Skip to main content

METHODS article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 25 May 2022
Sec. Motivation and Reward

A Protocol for the Non-invasive Method of Ultrasound Separation During the Sociosexual Vocal-Non-contact Model in Rats

  • Department of Experimental and Clinical Physiology, Laboratory of Centre for Preclinical Research, Medical University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

Ultrasonic vocalization (USV) is one of the measurable behavioral parameters of sociosexual interactions in rats. To precisely and accurately describe the neurobehavioral properties of USV and the potentially related specific emotional responsiveness of animals, we need to know which animals vocalize and what is their exact behavioral and physiological response. To this end, we modified the non-contact cage [non-contact erection model (NCE)] by adding a modification [vocalization-non-contact erection (VOC-NCE)] that makes it possible to assign emitted ultrasonic signals to a particular animal. Typically, the NCE cage consists of two compartments separated by perforated baffles. A male is placed in one section, and a receptive female is placed in the other section. This makes possible the accurate description of sexual parameters related to the cues controlled by the experimenter. In VOC-NCE, we completely separated the male USV from the female USV by three appropriately perforated baffles and located microphones combined with ultrasonic screening. We recorded emission in both typical bands, the so-called 22- and 50-kHz bands, with various subtypes, thus highlighting the utility of our protocol to investigate the sexual dimorphism of vocalization. Similar to the anticipatory model, we showed that emission can occur without acoustic feedback from concomitants during the VOC-NCE test. Therefore, we propose a relatively simple method for assigning individual vocalization. We discuss its usefulness and limitations in assessing vocal differentiation related to sexual parameters, adaptive changes during conditioning procedures, and further applications.

Introduction

Ultrasonic vocalization (USV) emitted by rats has gained considerable acceptance in recent decades as an applicable parameter in neurobehavioral research (Burgdorf and Panksepp, 2006; Taracha et al., 2012; Potasiewicz et al., 2019; Caruso et al., 2020; Costa et al., 2021). The frequency spectrum of the main emitted signals ranges from 20 kHz to about 70 kHz. The commonly accepted division according to the dominance frequency divides them into two groups, the so-called 22- and 50-kHz bands (Sales and Pye, 1974; Wright et al., 2010; Wöhr and Schwarting, 2013; Brudzynski, 2015), differing in the accompanying neurobehavioral context of the emission and stimulation of the reward-related neuronal structure of the rat brain (Brudzynski, 2007).

During sociosexual interactions in rats (arousing and highly appetitive situations), we can observe a wide variety of ultrasounds with all subtypes in the 22- and 50-kHz bands (Barfield et al., 1979; Burgdorf et al., 2008; Seffer et al., 2014; Ågmo and Snoeren, 2015; Bialy et al., 2019a; Bogacki-Rychlik et al., 2021).

One of the most significant problems of USV analysis is assigning particular signals to the animal that emitted them. This is necessary for the standardization of USV as a parameter reflecting a specific physiological process taking place during intraindividual interactions. It could only then serve as being fully applicable in preclinical models with a reasonable translational potential. To date, methods allowing this type of distinction are based on an analysis of the anticipatory vocalization of each individual (Bialy et al., 2000; Bogacki-Rychlik et al., 2021), the devocalization of one of the animals (Thomas et al., 1981; White et al., 1990; Matochik et al., 1992; Kisko et al., 2015), the anesthetization of one of them (Maggio and Whitney, 1985), or the use of a complex system based on multisource recording combined with the application of mathematical methods (Sangiamo et al., 2020; Warren et al., 2020).

Each of these methods has specific limitations due to the invasiveness of the procedure, stimuli deprivation of the animals, or a methodology that is hard to apply.

We propose a relatively simple way to distinguish ultrasounds during the so-called vocalization-non-contact erection test (VOC-NCE) in the experiment described here. The non-contact erection test (NCE) and dedicated chamber cage were developed and refined by Benjamin D. Sachs (Sachs et al., 1994; Sachs, 1997). It made it possible to standardize non-contact erections as a behavioral parameter and use them to study mechanisms of sexual arousal through the selective and controlled deprivation of tactile, visual, or olfactory stimuli. The standard NCE procedure was carried out by placing two animals in two distinct compartments separated by three plexiglass partitions with specific perforations. Shifts in the level of perforations between the baffles provided olfactory stimuli without direct contact between the animals. Furthermore, the transparent versus opaque material of the partition gave some (or a reduced) visual stimuli. Also, the exchange of non-specific auditory stimuli (to reach the spectrum heard by humans) was possible due to the movements of the animals. By airflow regulation, the experimenter could control the level of olfactory stimuli perceived by the subjects (Sachs, 1997). Altogether, it provided an opportunity to measure the parameters of sociosexual behavior during the absence of direct tactile stimulation with a controlled level of other classes of stimuli.

We proposed an additional expansion of this method by adding the possibility of analyzing the USV parameter of a particular animal and thus correlate it with other sexual parameters of males and females. We obtained this selectivity by appropriately positioning two microphones (one in each compartment) and adding a silencing screen above the chambers. Consequently, we isolated the acoustic stream derived from each animal.

Moreover, due to the high research interest in USV during sociosexual interactions, we present in detail our investigative protocol, which is based on rat sexual behavior. We believe that sexual behavior across the rodent order, including typical laboratory models (i.e., mice, gerbils, hamsters, and rats) and their corresponding USV, is controlled by similar mechanisms but differs significantly in the detail. Thus, we hoped that the presented VOC-NCE chamber with the behavioral protocol would help to avoid misinterpretations of the results between different experimental approaches by implementing the possibility of controlling modalities combined with sexual parameters (general arousal, sexual arousal, sexual motivation).

Nevertheless, the primary aim of this model was to obtain a clear USV separation during sociosexual interactions.

