–860. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.025
Parker, S. W., and Nelson, C. A.The Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core Group (2005a). An event-related potential study of the impact of institutional rearing on face recognition. Dev. Psychopathol. 17, 621–639. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050303
Parker, S. W., and Nelson, C. A.The Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core Group (2005b). The impact of early institutional rearing on the ability to discriminate facial expressions of emotion: an event-related potential study. Child Dev. 76, 54–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00829.x
Pechtel, P., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology 214, 55–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2
Rutter, M. (2012). Achievements and challenges in the biology of environmental effects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 17149–17153. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121258109
Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. I: disinhibited attachment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 17–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01688.x
Sheridan, M. A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., McLaughlin, K. A., and Nelson, C. A. (2012). Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 12927–12932. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200041109
Smith, K. E., and Pollak, S. D. (2021). Rethinking concepts and categories for understanding the neurodevelopmental effects of childhood adversity. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 67–93. doi: 10.1177/1745691620920725
Soares, I., Belsky, J., Oliveira, P., Silva, J., Marques, S., Baptista, J., et al. (2014). Does early family risk and current quality of care predict indiscriminate social behavior in institutionalized Portuguese children? Attach Hum. Dev. 16, 137–148. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.869237
Takiguchi, S., Fujisawa, T. X., Mizushima, S., Saito, D. N., Okamoto, Y., Shimada, K., et al. (2015). Ventral striatum dysfunction in children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder: functional MRI study. BJPsych Open 1, 121–128. doi: 10.1192/bjpo.bp.115.001586
Tarullo, A. R., Garvin, M. C., and Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Atypical EEG power correlates with indiscriminately friendly behavior in internationally adopted children. Dev. Psychol. 47, 417–431. doi: 10.1037/a0021363
Teicher, M. H., Dumont, N. L., Ito, Y., Vaituzis, C., Giedd, J. N., and Andersen, S. L. (2004). Childhood neglect is associated with reduced corpus callosum area. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.016
Teicher, M. H., and Samson, J. A. (2016). Annual research review: enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 241–266. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12507
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Millner, A., Gilhooly, T., Zevin, J. D., and Casey, B. J. (2011). Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation: neurodevelopment and adversity. Dev. Sci. 14, 190–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00971.x
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., et al. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation: previous institutionalization. Dev. Sci. 13, 46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x
Tottenham, N., and Sheridan, M. A. (2009). A review of adversity, the amygdala and the hippocampus: a consideration of developmental timing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:68. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.068.2009
Vanderwert, R. E., Marshall, P. J., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Fox, N. A. (2010). Timing of intervention affects brain electrical activity in children exposed to severe psychosocial neglect. PLoS One 5:e11415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011415
VanTieghem, M., Korom, M., Flannery, J., Choy, T., Caldera, C., Humphreys, K. L., et al. (2021). Longitudinal changes in amygdala, hippocampus and cortisol development following early caregiving adversity. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 48:100916. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100916
Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., Smyke, A. T., Marshall, P., Parker, S. W., et al. (2003). Designing research to study the effects of institutionalization on brain and behavioral development: the Bucharest early intervention project. Dev. Psychopathol. 15, 885–907. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000452
Keywords: brain development, looked after children/children in care, attachment, reactive attachment disorder (RAD), disinhibited social engagement disorder, institutionalization, foster care
Citation: Oliveira PS (2024) The impact of out-of-home care on brain development: a brief review of the neuroscientific evidence informing our understanding of children’s attachment outcomes. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 18:1332898. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1332898
Edited by:
Sarah Nazzari, University of Pavia, ItalyReviewed by:
Saara Nolvi, University of Turku, FinlandCopyright © 2024 Oliveira. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Paula S. Oliveira, paula.oliveira@annafreud.org