Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 23 January 2023
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards a Mechanistic Understanding of Depression, Anxiety, and Their Comorbidity: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience View all 9 articles

The unique face of comorbid anxiety and depression: increased interoceptive fearfulness and reactivity

  • 1Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, United States
  • 2Oxley College of Health Sciences, University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, United States

Anxiety and depression commonly co-occur, yet the underlying brain and behavioral processes are poorly understood. Here we examined the hypothesis that individuals with comorbid anxiety and depression would show increased fearful reactivity to an aversive interoceptive perturbation relative to depressed-only individuals. One-hundred and eighty anxious and/or depressed participants from the Tulsa 1000 study completed multi-level behavioral or functional magnetic resonance imaging assessments of interoception and nociception including breath-hold and cold-pressor challenges, and heartbeat perception and interoceptive attention tasks. One-hundred and four individuals with comorbid depression and anxiety disorders (Dep+Anx) were propensity matched with 52 individuals with depression-only (Dep). Data were analyzed using mixed-effects linear regression. The Dep+Anx group showed significantly greater self-reported fear of suffocation during breath holding (Wilcoxon r = 0.23) and reduced cold pain tolerance (R2 = 0.027) signified by hand removal during immersion. However, these groups did not differ with respect to neutrally-valenced behavioral indices of heartbeat perception or neural indices of interoceptive attention. Individuals with comorbid depression and anxiety, vs. those with only depression, show increased respiratory fearfulness and nociceptive reactivity during perturbations of these signals, whilst showing similar interoceptive awareness in the absence of perturbation. Our findings suggest that individuals with comorbid anxiety and depression process aversive interoceptive and nociceptive signals differently than those with depression alone, providing support for a process model of increased threat sensitivity and hyperarousal in anxious depression.

Introduction

Nearly half of all individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) also have an anxiety disorder (Kessler et al., 2015). Thus, comorbid MDD and anxiety disorders (e.g., generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder, panic disorder, or specific phobia) are among the most common presentations of affective psychopathology in psychiatric settings. Anxious depression is associated with greater treatment resistance (Ionescu et al., 2014), worse side effect profiles (Gaspersz et al., 2017), poorer treatment outcomes (Penninx et al., 2011), quicker symptom relapse (Fava et al., 2008) and higher levels of suicidal ideation (Kessler et al., 2015) than non-anxious depression. However, treatment engagement appears to be higher in this comorbid group (Kessler et al., 2015). Despite the pervasiveness of anxious depression, the neural and behavioral processes characterizing this form of psychopathology remain incompletely understood. A better understanding of the underlying process dysfunctions may be an important first step in the development of more mechanistically oriented interventions.

Depression and anxiety share abnormalities across a number of different neurocognitive processes, such as negative affect (Clark and Watson, 1991), life stress (Syed and Nemeroff, 2017), and cognitive function (Millan et al., 2012). However, studies comparing the mechanisms of anxious vs. non-anxious depression are rare, which may be one factor impeding improvements in treatment development. Hyperarousal, defined as an abnormal state of increased responsiveness to stimuli that is marked by various physiological and psychological symptoms, is a feature of both anxiety and anxious depression (Clark and Watson, 1991) that is not present in non-anxious depression. It may therefore be a useful intermediary target mechanism. Since anxiety disorders are also commonly characterized by abnormalities in the domain of interoception (Khalsa et al., 2018), the mechanistic probing of interoception in anxious depression might inform a deeper understanding of the underlying pathophysiology that could help to tease apart the differences between anxious and non-anxious depression.

Interoception refers to the process by which the nervous system senses, interprets, and integrates signals originating from within the body (Khalsa et al., 2018). Interoceptive awareness has many features, including: (1) interoceptive attention (i.e., cued or spontaneous focus on internal physiological sensations); (2) interoceptive accuracy (i.e., the match between true and consciously perceived internal physiological sensations); (3) interoceptive magnitude (i.e., the perceived intensity of internal physiological sensations (Khalsa and Lapidus, 2016; Khalsa et al., 2018); (4) interoceptive sensibility (i.e., self-perceived tendency to focus on interoceptive stimuli (Garfinkel et al., 2015); and (5) interoceptive insight (i.e., the metacognitive correspondence between self-perceived and objective measures of interoceptive accuracy). Whether, and how often interoceptive processes enter awareness varies according to the organ system and level of physiological arousal, and therefore multimodal assessments involving self-report, behavioral, and physiological forms of measurement across multiple domains may be important for gaining an informed perspective on this process. The insular cortex is a key hub within the neural circuitry implicated in interoception (Berntson and Khalsa, 2021; Nord et al., 2021). Related to this, models of embodied emotion (Craig, 2008; Smith and Lane, 2015; Barrett, 2017) have been proposed that integrate interoception with wider emotional processing and motivation. In this view, the co-activation of regions such as the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) during emotion processing (Thielscher and Pessoa, 2007) and across psychiatric disorders (McTeague et al., 2020) is consistent with the view that an emotional experience involves body states detected via interoceptive processes that may be altered in psychopathology. It follows from this that functional neuroimaging tasks which specifically perturb interoception can elucidate the neural circuits underlying interoceptive psychopathology (Smith et al., 2021; Teed et al., 2022). Furthermore, the anatomical overlap between circuits associated with fear conditioning (Maren and Quirk, 2004) and the central autonomic network (Thayer and Lane, 2000; Smith et al., 2017) includes the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), cingulate cortex, insula, and amygdala. This overlap forms the basis of a neurovisceral model of fear conditioning (Battaglia and Thayer, 2022) and makes the study of aversive interoception particularly relevant.