Materials and Equipment

Apparatus

The experimental chamber, made of plexiglass, had total dimensions of 50 cm × 31 cm × 27 cm. For the VOC-NCE test, the long axis of the chamber was bisected in the middle of its length by a set of three transparent partitions placed 1 cm apart (Figures 1, 2). The two outer partitions had evenly spaced rectangular perforations (2.5 cm × 7.5 cm) cut out at the bottom. The middle partition had four similar-sized rectangular perforations (2.5 cm × 9 cm), aligned about 3 cm above the perforations of the outer ones (Figure 1). We covered the top of the compartments with a wire grid to prevent the rats from escaping during the experimental trials. To acoustically separate the recording compartments, we mounted an acoustic screen (72 cm × 52 cm × 10 cm) in the central part of the lid, just above the three dividers. The microphones were attached 50 cm above the ground, one on each side, exactly opposite each other (Figures 35).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scheme of the standard NCE cage. Two compartments are separated by three partitions, namely, two outer partitions with identical perforations and one middle partition with perforations shifted upward.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. At the top of the figure are two middle partitions, and at the bottom are two outer partitions. The black opaque versions are alternatives to implement the deprivation of visual cues. The results presented in this study were obtained using the transparent version.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A scheme to illustrate the proposed expansion of the VOC-NCE cage with an acoustic screen.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The photograph of the VOC-NCE located in the behavioral recording room (the frontal view). For clarity, cables have been unplugged from the microphones, and the covering grid was removed.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The photograph of the VOC-NCE located in the behavioral recording room (the oblique view). For clarity, cables have been unplugged from the microphones, and the covering grid was removed.

Technical Assumption

In the experiment, we used a cage constructed of poly (methyl methacrylate) (PMMA), also known as plexiglass. The choice of material was dictated by its properties such as low weight, transparency (as PMMA transmits up to 94% of visible light) (Martín-de León et al., 2019), high flexibility (good resistance to cracking compared with polystyrene as used in some systems), and ease of cleaning even at high temperatures (Ali et al., 2015). However, in our experiment, the most important features are related to its sound insulation properties. In this context, PMMA is used in environmental engineering as sound walls to reduce industrial noise (Garai and Guidorzi, 2000), acoustic screens in recording studios, and commercially available isolation systems for animals (such as IVC). In the presented VOC-NCE system, two compartments were separated by three partitions made of plexiglass. This made it possible to obtain sufficient ultrasonic isolation due to the high acoustic impedance of the material. The main frequency range of the USV of the rats is between 20 kHz to about 70 kHz, so the wave energy is relatively low (comparable with the human audible sound wave range). We decided to leave open each compartment at the top and isolate them by the acoustic splitter screen (covered with polyurethane soundproofing foam). The open construction made it possible to avoid sound artifacts in the recordings generated due to multiple wave reflections. Apart from the ability to reflect, plexiglass has a significant sound absorption parameter, so we could probably create an acoustic shadow in the middle part of the cage (between the outer partitions). The floor of the VOC-NCE cage was not covered by bedding to reduce ultrasonic artifacts.

Methods

Animals and Behavioral Procedures

The subjects of this study were 6-month-old Long-Evans rats (eight males and eight females, weighted 480–570 g and 280–400 g, respectively). The animals were purchased from the Department of Experimental Medicine, the Medical University of Silesia, Katowice. The males and females were housed in separate rooms, with two animals per standard laboratory cage (55.6 cm × 33.4 cm × 19.5 cm, Animalab type IV). Chow and water were freely available. All cages were provided with wood shavings and dedicated plastic tubes as enrichment. The rats were maintained on a 12-h light-dark cycle (lights switched off at 9:00 p.m.) at a temperature around 22°C. All of the behavioral tests were conducted between 13:00 and 17:00 during the dark phase of the light-dark cycle. The experimental room was illuminated primarily by infrared light, with a total intensity of 25 lux.

The females were bilaterally ovariectomized [under intraperitoneal (IP) injection of ketamine 10 mg/100 g with xylazine 1 mg/100 g] before the experiment and then convalesced for 2 weeks (with analgetic and anti-inflammatory support). Estrus was hormonally induced with a single injection of estradiol benzoate [50 μg/rat subcutaneously (s.c.), Sigma, 48–72 h before the trial] and progesterone (500 μg/rat s.c., Sigma, 4–8 h before the trial). Hormones were dissolved in sesame oil and administered in 0.05 ml doses per individual. The induction was repeated no more than once a week and at least once every 2 weeks. The animals were habituated to the experimenters, to all behavioral procedures, and to the chamber in all versions (for the copulatory and VOC-NCE test). Before the trial phase, all of the animals acquired sexual experience during four preceding training sessions. Training copulatory sessions were performed in the experimental VOC-NCE chamber with the partitions removed. The male was placed for 5 min for acclimation (anticipatory phase), and then an estrus female was introduced for 25 min or up to the first intromission after the first ejaculation.

During the experimental trials, a male was placed in the first compartment for 5 min alone (anticipatory phase) and the recording equipment was switched on. After this time, a female was placed in the second compartment, and the recording process continued for the next 20 min.

Ultrasonic Recording and Analysis

All behavioral procedures were performed in two adjoining rooms. The recording room with the VOC-NCE cage and microphones and infrared camera was acoustically and visually isolated from the observers. A second room was used for observation, registration (computer with hardware), and further analysis. The rats were placed in the recording room during all of the trials while the experimenters stayed in the other room.

This separation isolated unwanted stimuli and reduced background noise generated by the electronic equipment and human activity (to maintain an optimal acoustic environment). The USV was recorded with a Sonotrack System (Sonotrack by Metris ver. 2.2) dedicated to this purpose, which enables recording on up to four independent parallel channels. The system consists of ultrasonic microphones (frequency range 15--125 kHz), with fully compatible hardware and software1. The microphones were connected by two identical cables, identical in all conductive parameters. All USV tracks were analyzed and compared manually. After the visual analysis based on spectrogram pictures, analog time windows were listened to, to exclude potential low-energy vestigial ultrasonic leaks.

