Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 24 August 2022
Sec. Pathological Conditions
This article is part of the Research Topic Animal Models of Neuropsychiatric Disorders: Validity, Strengths, and Limitations View all 7 articles

Using animal models for the studies of schizophrenia and depression: The value of translational models for treatment and prevention

  • 1Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Pharmacology, Ribeirão Preto Medical School, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brazil

Animal models of psychiatric disorders have been highly effective in advancing the field, identifying circuits related to pathophysiology, and identifying novel therapeutic targets. In this review, we show how animal models, particularly those based on development, have provided essential information regarding circuits involved in disorders, disease progression, and novel targets for intervention and potentially prevention. Nonetheless, in recent years there has been a pushback, largely driven by the US National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), to shift away from animal models and instead focus on circuits in normal subjects. This has been driven primarily from a lack of discovery of new effective therapeutic targets, and the failure of targets based on preclinical research to show efficacy. We discuss why animal models of complex disorders, when strongly cross-validated by clinical research, are essential to understand disease etiology as well as pathophysiology, and direct new drug discovery. Issues related to shortcomings in clinical trial design that confound translation from animal models as well as the failure to take patient pharmacological history into account are proposed to be a source of the failure of what are likely effective compounds from showing promise in clinical trials.

Background

Animal models have been highly effective in advancing our understanding of psychiatric disorders in terms of pathophysiology, disease development, symptomatology, and in proposing novel targets for therapeutic intervention. However, it is also clear that animal models cannot replicate in a precise manner the complex syndrome present in humans. For this reason, animal models must be carefully selected based on parallels to human conditions and circuit-specific alterations that can lead to pathophysiology. In this sense, it is proposed that, to be meaningful, animal models should (1) possess similarity in symptoms, such that the observed abnormalities of the model have clinical correlates in patients (face validity); (2) replicate the neurobiological bases of the human condition (construct validity); and (3) predict responsiveness to drugs currently used to treat the disorder (predictive validity) (Howland et al., 2019).

The initial animal models to study mental disorders were based on lesions or acute drug administration. Such studies have been influential in the early days of investigation of disease states. For example, it was the studies of amphetamine and other dopaminergic agents that gave us insight into the role of dopamine in schizophrenia (Smythies et al., 1967; Snyder, 1973). This was further substantiated by studies showing that antipsychotic drugs were primarily dopamine antagonists (Creese et al., 1976; Seeman et al., 1976). However, such acute models often failed to replicate the clinical conditions. Therefore, whereas baseline levels of dopamine in schizophrenia are not substantially different from controls (Abi-Dargham et al., 2000), amphetamine induces a 20–30-fold increase in dopamine turnover (Joel et al., 1997). Furthermore, although lesion studies, such as prefrontal cortical lesions, can replicate some executive function deficits found in schizophrenia (Joel et al., 1997), it is also apparent that patients do not have a “hole” in their prefrontal cortex.

There are also issues related to the types of behavioral tests to evaluate the models. Amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion has been advanced as a means to correlate the hyperdopaminergic state present in schizophrenia. However, recent studies have shown that this is not an accurate model (van den Buuse, 2010). Studies in humans have shown that the psychosis-relevant hyper-responsivity of the dopamine system is located primarily in the associative striatum (Kegeles et al., 2010; McCutcheon et al., 2018). However, amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion is instead shown to be a hyperfunction of dopamine transmission in the ventromedial striatum (Kelly et al., 1975), which is involved more in affective processes than in salience/psychosis (Floresco, 2015). In addition, acute amphetamine does not induce changes related to the negative symptoms and cognitive deficits seen in patients with schizophrenia (Krystal et al., 2005). Another example would be the studies used to assess related features of depression. Thus, the forced swim test has been long used in evaluating antidepressant medications; however, this has come under scrutiny with regard to its specificity for a negative affective state and despair related to the immobility measured in the test. Recently, it was associated with stress coping and learning processes which antidepressants would increase active coping over passive coping but do not necessarily reflect the negative affect (Molendijk and de Kloet, 2015; Commons et al., 2017; Armario, 2021). The same can be extended to sucrose preference; although it has been used as a standard in depression research, the typical deficits seen are very small in magnitude (Willner et al., 1987; He et al., 2020). Further, it can be argued that deciding between two options one of which is rewarding is not an effective means to assess anhedonia; a better approach would be to see if the animal will expend effort to reach a desired reward, which should be more translatable. Therefore, using validated behavioral models for assessing animal models is fraught with problems.

Another approach would be to utilize comparisons, both with respect to model generation and evaluation, that are based on more readily translatable measures. This is consistent with the NIMH’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework for investigating mental illnesses, which has been inspiring novel animal modeling strategies to conceptually deviate from a deterministic principle and to incorporate clinically relevant and objectively measurable endophenotypes. Thus, given that most disorders show genetic association, it is assumed that the pathology is present from birth, and represents a more developmental alteration. Below we describe examples based on this reasoning applied to research using animal models for the study of schizophrenia and depression.

Making a case for the use of animal models in schizophrenia research

Schizophrenia often emerges in late adolescence/early adulthood and is characterized by a variety of symptoms classically divided into three groups: positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms (Kahn et al., 2015). Positive symptoms include hallucinations, delusions, and thought disorders. Negative symptoms include blunted affect, alogia, anhedonia, social withdrawal, and avolition. Cognitive dysfunction involves deficits in working memory, attention, and processing speed, and trouble focusing. Schizophrenia has multifactorial etiology that includes genetic risk and exposure to socioenvironmental adversities (Miller et al., 2001; Henriksen et al., 2017; Popovic et al., 2019; Berthelot et al., 2022). Animal models for the study of schizophrenia comprehensively use genetic models, prenatal interventions, pharmacological models, and stress-based protocols during prepubertal periods to recapitulate the disorder features (Lipska and Weinberger, 2000; Meyer and Feldon, 2010; Modinos et al., 2015; Gomes et al., 2019).

Weinberger and colleagues used information regarding smaller hippocampal volumes in patients with schizophrenia (Weinberger, 1987) to induce an early-life hippocampal lesion in rats (neonatal ventral hippocampus lesion—NVHL—model), which upon examination as adults showed states consistent with schizophrenia (Lipska et al., 1993). In the NVHL model, lesion of the ventral hippocampus at PD7 induces behavioral alterations later in life consistent with schizophrenia, such as increased responsivity to psychostimulants and stress, decreased sociability, and impairments in cognitive function and prepulse inhibition (Lipska et al., 1993; Chambers et al., 1996; Becker et al., 1999; Grecksch et al., 1999; Lipska et al., 2002; Tseng et al., 2009). However, the lesion extent can impact the outcomes in which a small lesion is more effective to recapitulate hippocampal hyperexcitability (Patricio O’Donnell, personal communication) and increased dopamine responsivity (Swerdlow et al., 2001).

While a hippocampal lesion may not temporally replicate the pathology seen in humans and does not take genetic predisposition into account, it did provide insights into novel developmental changes. To explore preexisting neurodevelopmental factors in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, we have used a developmental disruption model based on the administration of the DNA alkylating agent methylazoxymethanol acetate (MAM) to pregnant rats at gestational day 17. This date was chosen since it is functionally equivalent to the human second trimester, during which exposure to severe influenza infections can confer susceptibility of the offspring to schizophrenia (Barr et al., 1990). The result was that rats exposed to MAM in gestation showed characteristics as adults consistent with schizophrenia, including deficits in executive function behaviors, impaired latent inhibition and social interactions, and hyper-responsivity to amphetamine and the NMDA receptor antagonist phencyclidine (PCP) (Lodge and Grace, 2009). But importantly, the correspondence extended beyond behavior, showing anatomical changes (cortical thinning due to loss of neuropil and loss of parvalbumin-containing GABAergic interneurons), limbic hippocampal hyperactivity, and increased dopamine neuron population activity (Moore et al., 2006; Lodge and Grace, 2007; Lodge et al., 2009), all consistent with what has been reported in patients with schizophrenia (Modinos et al., 2015). In addition, similar to the human condition, most of the schizophrenia-like changes in the MAM model appear in late adolescence/early adulthood (Gomes et al., 2016).

An alternate approach was to utilize known risk factors in schizophrenia. Thus, it has been shown that severe influenza infections during gestation can increase the incidence of schizophrenia births (Barr et al., 1990). To approximate this in rodents, studies were done to model maternal immune activation, mainly through the Poly I:C gestational injection. Poly I:C is a synthetic double-stranded RNA, which approximates the conditions present with severe viral infections (Meyer and Feldon, 2012). In rodents, offspring of pregnant rats treated with Poly I:C express behavioral dysfunctions associated with schizophrenia (Wolff et al., 2011; Meehan et al., 2017), such as deficits in working memory and sensorimotor gating, and increased sensitivity to amphetamine, which can be alleviated with antipsychotic drug treatment (Knuesel et al., 2014; Reisinger et al., 2015). Neurobiological features are also presented, including reduced cortical thickness and hippocampal volume, and aberrant dopaminergic function (Knuesel et al., 2014; Meyer, 2014).