Aberrant interoception has been strongly linked to many forms of psychopathology beyond anxious depression, including MDD (Avery et al., 2014; Wiebking et al., 2015), substance use disorders (Paulus and Stewart, 2014; Stewart et al., 2020), eating disorders (Khalsa et al., 2015; Kerr et al., 2016; Berner et al., 2018), suicidal behaviors (DeVille et al., 2020, 2021), and schizophrenia (Yao and Thakkar, 2022). Yet findings in depression are mixed. For example, in one study (Dunn et al., 2007), a severely depressed group did not differ from healthy comparators in heartbeat perception accuracy, whereas a more moderate group showed significantly lower accuracy, as also seen in other studies (Terhaar et al., 2012). This could suggest a non-linear effect of depression on interoception (Eggart et al., 2019). Alternatively, it might be explained by the heterogeneity of depression and/or the inclusion of co-morbid anxiety in the patient groups under investigation. Depression has been generally associated with blunted cardiac interoceptive awareness (Pollatos et al., 2009; Furman et al., 2013), whereas anxiety has often been conceptually associated with increased sensitivity (Van der Does et al., 2000; Domschke et al., 2010), perhaps driven by abnormally heightened interoceptive prediction signals of a somatic threat (Paulus and Stein, 2006; Khalsa and Feinstein, 2018). Interactions in the effect of depressive and anxiety symptoms on interoception have been suggested dimensionally in healthy individuals (Pollatos et al., 2009) and those with mood disorder symptoms (Dunn et al., 2010), although firm conclusions from such studies are limited by reliance on a measure of cardiac interoception with several drawbacks (Desmedt et al., 2018; Ring and Brener, 2018; Corneille et al., 2020; Ferentzi et al., 2022). Despite the available evidence of transdiagnostic interoceptive dysfunction, there is little work comparing interoception in individuals with MDD vs. those with MDD and comorbid anxiety.

The goal of the current cross-sectional investigation was to determine whether individuals with anxious depression show different sensitivity to interoceptive sensations of a threat compared to non-anxious depressed individuals. Our approach was to compare a sample of depressed individuals who were propensity-matched based on demographic characteristics to individuals with comorbid anxiety and depression using an interoceptive battery of tasks developed to measure different aspects of cardiorespiratory interoceptive and nociceptive processing. Our overarching hypothesis was that individuals with anxious depression would show increased interoceptive reactivity to aversive sensations relative to their depressed counterparts.

Materials and methods

Participants

Data analyzed for the current study included a group of 180 anxious and/or depressed participants (132 female) who were selected from the first 500 participant cohort of the Tulsa 1000 project (Victor et al., 2018), a naturalistic longitudinal study that recruited a community sample of 1,000 individuals with various forms of psychopathology, based on the dimensional NIMH Research Domain Criteria framework. Participants were between 18 and 55 years of age at the time of assessments (mean age = 35.6, standard deviation = 11.5). Depression and anxiety screening inclusion criteria included the following symptom scores: (1) Patient Health Questionnaire [PHQ-9, (Levis et al., 2019)] ≥10; and/or (2) Overall Anxiety Symptom and Impairment Scale [OASIS, (Campbell-Sills et al., 2009)] ≥8. Exclusion criteria were positive urine drug screen; lifetime bipolar, schizophrenia spectrum, antisocial personality, or obsessive compulsive disorders; current suicidal ideation with intent or plan; moderate to severe traumatic brain injury; severe and or unstable medical concerns; changes in psychiatric medication dose in the last 6 weeks; and Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) contraindications. Participants who met the criteria for substance use or eating disorders were excluded from analysis in the current study. Full inclusion/exclusion criteria can be found in the supplement and the parent project protocol article (Victor et al., 2018). Ethical approval was obtained from the Western Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and received financial compensation for participation. ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: #NCT02450240.

The initial analysis included 52 participants with non-anxious depression (Dep) and 128 participants with comorbid depression and anxiety disorder (Dep+Anx). To reduce potential confounds, 104 participants from the Dep+Anx group were propensity matched for age, sex, and education at a ratio of 2:1 with 52 participants from the Dep group using the MatchIt package in R (Ho et al., 2011). These two groups did not differ in their level of depression (PHQ-9), age, or body mass index (see Table 1) but, crucially, had significantly different levels of self-report anxiety sensitivity [Anxiety Sensitivity Index; ASI, (Taylor et al., 2007)] and anxiety severity (OASIS). There were significantly more racially white people in the Dep+Anx group but other racial groups did not significantly differ.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of propensity matched Depressed (Dep) and depressed and anxious (Dep+Anx) participants.

General procedure

General procedures included a clinical interview session, a neuroimaging session, and a behavioral (psychophysiology) session—all completed within 2 weeks on average. Although the parent project (i.e., the Tulsa 1000) consisted of a broader range of protocols (comprising ~23.5 h of testing), only details relevant to the current study (comprising ~7 h of testing) are presented here. See the supplement and protocol article (Victor et al., 2018) for details.

Study clinicians (masters or nurse level assistants, supervised by licensed clinical psychologists and board-certified psychiatrists) administered the MINI (Sheehan et al., 1998) structured clinical interview to establish the current presence of depressive episodes, anxiety disorder, substance use disorder or eating disorders and to rule out bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, personality disorders or obsessive compulsive disorder. During this session, participants also provided self-reported information on sociodemographics (i.e., age, education, income, ethnicity, and race), anxiety [OASIS, Anxiety Sensitivity Index; ASI (Taylor et al., 2007)], approach and avoidance motivation [Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Approach Scale; BIS/BAS (Carver and White, 1994)], depression (PHQ-9), emotional processing [Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; PANAS (Watson et al., 1988)], impulsivity [UPPS Impulse Behavior Scale (Whiteside et al., 2005)], pleasure [Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale; TEPS (Gard et al., 2006)], substance use (Patient Reported Outcome Measurement Information System; PROMIS), and trauma [Traumatic Events Questionnaire (Vrana and Lauterbach, 1994)]. For the current study, we focused on dimensional measures of anxiety/threat sensitivity (ASI), and depression [PROMIS depression scale (Cella et al., 2010)].

Interoceptive battery

The interoceptive battery of tasks consisted of all interoceptive tasks included in the T1000 study and are described fully in DeVille et al. (2020, 2021) and Lapidus et al. (2020). Brief descriptions follow below.