Results

The acoustic isolation was sufficient to separate all emitted ultrasonic calls and assign them to each animal. We analyzed the records from the male (channel 1) and female (channel 2) compartments obtained during non-contact sessions. Vocalizations were very intensive (hundreds of particular signals per track), and both males and females vocalized in all of the main types of ultrasounds. We presented some arbitrarily chosen samples of ultrasounds from two random records (Figures 69). The only criterion for selection was our intention to demonstrate various types in both 50- and 22-kHz frequencies. The visible color differentiation of the software was intentionally applied to improve the overall clarity. The timeline of the corresponding spectrograms was precisely adjusted to each other to reveal sufficient separation.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. (A,C) Different forms of 22-kHz specific for VOC-NCE: composite forms with different lengths of 22-kHz signal with co-occurring higher frequencies. (B) Short form of 22 kHz. All of them were emitted by males, and analogical parts of the female‘s spectrogram were blank.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. (A) Frequency-modulated non-trill (complex) emitted by the male. Female emitted two forms of short type. (B–D) Very intensive, frequency-modulated USV with trills and composite clusters of signals emitted by the female. The male was silent.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. (A–D) Various forms included flat-type, multistep, and step down on the male’s side On the female’s side, the ultrasonic spectrum is distinctly different. All ultrasounds can be unambiguously assigned to the emitter.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. (A–D) Another set of ultrasounds with split type, upward, and step down on the male’s side, and frequency-modulated trills on the female’s side.

Discussion

Our results indicate that the proposed VOC-NCE procedure is sufficient to separate vocalizations emitted by two individuals during sociosexual interaction. We have demonstrated that partitions with appropriately located perforations, combined with simple acoustic isolations, are sufficient to entirely reduce the ultrasonic stream while maintaining a flow of other stimuli. In the conducted sexual vocal-non-contact tests, emissions of USV were very intensive in the situation with reduced acoustic feedback, which implies the dominative role of cues other than auditory in initiating and maintaining USV. Therefore, this relatively simple model provides an opportunity to prioritize incentive modalities, crucial for emission during encounters. Moreover, it makes it possible to describe sex-dependent features in the spectrograms obtained due to reliable matching to the male or female. Furthermore, the presented separation should be useful for assigning an individual USV profile to other physiological parameters occurring during sexual behavior.

Expanding the NCE Test

In the classical procedure presented by Benjamin D. Sachs, the visual analysis of behavior enables the parameterization of sexual arousal (by latency to first non-contact erection as well as the number of non-contact erections) (Sachs et al., 1994; Sachs, 1997), sexual preferences (Kondo and Hayashi, 2021), sociosexual motivation (latency to start and duration of perforation sniffing), and general arousal (number of rearing, total locomotor activity). The experimental non-contact chamber provides visual contact (when the baffles are transparent), exchange of olfactory stimuli due to perforations (olfactory cues could also be reduced or enhanced by pumping air in or out), and non-specific acoustic cues as a result of animal movements (mainly in the human audible range). The previous experiments have shown that olfactory cues are sufficient and the most important to evoke non-contact erections (Sachs, 1997; Kondo et al., 1999) in mechanisms related to the medial amygdala activity (Bialy and Sachs, 2002; Kondo and Sachs, 2002; Bialy et al., 2011). Other stimuli appear to be less relevant in inducing a non-contact erection and sexual arousal in rats (Sachs, 1997). We can also modify the level of sexual arousal via the hormonal status of the female or male (Sachs et al., 1994; Manzo et al., 1999; Fernández-Vargas, 2018). Taken together, VOC-NCE gives opportunities to investigate USV during sociosexual activity in a more precise and selective established cue/sensory environment.

Ultrasonic Vocalization During VOC-NCE, Anticipatory, Copulation, and Postejaculatory Period: Behavioral Protocols

USV in the VOC-NCE Test

We started to measure the vocalization phase of the VOC-NCE test after 5 min of the anticipatory period with the introduction of the female. The duration of the anticipatory phase is determined during the preceding sexual training procedure. However, 5 min is commonly accepted due to optimal acclimation/conditioning properties during sexual behavior (Larsson, 1956; Bialy et al., 2000). In this and previous experiments, we end the NCE sessions after 20 min with a female (25 min in total, refer to Figure 10).

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Schematic protocol for the investigation of USV during particular phases of sociosexual interactions. *22-kHz shifts in the late phase of copulation occur relatively rarely. After the dominant flat-type postejaculatory 22-kHz signal, some forms of modulation could also be observed (end phase of 22-kHz postejaculatory vocalization, not depicted in the figure). Ej, ejaculation.

Previously, we found vocalizations specific to the NCE test in the so-called 22-kHz band. This variation included frequency-modulated signals and assembled types with coexisted prefixes or suffixes (Bialy et al., 2019a). By applying the VOC-NCE expansion, we have accurately identified particular emitters, in this case, by separating the ultrasound from the male and female. Thus, it probably could be functional in assessing some sexual dimorphic traits in spectrograms due to recent interest in this field (for a detailed review, refer to Lenell et al., 2021).

During 20 min of VOC-NCE, the USV of both male and female in physiological conditions should be abundant, with domination of the 50-kHz signals in the recordings. In this phase, we observed many high-frequency-modulated signals (trill-like types) co-occurring with all other subtypes of the 50-kHz group. These subtypes (trills and flat-trill) can be considered as additional markers of estrus occurrence. Our observations are consistent with the results of Barfield and Thomas (1986), which showed estrus clustered and a modulated type of USV. Similarly, Gerson et al. (2019) showed that direct clitoral stimulation elicits trill and flat-trill subtypes, and hormones significantly augment their emission. We demonstrate in Figures 7, 9 this characteristic pattern of estrus (aroused) female vocalization with highly modulated 50-kHz signals combined in clusters. This type of USV is relatively rare in the recordings of males during non-contact. In contrast, in the male spectrograms, we observed characteristics for the non-contact test variant of a 22-kHz, i.e., the 22-kHz shift-subtype consisting of a 22-kHz call combined with a 50-kHz prefix or suffix. They seem to reflect a fluctuation of arousal that can be interpreted as an emotional state related to sexual frustration. We have described these subtypes in detail previously (Bialy et al., 2019a). Different patterns of frequency-modulated 22-kHz USVs have also been demonstrated in many other neurobehavioral contexts (Miczek and Van Der Poel, 1991; Vivian and Miczek, 1993; Karwicka et al., 2021).

