Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 03 August 2023
Sec. Learning and Memory
This article is part of the Research Topic Animal-friendly methods for rodent behavioral testing in neuroscience research View all 21 articles

Making a case for the free exploratory paradigm: animal welfare-friendly assays that enhance heterozygosity and ecological validity

  • 1Department of Biological Sciences, Fordham University, Bronx, NY, United States
  • 2Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
  • 3Institute for Pharmacology and Toxicology, Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 4Laboratory of Veterinary Ethology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
  • 5Department of Animal Physiology, Institute of Functional Biology and Ecology, Faculty of Biology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
  • 6Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Rodents are not test tubes with whiskers”

                                       – author unknown

Introduction

Rodents, laboratory rats and mice, have been used as models in experimental research for almost two centuries (Keeler, 1947; Bolles and Woods, 1964; Nishioka, 1995; Guénet and Bonhomme, 2003). During this time, it has been assumed that rodent suffering was a necessary part of the tremendous scientific advancement, and thus the means justified the ends. After centuries of unregulated research, animal welfare committees were instituted in Europe, America and Australasia to limit animal suffering (Steneck, 1997; Curzer et al., 2016). While licenses to conduct research on animals are often burdensome to obtain (Curzer et al., 2016), there has been strong variance across nations in expectations for the license, the review process and compliance (Varga, 2013). Institutions in some nations, for instance, are not financially-equipped to perform random onsite inspections, or hire veterinarians to assess or enforce conditions of the license. Those critical of the process, such as Rollin (2002) have invoked the idiom “the fox guarding the hen-house” to describe the seemingly voluntary nature of compliance for researchers in these circumstances. Regardless, some common assays that can cause needless or unjustified suffering are still used (Mason et al., 2004; Carbone, 2019), and some licenses that are appropriately established are not followed closely-enough (Jerusalem Post, 2023). Meanwhile, science is more broadly communicated than ever, and the general public and media are becoming more aware of this suffering, particularly as we learn more about the animals themselves. For instance, rodents were historically viewed as vermin or pests. Yet it is now widely recognized that rodents are sentient (Bartal et al., 2011, 2014; Mogil, 2012; Mason, 2021), and like any animal, they deserve an expansion of our “compassion footprint” (Bekoff, 2010; Cochrane, 2013; Dunayer, 2013).

Amidst challenges to the current system (Varga, 2013) mounting data on animal suffering (Buckland and Nattrass, 2020; Webb et al., 2020), and calls from animal rights groups (McMahon et al., 2012) recommending the replacement of laboratory animals altogether (Gruber and Hartung, 2004; Langley et al., 2007; Robinson et al., 2019), there is a clear need to take additional steps to limit suffering. One such approach is to develop alternative assays. Fortunately, there are available assays which promote more positive affective states for rodents (Jirkof et al., 2019), while minimizing the number of animals bred into captivity and/or euthanized. Here we argue the value of one of many such approaches, the free exploratory paradigm (FEP; Griebel et al., 1993), which is a paradigm that allows animals to freely enter and exit a test apparatus. We suggest that the FEP can improve rodent welfare in both laboratory and field assays. We then discuss how the FEP can be utilized to improve the quality of data from some of these experiments.

As a team composed of field ecologists, ethologists, physiologists and neuroscientists, we study rodents in the field and laboratory. Our experiences with rodents do not align with historical attitudes and opinions. Rodents have traditionally had a reputation, particularly in some nations, as animals that “deserved to die” (Buckland and Nattrass, 2020). This poor perception of rodents was mostly worldwide, but it was epitomized by a survey of 200 households in Cape Town, South Africa. Almost one-fifth of participants answered they were “happy” for rodents to suffer before death, and only one third cared whether rodent control was humane (Buckland and Nattrass, 2020). In the centuries that unregulated rodent research took place, attitudes toward rodents used in scientific research could have reflected social attitudes. Common tests that have historically been known to cause suffering included moderate deprivation and reward studies, forced swim tests and forced copulation assays. In the latter case, sexually-receptive females are first paired with a male and later substituted with non-receptive females. These non-consensual copulatory assays were “justified” by the authors as a means to better understand (human) male sexual violence. Despite the unquestioned importance of laboratory animals to scientific progress over 200 years, suffering has become institutionalized. Not only is suffering bad for welfare, but stress within the laboratory causes data- distortion and reduces the justification of such studies (Bailey, 2018). Fortunately, we are now far enough along in advancements and technology, that we can raise the standard of justification for a few historic assays that have limited usefulness.

