Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 18 August 2022
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
This article is part of the Research Topic The Cerebellar Role in Psychiatric Disorders: Emerging Evidence and Future Perspectives View all 5 articles

A narrative sequencing and mentalizing training for adults with autism: A pilot study

\r\nTom Bylemans*Tom Bylemans1*Elien HelevenElien Heleven1Kris BaetensKris Baetens1Natacha DeroostNatacha Deroost1Chris Baeken,,Chris Baeken2,3,4Frank Van OverwalleFrank Van Overwalle1
  • 1Brain Body and Cognition, Department of Psychology, Center for Neuroscience, Vrije Universiteit Brussels, Brussels, Belgium
  • 2Ghent Experimental Psychiatry (GHEP) Lab, Department of Head and Skin (UZGent), Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University Hospital (UZ Brussel), Vrije Universiteit Brussel (VUB), Brussels, Belgium
  • 4Department of Electrical Engineering, Eindhoven University of Technology, Eindhoven, Netherlands

Adults diagnosed with autism experience difficulties with understanding the mental states of others, or themselves (mentalizing) and with adequately sequencing personal stories (narrative coherence). Given that the posterior cerebellum is implicated in both skills, as well as in the etiology of autism, we developed a narrative sequencing and mentalizing training for autistic adults. Participants with an official autism diagnosis were randomly assigned to a Training group (n = 17) or a waiting-list Control group (n = 15). The Training group took part in six weekly sessions in groups of three participants lasting each about 60 min. During training, participants had to (re)tell stories from the perspective of the original storyteller and answer questions that required mentalizing. We found significant improvements in mentalizing about others’ beliefs and in narrative coherence for the Training group compared to the Control group immediately after the training compared to before the training. Almost all participants from the Training group expressed beneficial effects of the training on their mood and half of the participants reported positive effects on their self-confidence in social situations. All participants recommended the current training to others. Results are discussed in light of cerebellar theories on sequencing of social actions during mentalizing. Further improvements to the program are suggested. Our results highlight the potential clinical utility of adopting a neuroscience-informed approach to developing novel therapeutic interventions for autistic populations.

Introduction

As social beings, we continuously share experiences through narratives–causally connected events and actions that are often goal-driven, embedded in temporal sequences, and which allow us to imbue personal experiences with meaning (Trabasso et al., 1992; Capps et al., 2000). Narratives that are coherently construed enable listeners to infer mental states (such as emotions, beliefs, and intentions) that are associated with personal experiences (Lind et al., 2020). The ability to infer such mental states is referred to as mentalizing (Van Overwalle, 2009; Schurz et al., 2014). Autism is a lifelong neurodevelopmental condition characterized by social and communicative difficulties (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Murphy et al., 2016), which involve difficulties with structuring personal stories (narrative coherence) and mentalizing. Even though autism is a lifelong diagnosis with increasing prevalence rates for adults (Baxter et al., 2015; Brugha et al., 2016), autism research remains largely focused on children (Kasari et al., 2014). Therefore, autistic adults are often unable to find proper evidence-based adult-specific services (Murphy et al., 2011; Howlin and Moss, 2012; Hahler and Elsabbagh, 2015; Vogan et al., 2016; Calleja et al., 2019). The few therapeutic programs that currently exist for autistic adults do not always yield the expected results (Gerhardt and Lainer, 2011; Nicolaidis et al., 2015; Dyrda et al., 2020). To the best of our knowledge, such programs are currently not based on neuroscientific insights. To remedy this neglect, we designed a novel narrative sequencing and mentalizing training for adults based on recent neuroscientific insights on the cerebellum to improve (a) mental state attribution of self and others and (b) coherent storytelling.

The design of the training program was based on recent neuroscientific insights on sequencing functions of the posterior cerebellum implicated in many non-motor behaviors such as language (De Smet et al., 2007; Mariën et al., 2014) and social cognition, including autobiographic memory and the chronological order of social events (Van Overwalle et al., 2014, 2019a; Heleven et al., 2021). Structural and functional deficits in the posterior cerebellum are responsible for most of the social and communicative difficulties observed in autism (D’Mello et al., 2015; Hampson and Blatt, 2015). They are key to autism development (Fatemi et al., 2012; Rogers et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2014; D’Mello and Stoodley, 2015; Sathyanesan et al., 2019) and are still observed in adulthood (Vargas et al., 2005; Hallahan et al., 2009). Furthermore, research has revealed atypical (e.g., hyper- or hypoconnectivity) cerebello-cortical connectivity in autism (Khan et al., 2015; Crippa et al., 2016; Ramos et al., 2019), which has been replicated in adult populations as well (Catani et al., 2008; Olivito et al., 2018).

According to the sequence detection hypothesis (Leggio and Molinari, 2015), the cerebellum functions by identifying temporal sequences in human behavior, automatizing them after repeated exposure leading to smooth behavior, and sending feedback signals to the neocortex when unexpected violations of these sequences occur. A similar cerebellar sequencing function has been suggested for social mentalizing (Van Overwalle et al., 2014, 2019a; Heleven et al., 2019). For example, to recognize a sarcastic remark, one needs to remember the relevant contextual information based on automatized event schemas, recognize a sudden, unexpected change in body language and in intonation, and evaluate verbal comments as reflecting inappropriate judgments of people, exaggerated statements, or non-literal statements. All these social inputs are happening in a chronological sequence. Understanding this sequence is a process subserved by cerebellar sequencing functions–a necessary process that leads to the recognition of social signals and requires mentalizing about the intentions or beliefs behind these social cues. This idea has been corroborated by a variety of sequence-based mentalizing tasks (e.g., Pu et al., 2020; Haihambo et al., 2021; Heleven et al., 2021; Li et al., 2021; Ma et al., 2021a,b). Several connectivity studies have uncovered bidirectional connections between the cerebellum and neocortical mentalizing areas which constitute the larger part of the default mode network (Habas et al., 2009; Krienen and Buckner, 2009; Buckner et al., 2011; Van Overwalle and Mariën, 2016; Van Overwalle et al., 2019b,2020). This confirms the close neural synchrony between the cerebellum and neocortex required for identifying sequences in social input and sending feedback signals.

As narratives consist of a temporal sequence of causally connected events, the sequence detection hypothesis also applies to narratives. Some researchers have detected cerebellar activation during narrative processing (Xu et al., 2005; AbdulSabur et al., 2014) and during the integration of narrative (movie) events into coherent event sequences (Lahnakoski et al., 2017). Importantly, areas that are involved in narrative production (Mar, 2004) are also connected to the cerebellum (Blatt et al., 2013; Watson et al., 2014; Palesi et al., 2017). Over- and underconnectivity between these areas has been observed in autism (Khan et al., 2015; Igelström et al., 2017; Olivito et al., 2018).

Both mentalizing and narrative coherence are social skills that remain difficult for autistic adults. First, evidence has shown continued mentalizing difficulties (Chung et al., 2014), such as problems with advanced mentalizing tasks that include non-literal expressions such as sarcasm and irony (Happé, 1994; Jolliffe and Baron-Cohen, 1999; Martinez et al., 2019; Morrison et al., 2019). Most of the difficulties are experienced during implicit (i.e., fast and automatic) mentalizing (Schuwerk et al., 2014), as has been observed in naturalistic tasks (Yoshida et al., 2010; Rosenblau et al., 2015) and in studies using eye-gaze tracking (Senju et al., 2009; Kirchner et al., 2011; Schneider et al., 2013). Some researchers have pointed to issues with explicit (i.e., reflective and conscious) mentalizing as well (Cole et al., 2018), however, these results are less clear-cut. Autistic adults are also often less fluent in reflective functioning and social metacognition (i.e., understanding your own mental states during interaction with others) (Roeyers and Demurie, 2010; Grainger et al., 2014)–important components of self-focused mentalizing.

Second, researchers have observed both macrostructural and microstructural narrative incoherence in autistic adults (McCabe et al., 2013; Geelhand et al., 2020). Macrostructure refers to overall narrative structure such as the temporal-causal order, while microstructure refers to internal linguistic elements such as transition words (e.g., “therefore”), cohesive devices (e.g., conjunctions such as “and”), and grammatical/syntactical organization. Autistic adults tend to focus more on non-essential details while narrating events (Barnes and Baron-Cohen, 2012), inaccurately use transition words (Colle et al., 2008), and tend to make less use of cohesive devices (McCabe et al., 2013).

