Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 01 August 2023
Sec. Learning and Memory

The thalamic reuniens is associated with consolidation of non-spatial memory too

  • 1School of Psychology, Speech and Hearing, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
  • 2Brain Research New Zealand – Rangahau Roro Aotearoa, a National Centre of Research Excellence, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • 3Brain Research New Zealand – Rangahau Roro Aotearoa, a National Centre of Research Excellence, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
  • 4New Zealand Brain Research Institute, Christchurch, New Zealand

The nucleus reuniens (RE) is situated in the midline thalamus and provides a key link between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. This anatomical relationship positions the Re as an ideal candidate to facilitate memory consolidation. However, there is no evidence that this role extends beyond spatial memory and contextual fear memory, which are both strongly associated with hippocampal function. We, therefore, trained intact male Long–Evans rats on an odor–trace–object paired-associate task where the explicit 10-s delay between paired items renders the task sensitive to hippocampal function. Neurons in the RE showed significantly increased activation of the immediate early gene (Zif268) when rats were re-tested for previous non-spatial memory 25 days after acquisition training, compared to a group tested at 5-days post-acquisition, as well as a control group tested 25 days after acquisition but with a new pair of non-spatial stimuli, and home cage controls. The remote recall group also showed relatively augmented IEG expression in the superficial layers of the medial PFC (anterior cingulate cortex and prelimbic cortex). These findings support the conclusion that the RE is preferentially engaged during remote recall in this non-spatial task and thus has a role beyond spatial memory and contextual fear memory.

1. Introduction

The nucleus reuniens of the thalamus (RE) is ideally situated to mediate information exchange between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (PFC) and participate in a variety of higher cognitive functions (reviews: Dolleman-van der Weel et al., 2019; Mathiasen et al., 2020; Ferraris et al., 2021). One of the most interesting lines of research suggests that RE is critical for long-term memory consolidation (Ferraris et al., 2021). Based on lesion studies in rats, the RE markedly reduces accurate recall 25 days after acquisition, but with little or no effect on short-term retrieval 1–5 days later, for spatial memory and contextual fear memory (Loureiro et al., 2012; Klein et al., 2019; Quet et al., 2020b).

Evidence from immediate early gene (IEG) expression also supports the involvement of RE in long-term consolidation. C-Fos in the RE and PFC, and not the hippocampus, shows a relatively large increase in the remote retrieval of spatial memory in the water maze (Loureiro et al., 2012; Klein et al., 2019). Another study found that the long-term retrieval of remote contextual fear memory also increased IEG activity in the RE, PFC, and amygdala (Silva et al., 2019), although the RE increase was not evident in a second study (Quet et al., 2020b). Together with lesion evidence, these findings suggest that the RE facilitates a dialogue between the PFC and the hippocampal system to support long-term memory consolidation and dynamic changes from a primarily hippocampal-dependent process to one that increasingly engages PFC activity.

Despite a role for the RE in remote memory recall when spatial information is required or context refers to a background of diffuse multimodal stimuli, the broader relevance of the RE for memory consolidation may be limited. Specifically, one lesion study reported that the RE was not critical for long-term social-olfactory memory (Quet et al., 2020a). These authors suggested that the social transmission of food memory may be supported by regions outside the hippocampus, which may explain why the RE was not involved in consolidation in this non-spatial task. The question remains, then, whether the engagement of the RE for long-term consolidation is limited to spatial and context memory specifically or more general processes that are sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction. The last point connects with the debate as to whether the hippocampal system is responsible primarily for the acquisition of spatial and context memory or more general cognitive and relational representations. One critical issue is the presence of an explicit temporal feature of the memory (Ranganath, 2019; Whittington et al., 2022). For example, learning object–odor non-spatial tasks when the two stimuli are presented simultaneously is not susceptible to hippocampal lesions in rats (Gilbert and Kesner, 2002). In contrast, the inclusion of a 10-s delay between the presentation of an object and the presentation of an odor renders non-spatial association memory susceptible to CA1 hippocampal lesions (Kesner et al., 2005), perhaps through mechanisms that link event sequences (Manns and Eichenbaum, 2005). In support of this, we found that dorsal CA1 neurons expressed more IEG Zif268 activity during a 5-day recall when the memory representation included a 10-s (trace) delay between an odor and its associated object (i.e., an odor–trace–object memory) than when the paired association memory was trained without the delay (Hamilton and Dalrymple-Alford, 2022). In this study, we show that the RE has increased IEG activation during remote, as relative to recent, recall of a previously acquired non-spatial association with such a temporal lag.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Animals and housing conditions