Genetics constitutes another domain of crucial risk factors for schizophrenia. Historically, genetic research on schizophrenia emphasized the search for “candidate gene(s)” in case-control studies. To date, over 1,000 genes have been tested but the overall results have been disappointing (Gejman et al., 2011; Mitchell et al., 2011; Henriksen et al., 2017), mainly due to issues of non-replication and/or lacking statistical power. Nonetheless, some of the most cited candidate genes have been targeted to generate genetic model animals, including disrupted-in-schizophrenia 1 (DISC1), neuregulin-1 (NRG1) and its receptor ERBB4, and dystobrevin-binding protein 1 (DTNBP1) (Johnstone et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2011). Although targeted genetic engineering (often deletion) of those “candidate genes” tend to phenotypically recapitulate behavioral alterations in schizophrenia to various extents, recent evidence from more advanced molecular genetic studies suggest that the genetic architecture in schizophrenia may involve only a little contribution from highly penetrant mutations (Henriksen et al., 2017). Indeed, recent genome-wide association studies support that the genetic risk in schizophrenia is conferred by a large number of common alleles (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014; Trubetskoy et al., 2022), which is extremely difficult to model in research animals due to difficulties to control allelic specificity and dosage (Mitchell et al., 2011).

Another prominent risk factor in schizophrenia is stress (van Winkel et al., 2008). Therefore, even in individuals without a family history of psychosis, childhood trauma is associated with a substantial increase in risk for schizophrenia (Popovic et al., 2019). Furthermore, in studies of children at risk for schizophrenia based on family history, Eve Johnstone and colleagues found that those children that were hyper-responsive to stress were more likely to transition to schizophrenia later in life (Miller et al., 2001). It was also found that clinical high-risk individuals have increased sensitivity to stress-induced dopamine release as same antipsychotic-naïve patients with schizophrenia which show antecedents of dopaminergic sensitivity to stress (Mizrahi et al., 2012). Drawing from these observations, we found that in the MAM model examined prepubertally, which is before the onset of the hyperdopaminergic state, the rats showed increased anxiety in the elevated plus maze, increased stress responsivity shown by heightened stress-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (Du and Grace, 2013; Zimmerman et al., 2013), and the basolateral amygdala (BLA), an area associated with stress and anxiety (Daviu et al., 2019), was firing at a constant high level (Du and Grace, 2016). Furthermore, if anxiety was alleviated in the MAM rats prepubertally either by administering diazepam, the pathology in the adult was circumvented (Du and Grace, 2013, 2016). Such insights provided a strong link between the human literature on early trauma and the MAM model. An increased responsivity to stress has also been described in the maternal immune activation and NVHL models (Lipska et al., 1993; Giovanoli et al., 2013).

Making a case for the use of animal models in depression research

Major depression represents the most prevalent psychiatric condition worldwide in which patients express symptoms, such as depressed mood, anhedonia, fatigue, weight changes, and suicidal idealization (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Otte et al., 2016). Adverse environmental factors, genetic background, and their interaction are the main risk factors for depression (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2013; Richter-Levin and Xu, 2018; Silva et al., 2021). Animal models for depression often involve stress exposure and evaluate anhedonia, helplessness, motivation, and despair behaviors, as behavioral features; some examples are the chronic mild stress (CMS) model, learned helplessness (LH), and maternal separation (Schmidt et al., 2011; Czéh et al., 2016; Maier and Seligman, 2016; Antoniuk et al., 2019; Kaufling, 2019; Su and Si, 2022).

Depression has been linked to hyperactivity of the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC) (Drevets et al., 2008b; Pizzagalli and Roberts, 2022), which is analogous to the infralimbic portion of the prefrontal cortex (ilPFC) in rodents (Laubach et al., 2018). All effective treatments were found to attenuate this sgACC hyperactivity (Drevets et al., 2008b), including local deep brain stimulation which is an effective intervention in treatment-resistant depression (Mayberg et al., 2005). Mimicking the human condition, animal models relevant for depression show increased ilPFC activity (Drevets et al., 2008b; Moreines et al., 2017b).

Preclinical studies have contributed to creating a strong base of circuit dysfunction, behavioral dysregulation, and antidepressants mechanism. For example, CMS commonly causes rodents to show changes related to anhedonia, amotivational behaviors, and decreased dopamine system activity (Willner et al., 1987; Belujon and Grace, 2017; Antoniuk et al., 2019). In addition, either direct ilPFC activation in normal rats or in rats exposed to CMS express this hypodopaminergic state (Patton et al., 2013; Chang and Grace, 2014) and ilPFC inactivation restores the normative dopamine activity in rats subjected to CMS (Moreines et al., 2017b), suggesting a relevant translational circuit dysfunction of the human condition.

ilPFC activity is associated with BLA excitability in rodents (Rosenkranz and Grace, 2002). Increased amygdala activity is another marked neurobiological feature in clinical depression (Drevets et al., 2008a; Yang et al., 2010; Murray et al., 2011) and it correlates with symptom severity (Mingtian et al., 2012). We found that the hypodopaminergic state caused by ilPFC activation is prevented by BLA inactivation (Patton et al., 2013). It may suggest a common circuit dysfunction feature involving the sgACC (in humans; or the ilPFC in rodents) and amygdala hyperexcitability (Figure 1A). This is important especially because these areas are pivotal in regulating stress responses. While the ilPFC drives the upregulation of BLA activity in response to stress, the plPFC has been associated with a decreased stress response by downregulating the BLA excitability (Rosenkranz and Grace, 2001, 2002). These have been associated with the opposite roles of the plPFC and ilPFC in modulating stress responses, reducing and facilitating, respectively (Lacroix et al., 2000; Radley et al., 2006; Tavares et al., 2009).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The circuit disruption underlying the hypodopaminergic state is reversed by ketamine. (A) ilPFC hyperactivity increases BLA activity. BLA excitatory drive induces RE hyperexcitability which also may be a resultant of ilPFC inputs through TRN. RE projections disrupt the vHIP activity and its connectivity with NAc. Impairment of vHIP activity decreases the GABAergic tone from NAc to VP. VP excitability is resulting from a decreased NAc inhibitory drive and increased BLA excitatory drive. The increased inhibitory output of VP inhibits VTA and leads to a hypodopaminergic state. (B) Ketamine normalizes the hypodopaminergic state by downregulating the BLA activity in response to ilPFC activity restoration. The inputs coming from BLA and ilPFC are proposed to regulate the RE activity normalization. RE may represent one important neurobiological pathway underlying the reestablishment of the vHIP and NAc connectivity. Stabilization of NAc and BLA activity decreases the activity of VP inhibitory inputs to the VTA which ultimately restores dopaminergic activity. ilPFC, the infralimbic portion of the medial prefrontal cortex; BLA, basolateral amygdala; TRN, thalamic reticular nucleus; RE, reuniens thalamic nucleus; vHIP, ventral hippocampus; NAc, nucleus accumbens; VP, ventral pallidum; VTA, ventral tegmental area. Dotted lines, decreased pathway drive.

The dysfunction of the plPFC, which is analogous to the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) in humans (Laubach et al., 2018), is also a feature described in animal models for depression. dACC disruption has been described in the human condition (Pizzagalli and Roberts, 2022) which may suggest that the inability to regulate stress response can lead to neuropathological conditions. We found that disrupting the ability of the plPFC to regulate stress during peripuberty cause animals to be more susceptible to helplessness and a hypodopaminergic state during adulthood (Uliana et al., 2020). It shows the importance of stress regulation during neurodevelopment in which unregulated stress response may lead to adult neuropathological conditions.

Importance of reverse translation approach in animal models: Neuroimaging and epidemiological findings as valuable resources

Compared to behavioral correlates for psychiatric disorders, neurobiological alterations directly observed in patients appear to be more objectively ascertainable in research animals. These endpoints have been increasingly employed in preclinical research to complement behavioral measures. With the technological advancement in human genomics, proteomics, metabolomics, and neuroimaging, our abilities to use non-invasive approaches to assess putative biomarkers have been markedly improved. Thus, it is imperative to validate existing models and develop novel models based on “reverse translation” of clinical findings to achieve optimal model construct validity. Notably, according to the NIMH’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative, one approach would be to focus animal research on neurobiological substrates relevant to normal brain functions, while being agnostic to categorical disease states. A key assumption of the RDoC framework is that psychiatric disorders arise from the deviation of essential brain functions that are defined as “domains” and “constructs,” which are controlled by neurobiological subsystems such as circuits, molecules, and genes. Thus, one benefit of the RDoC initiative is the potential to identify transdiagnostic brain dysfunctions and abnormal cognitive constructs, which could lead to insights fundamental to the development of mechanism-based targeted interventions. In addition to the emphasis on functional dimensions rather than compound disease states, the RDoC framework also explicitly advocate two equally important elements, development and environment, with circuit impact of particular environmental stressors being a central research question (Insel et al., 2010). Consistent with the RDoC framework, we argue that adolescent developmental stress may represent an effective “shared modeling strategy” to study the dopamine circuit disruptions involved in depression and schizophrenia (Grace, 2016). However, overly emphasizing transdiagnostic circuit dysfunctions could have a drawback, as this “top-down” approach could fail in its ability to understand a specifically perturbed system or rare etiological factors unique to certain disorders (Ross and Margolis, 2019) or disorders that impact multiple interacting systems, in which studying each in isolation would miss important data relevant to the disease state. This is where well-validated animal models specifically designed to study certain disorders are valuable.

The concept of “reverse translation” can be applied at various levels in preclinical psychiatry research, but traditionally it is mainly implemented via behavioral and/or functional characterization of risk genes. While this bottom-up approach has been undeniably useful for certain disorders with a relatively clear genetic basis, for psychiatric disorders that involve complex genetic liability and potential environmental modulation (e.g., schizophrenia and depression), it could be infeasible to comprehensively model their vast and often conflicting genetic basis (Henriksen et al., 2017). Indeed, in schizophrenia more than 200 risk loci have been identified by genome-wide association studies, which in theory could implicate even more risk genes. Moreover, schizophrenia disease risk is contributed by many common genetic variants each with a minor effect (Trubetskoy et al., 2022). Thus, at the current knowledge state, it has been argued that phenotypical impacts of common variants are difficult to model and interpret (McCarthy et al., 2014).