In the current training program, we looked at narrative coherence from a multidimensional perspective as proposed by Reese et al. (2011). According to this multidimensional model, narrative coherence can be subdivided into contextual coherence (i.e., when and where of the events), chronological coherence (i.e., temporally sequenced events), and thematic coherence (i.e., personal reflections and affective evaluations). Although research on autism using this model is non-existent, there is evidence pointing to a relationship with mentalizing. In general, coherent narratives have been related to improved mentalizing of the self and vice versa (Köber et al., 2018; Reese et al., 2011). More specifically, thematic coherence is associated with self-reflection through providing meaning to narrated events (Köber et al., 2018). Contextual coherence is related to perspective-taking (Baron-Cohen, 2001; Reese et al., 2011)–another subcomponent of mentalizing (Frith and Frith, 2006)–which allows a narrator to understand what the listener knows and doesn’t know. Chronological coherence is related to autobiographical memory because personal narratives are reproductions of our own memories. These autobiographical memories are reduced and less specific in autistic adults (Hare et al., 2007; Lind and Bowler, 2010; McDonnell et al., 2017; Anger et al., 2019). Research has furthermore shown a strong overlap between neural systems for mentalizing and for autobiographical memory, as they are all part of the default mode network in the neocortex and cerebellum (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Van Overwalle et al., 2014).

In sum, recent cerebellar findings demonstrated that mentalizing and narrative coherence are strongly interrelated capacities, and are both subserved by cerebellar sequencing functions. Moreover, cerebellar pathology is a key etiological factor in autism. Inspired by these findings, we developed a sequencing-based narrative mentalizing training for autistic adults. The training program aimed at improving self- and other-mentalizing and narrative coherence by asking participants to repeatedly (re)tell narratives. All training activities were inspired by prior theoretical and clinical work on narrative production and mentalizing. We selected elements of these earlier programs that were in line with the sequencing function of the cerebellum, including:

(a) using story grammar elements and visual aids to help structure the temporal order of stories (Moreau and Fidrych, 1994; Spencer and Slocum, 2010; Petersen et al., 2014),

(b) re-telling narratives and combining this with perspective-taking (García-Perez et al., 2008; Spencer and Slocum, 2010; Dodd et al., 2011; Petersen et al., 2014),

(c) focusing on causal relations within stories (Gillam et al., 2015),

(d) engaging in conversations that involve understanding the mental states of other participants and story protagonists by asking and answering story-based questions (Cavallini et al., 2015), and

(e) metacognitive reflection and perspective-taking conversations between participants (Trautwein et al., 2020).

All elements were adapted to an adult population. The current training strongly differs from this earlier work by relying on sound neuroscientific theories for the selection of training activities and by acknowledging the important relationship between mentalizing and narrative coherence.

Based on the sequence detection hypothesis (Leggio and Molinari, 2015), we hypothesized that repeatedly exposing participants to sequence-related narratives and mentalizing inferences during narratives, will improve narrative coherence and mentalizing capacities. We expected significant improvements in these skills in a Training group compared to a waiting-list Control group. Since the major focus of our program was to improve the sequential representation of narratives and embedment of thoughts and emotions within narratives, we expected most improvements on the coherence dimensions reflecting these skills, namely, the chronological and thematic dimensions, respectively.

Materials and methods

Participants

High-functioning autistic adults (i.e., with an average to high intelligence level) were recruited through several non-profit autism organizations, diagnostic centers, and autism coaches. Flyers were distributed through social media. A total of 32 participants (17 female, 15 male) with an age range from 18 to 62 years (M = 37.06, SD = 12.56) replied to the call and were all included in this study. A total of 17 participants were assigned to the Training group (9 female, 8 male; Mean age = 36.35 years, SD = 11.43), 15 participants were assigned to the Control group (8 female, 7 male; Mean age = 37.87 years, SD = 14.11). To make sure that age and gender were equally distributed across groups, for each new participant, assignment was determined on the sample characteristics (i.e., age and gender) of the already existing groups. When sample characteristics of both groups were balanced, new participants were randomly assigned.

Participants were officially diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) by independent multidisciplinary teams at several diagnostic centers based in Belgium. All participants provided proof of their diagnosis by sharing official documents with the primary investigator during a home visit (see section “Procedure”). All participants were free of concurrent neurological diagnoses or comorbid psychotic disorders. This was ascertained through the general questionnaire (see section “Measure”) and by interviewing the participants during the home visit. Other comorbid psychiatric disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity-disorder (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), anxiety, and depression, were accepted as such comorbidities are difficult to exclude from an autistic sample (Damiano et al., 2014). All participants received 20 euros for the completion of the pre-training testing and 20 euros for the completion of the post-training testing. Participants signed a written informed consent after being informed of the details of the study. This study was approved by the medical ethical committee of the University Hospital Brussels, in line with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki (2013).

Two participants dropped from the training program prior to starting the sessions due to unexpected schoolwork. Three other participants ended the training prematurely (i.e., after four sessions) because of a failure to find overlapping dates for the last two training sessions. Two of them completed a post-training test. All (incomplete) data of these five participants have been included as input in the analyses.

Procedure

Due to COVID 19 restrictions at the time of the study, the entire program was organized online, albeit with live interactions between participants and the experimenter. A graphic overview of the procedure is shown in Figure 1. After confirming participation in the study, participants signed the informed consent and completed several online questionnaires and a non-verbal intelligence test. Participants received a document and a video with detailed information about the training procedure. We attempted to attract and motivate potential participants by providing full transparency and maximal predictability. After completing the questionnaires and non-verbal intelligence test, participants were contacted again for a home visit. During this visit, under the supervision of the experimenter, participants completed two sequencing-based mentalizing tasks on a tablet, told two stories, and were administered an advanced mentalizing task. Further details on these measures are provided below. The participants followed each session of the training in small groups of three people (always the same group). They were invited to complete an online agenda in order to pinpoint dates for weekly training sessions. The total training for each participant was about 6 h, not including homework assignments and pre- and post-testing. Data collection started in February 2021 and ended in February 2022.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study Procedure. Pre-testing consisted of online questionnaires and intelligence test, and a home-visit. Participants in the Training group immediately started their 6-week training after pre-testing while participants in the Control group waited. Post-testing consisted of an online feedback questionnaire and a home visit. Participants in the Control group received a self-study bundle of the training.

Upon completion of the training program, participants were invited to fill in a feedback and satisfaction questionnaire. Participants again completed the two mentalizing tasks on a tablet during a home visit, told the two pre-training stories again [i.e., the same topic, henceforth referred to as Post (same) narratives], told two new narratives [henceforth referred to as Post (new) narratives], and completed the advanced mentalizing task. Participants received feedback on their pre-training results. Importantly, the time between the first and second assessment was the same for both groups, between 6 and 7 weeks.

The Control group did not participate in any form of training or specific activities during their waiting period. They received the training in a printed self-study version after the second assessment, in which all training information was carefully printed and exercises were accompanied with blank lines to be completed by the participants.

Training program

The program focused on improving mentalizing and narrative sequencing. Mentalizing was trained by retelling narratives from the original storyteller’s perspective while attending to the thoughts and emotions embedded in the overall storyline. Questions were asked that pertained to the mental states embedded within the story. Learning was further stimulated through verbal reinforcement. Narrative coherence was trained through mini lessons focusing on story grammar elements (i.e., setting, character, initiating event, problem, thoughts, emotions, actions, consequences, and resolution). Participants were visually aided by icons that depicted these elements and were reminded to use transition words. These visual aids were gradually removed to increase the internalization of applying story elements.

The program consisted of six sessions which were subdivided into two main parts; the first three sessions focused on theory (mini lessons; psychoeducation, tips and tricks, and impact of poorer mentalizing and narrative coherence) and retelling prewritten narratives, while the last three sessions focused on generating and retelling personally experienced narratives (see Table 1). After each session, participants received homework that was discussed at the beginning of the next session. For each session, an extensive manual was written to ensure consistency both between and within groups. In general, the main investigator followed an amount of pre-determined steps which can be consulted in Table 2. More detailed information on the training (i.e., specifics of each session and deviations from the general steps outlined in Table 2) is provided in the Supplementary material. We used SAFE recommendations to maximize training effects (Durlack et al., 2011)–including a sequenced, step-by-step training approach, active learning, focusing sufficient time on skill development, and having explicit learning goals.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Specific topics of each session and mini-lesson.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Steps followed in each session.

Measures

As noted earlier, some measures were administered online due to COVID restrictions.

General questionnaire

Participants completed an online general questionnaire which inquired about previous psychological and medical interventions, medical and sleeping problems, caffeine and alcohol intake, drug use, employment status, and educational background.

Autism questionnaire and social responsiveness scale for adults

Both self-report questionnaires were administered online and were included as covariates in the analyses to counteract potential confounding influences of a heterogeneous sample. An AQ score above 32 served as a cut-off for autism traits (Baron-Cohen et al., 2006). The SRS-a additionally identified the severity of social difficulties related to autism. An SRS-a score above 75 predicts (severe) autism traits (Bölte et al., 2011).

