- 1Department of Cognitive Psychology, Faculty for Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
- 2Department of Biopsychology, Faculty for Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
- 3Experimental Psychology II and Biological Psychology, Institute of Psychology, School of Human Sciences, Osnabrück University, Osnabrück, Germany
- 4Social Brain Lab, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Amsterdam, Netherlands
- 5Department of Psychology, MSH Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
- 6Institute for Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, MSH Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
Introduction
Research on functional brain lateralization made tremendous advances in the last decade (Ocklenburg et al., 2021), but shallow phenotyping is a continuing problem in large-scale studies using existing data. To specify, we understand shallow phenotyping as a minimal approach to assess phenotypes where the accuracy of the measurement is diminished and does not satisfy the complexity of the matter. For example, instead of using a full questionnaire or behavioral tool, the phenotype is assessed by a single item. This is often the case for e.g., handedness, which is one of the most prominent lateralized phenotypes in humans, with 89.4% of the population being right-handers (Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2020). Handedness is a complex trait emerging from multiple genetic, epigenetic, environmental, and interacting effects (Güntürkün and Ocklenburg, 2017; Ocklenburg et al., 2017; Kovel and Francks, 2019; Cuellar-Partida et al., 2021; Odintsova et al., 2022). About 25% of variance in human handedness has a genetic origin (Medland et al., 2006). Large-scale genome-wide-association studies (GWAS) have shown that handedness is a highly polygenic trait (Kovel and Francks, 2019; Wiberg et al., 2019; Cuellar-Partida et al., 2021). This indicates that many genes with small effect sizes contribute to handedness. In the largest GWAS, single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) explained between 3.45% and 5.9% of variance in handedness (Cuellar-Partida et al., 2021). However, GWAS require enormous sample sizes, which is not feasible for most institutions that want to investigate genetic underpinnings of laterality. Fortunately, the summary statistics of GWAS can be used to calculate polygenic scores (PGS) in smaller sample sizes (Dudbridge, 2013). PGS are sum scores calculated from allelic effects of thousands of SNPs and suit as an indicator of genetic predisposition for a certain phenotype (Choi et al., 2020). It has been shown that PGS can predict handedness in a smaller independent sample (Ocklenburg et al., 2022).
Since a large part of the variance in handedness cannot be explained by genetic variation, epigenetic factors likely come into play (Schmitz et al., 2017). A recent epigenome-wide association study (EWAS) investigated the association between handedness and several hundred thousand cytosine-phosphate-guanine nucleotide base pairings (CpGs) from whole-blood samples (Odintsova et al., 2022). Methylations of two regions were significantly associated with left-handedness: BLCAP and IAH1. The study also reported that CpGs located near SNPs associated with handedness were more strongly associated with left-handedness than other CpGs. However, effect sizes were small and explained little of the variance in handedness.
One environmental factor that has been proposed to influence hemispheric asymmetries is stress (Ocklenburg et al., 2016). Stress has multiple effects on the organism and can result in mental and physical disorders (McEwen, 1998; Pfeifer et al., 2021). In reaction to stress exposure, the body initiates a stress response driven by two major systems: the sympathetic–adrenal–medullary (SAM) complex (Mason, 1968) and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Aguilera, 2011). The end product of the HPA axis - the hormone cortisol - has been associated with cognitive and behavioral adaptations under stress (Vogel et al., 2016) but also with various disorders (de Kloet et al., 2005; Zorn et al., 2017; Zänkert et al., 2019). It has been proposed that maternal stress affects offspring lateralization by means of epigenetic processes in humans and rodents (Schmitz et al., 2017). Similarly, birth stress has been associated with non-right-handedness (Bakan, 1971; Hicks et al., 1980). Unraveling relations between stress and lateralization is highly relevant since several mental disorders feature atypical asymmetries (Berretz et al., 2020b) while stress is a crucial factor for the development and progression of psychopathology (Cohen et al., 2007). Studies focusing on the effect of acute stress on cognitive laterality are still rare. In a study by Brüne et al. (2013), participants displayed increased asymmetric response tendencies in a dot probe task using face stimuli after stress induction. A recent study with rats demonstrated an increase in asymmetric turning behavior under high stress (Mundorf et al., 2020). However, only rats who experienced early life stress via separation from their mothers as pups showed this effect. In a recent series of studies by our group, we could not find an effect of acute stress or stress hormones on indicators of language and emotional lateralization on the behavioral level (Berretz et al., 2020a, 2021). While acute stress led to more left-hemispheric activity during stress induction itself (Berretz et al., 2022b), it did not affect basic interhemispheric transfer afterwards (Berretz et al., 2022c). These inconsistencies in results indicate that the relationship between acute stress and changes in functional hemispheric asymmetries may be more complex (Berretz and Packheiser, 2022). In this context, focusing on other forms of lateralized behavior like social touch could constitute a worthwhile avenue to pursue (Malatesta et al., 2020; Berretz et al., 2022a).