Male Long–Evans rats, aged 12 months, were group-housed in Makrolon cages (48 × 28 × 22 cm) on a reversed 12-h light–dark cycle. Rats were maintained at 85% ad libitum body weight with testing during the dark phase. All procedures were approved (University of Canterbury Animal Ethics Committee and complied with ARRIVE guidelines). Three groups were established by random assignment. Post-acquisition, one group was tested for recent retention (5-days post-acquisition; n = 8), the second for remote retention (25-days post-acquisition; n = 8), and the third on a new association trained only at 25-days post-acquisition (25-day New, n = 6); training and testing for all groups used a 10-s delay between the presentation of an odor and the subsequent object. There was also a home cage control group (n = 9).

2.2. Non-spatial paired-associate memory task

For details on the apparatus and procedures, see Hamilton and Dalrymple-Alford (2022). Training on simple odor and simple object discriminations was conducted first to familiarize rats with the general “go/no-go” procedures. Acquisition for an odor–trace–object paired association was conducted in the same red Perspex runway (93 × 26 × 26 cm high) with vertically removable doors (26 × 26 cm; Figure 1). The odor (e.g., 20 ul of lemon or clove; Essential Oils, New Zealand) was mixed with 5 ml of sunflower oil and applied to a sponge (6.5 × 6 × 8 mm) surrounding a plastic cap at the center of the door at the end of the second compartment (15 cm; B in Figure 1A); the cap presented a 1 mg piece of chocolate, which was available after the rat was released from the 15 cm-long start compartment. After the removal of the door with the odorized sponge, the rat was retained for a 10-s delay in the third compartment (15 cm). It was then allowed to run to the end of the runway to interact with a lightweight and visually distinct object. The object's base was hinged to a food well beneath so the rat could search for an additional chocolate reward. The presence of the reward under the object depended on whether the specific odor–trace–object pairing was a “correct” association. Black curtains enclosed the test area to minimize spatial cues. The observer sat within the curtains, adjacent to the runway, and operated the doors.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Odor–trace–object paired-associate memory task. (A) Rats started an odor–trace–object paired-associate trial in Compartment A and held there for 120 s on the first trial per day and 20 s on the next 11 trials. Door X was removed to give access to the odor sponge (B) on door Y. On eating the food from the center of the sponge, Door Y was removed, and the rat was retained for a 10-s delay in Compartment C before Door Z was removed. Latency from the removal of Door Z to interaction with the object (C) at the end of Compartment D was recorded. (D) Acquisition and retention of the odor–trace–object paired-associate task in the three groups are expressed as the latency difference between non-rewarded and rewarded pairings. The retention session was conducted 5 days (Recent) or 25 days (Remote; 25-day New) after the rat either reached the criterion or the end of the acquisition period. Mean +/- standard errors. Recent, N = 8; Remote, N = 8; 25-day New, N = 6; §latencies were converted to the reciprocal (1/sec) to minimize non-homogeneity and the difference between “rewarded” and “non-rewarded” trials used to express performance; the resulting mean values have been inverted by multiplying by −1; *significance at p < 0.05; differs from the three other groups p < 0.01.

Each rat received 12 massed daily trials, six go (rewarded) and six no-go (non-rewarded) in a pseudo-randomized order, with no more than three of either type running consecutively within a session. Correct pairings for the paired-associate task were counterbalanced across rats. The critical measure was the latency to push (nose or paw) the object after opening the last door. Rats learned that two odor–trace–object pairings were rewarded under the object (e.g., odor 1 + object A and odor 2 + object B) and that the two alternate pairings were not rewarded (i.e., Odor 1 + Object B and Odor 2 + Object A). A trial was “correct” if the rat responded within 8 s on go trials or withheld responding for longer than 8 s on no-go trials. Rats that reached a criterion of 80% correct trials over 3 consecutive days, after a minimum of 25 sessions (N = 15), were removed from further training and retention tested either 5 days or 25 days later. There were eight rats that continued for the maximum of 50 days of training, but all of these showed clear evidence of acquisition (three rats each in Recent and Remote recall groups and two rats in the 25-day New group); their final latency difference between go and no-go trials was >3.3 s with a mean of 3.8 s.