Raven progressive matrices

The RPM (Raven and Court, 1998)–a non-verbal intelligence test–measured deductive reasoning and fluid intelligence. The RPM was administered online. An advantage of online RPM administration is that is avoids potential confounds such as stress and concentration problems due to a novel, potentially overstimulating test environment. Research has previously shown a positive correlation between RPM grades and standard IQ scores (O’Leary et al., 1991). Participants saw 60 incomplete symbol patterns and had to select the completing part. The RPM was administered to ensure at least average intelligence in the sample and preferred to conventional intelligence tests which can underestimate intelligence in autistic individuals (Dawson et al., 2007). Scores on the RPM were compared to norms collected in Great Britain (1992) based on a sample of 629 adults (Raven and Court, 1998).

Narrative coherence coding scheme

The NaCCS (Reese et al., 2011) was used to examine improvements in narrative coherence. At pre-test, participants told one story about their most positive and most negative life experiences (counterbalanced). We specifically elicited a positively and negatively valenced narrative because previous research has revealed that negative narratives are often more coherent than positive narratives (Fivush et al., 2008; Vanderveren et al., 2019). We, therefore, wanted to make sure that every participant told a story with a positive and a negative valence. This way, we could also explore whether the training would be able to improve global coherence or rather valence-specific coherence. At post-test, the topics of the stories at pre-test were retold [Post (same) narratives]. In addition, they told two completely new stories [Post (new) narratives]. Participants were explicitly asked for highly emotionally valenced narratives as this provided the highest chance of detecting any thematic elements. Participants could tell as long or short a story as they preferred while the investigator would passively listen. Narratives were recorded on a Sony audio recorder and transcribed for further analysis. Two raters (including the main investigator TB) coded the narratives on the 3 dimensions of the NaCCS as suggested by Reese et al. (2011); (a) Contextual coherence (i.e., when and where of the events), (b) chronological coherence (i.e., events sequenced on a narrative timeline), and (c) thematic coherence (i.e., self-focused mentalizing involving personal reflections and affective evaluations). Each dimension was scored on a scale from 0 to 3, and the overall score on a scale from 0 to 9.

First, 30% of all the narratives were coded by the two raters after which inter-rater reliability was assessed by calculating Intraclass Correlation (ICC) as in previous research (e.g., Chen et al., 2012; Sales et al., 2013; Vanaken et al., 2021). We also calculated Krippendorff’s alpha, a stricter reliability coefficient. Because inter-rater reliability was initially low and the main investigator was not blind to group assignment, 30% of all the stories were recoded by 3 trained, independent blinded raters and compared to the main investigator’s initial scores. As inter-rater reliability was sufficient to high (Chronological, ICC = 0.86, α = 0.73; Contextual, ICC = 0.93, α = 0.80; Thematic, ICC = 0.87, α = 0.70), ratings of the main investigator were kept as input for the final data analysis.

Verbal and pictorial sequencing tasks

To explore the program’s effectiveness, we used recently developed sequencing tasks (Heleven et al., 2019) which measure (other-focused) mentalizing in a sequential context. The recruitment of the cerebellum in these tasks has been validated with fMRI research (Heleven et al., 2019), in cerebellar patients (Van Overwalle et al., 2019a), and with cerebellar neurostimulation (Heleven et al., 2021). Participants had to generate the correct chronological order of four scrambled pictures or sentences. The pictural and verbal versions of the task were presented in a counterbalanced order across participants. Two sets of stimuli were created for each version to allow pre- to post-testing. Because we were only interested in training effects on sequence-based mentalizing, we limited our analyses to false- and true- belief scenarios which require mentalizing about another person’s belief in order to generate the correct chronological order (Heleven et al., 2019). In false-belief scenarios, a false or outdated belief occurs due to an unobserved change in reality as experienced from the perspective of a protagonist, while in true-belief scenarios, such a change in reality was observed. See Figure 2 for an example of a pictorial false belief scenario.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. An example of a false belief sequence in the Pictorial Sequencing Task (the correct order is 3–2–1–4). Participants have to select, in the correct order, the first picture on the screen, then the second picture, and so on. Each time, the pictures move in the order indicated by the participant (Heleven et al., 2019).

The sequencing tasks were administered via a Microsoft Surface Pro tablet with an attached keyboard and run on E-prime 3 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc, 2016, Pittsburgh, PA, United States). Participants viewed the pictures/sentences on the screen with a number from 1 to 4 in front of each stimulus. Responses were given with the numerical keys on the keyboard to select the first, second, third, and fourth picture/sentence in the sequence. After the third picture/sentence was selected, participants could restart the trial if they had made a mistake, or confirm their chosen order and continue to the next trial. Participants were asked to respond as accurately and as fast as possible. As high-functioning autistic adults have been known to pass false-belief tasks (Channon et al., 2014; Eddy, 2019), we expected ceiling effects for the accuracies of all scenarios. However, as autistic people often use compensatory strategies when time permits (Livingston et al., 2020), and given that such strategies are often inefficient and time-consuming (Livingston and Happé, 2017), we did expect changes regarding reaction times. All mean data, including reaction times and accuracies, can be consulted in the Supplementary material.

Animated triangles task

As some high-functioning autistic adults can pass certain false-belief tasks, we decided to administer the ATT (Abell et al., 2000) as well–an advanced (other-focused) mentalizing task in which participants watch nine short movie clips of moving triangles. For example, in one video, a small triangle is ostensibly trying to persuade a big triangle to move away from an exit, although there is no sound nor human movement to imply this, only the movements of the triangles. In this sequence, participants mentalize when they understand that the small triangle is trying to change the mental state of the big triangle (i.e., persuade the big triangle to move away). The videos were presented on a Microsoft Surface Pro tablet. Participants were asked to describe what they had seen immediately at the end of each clip. For the current study, we looked at verbal descriptions of the mentalizing movement sequences in which an interaction occurs between the triangles, and mental state appreciation is required to understand the sequence. Verbal descriptions were recorded on a small Sony audio recorder and transcribed for further analysis. Two raters scored each description on two dimensions; (1) Intentionality (i.e., use of mental state verbs to describe the intentional nature of the interactions) with a score between 0 and 5 for each video, and (2) appropriateness (i.e., understanding the meaning of the depicted sequence) with a score between 0 and 2 for each video. First, 30% of all the descriptions were scored by the two raters after which inter-rater reliability was assessed again by Intraclass Correlation (ICC) and Krippendorff’s alpha. Inter-rater reliability for both dimensions was sufficient to high (Appropriateness; ICC = 0.96, α = 0.67; Intentionality; ICC = 0.67, α = 0.93). After recoding these 30% verbal descriptions through consensus, the remaining 70% were coded by the main investigator.

Feedback and satisfaction questionnaire

We developed our own questionnaire to gather qualitative feedback on the training program. Participants rated their degree of satisfaction on a Likert Scale ranging from 1 (very unsatisfied) to 5 (very satisfied). Rated aspects included; Therapist, general training format, instructions, exercises, training length, training goals, personal benefits, training moments, general organization, training difficulty, and training content. Participants could elaborate on their ratings and could recommend improvements to the training program. Participants were further asked yes/no questions pertaining to: the likeliness of recommending this training to others, whether they felt that these kinds of training programs were currently lacking, whether or not they felt that the training was effective/helpful to overcome some of their daily difficulties, and whether or not the training had a positive effect on their mood and social self-confidence. Finally, we asked yes/no questions about their motivation and concentration levels during the training sessions.

Data analysis

We conducted several repeated measures MANCOVA’s with total AQ and SRS scores as covariates to control for heterogeneity in our sample. Missing data and outliers were replaced by Sampling Mean Estimation. An overview of missing data can be consulted in the Supplementary material. Outliers were visually detected through boxplots. If, after Sampling Mean Estimation, outliers were still detected in the boxplots, we logarithmically transformed the data prior to analysis. Note that the logarithmic transformation was not intended to rectify potential non-normal distribution of the data. For all our data, normality was verified.

We compared pre-training scores between the Training and the waiting-list Control group to ensure that they started at the same level. Pre- and Post-training scores were compared within and between groups, and Bonferroni-corrected. The waiting-list Control group did not receive any intervention between pre- and post-testing.

Results

Autism traits and intelligence

See Table 3 for an overview of average AQ and SRS scores, and proportions of RPM grades for both groups. RPM scores indicated that our sample consisted of high-functioning autistic adults.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Autism questionnaire (AQ), SRS, and RPM scores.

Narrative coherence

Given the distinct post-measures (same or new), we ran separate Repeated Measures MANCOVAs with Time (pre vs. post) and Group (training vs. control) on the pre-training measure and each distinct post-training measure. None of the covariates showed an interaction with any of the measures, indicating that none of the covariates mediated the observed effects. See Figure 3 for a visual overview of the data. Means and standard deviations for each dimension, as well as an additional Figure directly comparing scores between groups, can be consulted in the Supplementary material.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean narrative coherence scores comparing pre and post scores. The 0–3 scale on the left refers to the three distinct coherence dimensions, while the 0–9 scale on the right refers to the overall coherence. The post-measurement refers to narratives that are the “same” or “new” compared to the pre-measurement. Error bars represent the Standard Error of Means (SEM). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001 F-test between pre- and post-measurement. Asterisks refer to the comparison between pre vs. post (new) and between pre vs. post (same), and not between post (new) and post (same).