On the other hand, reverse translation from system-level human findings and epidemiological associations represents promising alternative strategies. One example of such “bedside-to-bench” translation is the use of neuroimaging findings to test animal models of dopamine dysregulations in schizophrenia. Two most replicated and convergent neuroimaging findings in schizophrenia are elevated striatal presynaptic dopamine function (McCutcheon et al., 2019) and structural and functional alterations in the hippocampus and the surrounding medial temporal lobes (MTL) (Lieberman et al., 2018). Although robust, these human findings are from independent studies, and whether and how these alterations are mechanistically linked to disease pathogenesis are unclear. Therefore, animal models reproducing these pathophysiological features are necessary to test for a causal relationship and/or other interactions. For example, as described previously, rodents prenatally exposed to MAM represent a promising research tool as it integrates those human findings (Modinos et al., 2015). In particular, the abnormal behavioral phenotypes of the MAM model are developed through extended trajectories consistent with the natural history of schizophrenia, with alterations related to positive symptoms that tend to manifest later compared to social withdrawal and cognitive deficits (Gomes et al., 2016). In addition, in utero MAM exposure is known to induce widespread, long-lasting DNA (Perez et al., 2016) and histone hypermethylation (Maćkowiak et al., 2014), which are factors supported by recent epigenetics research to be potential molecular links between genetic and environmental risk factors (Magwai et al., 2021). This gives the MAM model additional construct validity and valuable research potential, such as to identify hypermethylated genes that contribute to distinct behavioral or circuit alterations [example studies, see: Gulchina et al. (2017)]. Other reverse translation processes in schizophrenia research have been applied to generate dopamine D2 receptor overexpression and NVHL models, and their corresponding human neuroimaging findings have been reviewed extensively elsewhere (Tseng et al., 2009; Abi-Dargham, 2020). Discoveries from these models will not only causally confirm important neuroimaging findings, but also generate important testable hypotheses back to clinical research.

In addition to neuroimaging findings, epidemiological associations also represent a useful resource for reverse translation. For example, in schizophrenia, there is now compelling evidence indicating positive associations between adult psychosis risks and early exposure to diverse environmental risks (Millan et al., 2016), including age-dependent exposure to psychosocial stress, immune activation, and nutritional deficiency (Robinson and Bergen, 2021). Informed by these findings, the “epidemiology-based models” have crucial importance to advance preclinical research (Meyer and Feldon, 2010). Unlike the neuroimaging-informed models discussed above, these models tend to be based on experimental manipulations that closely mimic human environmental exposure, aiming to directly test causality implicated by human disease risk associations. These models are providing novel etiological and therapeutic insights for schizophrenia, especially on the neuroimmunological and neuroendocrine mechanisms. Furthermore, schizophrenia-associated gene and environment interaction is also being illuminated by these models, making them uniquely useful research tools to study the ontogeny of altered brain functions and possible early interventions (Meyer and Feldon, 2010).

In summary, the reverse translation of human findings into model animals represents an indispensable approach to determining if an altered neural system or environmental factor is causally linked to specific diseases. Although the RDoC framework has detailed “constructs” and units of analysis aiming to identify transdiagnostic biomarkers, it does not necessarily preclude the use of existing animal models initially designed to study a particular disease state. The key is to re-evaluate an animal model’s construct validity based on its ability to reflect the current understanding of the etiology of human disorders. In this aspect, non-invasive neuroimaging and epidemiologic risk associations have emerged to be useful resources, as they generate high-throughput data directly from human observations to update etiological theories and pathophysiological features of psychiatric disorders. These are especially important for complex disorders such as schizophrenia, for which at present there are no characterizing biomarkers and genetic phenotypes. Thus, for schizophrenia, if a model addresses a collection of consistent changes as observed in humans, ranging from risk exposure to circuit disruption and behavioral alterations, there is a higher probability that the model is of greater construct validity.

Integrating disease risk factors for the study of schizophrenia and depression

Stress as a major risk factor

Stress is a common environmental risk factor that can predispose individuals to various major psychiatric disorders, including depression and schizophrenia (Lupien et al., 2009; Kahn et al., 2015; Otte et al., 2016). History of adverse events across the lifespan was present in patients with depression (Richter-Levin and Xu, 2018; LeMoult et al., 2020). Indeed, stress experienced during early phases of development, such as neonatal and childhood period, also is associated with the emergence of depression later in life which may be due to the impact on the neurodevelopment trajectories of several brain areas involved with stress regulation (Liu, 2017; LeMoult et al., 2020). In animal research, stress-based models either during adulthood or early in life are widely used to approximate behavioral features of depression, such as anhedonia, amotivation, and helplessness (Pizzagalli and Roberts, 2022). These models also recapitulate core circuit dysfunction associated with depression, such as down-regulation of the dopaminergic system activity (Belujon and Grace, 2017; Kaufling, 2019). This common alteration in rodents and humans is proposed to drive depression-related behaviors (Reddy et al., 1992; Lambert et al., 2000; Nestler and Carlezon, 2006).

Schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that usually manifests in late adolescence/early adulthood (Kahn et al., 2015; Meyer and Lee, 2019). Indeed, early life stress is a common experience in individuals who later transition to psychosis (Holtzman et al., 2012; Croft et al., 2019; Popovic et al., 2019). Different from the stress schedule in the abovementioned models to study depression, stress during neurodevelopmental periods associated with cortical maturation, such as adolescence, can lead to VTA dopamine system hyperfunction and behaviors relevant to schizophrenia (Gomes et al., 2019), which corresponds to the clinical association between childhood trauma history and adult risks for psychosis conversion (Popovic et al., 2019). Furthermore, in the MAM neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia, it is proposed that the rats are more susceptible to stress when it is experienced during peripuberty (Zimmerman et al., 2013; Du and Grace, 2016), and their transition to adult hyperdopaminergic states can be prevented by alleviating the stress responses with an anxiolytic agent (Du and Grace, 2013) or environmental enrichment (Zhu and Grace, 2021). Drawing from these data, we postulated that sufficiently strong stressors in this prepubertal period could lead to the emergence of pathology in normal rats. This was indeed the case: exposing normal rats during this prepubertal state to combined stressors (daily footshock and three sessions of restraint stress over a 10-day period) could indeed lead to parvalbumin neuron loss, hippocampal hyperactivity, a hyperdopaminergic state, and cognitive deficits in these rats as adults (Gomes and Grace, 2017; Gomes et al., 2020). It demonstrates that the neurodevelopmental timing in which the adversity occurs may be determinant to the neuropathology, as stress is also a factor for other psychiatric conditions, with stress during the neonatal or adult period being related to depression emergence (Kendler et al., 1999; Andersen, 2015; Herbison et al., 2017; Silva et al., 2021). These convergent preclinical datasets have generated insights into the neurobiological origins of dopamine dysregulation in schizophrenia and the developmental interaction between environmental risk factors and antecedent predisposition.

Clinical evidence indicates that genetic risk for schizophrenia predicts structural and functional deficits in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Abé et al., 2021). We found that interfering with stress regulation by lesioning the prelimbic portion of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (plPFC) rendered normal rats more susceptible to more minor stressors (Gomes and Grace, 2017). This led to the postulate that a pathology related to deficits in stress regulation could be the predisposing factor that renders an individual at risk for the development of schizophrenia (Figure 2). Indeed, what this suggests is that genetic predisposition may not in itself lead to schizophrenia; instead the predisposition may lead to an inadequate regulation of stress either through prefrontal cortical disruption or other means, which, when combined with early stress, can lead to the emergence of schizophrenia (Gomes et al., 2019). This is better correlated with the human condition, in which identical twins with the same genetic predisposition only show a 50% concordance for schizophrenia (Cardno and Gottesman, 2000), and at least 1/3 of patients with schizophrenia do not have a family history of psychosis (Kahn et al., 2015). Therefore, animal models have been instrumental in linking genetic predisposition with life stressors as etiological factors in schizophrenia. The interaction between these factors may underlie the unregulated stress responses that precipitate pathological conditions, which potentially drive individual differences in the susceptibility to stress-related disorders. Although these variables have long been suggested in clinical association studies to modulate disease risks, how they contribute to the pathophysiological processes of diseases is unclear. Clinical research assessing stress-related risk factors for psychiatric conditions tends to be retrospective in nature, so that it typically precludes detailed segregation of variables, for example, the stressor type and intensity. However, these stress-specific characteristics could distinctively precipitate a particular neuropathological condition.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. plPFC-BLA stress circuit disruption during adolescence leads to hyperdopaminergic states. Unregulated stress response by plPFC hypofunction drives increased BLA activity. The increased BLA inputs to vHIP during peripuberty underlie the reduction of PV + cells which ultimately induces a vHIP hyperactivity. Its projections activate the NAc and as a GABAergic nucleus it generates an inhibitory drive to VP; a reduction in the activity of GABAergic VP protections to the VTA produces a hyperdopaminergic state. plPFC, prelimbic portion of the medial prefrontal cortex; BLA, basolateral amygdala; vHIP, ventral hippocampus; NAc, nucleus accumbens; VP, ventral pallidum; VTA, ventral tegmental area, PV, parvalbumin. Dotted lines, decreased pathway drive.