Breath-hold challenge

Each participant completed two inspiratory breath-hold trials (Asmundson and Stein, 1994), providing a brief measure of endogenous sensitivity to respiratory perturbation. Participants were seated in front of a computer screen, fitted with a respiration belt and electrocardiogram sensors (Biopac Systems, Inc.), and provided with a nose clip to prevent inadvertent respirations (see Lapidus et al., 2020 for a detailed description of the procedure). During normal breathing, concentrations of oxygen and carbon dioxide were analyzed from their exhaled air, providing a baseline measurement. Participants were then instructed to inhale maximally and, at the end of inhalation, to begin holding their breath for as long as they were able to tolerate. Participants were instructed to exhale into a capnometer-connected breathing tube (Oxigraf, Inc.) when they were no longer able to tolerate the breath hold. Following each breath hold, participants provided subjective ratings of the task (i.e., intensity, unpleasantness, and difficulty) as well as ratings of associated psychological experiences (i.e., stress, required effort, breathlessness, urge to breathe, sensations of suffocation, fear of suffocation) on visual analog scales (VAS) ranging from 0 (not at all) to 100 (extremely). We hypothesized that the Dep+Anx group would report more negative ratings/experiences of the breath-hold than the Dep group.

Cold-pressor challenge

Participants immersed their dominant hand in a circulating pool of water cooled to 6 degrees Celsius (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc., Arctic A25 Circulator). They were asked to keep their hand submerged for as long as they could tolerate. During the task, participants made continuous real-time pain intensity ratings on a scale ranging from 0 (no pain) to 100 (worst pain imaginable). These ratings were used to calculate each participant’s peak pain rating, as well as the time to reach ratings of mild (25/100), moderate (50/100), and peak pain. Afterward, each participant provided VAS ratings of unpleasantness, difficulty, and stress ranging from 0 (not at all) to 100 (extremely). See DeVille et al. (2020) for further details. We hypothesized that the Dep+Anx group would remove their hand more quickly than the Dep group and rate the experience more negatively.

Heartbeat-perception task

To assess cardiac interoception, participants performed a heartbeat-tapping task across three interoceptive conditions. During each condition, participants were instructed to press a key on a keyboard every time they felt their heartbeat, without taking their pulse. In the first trial (“guess”), subjects were instructed to tap every time they felt their heartbeat without taking their pulse. Guessing was encouraged if they felt unsure. In the next trial (“no guess”), guessing was discouraged, and participants were asked to tap only when they felt confident in feeling their heartbeat. In the final trial (“breath hold perturbation”), participants were instructed to inhale deeply, hold their breath, and tap along with their perceived heartbeats while sustaining the breath hold, without guessing. The breath-hold was expected to amplify cardiac sensations and presumably increase heartbeat-perception accuracy, and computational work in this task has shown a failure to adapt interoceptive processing in psychopathology (Smith et al., 2020). Afterward, participants provided VAS ratings ranging from 0 (not at all) to 100 (extremely) to indicate their perceived heartbeat intensity, confidence in their ability to accurately estimate their heartbeat, and their assessment of task-related difficulty. Interoceptive accuracy was quantified using the Beat-to-Tap consistency metric. This measure is described fully by Smith et al. (2021). Briefly, it quantifies how consistent the delay is between when a person taps and the event of interest (heartbeat or tone), compared to what would be expected if they tapped randomly (i.e., if taps did not track the timing of heartbeats or tones, irrespective of potential reaction time differences). This was regarded as an exploratory analysis to assess potential differences in the consistency of interoceptive estimates in non-threatening settings.

Interoceptive attention task

Participants completed two runs of the visceral interoceptive attention (VIA) task (Simmons et al., 2013), wherein they were presented with three conditions cued by a visually presented word (10 s duration): (1) “heart” cued internal attention toward heartbeat sensations; (2) “stomach” cued internal attention toward stomach sensations; and (3) “target” cued external attention toward word color changes at varying intensities. The VIA task has been previously shown to be effective at mapping the neural signal associated with interoceptive attention in several psychiatric and non-psychiatric populations (Kerr et al., 2016; DeVille et al., 2020; Stewart et al., 2020). Participants were asked to provide ratings of stimulus intensity (0 = “no sensation” to 6 = “extreme sensation”) after 50% of trials, which also helped to ensure they remained awake and were attending to the task. Each run included six trials per condition (intertrial interval range 2.5–12.5 s). Regions of interest (ROIs) from within the insula were chosen as these are typical regions evoked by this task (see above citations). The vmPFC was also examined following our recent findings showing reduced vmPFC activation was related to differences in interoception between individuals with generalized anxiety disorder compared to healthy comparisons (Teed et al., 2022). This was also regarded as an exploratory analysis to assess any differences in neural signals underlying interoceptive attention in non-threatening settings.

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data acquisition and preprocessing

Structural and functional MRI data were acquired on two identical GE Discovery MR750 3T scanners operating identical pulse sequences for functional [repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE) = 2,000/27 ms, the field of view (FOV)/slice = 240/2.9 mm, 128 × 128 matrix, 39 axial slices, 180 TRs] and structural scans [magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient recalled echo (MP-RAGE) TR/TE = 5/2.012 ms, FOV/slice = 240×192/0.9 mm, 186 axial slices]. Single-subject preprocessing was completed using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software (Cox, 1996). The first three TRs were discarded, followed by despiking, slice-timing correction, co-registration to anatomical volumes, motion correction, transformation to Montreal Neurological Institute space via an affine transformation, application of a 4 mm Gaussian full-width at half-max smoothing kernel, and a voxelwise general linear model analysis. Block regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function and used to model blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response for heart, stomach, and target conditions. Six motion parameters (three translations and three rotations) were included as nuisance regressors. Censoring was done at the regression step by removing volumes with either a Euclidean norm of the derivatives of the six motion parameters greater than 0.3 mm or greater than 10% outlier voxels, determined by 3dToutcount. Percent signal change during each condition was defined as the estimated beta coefficient from single-subject analysis, which was relative to the implicit baseline during unmodeled fixation and scaled by the zeroth order regressor to convert to percent signal change.

Data analysis

For analyses with repeated measures, linear mixed effects (LME) regression analyses were conducted using the lmerTest package in R. For analyses without repeated measures, data were analyzed using linear models in R. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied to each regression model to examine F tests for interactions and main effects. Significant interactions were followed up with an examination of fixed-effects. The Kenward–Roger approximation of degrees of freedom was used. Effect sizes (R2) for significant findings were estimated using the r2glmm package in R (Jaeger, 2017). Benjamini–Hochberg adjustments were applied within each set of analyses to account for repeated testing. The ggplot2 package (Wickham et al., 2016) was used to visualize data.