Repeated Measures MANCOVA revealed a significant multivariate interaction of Time × Group [F(8,21) = 2.9, p = 0.024, η2 = 0.525] for pre-training and post (same) narratives. At pre-training, there were no significant differences between groups. At post-training, simple main effects analysis revealed a significant difference between the Training group and the Control group for chronological coherence [t(31) = 4.13, p < 0.001] and overall coherence [t(31) = 3.23, p = 0.003] of positively valenced narratives. When comparing pre-training vs. post (same) narratives within groups, a significant improvement was found for the Training group on chronological coherence [t(16) = 2.82, p = 0.009], thematic coherence [t(16) = 5.19, p < 0.001], and overall coherence [t(16) = 3.38, p = 0.002] for positively valenced narratives, and on overall coherence for negatively valenced narratives [t(16) = 2.11, p = 0.044]. No significant differences were found for the Control group.

Repeated Measures MANCOVA also revealed a significant multivariate interaction of Time × Group [F(8,21) = 2.742, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.511] for pre-training narratives and post (new) narratives. At pre-training, simple effects analysis revealed no significant difference between both groups, except for thematic coherence of positively valenced narratives which was higher in the Control group compared to the Training group [t(31) = 5.55, p < 0.001]. At post-training, simple effects revealed a significant difference between the Training group and the Control group for chronological coherence [t(31) = 5.64, p < 0.001] and overall coherence [t(31) = 3.63, p = 0.001] of positively valenced narratives, and for chronological [t(31) = 3.82, p < 0.001] and overall coherence of negatively valenced narratives [t(31) = 2.82, p = 0.009]. When comparing pre-training narratives and post (new) narratives within groups, a significant improvement was found for the Training group on thematic coherence [t(16) = 3.38, p = 0.002; but note that this group had lower thematic scores to begin with compared to the Control group] and overall coherence of positively valenced narratives [t(16) = 2.86, p = 0.008], and on chronological coherence [t(16) = 2.47, p = 0.02], contextual coherence [t(16) = 3.05, p = 0.005], and overall coherence of negatively valenced narratives [t(16) = 3.90, p < 0.001]. No significant differences were found for the Control group.

Animated triangles task

Repeated Measures MANCOVA revealed no significant differences in pre- to post-training ATT scores for the Training group compared to the Control group.

Verbal and pictorial sequencing tasks

When looking at accuracy (Figure 4), repeated measures MANCOVA revealed a significant multivariate interaction of Time × Group [F(4,25) = 6.086, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.493]. At pre-training, simple main effects analysis revealed significantly better performance for the Training group compared to the Control group for verbal false belief sequencing [t(31) = 4.07 p < 0.001]. At post-measurement, these significant differences disappeared, probably due to a significant improvement in performance for the Control group [t(14) = 4.22, p < 0.001]. At post-testing, simple main effects analysis revealed significantly higher accuracies for the verbal and pictorial true belief sequences in the Training group relative to the Control group [t(31) = 2.71, p = 0.010, t(31) = 2.75, p = 0.008, respectively]. Of most importance, relative to baseline pre-testing, the Training group significantly improved on pictorial false belief sequencing [t(16) = 2.11, p = 0.034], while the Control group performed significantly less well on the pictorial true belief sequences [t(14) = −4.00, p < 0.001]. Note that the post-testing improvements of the Training group for pictorial false belief sequencing or for (verbal and pictorial) true belief sequencing, were not confounded by better performance at pre-training, where as noted above, simple effects revealed only better performance for verbal false belief sequencing of the Training group compared to the Control group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Accuracy of verbal and pictorial true- and false-belief sequencing comparing pre and post scores in function of Training and Control groups, expressed in mean total accuracy scores per condition (maximum score is 60). Error bars represent the Standard Error of Means (SEM). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001 F-test between pre- and post-measurement and between groups.

Repeated measures MANCOVA on accuracy furthermore revealed a significant multivariate interaction of Time × AQ [F(4,25) = 2.844, p = 0.018, η2 = 0.313], indicating that AQ total scores were associated with the effect of time on performance of the sequencing tasks. Closer inspection of AQ scores did not, however, reveal significant differences between groups on self-reported AQ scores, indicating that differences in AQ scores probably moderated outcomes within groups, but not between groups. A post-hoc correlational analysis only revealed a significant positive correlation between AQ scores and accuracy scores for verbal true-belief sequencing at post-test in the Training group (r = 0.60, p = 0.011).

When looking at response times, Repeated Measures MANCOVA revealed no significant differences in pre- to post-training verbal and pictorial sequencing response times for the Training group in comparison to the Control group.

Feedback and satisfaction questionnaire

There were 11 out of 12 participants who fully completed the training program and filled in the questionnaire. Table 4 shows an overview of their responses. In general, participants were very satisfied with the training program. Participants reported that the training duration was too short and not challenging enough. Participants liked the transparency of the investigator, the clear structure and repetitive nature of each session, and the duration of each session. Interestingly, with regards to the need and feasibility of future implementations of this training program in healthcare services, and of continued investigation with larger sample sizes, all participants would recommend the training to others and 91% felt that training programs such as these are currently lacking. All participants reported beneficial effects of the training in their daily lives, 91% reported a positive effect on their mood, and 46% reported a positive effect on their self-confidence in social interactions. Motivation remained high throughout the program for all participants.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Feedback and satisfaction questionnaire overview.

Discussion

Summary of findings

Social difficulties such as issues with mentalizing and narrative coherence are frequently experienced by autistic adults. To the best of our knowledge, however, existing interventions for this population are not informed by recent neuroscientific advances–a somewhat surprising observation given the strong neurodevelopmental character of autism. For this reason, we proposed a novel narrative sequencing and mentalizing training based on recent neuroscientific insights on sequencing functions subserved by the cerebellum. Participants were repeatedly exposed to narrative sequences. Narratives were (re)told from the perspective of the original storyteller (perspective-taking) and questions were asked that required mentalizing. We hypothesized that repeated exposure to sequences would improve mentalizing and narrative coherence in an autistic Training group compared to an autistic waiting-list Control group.

Autistic adults in the Training group significantly improved on several narrative dimensions. Chronological coherence was improved for both positively and negatively valenced narratives, indicating that participants were able to place narrative events in a more adequate temporal-sequential order after training. Thematic coherence was improved for positively valenced narratives, as participants provided significantly more personal reflections and evaluations after the training program than before, which reflects improved self-focused mentalizing. Contextual coherence only improved for negatively valenced narratives, as participants provided more specific information regarding the time and location of personally experienced events. Overall narrative coherence improved for both positively and negatively valenced narratives. Interestingly, the waiting-list Control group did not improve at post-measurement. In general, we detected improvements in both types of valenced narratives even though prior research suggested that negatively valenced narratives tend to be more coherent than positively valenced narratives (Fivush et al., 2008; Vanderveren et al., 2019). We recommend future studies to further explore valence differences when investigating narratives in autism. For now, we conclude that there is a need for training programs focusing on narrative coherence in autistic samples since both negative and positive narratives can show improvements. Especially given that coherent narratives play an important function in our social lives (Vanaken et al., 2021).

Regarding other-focused mentalizing abilities, the current training program was able to demonstrate improvements of autistic adults in the Training group regarding accuracy of pictorial (non-verbal) false belief sequencing relative to pre-training, and also showed improved accuracy relative to the control group regarding accuracy of verbal and pictorial true belief sequencing. The waiting-list Control group did however also significantly improve in accuracy of verbal false belief sequencing. Although pre- and post-sequencing tasks consisted of different stimuli, improvements might still be due to the mere effect of task repetition. However, we contend that the improvements in the Control group were observed in a different task modality than the Training group (i.e., verbal vs. pictorial sequencing). Although the tasks are related (subserved by sequencing processes), they are also different as non-verbal processing might require different strategies compared to verbal processing. Response times did not significantly improve after training. These are very promising results which demonstrate that the training program has potential to become an effective tool to teaching novel sequencing strategies that enhance mentalizing skills. Future research could compare scores on the sequencing tasks of autistic people after the training to performance by neurotypical individuals in order to investigate whether they achieved a similar performance level. Note that in contrast, accuracy to attribute intentionality to geometric figures moving in human-like (interactive) patterns was not improved. This might be due to the fact that this task does not involve explicitly generating the correct sequence of actions during mentalizing, while the training program focused on improving mentalizing skills combined with sequencing during storytelling. Another explanation might be that geometric figures first need to be anthropomorphized to understand their “social” behavior, while, in contrast, the sequencing tasks clearly deal with people and does not require this extra analytic step, rendering this task perhaps more sensitive to different levels of social sequencing for an autistic population. Other explanations are discussed in the limitations below.