For instance, all members of our international team have experience in field research. Several of our team members have been approached by laboratory researchers who wish to expand their studies to the field. The reasons for the transition are varied, yet one experience stands out. When MHP inquired about the forced swim test to a research team who recruited him to help, they explained that the assays were used to train future research students and for the benefit of any theoretical knowledge that was gained by using it. The principal researcher, who worked at a major research institution, had not considered whether there were tangible outcomes to medicine or society. However, some of these researchers-in-training would likely carry the same assays forward when they train their own students. One can see how this attitude, if embodied elsewhere, could become a cyclical process that perpetrates suffering. This occurs when students become desensitized to rodents' suffering (Balcombe, 2000), develop “compassion fatigue” (LaFollette et al., 2020), or falsely assume suffering is justifiable, because “vermin” are not thought of as having “feelings, emotions and/or memories.”

Research over the past 15 years, however, has shown laboratory rodents experience a wide range of feelings, emotions, regret and intelligence—being far more sentient than previously thought (Webb et al., 2020; Crump, 2022; Webster, 2022). While all animals, sentient or not, deserve our compassion (Bekoff, 2010), society has historically given more rights to animals thought to express memories, intelligence or sentience (Cochrane, 2013; Dunayer, 2013). For instance, we now know rats and mice show a high degree of empathy (Crawley, 2004; Bartal et al., 2011, 2014; Cox and Reichel, 2020) and remorse (Steiner and Redish, 2014). Rats are smart (Davis, 1996), have exceptional memories and can assess time (Kononowicz et al., 2022). Among rats driving robotic cars, those living in enriched environments had more robust driving skills (Crawford et al., 2020). All 17 rats of the latter study assayed had a higher concentration of dehydroepiandrosterone while driving, indicating they were experiencing the reward of learning a new skill (Crawford et al., 2020). The media, so important in steering social expectations, also widely-reported rats' ability to play, be tickled, and express joy through ultra-high frequency vocalizations (Mällo et al., 2007; Hammond et al., 2019; Burke et al., 2022). In a highly-cited, and attitude-shifting paper, these “chirps” were shown to be analogous to laughter (Panksepp and Burgdorf, 2000). Finally, in a finding that went “viral” on social media, researchers found that rats move to the beat (“danced”) of an eclectic range of popular music from Mozart to Michael Jackson (Ito et al., 2022). This public knowledge is helping shift attitudes, which is in turn compel animal rights advocates and researchers to explore alternatives. While a common concern of researchers is that their research outcomes could be compromised by welfare-friendly designs, we will argue that, in some situations, the FEP may actually improve data quality and research outcomes.

Ironically, the researchers who we referred to earlier, contacted us, not to improve the welfare of the animals, but to increase the value of their own research. The principal complaint was that “laboratory animals were a product of indolence and lacked genetic variability.” To eliminate variation, which might permit smaller effects to be detected, laboratory studies often used genetically-homogenous strains. Not only have laboratory animals been deprived of heterozygosity, the processes of domestication has modified the behavior and physiology of these animals. Such studies make it difficult to have broad conclusions. To eliminate further variation, laboratory studies also test animals in standardized environmental conditions that often do not reflect their natural environments in which they evolved. Thus, traditional tests purposefully remove interfering contextual variables (Rader, 1997, 2004; Würbel, 2000; Wolff, 2003; Voelkl et al., 2020). Yet these environmental variables intentionally removed from standardized assays may be essential for understanding treatment of some illnesses (Nesse, 1994; Mobbs and Kim, 2015; Oppenheim, 2019).

For instance, much neuroscience research focuses on fear or anxiety. These are natural states (Blumstein, 2020) that are elicited by cues of threats and may be modified in the absence of shelter, the presence of conspecifics, or when visibility changes (Orrock and Danielson, 2009; Parsons and Blumstein, 2010). The lack of these sorts of natural contexts in research assays, as well as a lack of genetic variation, has led to a perceived crises in some sub-disciplines when laboratory outcomes do not relate to practice (Manjili, 2013; Drucker, 2016; Fendt et al., 2020; Stryjek et al., 2021a). Furthermore, we understand that non-welfare-friendly designs may create uninterpretable data. This occurs when data is compromised after being collected from stressed animals (e.g., data distortion; Bailey, 2018). Yet the FEP, as we describe below, could improve research outcomes to address each of these crises.

The FEP can be a more welfare-friendly approach when used in the laboratory (Stryjek et al., 2012; Kohl et al., 2018; Mei et al., 2020; Kohler et al., 2022) or the field (Stryjek et al., 2018; Bedoya-Pérez et al., 2021; Parsons et al., 2023). This type of assay is similar in some respects to home cage testing (Grieco et al., 2021), where animals are tested in the place they live in order to minimize the stresses of transport and handling. It also allows animals to choose if and when they visit an experimental test. They are neither deprived nor punished beforehand, and they choose whether to remain or leave a test arena. There are many examples of FEP and we will give only generalized examples of how they might operate.