2.3. Post-acquisition retention test

Retention testing was conducted 5 days (recent recall) or 25 days (remote recall) after reaching the criterion or after 50 days of training. The retention session used the same previously learned association for the Recent and Remote groups. For the 25-day New learning group, a completely new odor–trace–object pairing was used per rat to assess the impact of a delayed return to testing conditions without explicit recall of the previously acquired association.

2.4. Histology

Immediately following the post-acquisition test (Recent, Remote, and 25-day New), or straight from an individual holding cage (Home Cage controls), rats were placed in a familiar dark, quiet room for 90 min prior to perfusion. Rats had experienced this procedure on two previous days. The rat was deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (125 mg/kg) and perfused transcardially with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB). Brains were post-fixed in 4% PFA and at least 48 h later placed in a long-term solution (20% glycerol in 0.1 M PB). Coronal 40-μm sections were collected using a freezing microtome (Thermo Fisher, UK) and stored in a cryo-protectant solution (30% glycerol, 30% ethylene glycol in 0.1 M PB) at −20°C prior to immunohistochemistry.

Free-floating sections were incubated in rabbit polyclonal Zif268 primary antibody (Egr-1; 1:1000 Cat# sc-110; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, USA) for 72 h at 4°C in PBSTx with 1% NGS, then incubated in biotinylated goat anti-rabbit secondary antibody (1:1000: Vector Laboratories BA-1000) overnight in PBSTx with 1% NGS. Following DAB visualization (16–20 min, pending visual check) and mounting, Zif268-positive cell staining in each region of interest was photographed using a light microscope (Leica, Germany) with a 10× objective. Automated cell counts were obtained through ImageJ analysis software (National Institute of Health, NIH, USA). Manual selections were made within the regions of interest, and images were converted to an 8-bit gray scale, background was subtracted (rolling = 40), converted to a mask, and the watershed function was applied. All neuronal cells above the threshold (“MaxEntropy” threshold, circularity 0.65–1.0) were counted.

Regions of interest were selected for their potential involvement in memory recall. Based on the Paxinos and Watson (2014) atlas, we included two regions of the medial PFC with sections at +3.00 to +3.72 from Bregma. These were A32D (anterior cingulate region, ACC) and A32V (prelimbic region). We followed the descriptions provided by Vogt and Paxinos (2014) who delineated cell layers II, III, and V in A32D, and layers II, III, V, and VI in A32V. Layer delineations provided by Vogt and Paxinos (2014) were also followed for the retrosplenial cortex (−5.04 to −6.48 from Bregma) to identify layers in granular Rgb and granular Rga (both sub-regions, layers II, III, IV, V, and VI; sections for Rga were centered at −6.36) and dysgranular Rdg (where layers II and III are deemed a single layer). For the midline thalamus, the RE and RH sections were taken between −1.80 and −2.16 from Bregma; the centromedial and paracentral intralaminar thalamic nuclei (ILt) were taken between −1.92 and −2.16 from Bregma. Sections for the dorsal and ventral hippocampal formation were based on atlas plates from −3.12 to −3.60 and −5.04 to −5.64 from Bregma, respectively.

Between two and six coronal sections containing a region of interest were selected. Within a cortical area with layers, such as A32D, the outermost layer of interest was identified, and a middle portion of this was manually outlined to ensure clear separation from any adjacent region (e.g., within a dorsal to ventral aspect in A32D; transitions across a different number of layers in retrosplenial cortex sub-regions). Then subsequent layers were established beneath this using the landmarks and approximate depths described by Vogt and Paxinos (2014) for the prefrontal cortex and retrosplenial cortex sub-regions. For regions without layers, as much of the region as possible within the photomicrograph was selected, while keeping adjacent regions separate. Expression within each relevant layer or region of interest was quantified as the number of neurons per mm2 and the average Zif268-positive cell count derived across the relevant sections. Sections through the ILt were counted manually due to technical issues on the microscope.