The advancements in mentalizing results are in line with the improvements found for narrative coherence, especially when considering significant improvements for the thematic dimension of narrative coherence (which requires self-focused mentalizing abilities), and the chronological dimension (which requires adequate temporal-sequencing abilities, such as in the sequencing task).

Feedback from participants in the Training group revealed overall satisfaction with the training program. All participants would recommend the current training to others and almost all participants reported a general lack of such programs in current healthcare. All participants reported that the training was helpful. Almost all participants reported beneficial effects of the training on their mood and half of the participants reported positive effects on their self-confidence in social situations. Although these responses cannot be compared against a control group, such feedback is interesting to estimate the feasibility and usefulness of the program. The positive responses of our participants point toward an important need to further develop and investigate the program.

Limitations and future research

High-functioning adults are generally able to pass simple (false-belief) mentalizing tasks (Channon et al., 2014; Eddy, 2019) but seem to have difficulties with “advanced” mentalizing tasks instead (Murray et al., 2017; Morrison et al., 2019). Such advanced mentalizing tasks often gauge their ability to understand sarcasm, attribute intentionality, detect lies, et cetera. It should be noted that we have not focused on these aspects of mentalizing in the current training program and rather kept a narrower focus.

In light of the cerebellar sequencing function (Leggio and Molinari, 2015), and in line with our hypotheses, we observed accuracy improvements on three out of four belief sequencing conditions. However, this result should be interpreted with some caution as the waiting-list Control group also showed some improvement in mentalizing, albeit in a different condition. In contrast, no improvements were found on the second so-called advanced mentalizing task (the Animated Triangles Task, Abell et al., 2000) in which participants verbally described moving triangles that interacted with each other in human-like patterns. A potential explanation of these non-significant results, noted earlier, is that this task is unrelated to sequencing, which is a fundamental novel aspect of the belief sequencing task. This attests to the importance of including task measures that test the critical novel component of the present training, namely the unfolding of sequences during narration of a story. Moreover, the Animated Triangles Task requires verbal responses which might hamper the sensitivity of detecting mentalizing difficulties in a sample characterized by communication difficulties. Furthermore, research has shown that such “advanced” mentalizing tasks often recruit additional abilities such as executive functions (Zalla and Korman, 2018) and that mentalizing is not always necessarily required to pass the tasks (e.g., Santiesteban et al., 2015). More naturalistic mentalizing tasks (e.g., Rosenblau et al., 2015) or more “advanced” aspects of mentalizing such as recognition of sarcasm, irony, and lie detection could have been more appropriate at detecting mentalizing improvements in the current study.

We should also note that the current sequencing tasks are not exempt from this critique as they likely recruit executive functions as well. Although we cannot exclude the possibility of some effects on executive functions, we assume these additional effects might be minor since we did not detect any improvements in reaction times. In the Supplementary material, we report two types of reaction times: Reaction times between stimulus onset and end of trial (RT-total), and reaction times between stimulus onset and first response (RT1). In previous studies of the sequencing tasks (Heleven et al., 2019), RT1 was considered the most critical processing time, while RT-total might mostly involve processes related to executive functioning, such as remembering, executing, and updating the sequence. Hence, improvements in executive functioning could have led to improvements in total reaction times, for example, through faster remembering of the sequence.

Furthermore, as this was a pilot study, we did not test the potential generalizability of the training to daily life. It has been argued that a lack of generalizability is often a limitation in clinical studies on autism (Carruthers et al., 2020). We are aware that this is a limitation in the current study as well. Note that qualitative feedback from the participants revealed that all participants reported beneficial effects of the training in their daily lives. We admit, however, that these beneficial effects are not necessarily related to improved mentalizing and/or narrative coherence, but could also be due to general training effects such as received empathy and the experience of sharing thoughts with other autistic people. In future developments of this training program, we, therefore, suggest implementing long-term outcomes of training effects, both looking at consistent improvements over time, as well as at effects on daily life functioning. We furthermore suggest implementing methods to increase generalizability. Such methods could include the use of material that is naturalistic and directly applicable to a participant’s personal life, as well as reinforcement of spontaneously occurring generalizations (see Stokes and Osnes, 1989).

Considering the cerebellar sequencing function (Leggio and Molinari, 2015), results regarding narrative coherence are in line with our hypotheses as well. During the training program, a major focus was placed on chronologically ordering narrative sequences and on embedding thematic elements such as thoughts and emotions within narratives. The contextual dimension (i.e., when and where) was covered to a lesser extent. The results of the current training study are reflective of this procedure, especially for chronological coherence as this dimension was improved both for positively and negatively valenced narratives, showing that temporal-sequencing abilities were strengthened.

Other, more general, limitations to the current training study need to be addressed. First, participants were always tested during home visits, and consequently, testing environments varied substantially between participants. However, this procedure had the benefit of reducing the chance of stress/anxiety during testing because of a safe/familiar testing environment. Our decision to test participants at their own place, was grounded in the belief that getting to know the investigator at their own terrain first would lower potential amounts of stress/anxiety. We believed that this would benefit the training program. In line with this cautionary statement, participants in the Training group got to know the investigator better (2 home visits + 6 sessions) than participants in the waiting-list Control group (only 2 home visits), which could have affected stress levels during post-training testing, and thus constitute a potential confound of the results. Future research could, for example, send a second, independent investigator to do the second testing in order to remove this confound (i.e., stress levels should remain the same at both testing times). Another potential option is to provide an equal amount of social attention to both groups (e.g., LaFave et al., 2019). Related to the latter suggestion, a critical limitation of the current study was the lack of participation in any training or activity for the Control group. Future research should provide either an alternative non-sequencing-based program to the Control group or provide means to increase active participation, for example through the organization of group contact without receiving specific training.

Second, it should be noted that we did not inquire about comorbid psychiatric conditions. We only made sure that none of our participants currently, and/or in the past, experienced psychotic or neurologic disorders. We included all other comorbidities in our sample as autism has been known to go hand in hand with a multitude of psychiatric conditions (Joshi et al., 2013; Damiano et al., 2014). Importantly, it has been shown that depression could, for example, influence the specificity of autobiographical memories (van Vreeswijk and de Wilde, 2004; Raes et al., 2006) which could have influenced narrative coherence outcomes. Similarly, attention and concentration difficulties (ADHD) could have had a negative influence on the performance of the sequencing tasks (reaction times were measured, and the tasks were quite lengthy). This is important to consider as ADHD and autism share a large comorbidity (Stevens et al., 2016). Dyslexia would have had a negative influence on the verbal sequencing tasks specifically. Unfortunately, we did not include such information in the final analyses. Future replications are therefore warranted to take information about comorbidities into consideration.

Third, It is worth noting that concerns may be raised regarding the intelligence levels of our sample as we have tested intelligence with a non-standard intelligence test. We are aware that RPM mainly taps into analytical (fluid) intelligence in contrast to crystallized intelligence (Carpenter et al., 1990) and that it overestimates intelligence in some subpopulations of autism compared to standard intelligence tests (Mottron, 2004; Dawson et al., 2011). However, specifically for autism, standard tests might also underestimate intelligence (Dawson et al., 2007). To corroborate the intelligence scores gained from the RPM, we asked all our participants about their education level and employment details. All participants that were employed had a job that required average intelligence and all participants were well-educated.

Fourth, it could be noted that our sample size might be too small to draw firm conclusions from the data. The small sample size of n = 32, could have reduced the power of the current study, consequently leading to an increased probability of falsely detecting true effects (Button et al., 2013) and/or falsely accepting a null hypothesis (Shreffler and Huecker, 2022). However, compared to previous intervention studies, our sample size still seems above average. To corroborate this, we calculated the average sample size for 48 unique intervention studies reported in two meta-analyses (Lorenc et al., 2017; Benevides et al., 2020). After further excluding 10 case report studies, the average sample size was 25.21 participants for the 38 remaining intervention studies. Our sample size of 32 is higher. Furthermore, we included only 46.88% of males in the current study compared to an average of 81% males in the meta-analyses, thereby moving away from the predominantly male-based samples found in previous research. This does not, however, justify the smaller sample size and potentially reduced power in the current pilot study. Future research should include a larger sample to draw firmer conclusions, and follow-up measures to track any long-term effects.

Finally, given that the cerebellum is implicated in non-motor processes directly related to our training, such as sequencing social actions and mentalizing, future interventions could benefit from applying simultaneous cerebellar neurostimulation as well. For example, cerebellar neurostimulation could benefit the current training program by boosting neural plasticity and cognitive flexibility, leading to the replacement of maladaptive patterns of social sequencing (e.g., replacing rigid social strategies) with more adaptive ones (Van Overwalle et al., 2021). Recent studies have already revealed beneficial effects of cerebellar neurostimulation on social sequence learning (Ballard et al., 2019; Heleven et al., 2021). A recent literature overview by Dedoncker et al. (2021) advises to apply neurostimulation during clinical intervention as its effects seem largest when applied to an already active brain region.