Preclinical studies have significantly advanced our understanding of how stress-specific variables influence the neurobiological/circuit mechanisms underlying complex behaviors involved in depression and schizophrenia (Lipska and Weinberger, 2000; Meyer and Feldon, 2010; Czéh et al., 2016; Muir et al., 2018). These animal models permit studies of potential prevention and therapeutic interventions, along with the ability to investigate early biomarkers. Although each single animal model certainly cannot recapitulate all the aspects of complex human disorders, they can bring unique and often unexpected insights that are the building blocks for novel therapies. Also, the clinical behavioral and neurobiological circuit dysfunction differ on the individual level (Otte et al., 2016; Su and Si, 2022), which reflects that even human conditions are not homogeneous. Therefore, regarding protocols used to understand some aspects of behavioral and neurobiological features of depression and schizophrenia, stress seems to be a mutual variable in both rodent models and the human condition.

The effect of stress can change over time, and the outcomes depend on when the stress is presented. For example, repeated stress acutely increases VTA dopamine system activity (Valenti et al., 2011b). However, repeated stress causes rats to show a hypodopaminergic state 1 week after the end of stress (Chang and Grace, 2014; Gomes et al., 2020). This switch in the dopaminergic state is likely due to cumulative neurobiological disruptions in the brain areas involved in stress regulation. The acute effect of stress is dependent on ventral hippocampus (vHip) activation (Valenti et al., 2011b) which drives the hyperdopaminergic state by activating the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and disinhibition of ventral pallidum GABAergic projection to the VTA (Grace, 2016). On the other hand, as mentioned above, the increased activity in the ilPFC underlies the hypodopaminergic state in rats subjected to CMS (Moreines et al., 2017b). The ilPFC excitability observed in animal models of affective dysregulation may disrupt the vHip activity indirectly via the reuniens thalamic nucleus (Dolleman-van der Weel and Witter, 2020), which also has been shown to regulate amotivational behaviors and dopaminergic activity (Zimmerman and Grace, 2016; Kafetzopoulos et al., 2018). Rats expressing helplessness have a disruption of long-term potentiation (LTP) in vHip-NAc plasticity (Belujon and Grace, 2014). In normal rats, vHip high-frequency stimulation induces LTP in NAc which is switched to long-term depression in helplessness (Belujon and Grace, 2014). It reflects abnormal vHip modulation of synaptic plasticity in the NAc. It can ultimately contribute to the decreased reward-related medial VTA dopamine system activity observed in helplessness rats (Belujon and Grace, 2014, 2017). All the circuit dysfunction information above takes advantage of animal models based on stress exposure to study changes related to depression, which cannot be observed in naïve animals.

Early life stress, such as childhood trauma, is associated with an increased risk for schizophrenia and depression later in life (Redman et al., 2017; Croft et al., 2019; Devi et al., 2019; Popovic et al., 2019; LeMoult et al., 2020). Stress early in life can be particularly detrimental because individuals are still undergoing neurodevelopmental changes (Bouwmeester et al., 2002; Marco et al., 2011; Gee et al., 2013b; Spear, 2013; Casey et al., 2019), and it can profoundly affect brain area maturation and connectivity (Bath et al., 2016; Johnson et al., 2018; Herzberg and Gunnar, 2020). One area implicated in the pathophysiology of depression and schizophrenia is the PFC (Perlstein et al., 2001; Rogers et al., 2004; Drevets et al., 2008a; Selemon and Zecevic, 2015), which is the last region to reach maturation during early adulthood (Gogtay et al., 2004; Davey et al., 2008). Dysfunctional activity in the PFC areas is observed in patients with depression and schizophrenia with a history of childhood adversity (Kim et al., 2019; Popovic et al., 2019; González-Acosta et al., 2020; Silva et al., 2021), which may be a long-term consequence of unregulated stress response. In the context that PFC is not adequately developed to regulate stress, it can allow the upregulation of amygdala activity early in life and affect their connectivity. In particular, early stress appears to induce precocious development of BLA-plPFC connectivity. Indeed, we have shown in rats that peripubertal stress induces precocious development of plasticity in the BLA projections to the plPFC (Uliana et al., 2021). This is consistent with clinical data showing accelerated connectivity between the amygdala and PFC in individuals with a history of maternal deprivation (Gee et al., 2013a). This early abnormal connectivity would interfere with the normal development of stress coping responses, which enables early coping skills at the cost of inefficient stress regulation in adulthood (Callaghan and Tottenham, 2016; Herzberg and Gunnar, 2020). Therefore, it may represent a common circuit dysregulation in rodents and humans after early life adversities (Gee et al., 2013a; Bath et al., 2016; Callaghan and Tottenham, 2016), which highlights the importance of using animal models to elucidate neurobiological features in the human conditions.

Genetic contribution to stress susceptibility

Schizophrenia and depression tend to run in the same families (Hovens et al., 2010), which suggests a common genetic link (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2013). Depression and schizophrenia share several genetic variations associated with these psychiatric disorders (Brainstorm Consortium et al., 2018). Individuals at ultra-high risk for schizophrenia that do not transition to psychosis show an increased incidence of affective disorders and depression in adulthood (Lin et al., 2015; Chan et al., 2018). The differences in susceptibility may be a product of interaction between genetic and environmental factors during a specific neurodevelopmental period (Grace, 2016; Meyer and Lee, 2019). Stress and its regulation may have a key role in this divergence, particularly depending on the developmental period (Green et al., 2010; McLaughlin et al., 2010; Andersen, 2015; Brenhouse and Bath, 2019). For example, the same repeated stress protocol induces a hyperdopaminergic schizophrenia-like state in adulthood when applied to adolescent rats and a hypodopaminergic depression-like state when applied to adult rats (Gomes et al., 2020). This reflects that both states may share the same risk factor with the age of exposure being a determinant of the outcome. Indeed, factors that impact stress regulation across the lifespan are essential for the neuropathological consequences (Lupien et al., 2009; McEwen, 2017; Shaw et al., 2020). The interference with plPFC regulation of stress during the peripubertal period leads to increased susceptibility to helplessness and hypodopaminergic state in adult rats (Uliana et al., 2020). However, the plPFC disruption during the same period when combined with mild stressor exposure leads to an adult hyperdopaminergic state (Gomes and Grace, 2017). The impairment in stress regulation may represent a preexisting vulnerability at the individual level in which the environmental factors would be determinant for the neuropathological state later in life. Alternately, if the early life trauma is sufficiently extreme, this can lead to adult pathology even without a family history (Devi et al., 2019; Popovic et al., 2019). We posit that the common developmental factor that underlies risk for multiple psychiatric disorders is either extreme peripubertal stress in normal individuals (e.g., severe childhood trauma) or developmentally driven peripubertal hyper-responsivity to mild stressors (e.g., stressful life events) in those at genetic risk or with other predispositions. Thus, adult stress resilience/susceptibility has been proposed to be attained through a developmental process (Nederhof and Schmidt, 2012), which can be critically shaped by early experience, brain development, and maturation (Andersen, 2015; Peña et al., 2019).

The preexistent genetic vulnerability to major psychiatric conditions may determine the impact of environmental influence during neurodevelopment. Animal models to study disease states are essential to study each factor’s contribution and their interaction in the disease pathogenesis. Although the animal models for depression and schizophrenia are important for new therapeutic discoveries, preventive interventions and early markers represent a promising and necessary field of study. The early detection of markers for specific neuropsychiatric conditions can inform the clinician of the timing for preventive measures, including targeting disease transition prevention or alleviating the severity of a future disease state. Therefore, animal models are a crucial tool to evaluate these aspects for better clinic and preclinic crosstalk.

Not our fault! Animal models should not be blamed for failures in drug development

While the back-and-forth translation of animal models and clinical studies has demonstrated value in advancing our understanding of disease states, there has been a move advanced by the US National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and others that eschews the use of animal models of human diseases. A substantial amount of this is due to the failure of animal models to provide novel targets for pharmacological interventions that are effective in schizophrenia. Thus, animal models have advanced a circuit in which loss of hippocampal parvalbumin interneurons leads to hippocampal hyperactivity and, via the striatum and ventral pallidum, disinhibition of the dopamine system and a resultant hyperdopaminergic state (Grace, 2016; Sonnenschein et al., 2020). This circuit is supported by strong clinical evidence, showing parvalbumin neuron loss in the hippocampus (Zhang and Reynolds, 2002; Konradi et al., 2011) and hippocampal hyperactivity (Schobel et al., 2013) that occurs in concert with increased dopamine function (Grace, 2012; Modinos et al., 2021). However, when pharmaceutical companies have targeted this circuit, the resultant agents have failed to demonstrate efficacy despite promising results from early trials. Thus, Lilly developed a metabotropic glutamate receptor 2/3 (mGluR2/3) agonist to attenuate glutamate hyperfunction (Rorick-Kehn et al., 2007); Roche developed a glycine uptake inhibitor to potentiate NMDA receptor drive presumably of the remaining parvalbumin interneurons (Alberati et al., 2012), and Pfizer developed a phosphodiesterase type 10 (PDE10) inhibitor to attenuate hippocampal overdrive of the striatum (Menniti et al., 2020). While each of these agents showed promise in animal models and early phase trials, each failed in the large multi-center phase three trials. The consequence is that many in industry and the NIMH concluded that animal models are not good predictors of the therapeutic efficacy of drugs. However, our studies have shown such a conclusion to be short-sighted; i.e., it is not the animal models that are failing, but instead a reliance on a pharmaceutical trial model that has been effective at identifying new dopaminergic modulatory drugs, but in all likelihood will fail to identify an effective drug acting on a novel target. This is due to the trial design—i.e., in these large trials, the companies typically rely heavily on chronic patients with schizophrenia that have been treated previously for many years with standard D2 antagonist drugs. As per protocol, the patients are withdrawn from their current medications for 1–2 weeks to wash out residual drug, before the novel agent is tested. However, this is problematic—treatment with a D2 antagonist will induce DA supersensitivity due to the system increasing in the number of D2 receptors to compensate for the blockade (Servonnet and Samaha, 2020). However, when these receptors are uncovered by washout of the drug, there remains a strong dopamine supersensitivity. As a result, when withdrawing a D2 antagonist, the only agent that can restore efficacy is another D2 antagonist. Indeed, we found that if we treated a MAM rat with the antipsychotic agent haloperidol for just 3 weeks and withdrew the drug for 1 week, a novel agent that was highly effective in the untreated MAM rat was now without efficacy (Gill et al., 2014)–i.e., by treating with haloperidol, we replaced hippocampal driven dopamine hyperactivity with a postsynaptic D2 supersensitivity. As a result, we believe that several potentially highly effective agents were prematurely dismissed not because the animal models were ineffective, but because the clinical trial design was not effective at evaluating agents with novel targets. Indeed, our studies have suggested that the mGluR2/3 agonist pomaglumetad should be a highly effective antipsychotic agent with minimal or no D2 antagonism-related side effects (Sonnenschein and Grace, 2020). Interestingly, although pomaglumetad failed to show efficacy in phase 3 trials, later analyses indicated this drug was effective in some patients, particularly those who were in the earlier disease stages (Kinon et al., 2015). These findings indicate that the efficacy of pomaglumetad in schizophrenia may depend on treatment history and disease progression.