2.5. Data analysis

ANOVA using Statistica (v13; Dell Inc.) evaluated mean differences across the groups (Recent, Remote, 25-day New, and Home Cage). Repeated measures factors were added for blocks of trials for the paired-associate memory task and Zif268 counts across related regions or sub-regions of interest. A reciprocal transformation of latency data for individual trials was used to establish homogeneity of variance. On each test day, the transformed latencies for individual trials generated a mean latency for the six rewarded trials and the six non-rewarded trials with the difference used to evaluate performance. Latencies were carried forward for the acquisition of rats that reached the criterion. The final 40 acquisition days were analyzed, as running was more consistent after the first 10 days. To account for multiple comparisons and balance Type I and Type II errors, we used a significance level of a p-value of < 0.02 for behavioral analyses and a P-value of < 0.01 across regions of interest for Zif268 expression. Post hoc Newman–Keuls (N–K) tests assessed pairwise group differences. Simple main effects analysis was used when there were significant interactions involving repeated measures factors. Insufficient or missing brain sections in two rats reduced the degrees of freedom for some analyses; the removal of these cases from other analyses did not change the findings. Effect sizes (Cohen's d) were used to describe pairwise group differences in Zif268 expression in the RE.

3. Results

3.1. Behavior

Rats in all groups rapidly acquired both the simple odor and simple object discrimination tasks at a similar rate [data not shown; simple odor discrimination, Group main effect, F(2,19) = 0.08, p = 0.92; simple object discrimination, F(2,19) = 2.11, p = 0.14].

The mean latency difference (based on reciprocal latencies) for paired-associate acquisition and performance on the retention test (Recent, Remote) and on the new odor–trace–object association (25-day New) is shown in Figure 1. An increasing latency difference was evident over training for the non-rewarded trials compared to the rewarded trials. All three groups acquired the odor–trace–object paired-associate task at a similar rate [Figure 1; Group main effect, F(2,19) = 0.52, p = 0.60; Block main effect, F(7,14) = 247.84, p < 0.001; Group x Block interaction F(14,133) = 0.71, p = 0.75]. The three groups did not differ on the final block of acquisition [Group at Block 10, F(2,19) = 0.26, p = 0.77]. There was no correlation between the mean latency difference in the final block of acquisition and the number of acquisition days [r(21) = −0.14, p > 0.5].

Rats in the Recent and Remote recall groups showed clear retention of the acquired association compared to rats given the 25-day New odor–trace–object pairing [F(2,19) = 9.63 p < 0.001]. Although the two recall groups exhibited similar responses on their last Block of acquisition (Block 10), the Recent recall group showed better retention than was shown by the Remote recall group (p < 0.02).

3.2. Zif268

Figure 2 shows Zif268 expression in the RE, RH, and the medial prefrontal A32D (ACC) and A32V (prelimbic) regions of interest for the four groups (Recent, Remote, 25-day New, and Home Cage controls). The RE showed higher expression in all three trained groups than in the Home Cage group [Group main effect F(3,25) = 64.72, p < 0.001], indicating that engagement in and general memory for the behavioral task per se are associated with increased activity in the RE. Critically, N–K post hoc analysis confirmed that all four groups differed from each other for Zif268 expression in the RE (p < 0.004). Remote recall of the odor–trace–object paired-associate memory resulted in significantly higher Zif268 expression in the RE compared to the home cage group (by a factor of 298%; effect size d = 9.91), recent recall (161% increase; d = 4.13), and exposure to a new association at 25 days (120% increase; d = 1.87). To a lesser extent, both recent recall and exposure to the new association also produced greater levels of expression compared to Home Cage (Recent: 185% increase; d = 4.25; 25-day New: 247% increase; d = 7.35). There was no correlation [r(19) = 0.21, p > 0.3] between the mean latency difference score on the retention test and zif268 expression in the RE within the individual groups: Remote, r(6) = 0.24, p > 0.6; Recent, r(7) = −0.60, p > 0.1; 25-day New, r(5) = −0.11, p > 0.8). The days taken to reach the criterion were also not associated with Zif268 expression in the RE [r(19) = −0.31, p > 0.17].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Zif268 expression per mm2 (mean +/– standard errors) assessed in the nucleus reuniens (RE), rhomboid nucleus (RH), and prefrontal cortex regions (A32D, anterior cingulate, Layers II, III, and V; A32V, prelimbic, Layers II, III, V, and VI) in the three groups immediately after testing at 5-days (Recent) or 25 days (Remote; 25-day New) after the acquisition, as well as Home Cage controls. (B) Schematic diagram showing the RE region assessed and expression in 10x magnification photomicrographs for each group. The RE was assessed from −1.80 to −2.16 by Bregma. (C) Schematic diagram of the mPFC (A32D and A32V; +3.00 to +3.72 from Bregma) regions and the corresponding layers assessed shown in 10x magnification photomicrographs, and expression in photomicrographs for each group. The Home Cage control group was processed separately, but using the same Zif268 primary antibody batch number. Variation in background staining was accommodated by adjusting the background threshold in ImageJ. Recent N = 8; Remote N = 8; 25-day New N = 6; Home Cage N = 9; black bar = 100 μm; *significance at p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; #all groups differ from each other at p < 0.01; differs from the three other groups p < 0.01.