Opinion: Broaden the scope - Research on the genetic and epigenetic key players in laterality needs more phenotypes than just handedness and language lateralization
Taken together, both genetic and epigenetic factors have a significant but small influence on human handedness, and there is a large gap of unexplained variance in the literature. We suggest that it is crucial to not only look into the ontogenetic factors, but also consider the phenotype when trying to understand this issue (Ocklenburg et al., 2014). Almost all large-scale studies on behavioral lateralization focus on handedness. Human handedness, however, is not an optimal phenotype to investigate either evolutionary, genetic or epigenetic questions in the broader scope of comparative laterality research for several reasons. The same is likely true for language lateralization, another widely used laterality phenotype (Hausmann et al., 2019). Importantly, both handedness and language lateralization are largely human specific. While limb preferences exist in many mammalian and non-mammalian species, animals often show individual-level asymmetry, but no population-level asymmetry (Ströckens et al., 2013; Ocklenburg et al., 2019; Manns et al., 2021). Even in those species that do show a significant population-level asymmetry for limb preferences, the left-right distribution is not as strongly skewed as in humans (Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2020). Still, discrepancies such as existing or missing population-level asymmetry that become apparent from a comparative perspective might also stimulate the question why laterality patterns differ across species and which factors play a role in exerting such differential influences. Another natural drawback of handedness or limb preferences in comparative laterality research is that it can only be observed in species that have limbs and use them to manipulate the environment.
We suggest that incorporating social laterality phenotypes (Marzoli et al., 2022) in studies on the ontogenesis and evolution of hemispheric asymmetries would benefit research in several ways. Laterality in social interactions has been found across many behavioral dimensions (Ocklenburg et al., 2018). These include walking side-by-side (Rodway and Schepman, 2022), hugging (Packheiser et al., 2019a), kissing (Ocklenburg and Güntürkün, 2009; Chapelain et al., 2015), and cradling children (Malatesta et al., 2019, 2020, 2021a; Packheiser et al., 2019b). Interestingly, studies show that these behaviors are all correlated with handedness, but only to a small to moderate extent in contrast to strong correlations with other forms of motor laterality, like footedness (Packheiser et al., 2020).
Importantly, social laterality can be observed in a wider range of species than limb preferences and is more comparable across species. For example, a wide variety of mammals show a side bias in mother-infant interactions (Karenina et al., 2017; Giljov et al., 2018; Karenina and Giljov, 2018). Moreover, fish schools show increased behavioral lateralization when predatory pressure is high, but fish are typically difficult to investigate regarding limb preferences, as they rarely use their fins to manipulate objects (Chivers et al., 2016). Several insect species such as bees or ants have also shown the need for social coordination in lateralized behavior (Anfora et al., 2011; Frasnelli et al., 2012, 2014; Rogers et al., 2013; Niven and Frasnelli, 2018) similar to the coordination needed during hugs and kisses in humans (Chapelain et al., 2015; Ocklenburg et al., 2018). This suggests that social laterality is phylogenetically old and conserved (Niven and Bell, 2018). Thus, social laterality could be a more suitable target behavior to uncover the mechanistic and genetic underpinnings of brain lateralization.
In addition to this benefit, social laterality may be a better phenotype for research on the evolution of laterality for another reason. In principle, two forms of evolutionary pressures to develop an asymmetrically organized nervous system exist. Firstly, there is an evolutionary pressure to develop an asymmetrically organized system per se, as it is more efficient and saves energy and neuronal tissue (Güntürkün and Ocklenburg, 2017). This form of evolutionary is independent of the direction, e.g., a leftward asymmetric function would be as beneficial for survival as a rightward asymmetric function. Secondly, there are also “social” evolutionary pressures for all individuals in a group (e.g., a shool of fish) to develop a lateral bias to the same side (Vallortigara, 2006). The direction of these biases becomes highly relevant when, for example, fleeing predators, as a single animal going to the other side than the rest of the group is easy prey (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2020). Thus, unlike the first form of evolutionary pressure to develop asymmetry, this second form is highly selective for direction of asymmetry. Importantly, handedness underlies the first form of evolutionary pressure, but it is debatable to what extent it underlies the second. For social laterality it is, however, rather clear that group coordination is highly relevant and empirical evidence shows that social pressure to go to one side does influence social laterality within groups (Chapelain et al., 2015). Thus, integrating social laterality phenotypes into research on the ontogenesis and evolution of hemispheric asymmetries could be highly beneficial as it likely underlies stronger evolutionary pressure than handedness, making it a behavioral phenotype that may have a stronger biological link to brain structure, which is determined by genetic and non-genetic factors.