One important issue is the necessity to use animal models of disorders to study the effectiveness of novel compounds. This is because a drug that targets a deficit state may not produce measurable results in a normal system, or the results may be altered in nature. One example would be the use of antipsychotic drugs. Administering antipsychotic drugs to normal rats has provided some insights into mechanisms, such as the importance of D2 blockade in their therapeutic action (Carlsson and Lindqvist, 1963) and the correlation between D2 affinity and clinical potency (Seeman et al., 1976). However, the drugs are likely not working the same in normal animals. For example, we had shown that it requires 3–4 weeks of treatment with first- or second-generation antipsychotic drugs to induce dopamine neuron depolarization block (Bunney and Grace, 1978; Chiodo and Bunney, 1983), which is proposed to be the therapeutic action via decreasing the pathologically high population activity of the dopamine system (Grace and Gomes, 2019). However, this is not consistent with the clinical picture, in which antipsychotic drugs can exert clinical efficacy within days of administration (Agid et al., 2006). When antipsychotic drugs are first administered to rats, the first effect is to increase dopamine neuron population activity (i.e., the number of neurons firing). However, in the MAM model, there is already an increased population activity at baseline (Lodge and Grace, 2007); now when the antipsychotic drug is administered to the MAM rat, it acts upon an already up-regulated system to rapidly induce depolarization block (Valenti et al., 2011a), unlike that observed in the control animal. The same is likely true for the rapid and sustained actions of the novel antidepressant ketamine. In our learned helplessness depression model in rats, ketamine rapidly reverses dopamine neuron down-regulation and hippocampal-accumbens long-term depression (LTD); an effect that is not observed in naïve animals (Belujon and Grace, 2014; proposed circuit in Figure 1B). In addition, the potential antidepressant effects of quetiapine on the dopamine system were again only observed in an animal model for depression; no consistent similar effect is seen in naïve animals (Moreines et al., 2017a). Therefore, while drug administration to naïve animals may provide some insight into targets and mechanism of specific behavior and circuit, administering it to a well-developed animal model is essential to provide insights into neurobiological mechanism leading to its therapeutic action in a pathological context.

Conclusion

In summary, whereas psychiatric disorders are traditionally modeled via acute pharmacological manipulation of neurotransmission and behavioral characterization, increasing animal models are now based on reverse translation of genetic association, neuroimaging, and epidemiological findings. By complementing behavioral readouts with clinically relevant findings, animal models based on diverse reverse translation strategies can have great construct validity. Notably, no model is “perfect” nor sufficient to capture all aspects of psychopathology (Lipska and Weinberger, 2000). Thus, it is important to emphasize clinically relevant dysfunctions, use multidisciplinary approaches, and rely on collaborative efforts to tackle complex disorders.

Author contributions

DU constructed the figures. All authors were involved in the concept, writing, citations, and finalization of the document and approved the submitted version.

Funding

AG was funded by US National Institutes of Health (NIH; MH57440). FG received a São Paulo Research Foundation Young Investigator grant (FAPESP—2018/17597-3). AG has received consulting fees from Alkermes, Lundbeck, Takeda, Roche, Lyra, Concert, Merck, and SynAgile and research funding from Lundbeck. Lundbeck was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abé, C., Petrovic, P., Ossler, W., Thompson, W. H., Liberg, B., Song, J., et al. (2021). Genetic risk for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia predicts structure and function of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 46, E441–E450. doi: 10.1503/jpn.200165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abi-Dargham, A. (2020). From “bedside” to “bench” and back: A translational approach to studying dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 110, 174–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abi-Dargham, A., Rodenhiser, J., Printz, D., Zea-Ponce, Y., Gil, R., Kegeles, L. S., et al. (2000). Increased baseline occupancy of D2 receptors by dopamine in schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 8104–8109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.14.8104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agid, O., Seeman, P., and Kapur, S. (2006). The “delayed onset” of antipsychotic action–an idea whose time has come and gone. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 31, 93–100.

Google Scholar

Alberati, D., Moreau, J. L., Lengyel, J., Hauser, N., Mory, R., Borroni, E., et al. (2012). Glycine reuptake inhibitor RG1678: A pharmacologic characterization of an investigational agent for the treatment of schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology 62, 1152–1161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, (5th Edn). Virginia, US: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Andersen, S. L. (2015). Exposure to early adversity: Points of cross-species translation that can lead to improved understanding of depression. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 477–491. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antoniuk, S., Bijata, M., Ponimaskin, E., and Wlodarczyk, J. (2019). Chronic unpredictable mild stress for modeling depression in rodents: Meta-analysis of model reliability. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 99, 101–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armario, A. (2021). The forced swim test: Historical, conceptual and methodological considerations and its relationship with individual behavioral traits. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 74–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, C. E., Mednick, S. A., and Munk-Jorgensen, P. (1990). Exposure to influenza epidemics during gestation and adult schizophrenia. A 40-year study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47, 869–874. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810210077012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bath, K., Manzano-Nieves, G., and Goodwill, H. (2016). Early life stress accelerates behavioral and neural maturation of the hippocampus in male mice. Horm. Behav. 82, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, A., Grecksch, G., Bernstein, H. G., Höllt, V., and Bogerts, B. (1999). Social behaviour in rats lesioned with ibotenic acid in the hippocampus: Quantitative and qualitative analysis. Psychopharmacology 144, 333–338. doi: 10.1007/s002130051015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., and Grace, A. A. (2014). Restoring mood balance in depression: Ketamine reverses deficit in dopamine-dependent synaptic plasticity. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 927–936. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., and Grace, A. A. (2017). Dopamine System Dysregulation in Major Depressive Disorders. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 1036–1046. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthelot, N., Garon-Bissonnette, J., Jomphe, V., Doucet-Beaupré, H., Bureau, A., and Maziade, M. (2022). Childhood trauma may increase risk of psychosis and mood disorder in genetically high-risk children and adolescents by enhancing the accumulation of risk indicators. Schizophr. Bull. Open 3:sgac017. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgac017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouwmeester, H., Wolterink, G., and van Ree, J. M. (2002). Neonatal development of projections from the basolateral amygdala to prefrontal, striatal, and thalamic structures in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 442, 239–249. doi: 10.1002/cne.10084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainstorm Consortium, V., Bulik-Sullivan, B., Finucane, H. K., Walters, R. K., Bras, J., et al. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360:eaa8757. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brenhouse, H. C., and Bath, K. G. (2019). Bundling the haystack to find the needle: Challenges and opportunities in modeling risk and resilience following early life stress. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 54:100768. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2019.100768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunney, B. S., and Grace, A. A. (1978). Acute and chronic haloperidol treatment: Comparison of effects on nigral dopaminergic cell activity. Life Sci. 23, 1715–1727. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(78)90471-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callaghan, B. L., and Tottenham, N. (2016). The Stress Acceleration Hypothesis: Effects of early-life adversity on emotion circuits and behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 7, 76–81. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardno, A. G., and Gottesman, I. I. (2000). Twin studies of schizophrenia: From bow-and-arrow concordances to star wars Mx and functional genomics. Am. J. Med. Genet. 97, 12–17.