Conclusion

Even though research has identified difficulties with mentalizing and narrative coherence as core characteristics of autism, and even though neuroscience has revealed a strong causal role of the cerebellum in these processes and in the etiology of autism, interventions for autistic adults that are based on this information are still lacking. The current narrative sequencing and mentalizing training was the first to incorporate these novel neuroscientific insights to develop an intervention for this population. And even though the training is perhaps not yet optimized for clinical implementation, the current findings revealed interesting effects. Narrative coherence was improved on several dimensions, especially regarding chronological and thematic (i.e., personal reflections and evaluations) coherence. Overall coherence was improved as well. Mentalizing during sequencing was also improved on several belief conditions, while mentalizing on a more “advanced” mentalizing task without explicit sequencing did not improve. Our results suggest, for the first time, that cerebellar sequencing theories could bridge an important gap between theoretical insights and clinical implementation and highlight the need for more neuroscience-informed interventions for autistic adults. Finally, participant feedback revealed a strong need for more adult-specific training programs and revealed promising markers for the feasibility of the current program. Future research with larger samples and improved training is advised.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the University Hospital Brussels. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

TB, FV, and EH contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection, and analysis were performed by TB with feedback from EH. TB prepared the first draft of the manuscript. All authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript, read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Spearheaded Research Program (SRP57) of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium, awarded to FV.

Acknowledgments

We thank the following master and honors students for their contribution in scoring and transcribing the stories and verbal descriptions; Verena Van der Heide, Firdaws Boutkabout, Zamira Maratovna Safiullina, Zoë Beatse, Alper Senel, Lisa Deflo, Margot Hecquet, Emma Asselman, and Joëlle Deschamps. We also thank the following autism coaches, autism enthusiasts, and organizations who helped recruiting participants; Leah Volders, Sabine Guitard, Ines Keppens, Celien De Pauw, LAVA vzw, UilenSpiegel vzw, and Autisme België.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.941272/full#supplementary-material

References

AbdulSabur, N. Y., Xu, Y., Liu, S., Ming Chow, H., Baxter, M., and Braun, R. A. (2014). Neural correlates and network connectivity underlying narrative production and comprehension: A combined fMRI and PET study. Cortex 57, 107–127. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abell, F., Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn. Dev. 15, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00014-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). “Neurodevelopmental disorders”, in diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association, 59–66.

Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, R. J., Saxe, R., and Yarkoni, T. (2014). Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses. Neuroimage 91, 324–335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anger, M., Wantzen, P., Le Vaillant, J., Malvy, J., Bon, L., and Guillery-Girard, B. (2019). Positive effect of visual cueing in episodic memory and episodic future thinking in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 10:1513. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, K. H., Goen, M. R. J., Maldonado, T., and Bernard, A. J. (2019). Effects of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on the cognitive stage of sequence learning. J. Neurophysiol. 122, 490–499. doi: 10.1152/jn.00036.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, I. J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). The big picture: Storytelling ability in adults with autism spectrum conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1557–1565. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1388-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind in normal development and autism. Prisme 34, 174–183.