The implied question arises about the possibility of experimentally controlling the frustration levels using VOC-NCE. Potentially, it could be achieved with the implantation of removable (openable) partitions, which would allow direct contact between the male and female after a fixed time of the non-contact procedure (Pfaus et al., 1990; similarly to Mas et al., 1990, 1995; Damsma et al., 1992; Lorrain et al., 1997, 1999). In such an experimental setup, the acquisition of sexual experience should reduce frustration effects via the conditioning processes during trials (Pfaus et al., 2001).

Overall, based on the presented VOC-NCE model, it is possible to conduct a quantitative and qualitative analysis of the 22- and 50-kHz bands to construct individual USV profiles, which can be used as an additional parameter to describe behavioral changes during sexual activity encounters.

During sexual behavior, we found that male rats in different phases of sexual behavior (anticipatory, postejaculatory period, and NCE test) display a specific pattern of vocalizations with dominant USV subtypes (Bialy et al., 2019a; Bogacki-Rychlik et al., 2021). This suggests that specific central responsiveness (emotional/motivational/arousal) influences the USV pattern. Therefore, we discuss in more detail these phases and compare them to the NCE and VOC-NCE tests.

Anticipatory Pre-Contact Vocalization

Anticipatory sexual behavior makes possible the analysis of individual rat USVs. During our experiments, males vocalize frequently, and such vocalizations are related to emotional memory, sexual motivation, conditioning to odor and background cues, sexual experience, and level of general arousal, but not sexual arousal per se (Bialy et al., 2019b). The total number of anticipatory precontact 50-kHz vocalizations (PVs) positively correlates with the acquisition of sexual experience, rewarding the value of previous sociosexual contact (Bialy et al., 2000), and the level of sexual motivation (Bogacki-Rychlik et al., 2021). Contrary to the total number of PVs, the pattern of vocalizations measured by the percentage of different subtypes seems to be stable and characteristic of anticipatory behavior with dominant complex/composite and flat calls (Bogacki-Rychlik et al., 2021). In the anticipatory phase, 22-kHz signals occur very rarely, and if they do then in short and unmodulated forms. Also, in this phase, erections of the penis, a sign of sexual arousal, are not observed in the standard 5-min period, supporting the thesis that PVs reflect sexual motivation and general arousal but not sexual arousal per se.

USV During Copulation

Copulation of male rats is a relatively complex but well-parameterized behavior (Sachs and Barfield, 1976; Sachs, 1978; Bialy et al., 2000, 2019b; Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). During copulation, many ultrasounds are emitted by both male and female, mostly at 50 kHz. The female vocalizes intensively during solicitation (darting, hopping, and ear wiggling), and the level of USV depends on the hormonal status. The male vocalizes during an approach to the female, especially before investigation and mounting (Barfield and Thomas, 1986). This USV significantly increases when having a problem with achieving intromissions and ejaculation, thus suggesting the significant effect on USV production of some frustration and enhanced general arousal rather than sexual arousal and sexual motivation per se (McIntosh and Barfield, 1980; Bialy et al., 1996). The role of the male and female USV in the coordination of copulation was investigated by using devocalized animals. A female exposed to a devocalized male displays less intensive soliciting behavior and more frequently moves out during intromission or ejaculation (Thomas et al., 1981). Such results suggest some coordinator role of the USV in the sexual interaction, although the effect of female or male devocalizations on copulatory parameters is not statistically significant (Ågmo and Snoeren, 2015).

USV During the Postejaculatory Interval

The PEI is the period between ejaculation and the next cycle of copulation, first mounting, or intromission after a refractory period (Sachs, 1978; Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). In the USV spectrograms of males, there is a domination of specific long-lasting 22-kHz calls with a very stable frequency (Barfield and Geyer, 1972; Bialy et al., 2019a). Postejaculatory vocalizations occur as the effect of an abrupt decrease in the arousal and motivation level with sleep-like patterns in EEG (Kurtz and Adler, 1973; Barfield and Geyer, 1975) and thus seem to reflect the relaxation state of the male after ejaculation (Bialy et al., 2016). The intensity of 22-kHz postejaculatory (latency to start and duration time) depends on the presence of the female (Sachs and Bialy, 2000). However, the crucial cues responsible for this reaction are not clear, but an enhanced level of anxiety inhibits this type of vocalization (Bialy et al., 2016). The 50-kHz types in PEI are uncommon when the 22-kHz signal occurs and are usually observed after it ends. Thus, their presence indicates increasing sexual motivation before the next copulation cycle (Figure 10).

Overall, we can distinguish behavioral parameters during PEI, which imply the time of enhancement of general arousal, sexual arousal, and sexual motivation (Sachs and Bialy, 2000; Bialy et al., 2019b), and the presented forms of USV are some of them.

VOC-NCE vs. Anticipatory PV, Copulatory and Postejaculatory USV

The VOC-NCE model presented here, as well as anticipatory PV, is a non-invasive method to analyze individual USV. The anticipatory PV model is convenient for investigating the male USV related to sexual motivation and general arousal (Bialy et al., 2019b). In both situations, the tactile and taste cues from the female are absent. The VOC-NCE model gives an additional opportunity, that is, we can investigate male and female USV individually in a state of the enhanced level of sexual arousal (together with general arousal and sexual motivation). In male rats, sexual arousal is evoked by odors from female urine and the body (Sachs, 1997; Kondo et al., 1999), and in estrus females by hormones, male odors, and the presence of males (Barfield and Thomas, 1986).

Both VOC-NCE and copulation trigger USV, and in both situations enhanced levels of sexual arousal, general arousal, and sexual motivation can be observed. In the VOC-NCE test, direct contact with tactile stimulation essential in copulation is absent (nor taste cues). There is a lack of consummatory elements of sexual interaction, namely, intromission, ejaculation, and hypothetically rewarding mounting. Thus, the rewarding and (eventually) some aversive aspects specific to copulation are not present. Also, we do not observe a relaxation state typically observed after ejaculation, and as an implication, a PEI does not occur (we can observe a rather gradual than a rapid decrease of arousal levels). These differences have a reflection in the changes in the 22-kHz USV mentioned above.