There were some missing data from certain tasks on the basis of nonadherence to task instructions (i.e., manually taking their pulse during the heartbeat-perception task; 19 participants), experimenter error (three participants) and refusal to complete the cold-pressor task, (one participant). Data for these individuals were excluded.

Analysis of behavioral tasks

For the breath-hold challenge, cold-pressor challenge, and heartbeat-tapping task, LME models were used to examine the relationship between the group and the dependent measure for each task. A random effect of participants (random intercept) was included within each model and covariates of Age, Sex, and Medication status were included. Individual fixed-effects specifications are provided for each model below. VAS ratings for each task were also compared between groups. Since a proportion of the VAS ratings were not normally distributed, Mann-Whitney tests, which are robust to deviations from normality, were used to compare ratings between groups, and a Wilcoxon R was reported for effect size.

Breath-hold and cold-pressor challenges

For the breath-hold task, duration in seconds was the primary dependent measure. Group, trial repetition, and the interaction between group and trial were included as fixed effects. For the cold pressor challenge, duration in seconds was the primary dependent measure. Group, time point (i.e., markers of mild pain, moderate pain, peak pain, and task discontinuation), and the interaction between group and time point were included as fixed effects.

Heartbeat-perception task

To counteract known limitations of the traditional formula used to measure heartbeat accuracy, we used our previously developed beat-to-tap consistency measure (Smith et al., 2021). We focused on the no-guess and perturbation conditions of this task due to the known limitations of the guessing condition (Desmedt et al., 2018). Group, condition (i.e., no-guess and perturbation), and the interaction between group and condition were included as fixed effects.

Interoceptive attention task

ROI analyses were performed focusing on insular and vmPFC subregions. Specifically, we focused on group differences in signal change in BOLD on the contrast of interoceptive vs. exteroceptive attention (INT-EXT) within six bilateral insula ROIs based on Brainntome atlas (Fan et al., 2016) subregions (hypergranular, ventral agranular, dorsal agranular, ventral dysgranular/granular, dorsal granular and dorsal dysgranular insula), and four vmPFC ROIs based on meta-analyses of fMRI studies related to valence processing (Lindquist et al., 2016), sympathetic autonomic control (Beissner et al., 2013) and self-processing (Murray et al., 2012). Group, laterality, and the interaction between group and laterality were included as fixed effects. An exploratory whole brain analysis was also carried out in AFNI using 3dMVM on the contrast of interoceptive vs. exteroceptive attention (INT-EXT), with an initial voxel height threshold of p < 0.001 and a cluster extent threshold of 20 voxels.

Power calculation

Power was calculated using G-Power software. Assuming small effect sizes (Cohen f = 0.1/Cohen d = 0.2), a critical alpha of p = 0.05, and the given sample size, the analyses had the following power to detect an effect: Cold-Pressor Challenge = 90%; Breath-Hold Challenge and Heartbeat-Perception Task = 76%; VIA Task = 33%.

Results

Breath-hold challenge

Anxious depression was significantly associated with VAS ratings of fear of suffocation, such that the Dep+Anx group reported greater fear of suffocation [mean (M) = 22.5, standard deviation (SD) = 25.0] than those in the Dep group (M = 12.0, SD = 18.2; p = 0.005, corrected p = 0.046, Wilcoxon r = 0.23; see Figure 1). Feelings of suffocation were also greater in the Dep+Anx group (M = 43.2, SD = 29.3) compared to the Dep group (M = 32.8, SD = 28.1), but this did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (p = 0.03, corrected p = 0.136, Wilcoxon r = 0.18). All other group comparisons of VAS ratings were nonsignificant (corrected p ranging from 0.583 to 0.821, see Table 2 for full details). For breath-hold duration, there was a significant main effect of trial, F(1,154) = 31.61, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.17, indicating that both groups held their breath for significantly longer in the second trial compared to the first trial. However, there was no significant effect of group, F(1,77) = 1.46, p = 0.22, and no Group × Trial interaction, F(1,154) = 0.56, p = 0.45.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Self-report ratings of suffocation fear in the Breath-Hold Task. Note that the Dep+Anx group reported significantly higher suffocation fear than the Dep group. Violin plots visualize distribution of the data, horizontal line represents the median. Dep+Anx, comorbid depression and anxiety disorders; Dep, depression only. **p < 0.01.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Full statistical results from Breath-hold Challenge, Heartbeat-Perception Task, Cold Pressor Challenge, and Interoceptive Attention Task.

Heartbeat-perception task

For heartbeat beat-to-tap consistency (see Supplementary Material), mixed-effects linear regression showed that there were no significant effects of group, F(1,238) = 1.26, p = 0.26, and the interaction between group × condition was also not significant, F(1,126) = 2.14, p = 0.15. Similarly, Wilcoxon rank sum tests indicated that there were no group differences in VAS ratings of overall difficulty, intensity, or confidence regarding the heartbeat-perception task (corrected p ranging from 0.716 to 0.992). See Table 2 for full details.

Cold-pressor challenge

Across all participants, mixed-effects linear regression found that the cold-pressor challenge elicited increased pain ratings over time (F(3,462) = 121.46, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.585). There was a significant interaction between timepoint × group (F(3,462) = 4.32, p = 0.005, R2 = 0.027). This was driven by the Dep group keeping their hands submerged in the cold water for significantly longer (approximately 16 s longer) than the Dep+Anx group after reaching their peak pain level (t(449) = 3.62, p = 0.0003, b = 15.73), without any statistically significant differences in the amount of time taken to reach self-reported mild, moderate, and peak pain levels (Figure 2 and Table 2). We report fixed effects and model summary values in Table 3. Wilcoxon rank sum tests indicated that there were no significant differences in average ratings of unpleasantness, pain, difficulty, and stress between the two groups (corrected p ranging from 0.581 to 0.930).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Self-report ratings of pain and behavioral hand removal in the Cold-Pressor Task. Note that the Dep+Anx group removed their hand significantly more quickly than the Dep group. Error bars represent ± 1 SE of the mean. Dep+Anx, comorbid depression and anxiety disorders; Dep, depression only. **p < 0.01.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Model parameters from the LME model for the Cold-Pressor Challenge.