For instance, an FEP test may involve experimental chambers whereby animals are attracted by food, shelter or conspecifics, and assayed under video surveillance or direct observation (Bedoya-Pérez et al., 2021; Parsons et al., 2023). In some circumstances, even more realistic assays may be constructed using the natural landscape such as common “rat runways” instead of chambers (Figure 1; Parsons et al., 2019). While we recognize these approaches are not sufficient for all research questions, we will highlight the benefits, a few types of hypotheses that may be addressed, and potential advantages over traditional tests.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Field FEP comprised of free ranging animals with continuous surveillance using passive microchip readers and video recorders in an urban environment. (A) Mobile outdoor laboratory; (B) gas anesthesia system for RFID implantation; (C) antennas placed within “rat runways”; (D) natural (non-modified) landscape within an urban warehouse setting.

Field

In addition to being welfare-friendly, FEP in the field offers other advantages. Free-living rodents are assumed to be genetically variable and possess their full faculties. This is in contrast to lab animals, which are inbred, have smaller brains, adrenal glands, and different sized brain structures including the basolateral complex of the amygdala, main olfactory bulb, and accessory olfactory bulb (Koizumi et al., 2018), among others. The differences are exacerbated by albinism which is frequent among laboratory rodents and causes impairment in various senses (e.g., Lockard, 1968; Sachs, 1996; Prusky et al., 2002). Additionally, such studies require limited (or no) handing, an unnatural stressor (Sensini et al., 2020) that can influence outcomes. This could be especially relevant to researchers that prefer to pre-identify subjects prior to testing. Additionally, studying animals in their natural environment allows more accurate study of environmental contexts that are missing in standardized laboratory trials. Context is essential because decision rules are often context-specific (e.g., Pinho et al., 2019; Heissenberger et al., 2020). Contexts, such as the availability of conspecifics, competitors, shelter and predators are expected to modify a variety of behaviors of interest. This can be especially important in fear and anxiety studies (Orrock et al., 2004; Orrock and Danielson, 2009; Parsons et al., 2018). Indeed, laboratory and field trial outcomes in olfactory-based research often differ (Apfelbach et al., 2005), and many of these differences can be explained by variable contexts (Parsons et al., 2018; Fendt et al., 2020; Stryjek et al., 2021b). The most parsimonious approach to improve welfare outcomes and increase experimental contexts may be to move laboratory-style chambers into the field (Figure 2; Modlinska and Stryjek, 2016; Stryjek et al., 2018; Bedoya-Pérez et al., 2021; Parsons et al., 2023). A wide range of possible experimental topics are discussed in Stryjek et al. (2021a) and these include studies of novelty, cognition, problem-solving, sensory acuity, behavioral responses to stress (stress resilience), and social behavior.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Field FEP in natural conditions demonstrating an assay that eliminates all animal handling. Laboratory-style boxes used for a study on free-ranging rodents (Apodemus mice; A. agrarius and A. flavicollis). (A, B) Two wooden chambers deployed near mice's habitat. (C) The inside of the experimental chamber with floor covered with sand layer and chocolate cream as a bait. (D) Video-still showing yellow-necked mice (A. flavicollis) during social interaction inside one of the chambers. (E) Video still showing yellow-necked mice during social interaction near the entrance pipes.

Millions of rodents are bred and sold to research laboratories each year. By testing wild animals in the wild, fewer rodents have to be bred and there is no need to kill free-living animals. Testing animals in their natural environment may help address the “crisis” of translational medicine as reported by Oppenheim (2019), where he argued that findings from the laboratory are not reliable predictors of clinical outcomes. Finally, studying animals in the wild may be a pathway to identify promising new model system that could be brought back into the laboratory, where they could be more systematically studied using a FEP.