Overall, the VOC-NCE protocol could be used as an expansion of the standard anticipatory PV + copulation, providing an opportunity to obtain individual USV profiles (for summarization of differences between models, refer to Table 1 and Figure 10).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of general arousal (GA), sexual arousal (SA), sexual motivation (SM), and genital tactile (GT) cues in different sexual situations.

Conclusion

Our modification of the non-contact test makes it possible to separate USV emitted by a pair of animals. It provides an opportunity to regulate the intensity of all modalities, including the ultrasonic stream, perceived during sociosexual interactions, and verify the influence of this change on USV profiles. We proposed the VOC-NCE cage as a new, non-invasive tool, expanding the classical sociosexual protocol (anticipatory plus copulatory). This makes it possible to describe changes in arousal and motivation during conditioned procedures by noticing differences in the USV, which should be useful in investigations of sociosexual behavior.

Additional Applications

Our results indicate that the ultrasound vocalization has been emitted intensively with significantly reduced ultrasonic feedback from another animal. In such a view, USV could be considered a behavioral reflex resulting from the activation of central autonomic nuclei (e.g., RVLM/CVLM, NTS, nucleus ambiguus) with effector response of the laryngeal and expiratory muscles (Hernandez-Miranda et al., 2017). Thus, it is comparable to the objective component of emotional response and has a non-discrete informational character perceived by concomitants. During sexual behavior, there is a remarkable parallel increase in sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system activity. Therefore, the vocalization corresponding to this behavior is a reflection of this autonomic equilibrium, and as such, can be applied as a preclinical parameter. It is extremely valuable, especially if we combine it with other autonomic parameters detected by telemetric systems, e.g., heart rate (Filipkowski et al., 2017; Olszyński et al., 2021) and arterial blood pressure (Vianna et al., 2008).

Based on our set of experiments, we tried to present an interesting differentiation in the 22-kHz signals. The results should be considered as an argument for separating them into distinct subgroups, namely, the modulated types of NCE procedure, and the flat, long-lasting postejaculatory. They correspond to very different neurobehavioral contexts and clustering them together with a “negative-feeling” reference seems to be inappropriate. Additionally, the VOC-NCE model might help to answer the question of the communication role of USV via influence on the behavior of the concomitant animal and, with that, assess its communicative function. There is a recurring issue about how much of this vocalization is communicative (Sales and Pye, 1974). To be more precise, what is the proportion between phylogenetically old, non-specific stem-derived motoric reactions of the laryngeal muscles as a result of fluctuating arousal (Delius, 1970; Bell, 1974; Pfaff, 2017) and the evolutionary younger, sociobiological messages controlled by emitters reflecting distinct emotional pathways (Panksepp, 2005; Brudzynski, 2013; Wöhr et al., 2021).

Although this system is dedicated to highly appetitive sociosexual interaction, it might be applicable in other behavioral contexts. For example, during social male-male or female-female interactions, the USV profile can correspond with same-sex preference (Van de Poll and Van Dis, 1979; Olvera-Hernández et al., 2015), social buffering, anxious-type behavior, or agonistic behavior/aggressive reactions.

Additionally, this model may be seen as useful in pharmacological and neurobehavioral experiments to evaluate the potential influence of various agents on sexual parameters, which includes selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, antipsychotic drugs, other 5-HTr modulators, dopamine receptor agents, autonomic nervous system agents, phosphodiesterase inhibitors, nitrates, other types of endogenic substances, and toxic factors (Olivier et al., 2006, 2017, 2022; Lin et al., 2015; Sanna et al., 2015; Chiang and Park, 2020; Esquivel-Franco et al., 2020; Kermath et al., 2022). Similarly, it could be useful in investigations of emotional responses, in addition to models of erectile dysfunction or premature ejaculation (Hull et al., 1994; Sachs, 2000; Waldinger and Olivier, 2005; Olayo-Lortia et al., 2014).

Limitations

In a few cases of singular ultrasounds (not presented here), some form of vocal artifacts was recorded, probably from the concomitant. However, appropriate matching was easy to apply due to the high difference in signal energy. We decided to leave this vestigial ultrasonic leak as an additional non-specific stimulus (blurred and weak, amorphic shapes of ultrasounds cannot be considered to be a distinct communicative signal). Nevertheless, it has to be emphasized that the isolation was nearly total, and the residual leak could be reduced by a slight change in perforation size or adjusting the threshold points in the recording settings. Other limitations stemmed directly from the non-contact procedure and have already been discussed.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Local Ethical Committee in Warsaw.

Author Contributions

WB-R and MB designed and performed the experiments, analyzed the data, and prepared the manuscript. AW analyzed the USV data and prepared the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Medical University of Warsaw and the Young Researcher Grant: 1MA/5/M/MB/N/20/20. The study was carried out with the use of the CePT infrastructure financed by the European Union—the European Regional Development Fund within the Operational Program “Innovative Economy” for 2007–2013.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ refer to https://www.metris.nl/en/products/sonotrack/sonotrack_specs/forspecifications.

References

Ågmo, A., and Snoeren, E. M. S. (2015). Silent or vocalizing rats copulate in a similar manner. PLoS One 10:e0144164. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, U., Karim, K. J. B. A., and Buang, N. A. (2015). A review of the properties and applications of poly (methyl methacrylate)(PMMA). Polymer Rev. 55, 678–705.