Interoceptive attention task

For the exploratory insula and vmPFC ROI analyses, mixed effects regression analyses indicated that there were no significant effects of group (all corrected p’s > 0.41, see Table 2) or group × laterality interactions (all corrected p’s > 0.552) for any of the six insula or four vmPFC ROIs. A subsequent exploratory whole brain analysis did not reveal any significant clusters of interest differentiating the groups. However, across both the groups there was clear evidence of dorsal mid-insula activation to the contrast of INT-EXT attention (all corrected p’s < 0.001), consistent with prior studies.

Discussion

This study examined whether individuals with anxious depression exhibit increased sensitivity to aversive interoceptive signals relative to demographically matched individuals with non-anxious depression. There were two main results: first, the Dep+Anx group reported greater fear of suffocation during a breath-hold task, despite similar breath-hold durations compared to the Dep group. Second, the Dep+Anx group removed their hand from painful cold water more quickly than the Dep group, despite having similar self-reported pain ratings over time. However, there were no differences between the groups on the heartbeat-perception task or the VIA task. This could be explained by the Dep+Anx group (relative to Dep only and possibly relative to healthy comparisons) perceiving body signals that accompany the breath-hold task as more threatening and reporting this subjectively. Then, in the cold-pressor task this Dep+Anx group reacted in such a way as to avoid threatening nociceptive signals by terminating the trial earlier. Together, these results support the hypothesis that anxious depression is characterized by an exaggerated response to aversive interoceptive signals relative to those with non-anxious depression and reduced tolerance of nociceptive threat. To our knowledge, this is the first time that differences in interoceptive reactivity between anxious vs. non-anxious depression have been identified, which was specifically enabled by our propensity-matching approach.

Previously, we showed increased emotional reactivity to threat in a startle EMG task in anxious depression compared to non-anxious depression, using the same propensity-matched sample (Ironside et al., 2022). An association between emotional reactivity and increased interoceptive awareness has also been previously shown (Pollatos et al., 2007; McTeague et al., 2011), which may suggest that a chronically increased outflow of sympathetic signals might be one variable contributing to the establishment of high interoceptive sensitivity/fearfulness, though potentially to the detriment of interoceptive accuracy (Paulus and Stein, 2010). Interestingly, in the cold pressor challenge, both groups had similar pain ratings over time, but the Dep+Anx group turned these feelings (pain) into action (hand removal) more quickly than the Dep group. This could be explained by reduced cognitive control or stronger emotional experiences in anxious depression for the same level of pain. Another possibility is a potential limbic-motor dysfunction in non-anxious depression, which prevents the interoceptive signal from activating motivational resources. In contrast, we see at least a preservation, if not an exaggeration of motivational activation to bodily sensations of threat in anxious depression. One explanation of altered interoception in psychiatric illnesses suggests a combination of rigid and exaggerated expectations of aversive interoceptive experiences and low signal-to-noise ratio of visceral input to the insula, resulting in erroneous evaluation of benign signal changes as significant, with accordingly motivated action (Paulus et al., 2019). It is unclear if the group difference seen here represents limbic motor dysfunction in non-anxious depression or erroneous evaluation in anxious depression, or (likely) a combination of the two.

Previous work based on symptom patterns supports a tripartite model (Clark and Watson, 1991), i.e., a bifactor model consisting of a general negative affect factor and specific depression and anxiety factors. The anxiety-specific factor in the tripartite model is hyperarousal. In the breath-hold and cold pressor tasks, the aversive interoceptive signal activates a defensive system. As expected from the tripartite model, the Dep+Anx group show hyperarousal, represented by higher fear of suffocation and quicker hand removal, which is not observed in the Dep group, suggesting a key difference between these sub-groups. In terms of behavior, the approach-withdrawal model helps tease apart the different factors present in anxious depression, with depression being associated with reduced approach and anxiety being associated with increased avoidance (Davidson, 1998). In the present study, the non-anxious depressed group showed a reduced degree of defensive action during a nociceptive threat (i.e., hand withdrawal), which was preserved in anxious depression. Future research should examine how those with anxious vs. non-anxious depression respond to treatments targeting the defensive action system. Compared to a group with anxiety only, it seems possible that an anxious depressed group might show both reduced approach and increased avoidance (e.g., using approach/avoidance threat paradigms). Further studies are needed to tease apart the potentially unique disease that the intersection of anxiety and depression represents.

We previously proposed (Ironside et al., 2022) a process model for anxiety and anxious depression to help distinguish disease modifiable processes that could be useful targets in treatment development. Individuals with anxiety may be characterized by an excessive defense system that takes incoming or internally generated stimuli and evaluates them with respect to threat-to-self, with a high prior expectation of aversive outcomes (Paulus et al., 2019). This process may be highly taxing and may result in exhausting affective processing capacities ultimately resulting in depression. This proposition is consistent with the observation that two-thirds of individuals with lifetime comorbid anxiety disorders and MDD reported an earlier age-of-onset of their first anxiety disorder than their MDD (Kessler et al., 2015). It is unclear whether this subsequent depression is characterized by a lack of response to both positive/negative affect, i.e., if anxious depression often becomes non-anxious depression eventually. It is also unlikely that the entire sub-group will show the same phased process. In comparison, depressed-only individuals might be characterized by a primary lack of reactivity to positive and negative stimuli, which results in a lack of anxious responding even when threatening stimuli are encountered. Therefore, whereas the primary disease-modifying process for anxious depression would be to attenuate threat-related processing, the primary disease-modifying process for non-anxious depression would be to enhance valence-independent motivation in general. A lot of focus is given to reward responsivity in depression, but we propose a more general factor of blunted motivation (both approach and avoidance motivation), supported by our prior (Ironside et al., 2022) and current findings. This would modify the tripartite model, transforming the anxiety-specific factor of hyperarousal into a general “arousal/motivation” factor, with depression and anxiety pulling in opposing directions. One consequence of this suggestion would be that treatments targeting hyperarousal in non-anxious depression could actually worsen an already blunted emotional reactivity. Interestingly, the same pattern of blunted aversive interoception was found when comparing the larger T500 study sample in terms of their past suicide attempt history, with the past attempters showing higher pain tolerance in the cold pressor challenge and less suffocation on a breath-hold task (DeVille et al., 2020, 2021). Perhaps this is descriptive of the process model suggested above, with initial anxious depression as a predecessor to the potentially more chronic state of non-anxious depression. However, this is at odds with worse reported outcomes and higher rates of suicidality in anxious depression.