Laboratory

FEP assays when conducted in the laboratory are also welfare-friendly, while the advantages for improving research outcomes are not as straight forward as those in the field. Wild animal studies can be important model systems and by bringing them into laboratory trials they can increase genetic variability and help us better understand natural behavioral variation (Stryjek et al., 2013; Dolivo and Taborsky, 2015; Kiyokawa et al., 2017; Koizumi et al., 2018; Schneeberger et al., 2020). However, animals are still captive. Additionally, animals in traditional paradigms are forced to explore/take part in the study, which elevates stress and distorts the result and can even eliminate behaviors under study. Yet, research in recent decades explored how the FEP could be applied in the lab. Laboratory FEP setups usually consist of a home cage and a testing device, e.g., operant walls (Kiryk et al., 2020), a touch screen box (Rivalan et al., 2017) or mazes such as the radial arm maze (Mei et al., 2020; Kohler et al., 2022). Figure 3 shows an example where the two are connected by a so-called sorter that recognizes the RFID-chipped mouse, ensures that only one animal enters the test device alone, and then starts individual tests in the test device via a connected computer. Mice or rats explore such a setup without food deprivation once they have access to it, quickly become accustomed to the set up, and learn within a few days that they are rewarded (e.g., with sucrose solution or pellets) for solving tasks in the test device. Such a FEP setup can theoretically run 24/7, and the animals visit the test device during both their active and passive phases, making more visits during the active period but surprisingly performing very similar in both phases. Compared to the classical procedure, where animals are manually placed in the boxes for training, the complete training procedure in such an application is several times faster. These studies show that the FEP is useful in field and laboratory conditions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Homecage (left), sorter system (middle), and touchscreen box (right). (B) Touchscreen box with pictures of a compound discrimination task on the screen (part of the attentional set shifting task measuring cognitive flexibility).

Conclusions

There is increasing desire from researchers (Jirkof et al., 2019; Buckland and Nattrass, 2020; d'Isa and Gerlai, 2023), animal welfare advocates, and the public to shift our attitudes about rodents used in experimental research. Indeed, a contingent of researchers and advocates are calling for the replacement of laboratory animals altogether (Gruber and Hartung, 2004; Langley et al., 2007). This has all been happening while traditional rodent research has been under scrutiny (Oppenheim, 2019; Voelkl et al., 2020, 2021) because laboratory animals lack genetic diversity, and because experimental laboratory situations are not similar enough to the “real world” to justify suffering if studies produce questionable results (Manjili, 2013; Drucker, 2016; Matusz et al., 2019; Oppenheim, 2019; Fendt et al., 2020). We recognize however, that laboratory animals are our only means for success in some areas of biomedicine. So, our position is not so strong that we recommend replacing animals altogether, and we recognize that not all research questions can be adequately addressed by the FEP. They do however offer, for some researchers, an intermediate, transitional step, whereby study protocols are explicitly designed to optimize animal welfare and to produce interpretable findings. Ultimately, the FEP, whether the designs we have highlighted, or new designs built to address new questions, can dramatically improve the welfare of rodents, while, when used in the wild, can reduce the number of animals bred and euthanized.

The most important misconception we have addressed relates to a common concern about adapting new practices relates to the false assumption that research outcomes will be compromised. FEP in the field may improve research outcomes because they incorporate genetic diversity, minimize animal handling, and take place in a natural environment where many contexts can be isolated or understood in concert with one another. This would satisfy animal welfare concerns and at the same time, address issues about translatability of findings (Drucker, 2016; Oppenheim, 2019). Laboratory FEP may be designed to improve outcomes and welfare in three ways: (1) by increasing heterozygosity when wild animals are brought into controlled settings and allowed to freely enter the designed apparatus; (2) when naturalistic contexts such as availability of conspecifics and shelter are incorporated into laboratory FEP settings; and (3) by minimizing animal handling, we decrease animal stress which is known to cause data distortion (Bailey, 2018). In short, improved welfare also increases data quality. While these types of assays have great potential to improve welfare and for more translatable outcomes, we would be remiss if we did not acknowledge potential shortfalls to be considered during the design and implementation. First, when used in the field where predators are nearby, we recommend deployment of video cameras to look for potential negative impacts on the subjects. In the laboratory, and when using wild-caught animals, care should be taken while catching them and acclimating them; some species may be unsuitable for captive living (Stryjek, 2010; Stryjek et al., 2021b). We hope in the next 10 years, that many variations of the FEP are created to continue addressing our most pressing research questions (for an exhaustive list see Stryjek et al., 2021a, and also Figure 4) in neuroscience, ethology and clinical medicine.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Infographic relating a few generalized types of hypotheses that can be addressed in a range of FEP assays in either the laboratory or the field. As more people work within this paradigm, more types of research questions will be addressed.

Author contributions

This invited opinion came about through extended discussions between MP, RS, MF, YK, PB, and DB. MP, RS, MF, YK, PB, and DB wrote and edited the draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Apfelbach, R., Blanchard, C. D., Blanchard, R. J., Hayes, R. A., and McGregor, I. S. (2005). The effects of predator odors in mammalian prey species: a review of field and laboratory studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1123–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, J. (2018). Does the stress of laboratory life and experimentation on animals adversely affect research data? A critical review. Altern. Lab. Anim. 46, 291–305. doi: 10.1177/026119291804600501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balcombe, J. (2000). The Use of Animals in Higher Education: Problems, Alternatives, & Recommendations. Arkansas: Humane Society Press.