Google Scholar

Barfield, R. J., Auerbach, P., Geyer, L. A., and Mcintosh, T. K. (1979). Ultrasonic vocalizations in rat sexual behavior. Am. Zool. 19, 469–480. doi: 10.1093/icb/19.2.469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barfield, R. J., and Geyer, L. A. (1972). Sexual behavior: ultrasonic postejaculatory song of the male rat. Science 176, 1349–1350. doi: 10.1126/science.176.4041.1349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barfield, R. J., and Geyer, L. A. (1975). The ultrasonic postejaculatory vocalization and postejaculatory refractory period of the male rat. J. Comparat. Physiol. Psychol. 88:723. doi: 10.1037/h0076435

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barfield, R. J., and Thomas, D. A. (1986). The role of ultrasonic vocalizations in the regulation of reproduction in rats. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 474, 33–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1986.tb27996.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, R. W. (1974). Ultrasounds in small rodents: arousal-produced and arousal-producing. Dev. Psychobiol. 7, 39–42. doi: 10.1002/dev.420070107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Beck, J., Abramczyk, P., Trzebskj, A., and Przybylski, J. (1996). Sexual behavior in male rats after nitric oxide synthesis inhibition. Physiol. Behav. 60, 139–143. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02272-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Bogacki-Rychlik, W., Kasarello, K., Nikolaev, E., and Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M. (2016). Modulation of 22-khz postejaculatory vocalizations by conditioning to new place: evidence for expression of a positive emotional state. Behav. Neurosci. 130, 415–421. doi: 10.1037/bne0000153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Bogacki-Rychlik, W., Przybylski, J., and Zera, T. (2019a). The sexual motivation of male rats as a tool in animal models of human health disorders. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:257. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Podobinska, M., Barski, J., Bogacki-Rychlik, W., and Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M. (2019b). Distinct classes of low frequency ultrasonic vocalizations in rats during sexual interactions relate to different emotional states. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars) 79, 1–12. doi: 10.21307/ane-2019-001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Nikolaev-Diak, A., Kalata, U., and Nikolaev, E. (2011). Blockade of androgen receptor in the medial amygdala inhibits noncontact erections in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 103, 295–301. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Rydz, M., and Kaczmarek, L. (2000). Precontact 50-kHz vocalizations in male rats during acquisition of sexual experience. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 983–990. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.5.983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., and Sachs, B. D. (2002). Androgen implants in medial amygdala briefly maintain noncontact erection in castrated male rats. Hormones Behav. 42, 345–355. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogacki-Rychlik, W., Rolf, M., and Bialy, M. (2021). Anticipatory 50-kHz precontact ultrasonic vocalizations and sexual motivation: characteristic pattern of ultrasound subtypes in an individual analyzed profile. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:722456. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.722456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brudzynski, S. M. (2007). Ultrasonic calls of rats as indicator variables of negative or positive states: acetylcholine-dopamine interaction and acoustic coding. Behav. Brain Res. 182, 261–273. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brudzynski, S. M. (2013). Ethotransmission: communication of emotional states through ultrasonic vocalization in rats. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brudzynski, S. M. (2015). Pharmacology of ultrasonic vocalizations in adult rats: significance, call classification and neural substrate. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 13, 180–192. doi: 10.2174/1570159x13999150210141444

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., Kroes, R. A., Moskal, J. R., Pfaus, J. G., Brudzynski, S. M., and Panksepp, J. (2008). Ultrasonic vocalizations of rats (Rattus norvegicus) during mating, play, and aggression: behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-administration of playback. J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 357–367. doi: 10.1037/a0012889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., and Panksepp, J. (2006). The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 173–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caruso, A., Ricceri, L., and Scattoni, M. L. (2020). Ultrasonic vocalizations as a fundamental tool for early and adult behavioral phenotyping of Autism Spectrum Disorder rodent models. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 116, 31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, V. S.-C., and Park, J. H. (2020). Glutamate in male and female sexual behavior: receptors, transporters, and steroid independence. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:589882. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.589882

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, G., Serra, M., and Simola, N. (2021). Association between novel object recognition/spontaneous alternation behavior and emission of ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: possible relevance to the study of memory. Brain Sci. 11:1053. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11081053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damsma, G., Pfaus, J. G., Wenkstern, D., Phillips, A. G., and Fibiger, H. C. (1992). Sexual behavior increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of male rats: comparison with novelty and locomotion. Behav. Neurosci. 106:181. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.1.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delius, J. D. (1970). Irrelevant behaviour, information processing and arousal homeostasis. Psychol. Forschung 33, 165–188. doi: 10.1007/bf00424983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esquivel-Franco, D. C., de Boer, S. F., Waldinger, M., Olivier, B., and Olivier, J. D. A. (2020). Pharmacological studies on the role of 5-HT1A receptors in male sexual behavior of wildtype and serotonin transporter knockout rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:40. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Vargas, M. (2018). Vocal signals of sexual motivation in male and female rodents. Curr. Sexual Health Rep. 10, 315–328. doi: 10.1007/s11930-018-0179-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipkowski, R., Polowy, R., Malz, M., Gawrys, O., and Olszynski, K. (2017). Ultrasonic and cardiovascular responses to ultrasonic sounds and vocalizations. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 77(Suppl. 1), 127–128.