The present study had several limitations. First, case-control designs and cross-sectional studies cannot support causal inferences. Longitudinal designs will be necessary to determine cause and effect. Second, the Dep+Anx group was twice as large, which may have increased the statistical power to detect effects in the Dep+Anx vs. the Dep-only group. However, as there were no group differences in the heartbeat task or the VIA tasks, this concern does not appear to have indiscriminately influenced the current findings. Third, we used a somewhat imprecise, albeit commonly employed, measure of nociceptive stimulation in the cold-pressor challenge. It would be advantageous to clarify whether anxious depression is associated with impaired processing of other pain signals, such as heat (Thompson et al., 2008) or pinprick forms of pain. Furthermore, heartbeat perception tasks have been the subject of criticism, reflecting the difficulty of assessing cardiac interoception at rest (Khalsa and Lapidus, 2016). We attempted to overcome this with our endogenous perturbation approach (i.e., breath-hold incorporation), but other exogenous perturbation approaches (e.g., Teed et al., 2022) are worth utilizing in future investigations. In addition, according to our power calculations our fMRI analyses were underpowered and therefore could only be deemed exploratory. Finally, without including an anxiety-disorder only comparator group it is unclear if the increased reactivity observed in our sample is due to anxiety, or whether the interaction of depression and anxiety has a unique profile. Future studies should assess differences in interoceptive and nociceptive processing between those with anxiety disorders only, depression only, and comorbid anxiety and depression. We did not find significant differences between the groups on a number of measures. However, those that involve aversive sensations such as the Cold-Pressor Challenge and the Breath-Hold Challenge showed group differences. This could be because of the limitations of the tasks outlined above, or, it could be indicative of the group differences stemming from the processing of aversive sensations only and thus driven by differences in threat sensitivity. Additionally, the fact that there were no statistically significant group differences in the activity of insula subregions during interoceptive attention (possibly due to low statistical power) limits our ability to dwell further on the possible neural correlates of differences in aversive interoceptive reactivity in individuals with anxious depression vs. those with depression alone. One possibility is that the observed differences in perception are related to activity in brain regions other than the insular cortex, although further study would be needed to verify this notion.

Conclusion

In sum, these findings provide further support for the view that anxious and non-anxious depression have distinct neurocognitive and neurophysiological profiles. One outcome of this arrangement may be that exaggerated reactivity to aversive interoceptive sensations in anxious depression could promote avoidance behavior, whereas a lack of valence-independent motivation could be a driving factor of similar outcomes in non-anxious depression. Our findings add to the growing body of evidence that interoceptive and nociceptive signals are processed differently in individuals with comorbid depression and anxiety than in individuals with depression alone, providing support for a process model of increased threat sensitivity and hyperarousal in anxious depression.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors pending the establishment of data sharing agreements between all parties.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Western Institutional Review Board. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study prior to study entry, and received compensation for their study involvement.

Author contributions

MP and SK: study conception and design. MI, DD, RK, AT, KB, RS, MP, and SK: analysis and interpretation of results. MI, DD, and SK: draft manuscript preparation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work has been supported in part by The William K. Warren Foundation and the National Institute of General Medical Sciences Center Grant (NIGMS) Award Number 1P20GM121312. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. MI, RK, and MP receive funding from the NIGMS center grant P20GM121312; SK has grant funding from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; K23MH112949; R01MH127225); RK has additional funding from National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA; R01DA050677); and MP has additional grant funding from NIDA (U01DA041089, R01DA050677).

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge Jerzy Bodurka, Ph.D. (1964–2021) for his intellectual and scientific contributions to the establishment of the structural and functional MRI processes that provided the foundation for the data collection, analysis, and interpretation of findings for the present work. The ClinicalTrials.gov identifier for the clinical protocol associated with data published in the current article is NCT02450240, “Latent Structure of Multi-level Assessments and Predictors of Outcomes in Psychiatric Disorders.”

Conflict of interest

MP is an advisor to Spring Care, Inc., a behavioral health startup, he has received royalties for an article about methamphetamine in UpToDate.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1083357/full#supplementary-material.

References

Asmundson, G. J., and Stein, M. B. (1994). Triggering the false suffocation alarm in panic disorder patients by using a voluntary breath-holding procedure. Am. J. Psychiatry 151, 264–266. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.2.264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avery, S. N., Clauss, J. A., Winder, D. G., Woodward, N., Heckers, S., and Blackford, J. U. (2014). BNST neurocircuitry in humans. Neuroimage 91, 311–323. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battaglia, S., and Thayer, J. F. (2022). Functional interplay between central and autonomic nervous systems in human fear conditioning. Trends Neurosci. 45, 504–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beissner, F., Meissner, K., BÄR, K.-J., and Napadow, V. (2013). The autonomic brain: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis for central processing of autonomic function. J. Neurosci. 33, 10503–10511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1103-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berner, L. A., Simmons, A. N., Wierenga, C. E., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Paulus, M. P., Bailer, U., et al. (2018). Altered interoceptive activation before, during and after aversive breathing load in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 48, 142–154. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntson, G. G., and Khalsa, S. S. (2021). Neural circuits of interoception. Trends Neurosci. 44, 17–28. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell-Sills, L., Norman, S. B., Craske, M. G., Sullivan, G., Lang, A. J., Chavira, D. A., et al. (2009). Validation of a brief measure of anxiety-related severity and impairment: the Overall Anxiety Severity and Impairment Scale (OASIS). J. Affect. Disord. 112, 92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 319–333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cella, D., Riley, W., Stone, A., Rothrock, N., Reeve, B., Yount, S., et al. (2010). The patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS) developed and tested its first wave of adult self-reported health outcome item banks: 2005-2008. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 63, 1179–1194. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., and Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 316–336. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.100.3.316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corneille, O., Desmedt, O., Zamariola, G., Luminet, O., and Maurage, P. (2020). A heartfelt response to Zimprich et al. (2020) and Ainley et al. (2020)’s commentaries: acknowledging issues with the HCT would benefit interoception research. Biol. Psychol. 152:107869. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2008). “Interoception and emotion: a neuroanatomical perspective,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 272–292.

Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cogn. Emot. 12, 307–330. doi: 10.1080/026999398379628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmedt, O., Luminet, O., and Corneille, O. (2018). The heartbeat counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Biol. Psychol. 138, 185–188. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVille, D. C., Khalsa, S. S., Lapidus, R. C., White, E., Paulus, M. P., and Aupperle, R. L. (2021). A transdiagnostic multilevel examination of interoceptive processing in individuals with a remote history of suicidal behavior. Behav. Ther. 52, 1080–1092. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2021.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVille, D. C., Kuplicki, R., Stewart, J. L., Tulsa, I., Paulus, M. P., and Khalsa, S. S. (2020). Diminished responses to bodily threat and blunted interoception in suicide attempters. eLife 9:e51593. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domschke, K., Stevens, S., Pfleiderer, B., and Gerlach, A. L. (2010). Interoceptive sensitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: an overview and integration of neurobiological findings. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., Ogilvie, A. D., and Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Heartbeat perception in depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1921–1930. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Stefanovitch, I., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Hawkins, A., and Dalgleish, T. (2010). Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety-and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1133–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggart, M., Lange, A., Binser, M. J., Queri, S., and Müller-Oerlinghausen, B. (2019). Major depressive disorder is associated with impaired interoceptive accuracy: a systematic review. Brain Sci. 9:131. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9060131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, L., Li, H., Zhuo, J., Zhang, Y., Wang, J., Chen, L., et al. (2016). The human brainnetome atlas: a new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3508–3526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fava, M., Rush, A. J., Alpert, J. E., Balasubramani, G. K., Wisniewski, S. R., Carmin, C. N., et al. (2008). Difference in treatment outcome in outpatients with anxious versus nonanxious depression: a STAR*D report. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 342–351. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06111868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferentzi, E., Wilhelm, O., and KÖteles, F. (2022). What counts when heartbeats are counted. Trends Cogn. Sci. 26, 832–835. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furman, D. J., Waugh, C. E., Bhattacharjee, K., Thompson, R. J., and Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Interoceptive awareness, positive affect and decision making in major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 151, 780–785. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Kring, A. M., and John, O. P. (2006). Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: a scale development study. J. Res. Pers. 40, 1086–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaspersz, R., Lamers, F., Kent, J. M., Beekman, A. T. F., Smit, J. H., Van Hemert, A. M., et al. (2017). Anxious distress predicts subsequent treatment outcome and side effects in depressed patients starting antidepressant treatment. J. Psychiatr. Res. 84, 41–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, D., Imai, K., King, G., and Stuart, E. A. (2011). MatchIt: nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. J. Stat. Softw. 42, 1–28. doi: 10.18637/jss.v042.i08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ionescu, D. F., Niciu, M. J., Richards, E. M., and Zarate, C. A., Jr. (2014). Pharmacologic treatment of dimensional anxious depression: a review. Prim. Care Companion CNS Disord. 16:PCC.13r01621. doi: 10.4088/PCC.13r01621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ironside, M., Kuplicki, R., Walker, E., Ramsey, C., Forthman, K., Nestor, M., et al. (2022). The unique face of anxious depression: exaggerated threat but preserved valence sensitivity. medRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2022.05.12.22275025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, B. (2017). R2glmm: computes R squared for mixed (multilevel) models. R package version 0.1, 2 Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=r2glmm.

Google Scholar

Kerr, K. L., Moseman, S. E., Avery, J. A., Bodurka, J., Zucker, N. L., and Simmons, W. K. (2016). Altered insula activity during visceral interoception in weight-restored patients with anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 521–528. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Sampson, N. A., Berglund, P., Gruber, M. J., Al-Hamzawi, A., Andrade, L., et al. (2015). Anxious and non-anxious major depressive disorder in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 24, 210–226. doi: 10.1017/S2045796015000189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Craske, M. G., Li, W., Vangala, S., Strober, M., and Feusner, J. D. (2015). Altered interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa: effects of meal anticipation, consumption and bodily arousal. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 48, 889–897. doi: 10.1002/eat.22387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., and Feinstein, J. S. (2018). “The somatic error hypothesis of anxiety,” in The Interoceptive Mind. From Homeostasis to Awareness, eds M. Tsakiris and H. De Preester (Oxford, England: Oxford University Press), 144–164.

Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., and Lapidus, R. C. (2016). Can interoception improve the pragmatic search for biomarkers in psychiatry? Front. Psychiatry 7:121. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapidus, R. C., Puhl, M., Kuplicki, R., Stewart, J. L., Paulus, M. P., Rhudy, J. L., et al. (2020). Heightened affective response to perturbation of respiratory but not pain signals in eating, mood and anxiety disorders. PLoS One 15:e0235346. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levis, B., Benedetti, A., and Thombs, B. D. (2019). Accuracy of patient health questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) for screening to detect major depression: individual participant data meta-analysis. BMJ 365:l1476. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l1476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., Wager, T. D., Weber, J., and Barrett, L. F. (2016). The brain basis of positive and negative affect: evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1910–1922. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maren, S., and Quirk, G. J. (2004). Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 844–852. doi: 10.1038/nrn1535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McTeague, L. M., Lang, P. J., Laplante, M.-C., and Bradley, M. M. (2011). Aversive imagery in panic disorder: agoraphobia severity, comorbidity and defensive physiology. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 415–424. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McTeague, L. M., Rosenberg, B. M., Lopez, J. W., Carreon, D. M., Huemer, J., Jiang, Y., et al. (2020). Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in emotional processing across psychiatric disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 177, 411–421. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18111271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., BrÜne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., et al. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 11, 141–168. doi: 10.1038/nrd3628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, R. J., Schaer, M., and DebbanÉ, M. (2012). Degrees of separation: a quantitative neuroimaging meta-analysis investigating self-specificity and shared neural activation between self-and other-reflection. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1043–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nord, C. L., Lawson, R. P., and Dalgleish, T. (2021). Disrupted dorsal mid-insula activation during interoception across psychiatric disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 178, 761–770. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20091340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., Feinstein, J. S., and Khalsa, S. S. (2019). An active inference approach to interoceptive psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 15, 97–122. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2006). An insular view of anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2010). Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 451–463. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., and Stewart, J. L. (2014). Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology 76, 342–350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penninx, B. W., Nolen, W. A., Lamers, F., Zitman, F. G., Smit, J. H., Spinhoven, P., et al. (2011). Two-year course of depressive and anxiety disorders: results from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). J. Affect. Disord. 133, 76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Herbert, B. M., Kaufmann, C., Auer, D. P., and Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive awareness, anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity to isometric exercise. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 65, 167–173. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E., and Schandry, R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depress. Anxiety 26, 167–173. doi: 10.1002/da.20504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ring, C., and Brener, J. (2018). Heartbeat counting is unrelated to heartbeat detection: a comparison of methods to quantify interoception. Psychophysiology 55:e13084. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59, 22–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Simmons, W. K., Avery, J. A., Barcalow, J. C., Bodurka, J., Drevets, W. C., and Bellgowan, P. (2013). Keeping the body in mind: insula functional organization and functional connectivity integrate interoceptive, exteroceptive and emotional awareness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2944–2958. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Feinstein, J. S., Kuplicki, R., Forthman, K. L., Stewart, J. L., Paulus, M. P., et al. (2021). Perceptual insensitivity to the modulation of interoceptive signals in depression, anxiety and substance use disorders. Sci. Rep. 11:2108. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81307-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Kuplicki, R., Feinstein, J., Forthman, K. L., Stewart, J. L., Paulus, M. P., et al. (2020). A Bayesian computational model reveals a failure to adapt interoceptive precision estimates across depression, anxiety, eating and substance use disorders. PLOS Comput. Biol. 16:e1008484. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., and Lane, R. D. (2015). The neural basis of one’s own conscious and unconscious emotional states. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 1–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Thayer, J. F., Khalsa, S. S., and Lane, R. D. (2017). The hierarchical basis of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 75, 274–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, J. L., Khalsa, S. S., Kuplicki, R., Puhl, M., Investigators, T., and Paulus, M. P. (2020). Interoceptive attention in opioid and stimulant use disorder. Addict. Biol. 25:e12831. doi: 10.1111/adb.12831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syed, S. A., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2017). Early life stress, mood and anxiety disorders. Chronic Stress (Thousand Oaks) 1:2470547017694461. doi: 10.1177/2470547017694461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S., Zvolensky, M. J., Cox, B. J., Deacon, B., Heimberg, R. G., Ledley, D. R., et al. (2007). Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol. Assess. 19, 176–188. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teed, A. R., Feinstein, J. S., Puhl, M., Lapidus, R. C., Upshaw, V., Kuplicki, R. T., et al. (2022). Association of generalized anxiety disorder with autonomic hypersensitivity and blunted ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity during peripheral adrenergic stimulation: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 79, 323–332. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.4225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terhaar, J., Viola, F. C., BÄR, K.-J., and Debener, S. (2012). Heartbeat evoked potentials mirror altered body perception in depressed patients. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1950–1957. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thielscher, A., and Pessoa, L. (2007). Neural correlates of perceptual choice and decision making during fear-disgust discrimination. J. Neurosci. 27, 2908–2917. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3024-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, T., Keogh, E., French, C. C., and Davis, R. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity and pain: generalisability across noxious stimuli. Pain 134, 187–196. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Does, A. W., Antony, M. M., Ehlers, A., and Barsky, A. J. (2000). Heartbeat perception in panic disorder: a reanalysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 47–62. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00184-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Victor, T. A., Khalsa, S. S., Simmons, W. K., Feinstein, J. S., Savitz, J., Aupperle, R. L., et al. (2018). Tulsa 1,000: a naturalistic study protocol for multilevel assessment and outcome prediction in a large psychiatric sample. BMJ Open 8:e016620. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-016620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrana, S., and Lauterbach, D. (1994). Prevalence of traumatic events and post-traumatic psychological symptoms in a nonclinical sample of college students. J. Trauma Stress 7, 289–302. doi: 10.1007/BF02102949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiteside, S. P., Lynam, D. R., Miller, J. D., and Reynolds, S. K. (2005). Validation of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale: a four-factor model of impulsivity. Eur. J. Pers. 19, 559–574. doi: 10.1002/per.556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H., Chang, W., and Wickham, M. H. (2016). Package ‘ggplot2’. Create Elegant Data Visualisations Using the Grammar of Graphics. Version 2, 1–189. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggplot2.

Google Scholar

Wiebking, C., De Greck, M., Duncan, N. W., Tempelmann, C., Bajbouj, M., and Northoff, G. (2015). Interoception in insula subregions as a possible state marker for depression—an exploratory fMRI study investigating healthy, depressed and remitted participants. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:82. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, B., and Thakkar, K. (2022). Interoception abnormalities in schizophrenia: a review of preliminary evidence and an integration with Bayesian accounts of psychosis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 132, 757–773. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anxiety, depression, fear, threat, interoception, nociception, pain, interoceptive awareness

Citation: Ironside M, DeVille DC, Kuplicki RT, Burrows KP, Smith R, Teed AR, Paulus MP and Khalsa SS (2023) The unique face of comorbid anxiety and depression: increased interoceptive fearfulness and reactivity. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:1083357. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1083357

Received: 28 October 2022; Accepted: 21 December 2022;
Published: 23 January 2023

Edited by:

Masaru Tanaka, University of Szeged (ELKH-SZTE), Hungary

Reviewed by:

Simone Battaglia, University of Turin, Italy
Piril Hepsomali, University of Roehampton London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Ironside, DeVille, Kuplicki, Burrows, Smith, Teed, Paulus and Khalsa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria Ironside, mironside@laureateinstitute.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.