A similar, albeit weaker, pattern of Zif268 expression to that of the RE was apparent in the RH [Group main effect, F(3,25) = 4.42, p = 0.01]. However, post hoc N–K revealed a significant difference only between the Remote and Home Cage groups (p = 0.01) with Recent and 25-day New producing non-significantly lower levels of expression compared to the Remote group (p < 0.10).

There was no Group main effect for the A32D (ACC) region, aggregated across all three layers [F(3,27) = 1.18, p = 0.35]. A Group x Layer interaction [F(6,54) = 3.49, p = 0.005] was, however, evident with the Remote recall group showing higher Zif268 expression in the superficial Layer II than in both the Recent recall group [F(1,27) = 5.62, p = 0.02] and the 25-day New learning group [F(1,27) = 6.16, p = 0.01]. Zif268 expression in the Home Cage group was not significantly different from any other group (p < 0.12). There were no group differences in the deeper layers.

Across the cell layers of the A32V (prelimbic) region of the medial PFC, Zif268 expression showed a similar pattern to that seen in A32D. That is, there was no Group main effect [F(3,27) = 1.95, p = 0.14] but a Group x Layer interaction [F(9,81) = 7.13, p < 0.001]. The interaction was again driven by elevated expression in superficial Layer II in the Remote recall group, with expression in this group significantly higher compared to both Recent [F(1,27) = 7.66, p = 0.01] and 25-day New groups [F(1,27) = 13.56, p < 0.002]. The Home Cage group showed an intermediate level, but significantly higher than both the Recent and 25-day New groups (p < 0.05). In the deeper Layer VI, however, the Home Cage group showed elevated Zif268 compared to all three trained groups (p < 0.01), but there were no differences in the intermediate layers (Layers III and V).

Figure 3 shows Zif268 expression in the hippocampus, ILt, and retrosplenial cortex in the four groups. Zif268 expression across the dorsal and ventral CA1 did not differ between the trained groups or the Home Cage group [Group main effect, F(3,27) = 1.39, p = 0.26]. There was also no Group by dorsal/ventral interaction [F(3,27) = 1.64, p = 0.19]. Across both the dorsal and ventral CA3, however, Home Cage rats expressed much higher Zif268 levels [Group main effect, F(3,26) = 44.12, p < 0.001], while the three trained groups did not differ from each other (N–K p > 0.1). There was no Group by dorsal/ventral interaction for CA3 [F(3,26) = 1.44, p = 0.25]. In the ILt, rats in the Home Cage group expressed less Zif268 than all three trained groups [Group main effect, F(3,25) = 5.36, p = 0.005], but the trained groups did not differ from each other (p > 0.5). There was no Group by ILt sub-region interaction [F(3,25) = 1.20, p = 0.32].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Zif268 expression per mm2 (mean +/– standard errors) assessed in the (A) dorsal (d) and ventral (v) hippocampal CA1 and CA3, (B) paracentral (PC) and centromedial (CM) intralaminar thalamic nuclei (ILt), (C) the retrosplenial cortex regions: granular Rgb, granular Rga, and (D) dysgranular Rdg in the three trained groups and the Home Cage group. (E) Schematic diagram of the caudal regions of the retrosplenial cortex with examples of layers II–VI shown in the photomicrograph. Note, Rga was quantified only in the posterior retrosplenial sections (−5.64 to −6.48 from Bregma), while Rgb and Rdg had sections compared from anterior and posterior retrosplenial cortex (−5.04 to −6.48 from Bregma). The deeper increase in thickness toward the more posterior aspects of the Rgb and Rdg. Recent N = 8; Remote N = 8; 25-day New N = 6; Home Cage N = 9; *significance at p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; differs from the three other groups p < 0.01.