Google Scholar

Carlsson, A., and Lindqvist, M. (1963). Effect of chlorpromazine or haloperidol on formation of 3methoxytyramine and normetanephrine in mouse brain. Acta Pharmacol. Toxicol. 20, 140–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0773.1963.tb01730.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Heller, A. S., Gee, D. G., and Cohen, A. O. (2019). Development of the emotional brain. Neurosci. Lett. 693, 29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, R. A., Moore, J., McEvoy, J. P., and Levin, E. D. (1996). Cognitive effects of neonatal hippocampal lesions in a rat model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 15, 587–594. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00132-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, C. T., Abdin, E., Subramaniam, M., Tay, S. A., Lim, L. K., and Verma, S. (2018). Two-Year Clinical and Functional Outcomes of an Asian Cohort at Ultra-High Risk of Psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 9:758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. H., and Grace, A. A. (2014). Amygdala-ventral pallidum pathway decreases dopamine activity after chronic mild stress in rats. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiodo, L. A., and Bunney, B. S. (1983). Typical and atypical neuroleptics: Differential effects of chronic administration on the activity of A9 and A10 midbrain dopaminergic neurons. J. Neurosci. 3, 1607–1619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-08-01607.1983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commons, K. G., Cholanians, A. B., Babb, J. A., and Ehlinger, D. G. (2017). The Rodent Forced Swim Test Measures Stress-Coping Strategy, Not Depression-like Behavior. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 8, 955–960. doi: 10.1021/acschemneuro.7b00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creese, I., Burt, D. R., and Snyder, S. H. (1976). Dopamine receptor binding predicts clinical and pharmacological potencies of antischizophrenic drugs. Science 192, 481–483. doi: 10.1126/science.3854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croft, J., Heron, J., Teufel, C., Cannon, M., Wolke, D., Thompson, A., et al. (2019). Association of Trauma Type, Age of Exposure, and Frequency in Childhood and Adolescence With Psychotic Experiences in Early Adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry 76, 79–86. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2013). Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: A genome-wide analysis. Lancet 381, 1371–1379. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62129-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czéh, B., Fuchs, E., Wiborg, O., and Simon, M. (2016). Animal models of major depression and their clinical implications. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 293–310. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davey, C. G., Yücel, M., and Allen, N. B. (2008). The emergence of depression in adolescence: Development of the prefrontal cortex and the representation of reward. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daviu, N., Bruchas, M. R., Moghaddam, B., Sandi, C., and Beyeler, A. (2019). Neurobiological links between stress and anxiety. Neurobiol. Stress 11:100191. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devi, F., Shahwan, S., Teh, W. L., Sambasivam, R., Zhang, Y. J., Lau, Y. W., et al. (2019). The prevalence of childhood trauma in psychiatric outpatients. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry 18:15. doi: 10.1186/s12991-019-0239-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolleman-van der Weel, M. J., and Witter, M. P. (2020). The thalamic midline nucleus reuniens: Potential relevance for schizophrenia and epilepsy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 119, 422–439. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C., Price, J. L., and Furey, M. L. (2008a). Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: Implications for neurocircuitry models of depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 213, 93–118. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0189-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C., Savitz, J., and Trimble, M. (2008b). The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders. CNS Spectr. 13, 663–681. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900013754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, Y., and Grace, A. A. (2013). Peripubertal diazepam administration prevents the emergence of dopamine system hyperresponsivity in the MAM developmental disruption model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1881–1888. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, Y., and Grace, A. A. (2016). Amygdala Hyperactivity in MAM Model of Schizophrenia is Normalized by Peripubertal Diazepam Administration. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 2455–2462. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floresco, S. B. (2015). The nucleus accumbens: An interface between cognition, emotion, and action. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 25–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Humphreys, K. L., Telzer, E. H., et al. (2013a). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 15638–15643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307893110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gee, D. G., Humphreys, K. L., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., et al. (2013b). A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry. J. Neurosci. 33, 4584–4593. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gejman, P. V., Sanders, A. R., and Kendler, K. S. (2011). Genetics of Schizophrenia: New Findings and Challenges. Annu. Rev. Genom. Hum. Genet. 12, 121–144. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genom-082410-101459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gill, K. M., Cook, J. M., Poe, M. M., and Grace, A. A. (2014). Prior antipsychotic drug treatment prevents response to novel antipsychotic agent in the methylazoxymethanol acetate model of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 40, 341–350. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovanoli, S., Engler, H., Engler, A., Richetto, J., Voget, M., Willi, R., et al. (2013). Stress in Puberty Unmasks Latent Neuropathological Consequences of Prenatal Immune Activation in Mice. Science 339, 1095–1099. doi: 10.1126/science.1228261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes, F. V., and Grace, A. A. (2017). Prefrontal Cortex Dysfunction Increases Susceptibility to Schizophrenia-Like Changes Induced by Adolescent Stress Exposure. Schizophr. Bull. 43, 592–600. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes, F. V., Rincón-Cortés, M., and Grace, A. A. (2016). Adolescence as a period of vulnerability and intervention in schizophrenia: Insights from the MAM model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 70, 260–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes, F. V., Zhu, X., and Grace, A. A. (2019). Stress during critical periods of development and risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 213, 107–113. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomes, F. V., Zhu, X., and Grace, A. A. (2020). The pathophysiological impact of stress on the dopamine system is dependent on the state of the critical period of vulnerability. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 3278–3291. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0514-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Acosta, C. A., Rojas-Cerón, C. A., and Buriticá, E. (2020). Functional Alterations and Cerebral Variations in Humans Exposed to Early Life Stress. Front. Public Health 8:536188. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.536188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grace, A. A. (2012). Dopamine system dysregulation by the hippocampus: Implications for the pathophysiology and treatment of schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology 62, 1342–1348. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grace, A. A. (2016). Dysregulation of the dopamine system in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 524–532. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grace, A. A., and Gomes, F. V. (2019). The Circuitry of Dopamine System Regulation and its Disruption in Schizophrenia: Insights Into Treatment and Prevention. Schizophr. Bull. 45, 148–157. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grecksch, G., Bernstein, H. G., Becker, A., Höllt, V., and Bogerts, B. (1999). Disruption of latent inhibition in rats with postnatal hippocampal lesions. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 525–532. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00081-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, J. G., McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P. A., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., et al. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gulchina, Y., Xu, S. J., Snyder, M. A., Elefant, F., and Gao, W. J. (2017). Epigenetic mechanisms underlying NMDA receptor hypofunction in the prefrontal cortex of juvenile animals in the MAM model for schizophrenia. J. Neurochem. 143, 320–333. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, L. W., Zeng, L., Tian, N., Li, Y., He, T., Tan, D. M., et al. (2020). Optimization of food deprivation and sucrose preference test in SD rat model undergoing chronic unpredictable mild stress. Anim. Model Exp. Med. 3, 69–78. doi: 10.1002/ame2.12107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henriksen, M. G., Nordgaard, J., and Jansson, L. B. (2017). Genetics of Schizophrenia: Overview of Methods, Findings and Limitations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:322. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbison, C. E., Allen, K., Robinson, M., Newnham, J., and Pennell, C. (2017). The impact of life stress on adult depression and anxiety is dependent on gender and timing of exposure. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 1443–1454. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herzberg, M. P., and Gunnar, M. R. (2020). Early life stress and brain function: Activity and connectivity associated with processing emotion and reward. Neuroimage 209:116493. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtzman, C. W., Shapiro, D. I., Trotman, H. D., and Walker, E. F. (2012). Stress and the prodromal phase of psychosis. Curr. Pharm. Des. 18, 527–533. doi: 10.2174/138161212799316280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hovens, J. G. F. M., Wiersma, J. E., Giltay, E. J., van Oppen, P., Spinhoven, P., Penninx, B. W. J. H., et al. (2010). Childhood life events and childhood trauma in adult patients with depressive, anxiety and comorbid disorders vs. controls. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 122, 66–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01491.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howland, J. G., Greenshaw, A. J., and Winship, I. R. (2019). Practical Aspects of Animal Models of Psychiatric Disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry 64, 3–4. doi: 10.1177/0706743718771833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joel, D., Weiner, I., and Feldon, J. (1997). Electrolytic lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex in rats disrupt performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, but do not disrupt latent inhibition: Implications for animal models of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 85, 187–201. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)87583-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, F. K., Delpech, J. C., Thompson, G. J., Wei, L., Hao, J., Herman, P., et al. (2018). Amygdala hyper-connectivity in a mouse model of unpredictable early life stress. Transl. Psychiatry 8:49. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0092-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnstone, M., Thomson, P. A., Hall, J., McIntosh, A. M., Lawrie, S. M., and Porteous, D. J. (2011). DISC1 in schizophrenia: Genetic mouse models and human genomic imaging. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 14–20. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, C. A., Watson, D. J. G., and Fone, K. C. F. (2011). Animal models of schizophrenia. Br. J. Pharmacol. 164, 1162–1194. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01386.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kafetzopoulos, V., Kokras, N., Sotiropoulos, I., Oliveira, J. F., Leite-Almeida, H., Vasalou, A., et al. (2018). The nucleus reuniens: A key node in the neurocircuitry of stress and depression. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 579–586. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, R. S., Sommer, I. E., Murray, R. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D. R., Cannon, T. D., et al. (2015). Schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 1:15067. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufling, J. (2019). Alterations and adaptation of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons in animal models of depression. Cell Tissue Res. 377, 59–71. doi: 10.1007/s00441-019-03007-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kegeles, L. S., Abi-Dargham, A., Frankle, W. G., Gil, R., Cooper, T. B., Slifstein, M., et al. (2010). Increased synaptic dopamine function in associative regions of the striatum in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 231–239. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, P. H., Seviour, P. W., and Iversen, S. D. (1975). Amphetamine and apomorphine responses in the rat following 6-OHDA lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi and corpus striatum. Brain Res. 94, 507–522. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90233-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., and Prescott, C. A. (1999). Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 837–841. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.6.837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. K., Ham, B. J., and Han, K. M. (2019). Interactive effects of genetic polymorphisms and childhood adversity on brain morphologic changes in depression. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 91, 4–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinon, B. J., Millen, B. A., Zhang, L., and McKinzie, D. L. (2015). Exploratory analysis for a targeted patient population responsive to the metabotropic glutamate 2/3 receptor agonist pomaglumetad methionil in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 754–762. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knuesel, I., Chicha, L., Britschgi, M., Schobel, S. A., Bodmer, M., Hellings, J. A., et al. (2014). Maternal immune activation and abnormal brain development across CNS disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 10, 643–660. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konradi, C., Yang, C. K., Zimmerman, E. I., Lohmann, K. M., Gresch, P., Pantazopoulos, H., et al. (2011). Hippocampal interneurons are abnormal in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 131, 165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krystal, J. H., Perry, E. B., Gueorguieva, R., Belger, A., Madonick, S. H., Abi-Dargham, A., et al. (2005). Comparative and interactive human psychopharmacologic effects of ketamine and amphetamine: Implications for glutamatergic and dopaminergic model psychoses and cognitive function. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 985–994. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.9.985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacroix, L., Spinelli, S., Heidbreder, C. A., and Feldon, J. (2000). Differential role of the medial and lateral prefrontal cortices in fear and anxiety. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 1119–1130. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.6.1119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, G., Johansson, M., Agren, H., and Friberg, P. (2000). Reduced brain norepinephrine and dopamine release in treatment-refractory depressive illness: Evidence in support of the catecholamine hypothesis of mood disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 787–793. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.8.787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laubach, M., Amarante, L. M., Swanson, K., and White, S. R. (2018). What, If Anything, Is Rodent Prefrontal Cortex? eNeuro 5, ENEURO.0315–18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0315-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Tracy, A., Hoffmeister, J. A., Ip, E., and Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Meta-analysis: Exposure to Early Life Stress and Risk for Depression in Childhood and Adolescence. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 59, 842–855. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, J. A., Girgis, R. R., Brucato, G., Moore, H., Provenzano, F., Kegeles, L., et al. (2018). Hippocampal dysfunction in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia: A selective review and hypothesis for early detection and intervention. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1764–1772. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, A., Wood, S. J., Nelson, B., Beavan, A., McGorry, P., and Yung, A. R. (2015). Outcomes of nontransitioned cases in a sample at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatry 172, 249–258. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13030418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipska, B. K., Aultman, J. M., Verma, A., Weinberger, D. R., and Moghaddam, B. (2002). Neonatal damage of the ventral hippocampus impairs working memory in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00282-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipska, B. K., Jaskiw, G. E., and Weinberger, D. R. (1993). Postpubertal emergence of hyperresponsiveness to stress and to amphetamine after neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage: A potential animal model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 9, 67–75. doi: 10.1038/npp.1993.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipska, B. K., and Weinberger, D. R. (2000). To model a psychiatric disorder in animals: Schizophrenia as a reality test. Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 223–239. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00137-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, R. T. (2017). Childhood Adversities and Depression in Adulthood: Current Findings and Future Directions. Clin. Psychol. 24, 140–153. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodge, D. J., Behrens, M. M., and Grace, A. A. (2009). A loss of parvalbumin-containing interneurons is associated with diminished oscillatory activity in an animal model of schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 29, 2344–2354. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5419-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodge, D. J., and Grace, A. A. (2007). Aberrant hippocampal activity underlies the dopamine dysregulation in an animal model of schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 27, 11424–11430. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodge, D. J., and Grace, A. A. (2009). Gestational methylazoxymethanol acetate administration: A developmental disruption model of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 204, 306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., and Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 434–445. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maćkowiak, M., Bator, E., Latusz, J., Mordalska, P., and Wędzony, K. (2014). Prenatal MAM administration affects histone H3 methylation in postnatal life in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 24, 271–289. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magwai, T., Shangase, K. B., Oginga, F. O., Chiliza, B., Mpofana, T., and Xulu, K. R. (2021). DNA Methylation and Schizophrenia: Current Literature and Future Perspective. Cells 10:2890. doi: 10.3390/cells10112890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, S. F., and Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Learned helplessness at fifty: Insights from neuroscience. Psychol. Rev. 123, 349–367. doi: 10.1037/rev0000033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marco, E. M., Macrì, S., and Laviola, G. (2011). Critical age windows for neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders: Evidence from animal models. Neurotox. Res. 19, 286–307. doi: 10.1007/s12640-010-9205-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayberg, H. S., Lozano, A. M., Voon, V., McNeely, H. E., Seminowicz, D., Hamani, C., et al. (2005). Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Neuron 45, 651–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, S. E., McCombie, W. R., and Corvin, A. (2014). Unlocking the treasure trove: From genes to schizophrenia biology. Schizophr. Bull. 40, 492–496. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCutcheon, R., Beck, K., Jauhar, S., and Howes, O. D. (2018). Defining the Locus of Dopaminergic Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: A Meta-analysis and Test of the Mesolimbic Hypothesis. Schizophr. Bull. 44, 1301–1311. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCutcheon, R. A., Abi-Dargham, A., and Howes, O. D. (2019). Schizophrenia, Dopamine and the Striatum: From Biology to Symptoms. Trends Neurosci. 42, 205–220. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2017). Neurobiological and Systemic Effects of Chronic Stress. Chronic Stress 1:2470547017692328. doi: 10.1177/2470547017692328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., and Kessler, R. C. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication II: Associations with persistence of DSM-IV disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 124–132. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meehan, C., Harms, L., Frost, J. D., Barreto, R., Todd, J., Schall, U., et al. (2017). Effects of immune activation during early or late gestation on schizophrenia-related behaviour in adult rat offspring. Brain Behav. Immun. 63, 8–20. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.07.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menniti, F. S., Chappie, T. A., and Schmidt, C. J. (2020). PDE10A Inhibitors-Clinical Failure or Window Into Antipsychotic Drug Action? Front. Neurosci. 14:600178. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.600178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, H. C., and Lee, F. S. (2019). Translating Developmental Neuroscience to Understand Risk for Psychiatric Disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 176, 179–185. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19010091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, U. (2014). Prenatal poly(i:C) exposure and other developmental immune activation models in rodent systems. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 307–315. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, U., and Feldon, J. (2010). Epidemiology-driven neurodevelopmental animal models of schizophrenia. Prog. Neurobiol. 90, 285–326. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, U., and Feldon, J. (2012). To poly(I:C) or not to poly(I:C): Advancing preclinical schizophrenia research through the use of prenatal immune activation models. Neuropharmacology 62, 1308–1321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millan, M. J., Andrieux, A., Bartzokis, G., Cadenhead, K., Dazzan, P., Fusar-Poli, P., et al. (2016). Altering the course of schizophrenia: Progress and perspectives. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 15, 485–515. doi: 10.1038/nrd.2016.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, P., Lawrie, S. M., Hodges, A., Clafferty, R., Cosway, R., and Johnstone, E. C. (2001). Genetic liability, illicit drug use, life stress and psychotic symptoms: Preliminary findings from the Edinburgh study of people at high risk for schizophrenia. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 36, 338–342. doi: 10.1007/s001270170038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mingtian, Z., Shuqiao, Y., Xiongzhao, Z., Jinyao, Y., Xueling, Z., Xiang, W., et al. (2012). Elevated amygdala activity to negative faces in young adults with early onset major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 201, 107–112. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, K. J., Huang, Z. J., Moghaddam, B., and Sawa, A. (2011). Following the genes: A framework for animal modeling of psychiatric disorders. BMC Biol. 9:76. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-9-76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizrahi, R., Addington, J., Rusjan, P. M., Suridjan, I., Ng, A., Boileau, I., et al. (2012). Increased stress-induced dopamine release in psychosis. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 561–567. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modinos, G., Allen, P., Grace, A. A., and McGuire, P. (2015). Translating the MAM model of psychosis to humans. Trends Neurosci. 38, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modinos, G., Richter, A., Egerton, A., Bonoldi, I., Azis, M., Antoniades, M., et al. (2021). Interactions between hippocampal activity and striatal dopamine in people at clinical high risk for psychosis: Relationship to adverse outcomes. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 1468–1474. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01019-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molendijk, M. L., and de Kloet, E. R. (2015). Immobility in the forced swim test is adaptive and does not reflect depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 62, 389–391. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, H., Jentsch, J. D., Ghajarnia, M., Geyer, M. A., and Grace, A. A. (2006). A neurobehavioral systems analysis of adult rats exposed to methylazoxymethanol acetate on E17: Implications for the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 253–264. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreines, J. L., Owrutsky, Z. L., and Grace, A. A. (2017b). Involvement of Infralimbic Prefrontal Cortex but not Lateral Habenula in Dopamine Attenuation After Chronic Mild Stress. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 904–913. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreines, J. L., Owrutsky, Z. L., Gagnon, K. G., and Grace, A. A. (2017a). Divergent effects of acute and repeated quetiapine treatment on dopamine neuron activity in normal vs. chronic mild stress induced hypodopaminergic states. Transl. Psychiatry 7:1275. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0039-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muir, J., Lopez, J., and Bagot, R. C. (2018). Wiring the depressed brain: Optogenetic and chemogenetic circuit interrogation in animal models of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 1013–1026. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0291-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, E. A., Wise, S. P., and Drevets, W. C. (2011). Localization of dysfunction in major depressive disorder: Prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e43–e54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nederhof, E., and Schmidt, M. V. (2012). Mismatch or cumulative stress: Toward an integrated hypothesis of programming effects. Physiol. Behav. 106, 691–700. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J., and Carlezon, W. A. (2006). The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 1151–1159. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otte, C., Gold, S. M., Penninx, B. W., Pariante, C. M., Etkin, A., Fava, M., et al. (2016). Major depressive disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2:16065. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2016.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, M. H., Bizup, B. T., and Grace, A. A. (2013). The infralimbic cortex bidirectionally modulates mesolimbic dopamine neuron activity via distinct neural pathways. J. Neurosci. 33, 16865–16873. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2449-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peña, C. J., Nestler, E. J., and Bagot, R. C. (2019). Environmental Programming of Susceptibility and Resilience to Stress in Adulthood in Male Mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:40. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, S. M., Aguilar, D. D., Neary, J. L., Carless, M. A., Giuffrida, A., and Lodge, D. J. (2016). Schizophrenia-Like Phenotype Inherited by the F2 Generation of a Gestational Disruption Model of Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 477–486. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Noll, D. C., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1105–1113. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pizzagalli, D. A., and Roberts, A. C. (2022). Prefrontal cortex and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 225–246. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01101-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popovic, D., Schmitt, A., Kaurani, L., Senner, F., Papiol, S., Malchow, B., et al. (2019). Childhood Trauma in Schizophrenia: Current Findings and Research Perspectives. Front. Neurosci. 13:274. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radley, J. J., Arias, C. M., and Sawchenko, P. E. (2006). Regional differentiation of the medial prefrontal cortex in regulating adaptive responses to acute emotional stress. J. Neurosci. 26, 12967–12976. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4297-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddy, P. L., Khanna, S., Subhash, M. N., Channabasavanna, S. M., and Rao, B. S. (1992). CSF amine metabolites in depression. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 112–118.