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, A. R., Knickmeyer, R., and Wheelwright, S. (2006). The autism- spectrum quotient (AQ) – adolescent version. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 343–350. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0073-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baxter, A. J., Brugha, T. S., Erskine, H. E., Scheurer, R. W., Vos, T., and Scott, J. G. (2015). The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Med. 45, 601–613. doi: 10.1017/S00332917400172X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benevides, W. T., Shore, M. S., Andresen, M.-L., Caplan, R., Cook, B., and Wittig, K. (2020). Interventions to address health outcomes among autistic adults: A systematic review. Autism 24, 1345–1359. doi: 10.117/1362361320913664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blatt, G. J., Oblak, A. L., and Schmahmann, J. D. (2013). “Cerebellar connections with limbic circuits: Anatomy and functional implications,” in Handbook of the cerebellum and cerebellar disorders, eds M. Manto, J. D. Schmahmann, F. Rossi, D. L. Gruol, and N. Koibuchi (Dordrecht: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-1333-8_22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bölte, S., Westerwald, E., Holtmann, M., Freitag, C., and Poustka, F. (2011). Autistic traits and autism spectrum disorders: The clinical validity of two measures presuming a continuum of social communication skills. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 66–72. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1024-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brugha, S. T., Spiers, N., Bankart, J., Cooper, S.-A., McManus, S., and Tyrer, F. (2016). Epidemiology of autism in adults across age groups and ability levels. Br. J. Psychiatry 209, 498–503. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.174649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R., Krienen, F., Castellanos, A., Diaz, J. C., and Yeo, B. T. (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 2322–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Button, S. K., Loannidis, P. A. J., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, A. B., Flint, J., and Munafò, R. M. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 365–376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calleja, S., Islam, A. M. F., Kingsley, J., and McDonald, R. (2019). The disparities of healthcare access for adults with autism spectrum disorders. Medicine 98:e14480. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000014480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capps, L., Losh, M., and Thurber, C. (2000). “The frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autism. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 28, 193–204. doi: 10.1023/A:1005126915631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., and Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices test. Psychol. Rev. 97, 404–431. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carruthers, S., Pickles, A., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., and Charman, T. (2020). Beyond intervention into daily life: A systematic review of generalisation following social communication interventions for young children with autism. Autism Res. 13, 506–522. doi: 10.1002/aur.22654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Jones, K. D., Daly, E., Embiricos, N., Deeley, Q., Pugliese, L., et al. (2008). Altered cerebellar feedback projections in Asperger syndrome. Neuroimage 41, 1184–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavallini, E., Bianco, F., Bottiroli, S., Rosi, A., Vecchi, T., and Lecce, S. (2015). Training for generalization in ToM: A study with older adults. Front. Psychol. 6:1123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Channon, S., Crawford, S., Orlowska, D., Parikh, N., and Thoma, P. (2014). Mentalizing and social problem solving in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 19, 149–163. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2013.809659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., McAnally, M. H., Wang, Q., and Reese, E. (2012). The coherence of critical event narratives and adolescents’ psychological functioning. Memory 20, 667–681. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.693934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, Y. S., Barch, D., and Strube, M. (2014). A meta-analysis of mentalizing impairments in adults with schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder. Schizophr. Bull. 40, 602–616. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, J. E., Slocombe, E. K., and Barraclough, E. N. (2018). Abilities to explicitly and implicitly infer intentions from actions in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 1712–1726. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3425-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., and Van Der Lely, H. K. J. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 28–40. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0357-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crippa, A., Del Vecchio, G., Ceccarelli, B. S., Nobile, M., Arrigoni, F., and Brambila, P. (2016). Cortico-cerebellar connectivity in autism spectrum disorder: What do we know so far? Front. Psychiatry 7:20. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damiano, R. C., Mazfksy, A. C., White, W. S., and Dichter, S. G. (2014). Future directions for research in autism spectrum disorders. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 43, 828–843. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.945214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, M., Gernsbacher, M. A., and Mottron, L. (2011). The level and nature of autistic intelligence II: What about Asperger syndrome? PLoS One 6:e25372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Gernsbacher, M. A., and Mottron, L. (2007). The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 18, 657–662. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01954.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Smet, J. H., Baillieux, H., De Deyn, P. P., Mariën, P., and Paquier, P. (2007). The cerebellum and language: The story so far. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 59, 165–170. doi: 10.1159/000102927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dedoncker, J., Baeken, C., De Raedt, R., and Vanderhasselt, M.-A. (2021). Combined transcranial direct current stimulation and psychological interventions: State of the art and promising perspectives for clinical psychology. Biol. Psychol. 158:107991. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Mello, M. A., Crocetti, D., Mostofsky, H. S., and Stoodley, J. S. (2015). Cerebellar gray matter and lobular volumes correlate with core autism symptoms. Neuroimage Clin. 7, 631–639. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Mello, M. A., and Stoodley, C. J. (2015). Cerebro-cerebellar circuits in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Neurosci. 9:408. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodd, L. J., Ocampo, A., and Kennedy, S. K. (2011). Perspective taking through narratives: An intervention for students with ASD. Commun. Disord. Q. 33, 23–33. doi: 10.1177/1525740110395014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durlack, A. J., Weissberg, P. R., Dymnicki, B. A., Taylor, D. R., and Schellinger, B. K. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school- based universal interventions. Child Dev. 82, 405–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyrda, K., Lucci, D. K., Bieniek, R., and Brynska, A. (2020). Therapeutic programs aimed at developing theory of mind in patients with autism spectrum disorders – available methods and their effectiveness. Psychiatr. Pol. 54, 591–602. doi: 10.12740/PP/108493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eddy, C. M. (2019). What do you have in mind? Measures to assess mental state reasoning in neuropsychiatric populations. Front. Psychiatry 10:425. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fatemi, H. S., Aldiner, A. K., Ashwood, P., Bauman, L. M., Blaha, D. C., Blatt, J. G., et al. (2012). Consensus paper: Pathological role of the cerebellum in autism. Cerebellum 11, 777–807. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0355-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fivush, R., McDermott, S. J., and Bohanek, J. G. (2008). Meaning making in mothers’ and children’s narratives of emotional events. Memory 16, 579–594. doi: 10.1080/09658210802150681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, D. C., and Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Perez, R. M., Hobson, R. P., and Lee, A. (2008). Narrative role-taking in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 156–168. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0379-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geelhand, P., Papastamou, F., Deliens, G., and Kissine, M. (2020). Narrative production in autistic adults: A systematic analysis of the microstructure, macrostructure and internal state language. J. Pragmat. 164, 57–81. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2020.04.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerhardt, F. P., and Lainer, I. (2011). Addressing the needs of adolescents and adults with autism: A crisis on the horizon. J. Contemp. Psychother. 41, 37–45. doi: 10.1007/s10879-010-9160-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillam, L. S., Hartzheim, D., Studenka, B., Simonsmeier, V., and Gillam, R. (2015). Narrative intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58, 920–933. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, C., Williams, M. D., and Lind, E. S. (2014). Metacognition, metamemory, and mindreading in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 123, 650–659. doi: 10.1037/a0036531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habas, C., Kamdar, N., Nguyen, D., Prater, K., Beckmann, C. F., Menon, V., et al. (2009). Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J. Neurosci. 29, 8586–8594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahler, E.-M., and Elsabbagh, M. (2015). Autism: A global perspective. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2, 58–64. doi: 10.1007/s40474-014-0033-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haihambo, N., Ma, Q., Baeken, C., Deroost, N., Baetens, K., and Van Overwalle, F. (2021). Social thinking is for doing: The posterior cerebellum supports prediction of social actions based on personality traits. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 17, 241–251. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallahan, B., Daly, E. M., McAlonan, G., Loth, E., Toal, F., O’Brien, F., et al. (2009). Brain morphometry volume in autistic spectrum disorder: A magnetic resonance imaging study of adults. Psychol. Med. 39, 337–346. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampson, R. D., and Blatt, J. G. (2015). Autism spectrum disorders and neuropathology of the cerebellum. Front. Neurosci. 9:420. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hare, J. D., Mellor, C., and Azmi, S. (2007). Episodic memory in adults with autistic spectrum disorders: Recall for self- versus other-experiences events. Res. Dev. Disabil. 28, 317–329. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heleven, E., van Dun, K., De Witte, S., Baeken, C., and Van Overwalle, F. (2021). The role of the cerebellum in social and non-social action sequences: A preliminary LF-rTMS study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:593821. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.593821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heleven, E., van Dun, K., and Van Overwalle, F. (2019). The posterior cerebellum is involved in constructing social action sequences: An fMRI study. Sci. Rep. 9:11110. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46962-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howlin, P., and Moss, P. (2012). Adults with autism spectrum disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry 57, 275–283. doi: 10.1177/070674371205700502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igelström, M. K., Webb, W. T., and Graziano, S. A. M. (2017). Functional connectivity between the temporoparietal cortex and cerebellum in autism spectrum disorder. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2617–2627. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolliffe, T., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The strange stories test: A replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29, 395–406. doi: 10.1023/a:102308292836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, G., Wozniak, J., Petty, C., Martelon, K. M., Fried, R., Bolfek, A., et al. (2013). Psychiatric comorbidity and functioning in a clinically referred population of adults with autism spectrum disorders: A comparative study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1314–1325. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1679-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasari, C., Shire, S., Factor, R., and McCracken, C. (2014). Psychosocial treatments for individuals with autism spectrum disorder across the lifespan: New developments and underlying mechanisms. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 16:512. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0512-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, A. J., Nair, A., Keown, C. L., Datko, M. C., Lincoldn, A. J., and Müller, R.-A. (2015). Cerebro- cerebellar resting-state functional connectivity in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 625–634. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirchner, J. C., Hatri, A., Heekeren, H. R., and Dziobek, I. (2011). Autistic symptomatology, face processing abilities, and eye fixation patterns. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 158–167. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1032-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köber, C., Kuhn, M., Peters, I., and Habermas, T. (2018). Mentalizing oneself: Detecting reflective functioning in life narratives. Attach. Hum. Dev. 21, 313–331. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1473886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krienen, F. M., and Buckner, R. L. (2009). Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2485–2497. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFave, E. S., Granbom, M., Cudjoe, K. M. T., Gottsch, A., Shorb, G., and Szanton, L. S. (2019). Attention control group activities and perceived benefit in a trial of a behavioral intervention for older adults. Res. Nurs. Health 42, 476–482. doi: 10.1002/nur.21992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahnakoski, M. J., Jääskeläinen, P. L., Sams, M., and Nummenmaa, L. (2017). Neural mechanisms for integrating consecutive and interleaved natural events. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 3360–3376. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leggio, M., and Molinari, M. (2015). Cerebellar sequencing: A trick for predicting the future. Cerebellum 14, 35–38. doi: 10.1007/s12311-014-0616-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., Ma, Q., Baetens, K., Pu, M., Deroost, N., and Van Overwalle, F. (2021). Social cerebellum in goal-directed navigation. Soc. Neurosci. 16, 467–485. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2021.1970017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lind, E. S., and Bowler, M. D. (2010). Episodic memory and episodic future thinking in adults with autism. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 896–905. doi: 10.1037/a0020631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lind, M., Vanwoerden, S., Penner, F., and Sharp, C. (2020). Narrative coherence in adolescence: Relations with attachment, mentalization, and psychopathology. J. Pers. Assess. 102, 380–389. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2019.1574805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, A. L., and Happé, F. (2017). Conceptualizing compensation in neurodevelopmental disorders: Reflections from autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 729–742. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, A. L., Shah, P., Milner, V., and Happé, F. (2020). Quantifying compensatory strategies in adults with and without diagnoses autism. Mol. Autism 11:15. doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0308-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenc, T., Rodgers, M., Marshall, D., Melton, H., Rees, R., and Sowden, A. (2017). Support for adults with autism spectrum disorder without intellectual impairment: Systematic review. Autism 22, 654–668. doi: 10.1177/1362361317698939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Q., Pu, M., Haihambo, P. N., Baetens, K., Heleven, E., and Van Overwalle, F. (2021a). The posterior cerebellum and temporoparietal junction support explicit learning of social belief sequences. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 22, 467–491. doi: 10.3758/s13415-021-00966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Q., Pu, M., Heleven, E., Haihambo, P. N., Baetens, K., and Van Overwalle, F. (2021b). The posterior cerebellum supports implicit learning of social belief sequences. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 21, 970–992. doi: 10.3758/s13415-021-00910-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mar, A. R. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia 42, 1414–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariën, P., Ackermann, H., Adamaszek, M., Barwood, H. S. C., Beaton, A., and Ziegler, W. (2014). Consensus paper: Language and the cerebellum: An ongoing enigma. Cerebellum 13, 386–410. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0540-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez, G., Mosconi, E., Daban-Huard, C., Parellada, M., Fananas, L., and Amado, I. (2019). “A circle and a triangle dancing together”: Alteration of social cognition in schizophrenia compared to autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Res. 210, 94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.05.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, A., Hillier, A., and Shapiro, C. (2013). Brief report: Structure of personal narratives of adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Abnorm. Dev. Disord. 43, 733–738. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1585-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, G. C., Valentino, K., and Diehl, J. J. (2017). A developmental psychopathology perspective on autobiographical memory in autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Rev. 44, 59–81. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2017.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, M. R., and Fidrych, H. (1994). The story grammar marker: Teacher’s manual. Springfield, MA: MindWing Concepts.