Google Scholar

Garai, M., and Guidorzi, P. (2000). European methodology for testing the airborne sound insulation characteristics of noise barriers in situ: experimental verification and comparison with laboratory data. J. Acoustical Soc. Am. 108, 1054–1067. doi: 10.1121/1.1286811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerson, C. A., Mac Cionnaith, C. E., Quintana, G. R., and Pfaus, J. G. (2019). Effects of ovarian hormones on the emission of 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations during distributed clitoral stimulation in the rat. Hormones Behav. 109, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernandez-Miranda, L. R., Ruffault, P.-L., Bouvier, J. C., Murray, A. J., Morin-Surun, M.-P., Zampieri, N., et al. (2017). Genetic identification of a hindbrain nucleus essential for innate vocalization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 8095–8100. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702893114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, E., Lumley, L., Matuszewich, L., Dominguez, J., Moses, J., and Lorrain, D. (1994). The roles of nitric oxide in sexual function of male rats. Neuropharmacology 33, 1499–1504. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(94)90054-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, E. M., and Rodríguez-Manzo, G. (2017). “1.01 - Male sexual behavior,” in Hormones, Brain and Behavior, 3rd Edn, eds D. W. Pfaff and M. Joëls (Oxford: Academic Press), 1–57. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-803592-4.00001-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karwicka, W., Wiatrowska, M., Kondrakiewicz, K., Knapska, E., Kursa, M. B., and Hamed, A. (2021). Relaying aversive ultrasonic alarm calls depends on previous experience. empathy, social buffering, or panic? Brain Sci. 11:759. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11060759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kermath, B. A., Thompson, L. M., Jefferson, J. R., Ward, M. H. B., and Gore, A. C. (2022). Transgenerational effects of prenatal endocrine disruption on reproductive and sociosexual behaviors in sprague dawley male and female rats. Toxics 10:47. doi: 10.3390/toxics10020047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kisko, T. M., Himmler, B. T., Himmler, S. M., Euston, D. R., and Pellis, S. M. (2015). Are 50-kHz calls used as play signals in the playful interactions of rats? II. Evidence from the effects of devocalization. Behav. Processes 111, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kondo, Y., and Hayashi, H. (2021). Neural and hormonal basis of opposite-sex preference by chemosensory signals. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22:8311. doi: 10.3390/ijms22158311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kondo, Y., and Sachs, B. D. (2002). Disparate effects of small medial amygdala lesions on noncontact erection, copulation, and partner preference. Physiol. Behav. 76, 443–447. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00682-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kondo, Y., Tomihara, K., and Sakuma, Y. (1999). Sensory requirements for noncontact penile erection in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 113:1062. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.113.5.1062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtz, R. G., and Adler, N. T. (1973). Electrophysiological correlates of copulatory behavior in the male rat: evidence for a sexual inhibitory process. J. Comparat. Physiol. Psychol. 84:225. doi: 10.1037/h0035265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsson, K. (1956). Conditioning and Sexual Behavior in the Male Albino Rat. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.

Google Scholar

Lenell, C., Broadfoot, C. K., Schaen-Heacock, N. E., and Ciucci, M. R. (2021). Biological and acoustic sex differences in rat ultrasonic vocalization. Brain Sci. 11:459. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11040459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, L. L., Wang, D., Wang, W., Cheng, Y. Q., Su, D. F., and Liu, A. J. (2015). Long-term treatment of clonidine, atenolol, amlodipine and dihydrochlorothiazide, but not enalapril, impairs the sexual function in male spontaneously hypertensive rats. PLoS One 10:e0116155. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorrain, D. S., Matuszewich, L., Friedman, R. D., and Hull, E. M. (1997). Extracellular serotonin in the lateral hypothalamic area is increased during the postejaculatory interval and impairs copulation in male rats. J. Neurosci. 17, 9361–9366. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-09361.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorrain, D. S., Riolo, J. V., Matuszewich, L., and Hull, E. M. (1999). Lateral hypothalamic serotonin inhibits nucleus accumbens dopamine: implications for sexual satiety. J. Neurosci. 19, 7648–7652. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-17-07648.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maggio, J. C., and Whitney, G. (1985). Ultrasonic vocalizing by adult female mice (Mus musculus). J. Comp. Psychol. 99, 420–436. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.99.4.420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzo, J., Cruz, M., Hernandez, M., Pacheco, P., and Sachs, B. (1999). Regulation of noncontact erection in rats by gonadal steroids. Hormones Behav. 35, 264–270. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-de León, J., Pura, J. L., Bernardo, V., and Rodríguez-Pérez, M. A. (2019). Transparent nanocellular PMMA: characterization and modeling of the optical properties. Polymer 170, 16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.polymer.2019.03.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mas, M., Fumero, B., and González-Mora, J. (1995). Voltammetric and microdialysis monitoring of brain monoamine neurotransmitter release during sociosexual interactions. Behav. Brain Res. 71, 69–79. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00043-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mas, M., Gonzalez-Mora, J. L., Louilot, A., Solé, C., and Guadalupe, T. (1990). Increased dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of copulating male rats as evidenced by in vivo voltammetry. Neurosci. Lett. 110, 303–308. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(90)90864-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matochik, J. A., Barfield, R. J., and Nyby, J. (1992). Regulation of sociosexual communication in female Long-Evans rats by ovarian hormones. Hormones Behav. 26, 545–555. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(92)90021-m

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, T. K., and Barfield, R. J. (1980). The temporal patterning of 40–60 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations and copulation in the rat (Rattus norvegicus). Behav. Neural Biol. 29, 349–358. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(80)90259-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miczek, K. A., and Van Der Poel, A. M. (1991). Long ultrasonic calls in male rats following mating, defeat and aversive stimulation: frequency modulation and bout structure. Behaviour 119, 127–142. doi: 10.1163/156853991X00409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olayo-Lortia, J., Ferreira-Nuño, A., Velázquez-Moctezuma, J., and Morales-Otal, A. (2014). Further definition on the multiple partner choice arena: a potential animal model for the study of premature ejaculation. J. Sexual Med. 11, 2428–2438. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivier, B., Chan, J. S. W., Pattij, T., de Jong, T. R., Oosting, R. S., Veening, J. G., et al. (2006). Psychopharmacology of male rat sexual behavior: modeling human sexual dysfunctions? Int. J. Impotence Res. 18, S14–S23. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijir.3901330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivier, J. D., Esquivel Franco, D. C., Oosting, R., Waldinger, M., Sarnyai, Z., and Olivier, B. (2017). Tramadol: effects on sexual behavior in male rats are mainly caused by its 5-HT reuptake blocking effects. Neuropharmacology 116, 50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.11.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivier, J. D., Janssen, J., Pattij, T., Prêtre, S. D., and Olivier, B. (2022). “Antidepressants, sexual behavior, and translational models for male sexual dysfunction: development of animal models, pharmacology, and genetics,” in Translational Research Methods for Major Depressive Disorder, eds Y. K. Kim and M. Amidfar (Berlin: Springer), 451–515. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.02.098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olszyński, K. H., Polowy, R., Wardak, A. D., Grymanowska, A. W., and Filipkowski, R. K. (2021). Increased vocalization of rats in response to ultrasonic playback as a sign of hypervigilance following fear conditioning. Brain Sci. 11:970. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11080970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olvera-Hernández, S., Chavira, R., and Fernández-Guasti, A. (2015). Prenatal letrozole produces a subpopulation of male rats with same-sex preference and arousal as well as female sexual behavior. Physiol. Behav. 139, 403–411. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2005). Psychology. Beyond a joke: from animal laughter to human joy? Science 308, 62–63. doi: 10.1126/science.1112066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaff, D. (2017). How the Vertebrate Brain Regulates Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Pfaus, J., Damsma, G., Nomikos, G. G., Wenkstern, D., Blaha, C., Phillips, A., et al. (1990). Sexual behavior enhances central dopamine transmission in the male rat. Brain Res. 530, 345–348. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91309-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., and Centeno, S. (2001). Conditioning and sexual behavior: a review. Hormones Behav. 40, 291–321. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potasiewicz, A., Holuj, M., Piotrowska, D., Zajda, K., Wojcik, M., Popik, P., et al. (2019). Evaluation of ultrasonic vocalizations in a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia during the early life stages of rats. Neuropharmacology 146, 28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.11.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D. (1978). “Conceptual and neural mechanisms of masculine copulatory behavior,” in Sex and Behavior, eds T. E. McGill, D. A. Dewsbury, and B. D. Sachs (Berlin: Springer), 267–295

Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D. (1997). Erection evoked in male rats by airborne scent from estrous females. Physiol. Behav. 62, 921–924. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00307-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D. (2000). Contextual approaches to the physiology and classification of erectile function, erectile dysfunction, and sexual arousal. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 541–560. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00022-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D., Akasofu, K., Citron, J. H., Daniels, S. B., and Natoli, J. H. (1994). Noncontact stimulation from estrous females evokes penile erection in rats. Physiol. Behav. 55, 1073–1079. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90390-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D., and Barfield, R. J. (1976). “Functional analysis of masculine copulatory behavior in the rat,” in Advances in the Study of Behavior, eds J. S. Rosenblatt, R. A. Hinde, E. Shaw, and C. Beer (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 91–154. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(08)60166-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D., and Bialy, M. (2000). Female presence during postejaculatory interval facilitates penile erection and 22-kHz vocalization in male rats. Behav. Neurosci. 114:1203. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.6.1203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sales, G., and Pye, D. (1974). “Ultrasound in rodents,” in Ultrasonic Communication by Animals, eds G. Sales and D. Pye (Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands), 149–201. doi: 10.1007/978-94-011-6901-1_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sangiamo, D. T., Warren, M. R., and Neunuebel, J. P. (2020). Ultrasonic signals associated with different types of social behavior of mice. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 411–422. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0584-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanna, F., Contini, A., Melis, M. R., and Argiolas, A. (2015). Role of dopamine D4 receptors in copulatory behavior: studies with selective D4 agonists and antagonists in male rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 137, 110–118. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2015.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seffer, D., Schwarting, R. K. W., and Wöhr, M. (2014). Pro-social ultrasonic communication in rats: insights from playback studies. J. Neurosci. Methods 234, 73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.01.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taracha, E., Hamed, A., Krza̧ścik, P., Lehner, M., Skórzewska, A., Płaźnik, A., et al. (2012). Inter-individual diversity and intra-individual stability of amphetamine-induced sensitization of frequency-modulated 50-kHz vocalization in Sprague–Dawley rats. Psychopharmacology 222, 619–632. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2658-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, D. A., Talalas, L., and Barfield, R. J. (1981). Effect of devocalization of the male on mating behavior in rats. J. Comparat. Physiol. Psychol. 95:630. doi: 10.1037/h0077803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Poll, N., and Van Dis, H. (1979). The effect of medial preoptic—anterior hypothalamic lesions on bisexual behavior of the male rat. Brain Res. Bull. 4, 505–511. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(79)90035-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vianna, D. M., Allen, C., and Carrive, P. (2008). Cardiovascular and behavioral responses to conditioned fear after medullary raphe neuronal blockade. Neuroscience 153, 1344–1353. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.03.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vivian, J. A., and Miczek, K. A. (1993). Morphine attenuates ultrasonic vocalization during agonistic encounters in adult male rats. Psychopharmacology 111, 367–375. doi: 10.1007/BF02244954

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldinger, M. D., and Olivier, B. (2005). Animal models of premature and retarded ejaculation. World J. Urol. 23, 115–118. doi: 10.1007/s00345-004-0493-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, M., Clein, R., Spurrier, M., Roth, E., and Neunuebel, J. (2020). Ultrashort-range, high-frequency communication by female mice shapes social interactions. Sci. Rep. 10:2637. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59418-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, N. R., Cagiano, R., Moises, A. U., and Barfield, R. J. (1990). Changes in mating vocalizations over the ejaculatory series in rats (Rattus norvegicus). J. Comparat. Psychol. 104:255. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.104.3.255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wöhr, M., Kisko, T. M., and Schwarting, R. K. W. (2021). Social behavior and ultrasonic vocalizations in a genetic rat model haploinsufficient for the cross-disorder risk gene Cacna1c. Brain Sci. 11:724. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11060724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wöhr, M., and Schwarting, R. K. W. (2013). Affective communication in rodents: ultrasonic vocalizations as a tool for research on emotion and motivation. Cell Tissue Res. 354, 81–97. doi: 10.1007/s00441-013-1607-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, J. M., Gourdon, J. C., and Clarke, P. B. (2010). Identification of multiple call categories within the rich repertoire of adult rat 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations: effects of amphetamine and social context. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 211, 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1859-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ultrasonic vocalization, ultrasound separation, sexual behavior, 22-kHz, 50-kHz, rats

Citation: Bogacki-Rychlik W, Wrona A and Bialy M (2022) A Protocol for the Non-invasive Method of Ultrasound Separation During the Sociosexual Vocal-Non-contact Model in Rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:910591. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.910591

Received: 01 April 2022; Accepted: 21 April 2022;
Published: 25 May 2022.

Edited by:

Luigia Trabace, University of Foggia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Theresa Kisko, University of Marburg, Germany
Hideaki Inagaki, Aichi Medical University, Japan

Copyright © 2022 Bogacki-Rychlik, Wrona and Bialy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michal Bialy, michal.bialy@wum.edu.pl, michalbialy@yahoo.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.