Integrating social laterality: A research proposal
While a few studies have studied the influence of stress on social laterality (Suter et al., 2007; Reissland et al., 2009) to this day, not a single study has investigated social laterality in the context of genetics and epigenetics. Thus, we believe that it is critical to include them in future large-scale investigations to uncover the genetic and epigenetic mechanisms of structural and functional brain lateralization. As extremely high sample sizes are needed to study the relationship between genes and behavior, we therefore propose to fill the gap in the literature by integrating especially behavioral measures of social laterality into test batteries for population-based genetic profiling such as the UK Biobank. Behavioral social laterality phenotypes can be easily acquired by asking participants to imagine, for example, to cradle a child, which has been demonstrated to reliably produce the universally found left-sided cradling bias (Malatesta et al., 2021b; Vauclair, 2022) or imagine to which side they turn their heads during a kiss. Such methods are highly economical as they can be included in survey questionnaires. We furthermore propose that comparative animal studies should be conducted to have mechanistic insight into the role of stress and determine the genetic basis of social laterality. This complementary approach to study social laterality will thus inform about potential genetic loci through human genome-wide association studies that can then be causally investigated in animal models through modern transgenic approaches such as CRISPR/Cas (Pickar-Oliver and Gersbach, 2019). Importantly however, we argue that the inclusion of social phenotypes would also advance laterality research when not combined with genetic or epigenetic approaches. Since one can assume that asymmetrically organized social behavior results from a principal functional lateralization of the brain, it may give insights into the hemispheric division of functions such as emotion processing and social cognition (Karenina and Giljov, 2018). With that, social laterality phenotypes may further shed light onto the groundwork of lateralization and its representation in the brain linking a range of behavioral asymmetries. As an alternative to genetic and epigenetic markers, studies on social laterality may consider the integration of imaging techniques such as electroencephalography (EEG) (e.g., Packheiser et al., 2021).
Author contributions
LSP and KH wrote the section on stress. GB wrote the section on stress and laterality. DM wrote the section on genetic analyses. JP wrote the section on social laterality. SO conceptualized the article, wrote the first outline of the article, and the opinion section. All authors have read and critically revised the final submitted version of the article.
Funding
The contribution of LSP was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) within project B4 of the Collaborative Research Center (SFB) 874 Integration and Representation of Sensory Processes (project-ID 122679504 SFB 874). KH was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) - project number GRK-2185/1 (DFG Research Training Group Situated Cognition)/Gefördert durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) - Projektnummer GRK-2185/1 (DFG-Graduiertenkolleg Situated Cognition). JP was supported by the German National Academy of Sciences Leopoldina (LPDS 2021-05).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Aguilera, G. (2011). HPA axis responsiveness to stress: implications for healthy aging. Exp. Gerontol. 46, 90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2010.08.023
Anfora, G., Rigosi, E., Frasnelli, E., Ruga, V., Trona, F., Vallortigara, G., et al. (2011). Lateralization in the invertebrate brain: left-right asymmetry of olfaction in bumble bee, Bombus terrestris. PLoS ONE 6, e18903. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018903
Berretz, G., Cebula, C., Wortelmann, B. M., Papadopoulou, P., Wolf, O. T., Ocklenburg, S., et al. (2022a). Romantic partner embraces reduce cortisol release after acute stress induction in women but not in men. PLoS ONE 17, e0266887. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266887
Berretz, G., and Packheiser, J. (2022). Altered hemispheric asymmetries as an endophenotype in psychological and developmental disorders: a theory on the influence of stress on brain lateralization. psyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/3abqp
Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., Höffken, O., Wolf, O. T., and Ocklenburg, S. (2021). Dichotic listening performance and interhemispheric integration after administration of hydrocortisone. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-00896-1
Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., Wolf, O. T., and Ocklenburg, S. (2020a). Dichotic listening performance and interhemispheric integration after stress exposure. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-77708-5
Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., Wolf, O. T., and Ocklenburg, S. (2022b). Acute stress increases left hemispheric activity measured via changes in frontal alpha asymmetries. Iscience 25, 103841. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.103841
Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., Wolf, O. T., and Ocklenburg, S. (2022c). Improved interhemispheric connectivity after stress during lexical decision making. Behav. Brain Res. 418, 113648. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113648
Berretz, G., Wolf, O. T., Güntürkün, O., and Ocklenburg, S. (2020b). Atypical lateralization in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders: what is the role of stress? Cortex 125, 215–232. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.019