Google Scholar

Morrison, E. K., Pinkham, E. A., Kelsven, S., Ludwig, K., Penn, L. D., and Sasson, J. N. (2019). Psychometric evaluation of social cognitive measures for adults with autism. Autism Res. 12, 766–778. doi: 10.1002/aur.2084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mottron, L. (2004). Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Current practices, instrument biases, and recommendations. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 19–27. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000018070.88380.83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, C., Wilson, E. C., Robertson, M. D., Ecker, C., Daly, M. E., and McAlonan, M. G. (2016). Autism spectrum disorder in adults: Diagnosis, management, and health services development. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 12, 1669–1686. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S65455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, M. G. D., Beecham, J., Craig, M., and Ecker, C. (2011). Autism in adults: New biological findings and their translational implications to the cost of clinical services. Brain Res. 1380, 22–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, K., Johnston, K., Cunane, H., Kerr, C., Spain, D., and Happé, F. (2017). A new test of advanced theory of mind: The “Strange Stories Film Task” captures social processing differences in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 10, 1120–1132. doi: 10.1002/aur.1744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolaidis, C., Raymaker, M. D., McDonald, E. K., Baggs, A. W., Dern, S., and Ashkenazy, E. (2015). “Respect the way I need to communicate with you”: Healthcare experiences of adults on the autism spectrum. Autism 19, 824–831. doi: 10.1177/1362361315576221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Leary, M. U., Rusch, M. K., and Guastello, J. S. (1991). Estimating age-stratified WAIS-R IQS from scores on the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices. J. Clin. Psychol. 47, 277–284. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2277::aid-jclp2270470215>3.0.co;2-i

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivito, G., Lupo, M., Laghi, F., Clausi, S., Baiocco, R., Cercignani, M., et al. (2018). Lobular patterns of cerebellar resting-state connectivity in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Eur. J. Neurosci. 47, 729–735. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palesi, F., De Rinaldis, A., Castellazzi, G., Calamante, F., Muhlert, N., and Gandini Wheeler-Kingshott, A. M. C. (2017). Contralateral cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathways reconstruction in humans in vivo: Implications for reciprocal cerebro-cerebellar structural connectivity in motor and non-motor areas. Sci. Rep. 7:12841. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13079-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, B. D., Brown, L. C., Ukrainetz, A. T., Wise, C., Spencer, D. T., and Zebre, J. (2014). Systematic individualized narrative language intervention on the personal narratives of children with autism. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 45, 67–86. doi: 10.1044/2013_LSHSS-12-0099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Psychology Software Tools, Inc (2016). [E-Prime 3.0]. Available online at: https://support.pstnet.com/

Google Scholar

Pu, M., Heleven, E., Delplanque, J., Gilbert, N., Ma, Q., Funghi, G., et al. (2020). The posterior cerebellum supports the explicit sequence learning linked to trait attribution. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 20, 798–815. doi: 10.3758/s13415-020-00803-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Beyers, W., Eelen, P., and Brunfaut, E. (2006). Reduced autobiographical memory specificity and rumination in predicting the course of depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 699–704. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos, C. T., Balardin, B. J., Sato, R. J., and Fujita, A. (2019). Abnormal cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 12:74. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J. J. R., and Court, J. (1998). Raven manual: Section 3. The standard progressive matrices. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc.

Google Scholar

Reese, E., Haden, A. C., Baker-Ward, L., Bauer, P., Fivush, R., and Ornstein, A. P. (2011). Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: A multidimensional model and coding method. J. Cogn. Dev. 12, 424–462. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.587854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeyers, H., and Demurie, E. (2010). How impaired is mind-reading in high-functioning adolescents and adults with autism? Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 7, 123–134. doi: 10.1080/17405620903425924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, D. T., McKimm, E., Dickson, E. P., Goldowitz, D., Blaha, D. C., and Mittleman, G. (2013). Is autism a disease of the cerebellum? An integration of clinical and pre-clinical research. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:15. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenblau, G., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, R. H., and Dziobek, I. (2015). Approximating implicit and explicit mentalizing with two naturalistic video-based tasks in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 953–965. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2249-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sales, M. J., Merrill, A. N., and Fivush, R. (2013). Does making meaning make it better? Narrative meaning making and well-being in at-risk African-American adolescent females. Memory 21, 97–110. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.706614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santiesteban, I., Shah, P., White, S., Bird, G., and Heyes, C. (2015). Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera control. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 844–849. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0716-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sathyanesan, A., Zhou, J., Scafidi, J., Heck, H. D., Sillitoe, V. R., and Gallo, V. (2019). Emerging connections between cerebellar development, behavior and complex brain disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 298–313. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0152-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, D., Slaughter, P. V., Bayliss, P. A., and Dux, E. P. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition 129, 410–417. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., and Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuwerk, T., Vuori, M., and Sodian, B. (2014). Implicit and explicit theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: The impact of experience. Autism 19, 459–468. doi: 10.1177/1362361314526004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., and Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science 325, 883–885. doi: 10.1126/science.1176170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shreffler, J., and Huecker, M. R. (2022). “Type I and type II errors and statistical power,” in StatPearls [Internet] (Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing). Available online at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK557530/ (accessed March 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Spencer, D. T., and Slocum, A. T. (2010). The effect of a narrative intervention on story retelling and personal story generation skills of preschoolers with risk factors and narrative language delays. J. Early Interv. 32, 178–199. doi: 10.1177/1053818110379124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, T., Peng, L., and Barnard-Brak, L. (2016). The comorbidity of ADHD in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 31, 11–18. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2016.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokes, F. T., and Osnes, G. P. (1989). An operant pursuit of generalization. Behav. Ther. 20, 337–355. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80054-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trabasso, T., Stein, N. L., Rodkin, P. C., Park Munger, M., and Baughn, C. R. (1992). Knowledge of goals and plans in the on-line narration of events. Cogn. Dev. 7, 133–170. doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(92)90009-G

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trautwein, F.-M., Kanske, P., Böckler, A., and Singer, T. (2020). Differential benefits of mental training types for attention, compassion, and theory of mind. Cognition 194:104039. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., Baeken, C., Campanella, S., Crunelle, L. C., Heleven, E., and Baetens, K. (2021). The role of the posterior cerebellum in dysfunctional social sequencing. Cerebellum. doi: 10.1007/s12311-021-01330-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., Baetens, K., Mariën, P., and Vandekerckhove, M. (2014). Social cognition and the cerebellum: A meta-analysis of over 350 fMRI studies. Neuroimage 86, 554–572. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., and Mariën, P. (2016). Functional connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum in social cognition: A multi-study analysis. Neuroimage 124(Pt A), 248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., De Coninck, S., Heleven, E., Perrotta, G., Ben Taib, O. N., and Mariën, P. (2019a). The role of the cerebellum in reconstructing social action sequences: A pilot study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 549–558. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., Van de Steen, F., and Mariën, P. (2019b). Dynamic causal modeling of the effective connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum in social mentalizing across five studies. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 19, 211–223. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-00659-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., Van de Steen, F., van Dun, K., and Heleven, E. (2020). Connectivity between the cerebrum and cerebellum during social and non-social sequencing using dynamic causal modeling. Neuroimage 206:116326. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Vreeswijk, F. M., and de Wilde, J. E. (2004). Autobiographical memory specificity, psychopathology, depressed mood and the use of the Autobiographical Memory Test: A meta- analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 731–743. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00194-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanaken, L., Bijttebier, P., Fivush, R., and Hermans, D. (2021). Narrative coherence predicts emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: A two-year longitudinal study. Cogn. Emot. 36, 70–81. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.1902283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderveren, E., Bijttebier, P., and Hermans, D. (2019). Autobiographical memory coherence and specificity: Examining their reciprocal relation and their associations with internalizing symptoms and rumination. Behav. Res. Ther. 116, 30–35. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, L. D., Nascimbene, C., Krishnan, C., Zimmerman, W. A., and Pardo, A. C. (2005). Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann. Neurol. 57, 67–81. doi: 10.1002/ana.20315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogan, V., Lake, K. J., Tint, A., Weiss, A. J., and Lunsky, Y. (2016). Tracking health care service use and the experiences of adults with autism spectrum disorders without intellectual disability: A longitudinal study of service rates, barriers and satisfaction. Disabil. Health J. 10, 264–270. doi: 10.10116/j.dhjo.2016.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S. S. H., Kloth, D. A., and Badura, A. (2014). The cerebellum, sensitive periods, and autism. Neuron 83, 518–532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, C. T., Becker, N., Apps, R., and Jones, W. M. (2014). Back to front: Cerebellar connections and interactions with the prefrontal cortex. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:4. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J., Kemeny, S., Park, G., Frattali, C., and Braun, A. (2005). Language in context: Emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension. Neuroimage 25, 1002–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida, W., Dziobek, I., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, R. H., Friston, J. K., and Dolan, J. R. (2010). Cooperation and heterogeneity of the autistic mind. J. Neurosci. 30, 8815–8818. doi: 10.3389/fpsyh.2018.00752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalla, T., and Korman, J. (2018). Prior knowledge, episodic control and theory of mind in autism: Toward an integrative account of social cognition. Front. Psychol. 9:752. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, training, cerebellum, sequencing, mentalizing, narrative coherence, adults

Citation: Bylemans T, Heleven E, Baetens K, Deroost N, Baeken C and Van Overwalle F (2022) A narrative sequencing and mentalizing training for adults with autism: A pilot study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:941272. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.941272

Received: 11 May 2022; Accepted: 01 August 2022;
Published: 18 August 2022.

Edited by:

Giusy Olivito, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Viola Oldrati, Eugenio Medea (IRCCS), Italy
Chiara Ferrari, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Bylemans, Heleven, Baetens, Deroost, Baeken and Van Overwalle. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tom Bylemans, VG9tLmJ5bGVtYW5zQHZ1Yi5iZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.