In the Rgb sub-region of the retrosplenial cortex, the 25-day New group (i.e., with a new odor–trace–object pairing in the retention test) showed lower Zif268 expression compared to the other groups [Group main effect, F(3,27) = 6.28, p = 0.002]. This difference was larger relative to the Home Cage group (N–K, p < 0.001) than the Recent (p = 0.02) and Remote (p = 0.003) groups. The group differences, however, also varied across Rgb layers [Group x Layer interaction, F(12,108) = 3.65, p < 0.001], diminishing across the depth of the layer. In both Layer II and III, the 25-day New group was different from the other three groups; in layer III, the Remote Group also showed increased levels compared to the Recent Group. There were no group differences for layers V and VI (F < 1.61).

For the Rga sub-region, there was also a Group main effect [F(3,25) = 7.44, p = 0.001] with a graded decrease in Zif268 expression from Home Cage to Remote Group, Recent Group, and 25-day New group. The Home Cage group showed significantly higher expression than both Recent (N–K, p = 0.006) and 25-day New groups (N–K, p = 0.001), but no other differences reached significance. Although these differences were reduced with an increase in the depth of layer, the Group-by-Layer interaction was not significant [F(12,100) = 1.81, p < 0.06].

In the Rdg sub-region, the 25-day New group again expressed lower levels of Zif268 than the other three groups [Group main effect, F(3,27) = 19.09, p < 0.001; pairwise N–K, p < 0.001]. The Recent, Remote, and Home Cage groups did not differ (N–K p > 0.1). There was also a Group-by-Layer interaction [F(9,81) = 5.91, p < 0.001] due to the differences with the 25-day New Group diminishing across deeper layers (p < 0.005 except layer VI p = 0.07). In Layer IV, the Home Cage group expressed an intermediate level of Zif268, which was significantly different from the Recent (p = 0.02) and Remote (p < 0.001) groups. No other differences reached significance.

4. Discussion

The current study builds on evidence that the RE makes an important contribution to consolidation and remote recall for spatial memory (Loureiro et al., 2012; Klein et al., 2019) and contextual fear memory (Quet et al., 2020b). We found a robust increase in RE activity during long term, but not short term, retrieval of a non-spatial associative memory in which the inclusion of a brief (10-s trace) between the presentation of the odor and the rat's interaction with the object renders this task hippocampal-dependent (Kesner et al., 2005). The current study found a large effect size for Zif268 expression in the RE for remote recall when compared to when retention was tested 5 days after acquisition, and when compared to two control conditions (25-day retesting but with a new association; and home cage control). There was only weak evidence that the RH was engaged in a similar fashion for remote recall, although a broadly similar pattern of IEG activation to that of RE expression was found across the three trained groups. The value of this paired-associate task is that it offers the opportunity to explore memory for an association between arbitrary non-spatial stimuli, which is a core feature of the amnesic syndrome (Turriziani et al., 2004).

The engagement of the RE may also be relevant to the performance of the odor–trace–object paired-associate task because all three trained groups showed a marked increase in Zif268 at the time of testing compared to home cage controls. In addition, all three trained groups showed increased Zif268 activation in both paracentral and centromedial sub-regions of the ILt compared to home cage controls. In contrast, inhibition of the superficial layer of the retrosplenial cortex may be relevant to new learning in this task, as there was reduced IEG activity in the 25-day New group compared to the two recall groups and the home cage controls.

Increased IEG activity in the medial PFC superficial layer with remote recall in our non-spatial task, using Zif268 as a neuronal marker, is also consistent with previous studies on memory consolidation. Remote recall in a spatial water maze task was accompanied by increased c-Fos expression in the prelimbic cortex and ACC, as well as increased spinogenesis in the superficial layers; these effects were attenuated by RE lesions (Klein et al., 2019). Together, the findings suggest that communication between the RE, the medial prefrontal cortex, and the hippocampus may be relevant for remote retrieval beyond spatial and contextual memory. This broadens the role of the RE and mPFC in hippocampal–diencephalic–cortical connectivity for consolidation and memory persistence across different kinds of memory tasks.