Brüne, M., Nadolny, N., Güntürkün, O., and Wolf, O. T. (2013). Stress induces a functional asymmetry in an emotional attention task. Cogn. Emot. 27, 558–566. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.726211
Chapelain, A., Pimbert, P., Aube, L., Perrocheau, O., Debunne, G., Bellido, A., et al. (2015). Can population-level laterality stem from social pressures? Evidence from cheek kissing in humans. PLoS ONE 10, e0124477. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124477
Chivers, D. P., McCormick, M. I., Allan, B. J. M., Mitchell, M. D., Gonçalves, E. J., Bryshun, R., et al. (2016). At odds with the group: changes in lateralization and escape performance reveal conformity and conflict in fish schools. Proc. Biol. Sci. 283. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.1127
Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S.-H., and O'Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: a guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nat. Protoc. 15, 2759–2772. doi: 10.1038/s41596-020-0353-1
Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., and Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological stress and disease. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 298, 1685–1687. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.14.1685
Cuellar-Partida, G., Tung, J. Y., Eriksson, N., Albrecht, E., Aliev, F., Andreassen, O. A., et al. (2021). Genome-wide association study identifies 48 common genetic variants associated with handedness. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00956-y
de Kloet, E. R., Joëls, M., and Holsboer, F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 463–475. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683
Dudbridge, F. (2013). Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet. 9, e1003348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348
Frasnelli, E., Haase, A., Rigosi, E., Anfora, G., Rogers, L. J., Vallortigara, G., et al. (2014). The bee as a model to investigate brain and behavioural asymmetries. Insects 5, 120–138. doi: 10.3390/insects5010120
Frasnelli, E., Iakovlev, I., and Reznikova, Z. (2012). Asymmetry in antennal contacts during trophallaxis in ants. Behav. Brain Res. 232, 7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.03.014
Giljov, A., Karenina, K., and Malashichev, Y. (2018). Facing each other: mammal mothers and infants prefer the position favouring right hemisphere processing. Biol. Lett. 14, 20170707. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2017.0707
Güntürkün, O., and Ocklenburg, S. (2017). Ontogenesis of lateralization. Neuron 94, 249–263. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.045
Hausmann, M., Brysbaert, M., van der Haegen, L., Lewald, J., Specht, K., Hirnstein, M., et al. (2019). Language lateralisation measured across linguistic and national boundaries. Cortex 111, 134–147. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.020
Hicks, R. A., Dusek, C., Larsen, F., Williams, S., and Pellegrini, R. J. (1980). Birth complications and the distribution of handedness. Cortex 16, 483–486. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(80)80049-9
Karenina, K., and Giljov, A. (2018). Mother and offspring lateralized social behavior across mammalian species. Prog. Brain Res. 238, 115–141. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.003
Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Ingram, J., Rowntree, V. J., and Malashichev, Y. (2017). Lateralization of mother-infant interactions in a diverse range of mammal species. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 30. doi: 10.1038/s41559-016-0030
Kovel, C. G. F., and Francks, C. (2019). The molecular genetics of hand preference revisited. Sci. Rep. 9, 5986. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42515-0
Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Apicella, F., Abiuso, C., Muratori, F., Forrester, G. S., et al. (2020). Received cradling bias during the first year of life: a retrospective study on children with typical and atypical development. Front. Psychiatry 11, 91. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00091
Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Morelli, L., Pivetti, M., and Tommasi, L. (2021a). The role of ethnic prejudice in the modulation of cradling lateralization. J. Nonverbal Behav. 45, 187–205. doi: 10.1007/s10919-020-00346-y
Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Piccioni, C., and Tommasi, L. (2019). The relationship between the left-cradling bias and attachment to parents and partner. Evol. Psychol. 17, 1474704919848117. doi: 10.1177/1474704919848117
Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Prete, G., and Tommasi, L. (2021b). Human lateralization, maternal effects and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 668520. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.668520
Manns, M., Basbasse, Y. E., Freund, N., and Ocklenburg, S. (2021). Paw preferences in mice and rats: meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 127, 593–606. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.011
Marzoli, D., D'Anselmo, A., Malatesta, G., Lucaf,ò, C., Prete, G., Tommasi, L., et al. (2022). The intricate web of asymmetric processing of social stimuli in humans. Symmetry 14, 1096. doi: 10.3390/sym14061096
Mason, J. W. (1968). A review of psychoendocrine research on the pituitary-adrenal cortical system. Psychosom. Med. 30, 576–607. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196809000-00020
McEwen, B. S. (1998). “Stress, adaptation, and disease: allostasis and allostatic load,” in Molecular Aspects, Integrative Systems, and Clinical Advances, eds S. M. McCann, J. M. Lipton, E. M. Sternberg, G. P. Chrousos, P. W. Gold, and C. C. Smith (New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences), 33–44.