Google Scholar

Bartal, I. B.-A., Decety, J., and Mason, P. (2011). Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science 334 1427–1430. doi: 10.1126/science.1210789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartal, I. B.-A., Rodgers, D. A., Sarria, M. S. B., Decety, J., and Mason, P. (2014). Pro-social behavior in rats is modulated by social experience. Elife 3, e01385. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedoya-Pérez, M. A., Le, A., McGregor, I. S., and Crowther, M. S. (2021). Antipredator responses toward cat fur in wild brown rats tested in a semi-natural environment. Behav. Ecol. 32, 835–844. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arab038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekoff, M. (2010). The Animal Manifesto: Six Reasons for Expanding Our Compassion Footprint. California: New World Library.

Google Scholar

Blumstein, D. T. (2020). The Nature of Fear: Survival Lessons From the Wild. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Bolles, R. C., and Woods, P. J. (1964). The onotogeny of behavior in the albino rat. Anim. Behav. 12, 427–441. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(64)90062-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckland, A., and Nattrass, N. (2020). Understanding preferences for humane and cruel treatment of pest rodents in Site C, Khayelitsha, South Africa. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 23, 315–324. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2019.1666008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, C., Pellis, S., and Achterberg, E. (2022). Who's laughing? Play, tickling and ultrasonic vocalizations in rats. Philos. Transact. R. Soc. B 377, 20210184. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbone, L. (2019). Ethical and IACUC considerations regarding analgesia and pain management in laboratory rodents. Comp. Med. 69, 443–450. doi: 10.30802/AALAS-CM-18-000149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochrane, A. (2013). From human rights to sentient rights. Crit. Rev. Int. Soc. Polit. Philos. 16, 655–675. doi: 10.1080/13698230.2012.691235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, S. S., and Reichel, C. M. (2020). Rats display empathic behavior independent of the opportunity for social interaction. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 1097–1104. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0572-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, L., Knouse, L., Kent, M., Vavra, D., Harding, O., LeServe, D., et al. (2020). Enriched environment exposure accelerates rodent driving skills. Behav. Brain Res. 378, 112–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawley, J. N. (2004). Designing mouse behavioral tasks relevant to autistic-like behaviors. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 10, 248–258. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crump, A. (2022). Animal sentience science and policy. Anim. Sent. 6, 15. doi: 10.51291/2377-7478.1748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curzer, H. J., Perry, G., Wallace, M. C., and Perry, D. (2016). The three Rs of animal research: what they mean for the institutional animal care and use committee and why. Sci. Eng. Ethics 22, 549–565. doi: 10.1007/s11948-015-9659-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, H. (1996). Underestimating the rat's intelligence. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 291–298. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(96)00014-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

d'Isa, R., and Gerlai, R. (2023). Designing animal-friendly behavioral tests for neuroscience research: the importance of an ethological approach. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 1090248. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1090248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolivo, V., and Taborsky, M. (2015). Norway rats reciprocate help according to the quality of help they received. Biol. Lett. 11, 20140959. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drucker, D. J. (2016). Never waste a good crisis: confronting reproducibility in translational research. Cell Metab. 24, 348–360. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2016.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunayer, J. (2013). “The rights of sentient beings: Moving beyond old and new speciesism,” in The Politics of Species: Reshaping Our Relationships With Other Animals, eds R. Corbey, and A. Lanjouw (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 27–39.