We selected Zif268 because this IEG marker provided prior evidence of engagement by dorsal CA1 neurons for 5-day recall after training with a 10-s trace between the odor and object stimuli compared to 5-day retention after training without the inter-item delay (Hamilton and Dalrymple-Alford, 2022). Previous research on IEG activation in the RE has used c-Fos as the functional marker (e.g., Loureiro et al., 2012; Klein et al., 2019). However, similarities rather than differences can usually be expected across the two functional markers, and in any case, our results were positive with Zif268. For example, direct comparison of Zif268 and c-Fos in the cortex following remote recall compared to recent recall of a five-arm spatial reference memory produced equivalent effects (Maviel et al., 2004). Thus, either IEG marker seems suitable for functional brain imaging associated with memory consolidation.

A previous study suggested that the RE may not be critical for the long-term consolidation of non-spatial memory (Quet et al., 2020a). However, there are differences in task characteristics between this previous study and those used in the current study. Quet et al. (2020a) determined that pre-acquisition lesions of the RE had no effect on social-olfactory memory, and they commented on the mixed evidence for the role of the hippocampus in that task as an explanation for this lack of effect. In contrast, the current study used very different task procedures and focused on neuronal activity at the time of recall in a specific odor–trace–object task that other studies suggests preferentially engages dorsal hippocampal CA1 neurons (Kesner et al., 2005; Hamilton and Dalrymple-Alford, 2022). Moreover, the current study was focused on the presence and pattern of neuronal activation associated with remote recall and was not designed to test the criticality of the RE per se for remote retrieval.

While RE activity during recall is associated with remote retrieval of contextual memory, spatial memory, and now non-spatial odor–trace–object association memory, multiple thalamic sites appear to support remote retrieval in the absence of RE activity during recall. There is evidence that the disengagement of the anterodorsal thalamic nuclei facilitates remote contextual fear memory recall (Vetere et al., 2021), whereas the activation of the anteromedial thalamic nuclei and ILt is relevant for remote memory of differentially reinforced (salient) contexts and spatial cues, respectively (Lopez et al., 2009; Toader et al., 2023). Unfortunately, we had minimal Zif268 staining in the anterior thalamic nuclei in our material, so we cannot address their involvement in our study. Nonetheless, alternate thalamic sites may reduce the critical importance of the RE during either systems-level consolidation or remote recall. If several nuclei contribute to the consolidation and recall of a remotely acquired memory, this could explain evidence that environmental enrichment reinstated remote recall and elevated medial PFC activation in rats with RE lesions (Ali et al., 2017). Similarly, the failure of RE lesions to impair remote recall of social-olfactory memory may be due to the relative importance of alternate thalamic structures for that memory (Quet et al., 2020a). Future studies are needed to determine how multiple neural circuits that engage the midline thalamus cooperate in the processes of acquisition, consolidation, and long-term memory retrieval for different types of events.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by University of Canterbury Animal Ethics Committee.