Google Scholar

Redman, S. L., Corcoran, C. M., Kimhy, D., and Malaspina, D. (2017). Effects of early trauma on psychosis development in clinical high-risk individuals and stability of trauma assessment across studies: A review. Arch. Psychol. 1:28.

Google Scholar

Reisinger, S., Khan, D., Kong, E., Berger, A., Pollak, A., and Pollak, D. D. (2015). The poly(I:C)-induced maternal immune activation model in preclinical neuropsychiatric drug discovery. Pharmacol. Ther. 149, 213–226. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2015.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter-Levin, G., and Xu, L. (2018). How could stress lead to major depressive disorder? IBRO Rep. 4, 38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ibror.2018.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, N., and Bergen, S. E. (2021). Environmental Risk Factors for Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder and Their Relationship to Genetic Risk: Current Knowledge and Future Directions. Front. Genet. 12:686666. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2021.686666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, M. A., Kasai, K., Koji, M., Fukuda, R., Iwanami, A., Nakagome, K., et al. (2004). Executive and prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: A review of neuropsychological and imaging evidence. Neurosci. Res. 50, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rorick-Kehn, L. M., Johnson, B. G., Knitowski, K. M., Salhoff, C. R., Witkin, J. M., Perry, K. W., et al. (2007). In vivo pharmacological characterization of the structurally novel, potent, selective mGlu2/3 receptor agonist LY404039 in animal models of psychiatric disorders. Psychopharmacology 193, 121–136. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0758-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenkranz, J. A., and Grace, A. A. (2001). Dopamine attenuates prefrontal cortical suppression of sensory inputs to the basolateral amygdala of rats. J. Neurosci. 21, 4090–4103. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-11-04090.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenkranz, J. A., and Grace, A. A. (2002). Cellular mechanisms of infralimbic and prelimbic prefrontal cortical inhibition and dopaminergic modulation of basolateral amygdala neurons in vivo. J. Neurosci. 22, 324–337. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-01-00324.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C. A., and Margolis, R. L. (2019). Research Domain Criteria: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Potential Alternatives for Future Psychiatric Research. Mol. Neuropsychiatry 5, 218–236. doi: 10.1159/000501797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 511, 421–427. doi: 10.1038/nature13595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M. V., Wang, X.-D., and Meijer, O. C. (2011). Early life stress paradigms in rodents: Potential animal models of depression? Psychopharmacology 214, 131–140. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2096-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schobel, S. A., Chaudhury, N. H., Khan, U. A., Paniagua, B., Styner, M. A., Asllani, I., et al. (2013). Imaging patients with psychosis and a mouse model establishes a spreading pattern of hippocampal dysfunction and implicates glutamate as a driver. Neuron 78, 81–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeman, P., Lee, T., Chau-Wong, M., and Wong, K. (1976). Antipsychotic drug doses and neuroleptic/dopamine receptors. Nature 261, 717–719. doi: 10.1038/261717a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selemon, L. D., and Zecevic, N. (2015). Schizophrenia: A tale of two critical periods for prefrontal cortical development. Transl. Psychiatry 5:e623. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Servonnet, A., and Samaha, A. N. (2020). Antipsychotic-evoked dopamine supersensitivity. Neuropharmacology 163:107630. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, G. A., Dupree, J. L., and Neigh, G. N. (2020). Adolescent maturation of the prefrontal cortex: Role of stress and sex in shaping adult risk for compromise. Genes Brain Behav. 19:e12626. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, R. C., Maffioletti, E., Gennarelli, M., Baune, B. T., and Minelli, A. (2021). Biological correlates of early life stressful events in major depressive disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 125:105103. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.105103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smythies, J. R., Johnston, V. S., Bradley, R. J., Benington, F., Morin, R. D., and Clark, L. C. (1967). Some new behaviour-disrupting amphetamines and their significance. Nature 216, 128–129. doi: 10.1038/216128a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, S. H. (1973). Amphetamine psychosis: A “model” schizophrenia mediated by catecholamines. Am. J. Psychiatry 130, 61–67. doi: 10.1176/ajp.130.1.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnenschein, S. F., Gomes, F. V., and Grace, A. A. (2020). Dysregulation of Midbrain Dopamine System and the Pathophysiology of Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 11:613. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnenschein, S. F., and Grace, A. A. (2020). The mGluR2/3 agonist pomaglumetad methionil normalizes aberrant dopamine neuron activity via action in the ventral hippocampus. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 2106–2113. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0764-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2013). Adolescent neurodevelopment. J. Adolesc. Health 52, S7–S13. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, Y. A., and Si, T. (2022). Progress and challenges in research of the mechanisms of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Gen. Psychiatr. 35:e100724. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2021-100724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swerdlow, N. R., Halim, N., Hanlon, F. M., Platten, A., and Auerbach, P. P. (2001). Lesion size and amphetamine hyperlocomotion after neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions: More is less. Brain Res. Bull. 55, 71–77. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00492-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavares, R. F., Corrêa, F. M. A., and Resstel, L. B. M. (2009). Opposite role of infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the tachycardiac response evoked by acute restraint stress in rats. J. Neurosci. Res. 87, 2601–2607. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trubetskoy, V., Pardiñas, A. F., Qi, T., Panagiotaropoulou, G., Awasthi, S., Bigdeli, T. B., et al. (2022). Mapping genomic loci implicates genes and synaptic biology in schizophrenia. Nature 604, 502–508. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04434-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, K. Y., Chambers, R. A., and Lipska, B. K. (2009). The neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion as a heuristic neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 204, 295–305. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uliana, D. L., Gomes, F. V., and Grace, A. A. (2020). Prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex disruption during adolescence increases susceptibility to helpless behavior in adult rats. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 35, 111–125. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uliana, D. L., Gomes, F. V., and Grace, A. A. (2021). Stress impacts corticoamygdalar connectivity in an age-dependent manner. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 731–740. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00886-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenti, O., Cifelli, P., Gill, K. M., and Grace, A. A. (2011a). Antipsychotic drugs rapidly induce dopamine neuron depolarization block in a developmental rat model of schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 31, 12330–12338. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2808-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenti, O., Lodge, D. J., and Grace, A. A. (2011b). Aversive stimuli alter ventral tegmental area dopamine neuron activity via a common action in the ventral hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 31, 4280–4289. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5310-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Buuse, M. (2010). Modeling the positive symptoms of schizophrenia in genetically modified mice: Pharmacology and methodology aspects. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 246–270. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Winkel, R., Stefanis, N. C., and Myin-Germeys, I. (2008). Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interaction. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 1095–1105. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberger, D. R. (1987). Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 660–669. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800190080012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, P., Towell, A., Sampson, D., Sophokleous, S., and Muscat, R. (1987). Reduction of sucrose preference by chronic unpredictable mild stress, and its restoration by a tricyclic antidepressant. Psychopharmacology 93, 358–364. doi: 10.1007/BF00187257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, A. R., Cheyne, K. R., and Bilkey, D. K. (2011). Behavioural deficits associated with maternal immune activation in the rat model of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 225, 382–387. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, T. T., Simmons, A. N., Matthews, S. C., Tapert, S. F., Frank, G. K., Max, J. E., et al. (2010). Adolescents with major depression demonstrate increased amygdala activation. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 42–51.

Google Scholar

Zhang, Z. J., and Reynolds, G. P. (2002). A selective decrease in the relative density of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the hippocampus in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 55, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00188-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., and Grace, A. A. (2021). Prepubertal Environmental Enrichment Prevents Dopamine Dysregulation and Hippocampal Hyperactivity in MAM Schizophrenia Model Rats. Biol. Psychiatry 89, 298–307. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, E. C., Bellaire, M., Ewing, S. G., and Grace, A. A. (2013). Abnormal stress responsivity in a rodent developmental disruption model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 2131–2139. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, E. C., and Grace, A. A. (2016). The Nucleus Reuniens of the Midline Thalamus Gates Prefrontal-Hippocampal Modulation of Ventral Tegmental Area Dopamine Neuron Activity. J. Neurosci. 36, 8977–8984. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, depression, hippocampus, plasticity, prefrontal cortex, dopamine

Citation: Uliana DL, Zhu X, Gomes FV and Grace AA (2022) Using animal models for the studies of schizophrenia and depression: The value of translational models for treatment and prevention. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:935320. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.935320

Received: 03 May 2022; Accepted: 04 August 2022;
Published: 24 August 2022.

Edited by:

Jen Wenzel, University of San Diego, United States

Reviewed by:

Bryan W. Jenkins, University of Guelph, Canada
Duncan Sinclair, University of Tasmania, Australia
Juliet Richetto, University of Zurich, Switzerland
James P. Kesby, The University of Queensland, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Uliana, Zhu, Gomes and Grace. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anthony A. Grace, Z3JhY2VhYUBwaXR0LmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.