Google Scholar

Fendt, M., Parsons, M. H., Apfelbach, R., Carthey, A. C. R., dickman, Endres, T., Frank, A., et al. (2020). Context and trade-offs characterize real-world threat detection systems: a review and comprehensive framework to improve research practice and resolve the translational crisis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 115, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griebel, G., Belzungz, C., Misslin, R., and Vogel, E. (1993). The free-exploratory paradigm: an effective method for measuring neophobic behaviour in mice. Behav. Pharmacol. 4, 637–644. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199312000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grieco, F., Bernstein, B. J., Biemans, B., Bikovski, L., Burnett, C. J., Cushman, J. D., et al. (2021). Measuring behavior in the home cage: study design, applications, challenges, and perspectives. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 735387. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.735387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruber, F. P., and Hartung, T. (2004). Alternatives to animal experimentation in basic research. Altern. Anim. Exp. 21, 3–31. doi: 10.14573/altex.2004.suppl.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guénet, J.-L., and Bonhomme, F. (2003). Wild mice: an ever-increasing contribution to a popular mammalian model. Trends Genet. 19, 24–31. doi: 10.1016/S0168-9525(02)00007-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, T., Bombail, V., Nielsen, B. L., Meddle, S. L., Lawrence, A. B., and Brown, S. M. (2019). Relationships between play and responses to tickling in male juvenile rats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 221, 104879. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heissenberger, S., de Pinho, G. M., Martin, J. G., and Blumstein, D. T. (2020). Age and location influence the costs of compensatory and accelerated growth in a hibernating mammal. Behav. Ecol. 31, 826–833. doi: 10.1093/beheco/araa013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, Y., Shiramatsu, T. I., Ishida, N., Oshima, K., Magami, K., and Takahashi, H. (2022). Spontaneous beat synchronization in rats: Neural dynamics and motor entrainment. Sci. Adv. 8, eabo7019. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abo7019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jerusalem Post. (2023, May 23). Treatment of cocaine-addicted rats sparks ethics investigation at US University. Jerusalem Post.Available online at: https://www.jpost.com/omg/article-743917 (accessed July 25, 2023).

Google Scholar

Jirkof, P., Rudeck, J., and Lewejohann, L. (2019). Assessing affective state in laboratory rodents to promote animal welfare—what is the progress in applied refinement research? Animals 9, 1026. doi: 10.3390/ani9121026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keeler, C. E. (1947). Modification of brain and endocrine glands; as an explanation of altered behavior trends, in coat-character mutant strains of the Norway rat. J. Tennessee Acad. Sci. 22, 202.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kiryk, A., Janusz, A., Zglinicki, B., Turkes, E., Knapska, E., Konopka, W., et al. (2020). IntelliCage as a tool for measuring mouse behavior−20 years perspective. Behav. Brain Res. 388, 112620. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiyokawa, Y., Tanaka, K. D., Ishii, A., Mikami, K., Katayama, M., Koizumi, R., et al. (2017). Two strains of roof rats as effective models for assessing new-object reaction. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 79, 702–708. doi: 10.1292/jvms.17-0002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, M. T., Stahler, D. R., Metz, M. C., Forester, J. D., Kauffman, M. J., Varley, N., et al. (2018). Diel predator activity drives a dynamic landscape of fear. Ecol. Monogr. 88, 638–652. doi: 10.1002/ecm.1313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohler, J., Mei, J., Banneke, S., Winter, Y., Endres, M., and Emmrich, J. V. (2022). Assessing spatial learning and memory in mice: classic radial maze versus a new animal-friendly automated radial maze allowing free access and not requiring food deprivation. Front Behav Neurosci. 16, 1013624. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1013624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koizumi, R., Kiyokawa, Y., Mikami, K., Ishii, A., Tanaka, K. D., Tanikawa, T., et al. (2018). Structural differences in the brain between wild and laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus): potential contribution to wariness. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 80, 1054–1060. doi: 10.1292/jvms.18-0052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kononowicz, T. W., van Wassenhove, V., and Doyère, V. (2022). Rodents monitor their error in self-generated duration on a single trial basis. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 119, e2108850119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2108850119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFollette, M. R., Riley, M. C., Cloutier, S., Brady, C. M., O'Haire, M. E., and Gaskill, B. N. (2020). Laboratory animal welfare meets human welfare: A cross-sectional study of professional quality of life, including compassion fatigue in laboratory animal personnel. Front. Vet. Sci. 114. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.00114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langley, G., Evans, T., Holgate, S. T., and Jones, A. (2007). Replacing animal experiments: choices, chances and challenges. Bioessays 29, 918–926. doi: 10.1002/bies.20628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockard, R. B. (1968). The albino rat: a defensible choice or a bad habit? Am. Psychol. 23, 734. doi: 10.1037/h0026726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mällo, T., Matrov, D., Herm, L., Kõiv, K., Eller, M., Rinken, A., et al. (2007). Tickling-induced 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalization is individually stable and predicts behaviour in tests of anxiety and depression in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 184, 57–71. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manjili, M. (2013). Opinion: Translational Research in Crisis. The Scientist.com. Available online at: https://www.the-scientist.com/opinion/opinion-translational-research-in-crisis-38724 (accessed June 15, 2023).