Author contributions

JH and JD-A: funding, concept and design, writing and editing drafts, statistical analysis, and interpretation. JH: conducting an experiment. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by University of Canterbury Equipment and Research Grants and Early Career support (JH) from Brain Research New Zealand – Rangahau Roro Aotearoa and Royal Society of New Zealand, Canterbury Branch.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ali, M., Cholvin, T., Muller, M. A., Cosquer, B., Kelche, C., Cassel, J. C., et al. (2017). Environmental enrichment enhances systems-level consolidation of a spatial memory after lesions of the ventral midline thalamus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 141, 108–123. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolleman-van der Weel, M. J., Griffin, A. L., Ito, H. T., Shapiro, M. L., Witter, M. P., Vertes, R. P., et al. (2019). The nucleus reuniens of the thalamus sits at the nexus of a hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex circuit enabling memory and behavior. Learn. Mem. 26, 191–205. doi: 10.1101/lm.048389.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraris, M., Cassel, J. C., de Vasconcelos, A. P., Stephan, A., and Quilichini, P. P. (2021). The nucleus reuniens, a thalamic relay for cortico-hippocampal interaction in recent and remote memory consolidation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 125, 339–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. E., and Kesner, R. P. (2002). Role of rodent hippocampus in paired-associate learning involving associations between a stimulus and a spatial location. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 63. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.1.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, J. J., and Dalrymple-Alford, J. C. (2022). Anterior thalamic nuclei: A critical substrate for non-spatial paired-associate memory in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 56, 5014–5032. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesner, R. P., Hunsaker, M. R., and Gilbert, P. E. (2005). The role of CA1 in the acquisition of an object-trace-odor paired associate task. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 781. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, M. M., Cholvin, T., Cosquer, B., Salvadori, A., Le Mero, J., Kourouma, L., et al. (2019). Ventral midline thalamus lesion prevents persistence of new (learning-triggered) hippocampal spines, delayed neocortical spinogenesis, and spatial memory durability. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1659–1676. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01865-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, J., Wolff, M., Lecourtier, L., Cosquer, B., Bontempi, B., Dalrymple-Alford, J. C., et al. (2009). The intralaminar thalamic nuclei contribute to remote spatial memory. J. Neurosci. 29, 3302–3306. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5576-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loureiro, M., Pereira de Vasconcelos, A., Cholvin, T., Lopez, J., Merienne, N., Latreche, A., et al. (2012). The ventral midline thalamus (reuniens and rhomboid nuclei) contributes to the persistence of spatial memory in rats. J. Neurosci. Official J. Soc. Neurosci. 32, 9947–9959. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0410-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manns, J. R., and Eichenbaum, H. (2005). Time and treason to the trisynaptic teachings: Theoretical comment on Kesner et al. Behav Neuroscience, 119, 1140–1143.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mathiasen, M. L., O'Mara, S. M., and Aggleton, J. P. (2020). The anterior thalamic nuclei and nucleus reuniens: so similar but so different. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 119,268–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maviel, T., Durkin, T. P., Menzaghi, F., and Bontempi, B. (2004). Sites of neocortical reorganization critical for remote spatial memory. Science 305, 96–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1098180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (2014). Paxinos and Watson's The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 7th ed. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Quet, E., Cassel, J. C., Cosquer, B., Galloux, M., Pereira De Vasconcelos, A., and Stéphan, A. (2020a). Ventral midline thalamus is not necessary for systemic consolidation of a social memory in the rat. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 4, 2398212820939738. doi: 10.1177/2398212820939738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quet, E., Majchrzak, M., Cosquer, B., Morvan, T., Wolff, M., Cassel, J. C., et al. (2020b). The reuniens and rhomboid nuclei are necessary for contextual fear memory persistence in rats. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 955–968. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02048-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ranganath, C. (2019). Time, memory, and the legacy of Howard Eichenbaum. Hippocampus 29, 146–161. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, B. A., Burns, A. M., and Gräff, J. (2019). A cFos activation map of remote fear memory attenuation. Psychopharmacology 236, 369–381. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5000-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toader, A. C., Regalado, J. M., Li, Y. R., Terceros, A., Yadav, N., Kumar, S., et al. (2023). Anteromedial thalamus gates the selection and stabilization of long-term memories. Cell 186, 1369–1381. e1317. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2023.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turriziani, P., Fadda, L., Caltagirone, C., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2004). Recognition memory for single items and for associations in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 42, 426–433. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetere, G., Xia, F., Ramsaran, A. I., Tran, L. M., Josselyn, S. A., and Frankland, P. W. (2021). An inhibitory hippocampal–thalamic pathway modulates remote memory retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 685–693. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00819-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogt, B. A., and Paxinos, G. (2014). Cytoarchitecture of mouse and rat cingulate cortex with human homologies. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 185–192. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0493-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittington, J. C., McCaffary, D., Bakermans, J. J., and Behrens, T. E. (2022). How to build a cognitive map. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1257–1272. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01153-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: nucleus reuniens, memory, consolidation, recall, paired-associate, non-spatial

Citation: Hamilton JJ and Dalrymple-Alford JC (2023) The thalamic reuniens is associated with consolidation of non-spatial memory too. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 17:1215625. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1215625

Received: 02 May 2023; Accepted: 12 July 2023;
Published: 01 August 2023.

Edited by:

Szu-Han Wang, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Gareth Robert Isaac Barker, University of Bristol, United Kingdom
Gisella Vetere, École Supérieure de Physique et de Chimie Industrielles de la Ville de Paris, France

Copyright © 2023 Hamilton and Dalrymple-Alford. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jennifer J. Hamilton, jenny.hamilton@canterbury.ac.nz; John C. Dalrymple-Alford, john.dalrymple-alford@canterbury.ac.nz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.