Google Scholar

Mason, G., Hawkins, P., Grant, G., Raymond, R., Hughes, G., Morton, D., et al. (2004). Reducing suffering through refinement of procedures: report of the 2003 RSPCA/UFAW Rodent Welfare Group meeting. Anim. Technol. Welfare. 3, 79–85. Available online at: https://atrium.lib.uoguelph.ca/server/api/core/bitstreams/8d0a71e3-4a85-4359-8f2c-38afc8721cc3/content

Google Scholar

Mason, P. (2021). Lessons from helping behavior in rats. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 52–56. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matusz, P. J., Dikker, S., Huth, A. G., and Perrodin, C. (2019). Are we ready for real-world neuroscience? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 327–338. doi: 10.1162/jocn_e_01276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMahon, C. R., Harcourt, R., Bateson, P., and Hindell, M. A. (2012). Animal welfare and decision making in wildlife research. Biol. Conserv. 153, 254–256. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2012.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mei, J., Kohler, J., Winter, Y., Spies, C., Endres, M., Banneke, S., et al. (2020). Automated radial 8-arm maze: a voluntary and stress-free behavior test to assess spatial learning and memory in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 381, 112352. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobbs, D., and Kim, J. J. (2015). Neuroethological studies of fear, anxiety, and risky decision-making in rodents and humans. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 5, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modlinska, K., and Stryjek, R. (2016). Food neophobia in wild rats (Rattus norvegicus) inhabiting a changeable environment—a field study. PLoS ONE 11, e0156741. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0156741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogil, J. S. (2012). The surprising empathic abilities of rodents. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 143–144. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: an evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethol. Sociobiol. 15, 247–261. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)90002-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishioka, Y. (1995). The origin of common laboratory mice. Genome 38, 1–7. doi: 10.1139/g95-001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oppenheim, R. W. (2019). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mammals (and humans): the death of a central dogma in neuroscience and its replacement by a new dogma. Dev. Neurobiol. 79, 268–280. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orrock, J. L., and Danielson, B. J. (2009). Temperature and cloud cover, but not predator urine, affect winter foraging of mice. Ethology 115, 641–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01654.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orrock, J. L., Danielson, B. J., and Brinkerhoff, R. J. (2004). Rodent foraging is affected by indirect, but not by direct, cues of predation risk. Behav. Ecol. 15, 433–437. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arh031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Burgdorf, J. (2000). 50-kHz chirping (laughter?) in response to conditioned and unconditioned tickle-induced reward in rats: effects of social housing and genetic variables. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 25–38. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00238-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, M. H., Apfelbach, R., Banks, P. B., Cameron, E. Z., Dickman, C. R., Frank, A. S., et al. (2018). Biologically meaningful scents: a framework for understanding predator–prey research across disciplines. Biol. Rev. 93, 98–114. doi: 10.1111/brv.12334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, M. H., and Blumstein, D. T. (2010). Feeling vulnerable? Indirect risk cues differently influence how two marsupials respond to novel dingo urine. Ethology 116, 972–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01810.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, M. H., Deutsch, M. A., Dumitriu, D., and Munshi-South, J. (2019). Differential responses by city rats (Rattus norvegicus) toward male or female-produced pheromones in sheltered and high-risk presentations. J. Urban Ecol. doi: 10.1093/jue/juz009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, M. H., Stryjek, R., Bebas, P., Fendt, M., Blumstein, D. T., Kiyokawa, Y., et al. (2023). Why are predator cues in the field not more evocative? A ‘real world'assay elicits subtle, but meaningful, responses by wild rodents to predator scents. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10, 1259. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.1054568

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinho, G. M., Ortiz-Ross, X., Reese, A. N., and Blumstein, D. T. (2019). Correlates of maternal glucocorticoid levels in a socially flexible rodent. Horm. Behav. 116, 104577. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prusky, G. T., Harker, K. T., Douglas, R. M., and Whishaw, I. Q. (2002). Variation in visual acuity within pigmented, and between pigmented and albino rat strains. Behav. Brain Res. 136, 339–348. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00126-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rader, K. (2004). Making Mice: Standardizing Animals for American Biomedical Research, 1900-1955. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Rader, K. A. (1997). The origins of mouse genetics: beyond the bussey institution I. cold spring harbor: the station for experimental evolution and the ‘mouse club of america'. Mamm. Genome 8, 464–466. doi: 10.1007/s003359900477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivalan, M., Munawar, H., Fuchs, A., and Winter, Y. (2017). Correction: an automated, experimenter-free method for the standardised, operant cognitive testing of rats. PLoS ONE 12, e0176807. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, N. B., Krieger, K., Khan, F. M., Huffman, W., Chang, M., Naik, A., et al. (2019). The current state of animal models in research: a review. Int. J. Surg. 72, 9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2019.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rollin, B. E. (2002). Ethics, Animal Welfare, and ACUCs. Applied Ethics in Animal Research: Philosophy, Regulation, and Laboratory Applications (West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press), 113.

Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D. (1996). Penile erection in response to remote cues from females: albino rats severely impaired relative to pigmented strains. Physiol. Behav. 60, 803–808. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(96)00158-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneeberger, K., Röder, G., and Taborsky, M. (2020). The smell of hunger: Norway rats provision social partners based on odour cues of need. PLoS Biol. 18, e3000628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sensini, F., Inta, D., Palme, R., Brandwein, C., Pfeiffer, N., Riva, M. A., et al. (2020). The impact of handling technique and handling frequency on laboratory mouse welfare is sex-specific. Sci. Rep. 10, 17281. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74279-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, A. P., and Redish, A. D. (2014). Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of regret in rat decision-making on a neuroeconomic task. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 995–1002. doi: 10.1038/nn.3740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steneck, N. H. (1997). Role of the institutional animal care and use committee in monitoring research. Ethics Behav. 7, 173–184. doi: 10.1207/s15327019eb0702_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryjek, R. (2010). A transportation device for rats. Lab Anim. 39, 279. doi: 10.1038/laban0910-279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryjek, R., Mioduszewska, B., Spaltabaka-Gedek, E., and Juszczak, G. R. (2018). Wild Norway rats do not avoid predator scents when collecting food in a familiar habitat: a field study. Sci. Rep. 8, 9475. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27054-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryjek, R., Modlińska, K., and Pisula, W. (2012). Species specific behavioural patterns (digging and swimming) and reaction to novel objects in wild type, Wistar, Sprague-Dawley and Brown Norway rats. PLoS ONE 7, e40642. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryjek, R., Modlińska, K., Turlejski, K., and Pisula, W. (2013). Circadian rhythm of outside-nest activity in wild (WWCPS), albino and pigmented laboratory rats. PLoS ONE 8, e66055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryjek, R., Parsons, M. H., Fendt, M., Swiecicki, J., and Bebas, P. (2021a). Let's get wild: a review of free-ranging rat assays as context-enriched supplements to traditional laboratory models. J. Neurosci. Methods 362, 109303. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2021.109303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stryjek, R., Parsons, M. H., Fendt, M., Swiecicki, J., and Bebas, P. (2021b). A methodological review of free-ranging rat assays as context-enriched supplements to traditional laboratory models. J. Neurosci. Methods 362, 109303.

Google Scholar

Varga, O. (2013). Critical analysis of assessment studies of the animal ethics review process. Animals 3, 907–922. doi: 10.3390/ani3030907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelkl, B., Altman, N. S., Forsman, A., Forstmeier, W., Gurevitch, J., Jaric, I., et al. (2020). Reproducibility of animal research in light of biological variation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 384–393. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0313-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelkl, B., Würbel, H., Krzywinski, M., and Altman, N. (2021). The standardization fallacy. Nat. Methods 18, 5–7. doi: 10.1038/s41592-020-01036-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, C. E., Woodford, P., and Huchard, E. (2020). The study that made rats jump for joy, and then killed them: the gap between knowledge and practice widens when scientists fail to engage with the ethical implications of their own work. Bioessays 42, 2000030. doi: 10.1002/bies.202000030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, J. (2022). Animal Welfare: Understanding Sentient Minds and Why It Matters. John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Wolff, J. O. (2003). Laboratory studies with rodents: facts or artifacts? Bioscience 53, 421–427. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2003)053[0421:LSWRFO]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Würbel, H. (2000). Behaviour and the standardization fallacy. Nat. Genet. 26, 263–263. doi: 10.1038/81541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: animal welfare, research animals, rodents, laboratory mice, laboratory rats, behavioral protocols

Citation: Parsons MH, Stryjek R, Fendt M, Kiyokawa Y, Bebas P and Blumstein DT (2023) Making a case for the free exploratory paradigm: animal welfare-friendly assays that enhance heterozygosity and ecological validity. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 17:1228478. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1228478

Received: 25 May 2023; Accepted: 12 July 2023;
Published: 03 August 2023.

Edited by:

Kai Diederich, German Federal Institute for Risk Assessment, Germany

Reviewed by:

Sara Elizabeth Wells, Medical Research Council, United Kingdom
Gabrielle Christine Musk, University of Western Australia, Australia

Copyright © 2023 Parsons, Stryjek, Fendt, Kiyokawa, Bebas and Blumstein. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael H. Parsons, parsons.hmichael@gmail.com; Rafal Stryjek, rstryjek@wp.pl

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.