Medland, S. E., Duffy, D. L., Wright, M. J., Geffen, G. M., and Martin, N. G. (2006). Handedness in twins: joint analysis of data from 35 samples. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 9, 46–53. doi: 10.1375/twin.9.1.46
Mundorf, A., Matsui, H., Ocklenburg, S., and Freund, N. (2020). Asymmetry of turning behavior in rats is modulated by early life stress. Behav. Brain Res. 393, 112807. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112807
Niven, J. E., and Bell, A. T. A. (2018). Lessons in lateralisation from the insects. Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 486–488. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.04.008
Niven, J. E., and Frasnelli, E. (2018). Insights into the evolution of lateralization from the insects. Prog. Brain Res. 238, 3–31. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.001
Ocklenburg, S., Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., and Friedrich, P. (2021). Laterality 2020: entering the next decade. Laterality 26, 265–297. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2020.1804396
Ocklenburg, S., Beste, C., and Arning, L. (2014). Handedness genetics: considering the phenotype. Front. Psychol. 5, 1300. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01300
Ocklenburg, S., and Güntürkün, O. (2009). Head-turning asymmetries during kissing and their association with lateral preference. Laterality 14, 79–85. doi: 10.1080/13576500802243689
Ocklenburg, S., Isparta, S., Peterburs, J., and Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2019). Paw preferences in cats and dogs: meta-analysis. Laterality 24, 647–677. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2019.1578228
Ocklenburg, S., Korte, S. M., Peterburs, J., Wolf, O. T., and Güntürkün, O. (2016). Stress and laterality - the comparative perspective. Physiol. Behav. 164, 321–329. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.06.020
Ocklenburg, S., Metzen, D., Schlüter, C., Fraenz, C., Arning, L., Streit, F., et al. (2022). Polygenic scores for handedness and their association with asymmetries in brain structure. Brain Struct. Funct. 227, 515–527. doi: 10.1007/s00429-021-02335-3
Ocklenburg, S., Packheiser, J., Schmitz, J., Rook, N., Güntürkün, O., Peterburs, J., et al. (2018). Hugs and kisses - the role of motor preferences and emotional lateralization for hemispheric asymmetries in human social touch. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 95, 353–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.007
Ocklenburg, S., Schmitz, J., Moinfar, Z., Moser, D., Klose, R., Lor, S., et al. (2017). Epigenetic regulation of lateralized fetal spinal gene expression underlies hemispheric asymmetries. Elife 6, e22784. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22784.013
Odintsova, V. V., Suderman, M., Hagenbeek, F. A., Caramaschi, D., Hottenga, J.-J., Pool, R., et al. (2022). DNA methylation in peripheral tissues and left-handedness. Sci. Rep. 12, 5606. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-08998-0
Packheiser, J., Berretz, G., Rook, N., Bahr, C., Schockenhoff, L., Güntürkün, O., et al. (2021). Investigating real-life emotions in romantic couples: a mobile EEG study. Sci. Rep. 11, 1142. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-80590-w
Packheiser, J., Rook, N., Dursun, Z., Mesenhöller, J., Wenglorz, A., Güntürkün, O., et al. (2019a). Embracing your emotions: affective state impacts lateralisation of human embraces. Psychol. Res. 83, 26–36. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-0985-8
Packheiser, J., Schmitz, J., Berretz, G., Carey, D. P., Paracchini, S., Papadatou-Pastou, M., et al. (2020). Four meta-analyses across 164 studies on atypical footedness prevalence and its relation to handedness. Sci. Rep. 10, 14501. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-71478-w
Packheiser, J., Schmitz, J., Berretz, G., Papadatou-Pastou, M., and Ocklenburg, S. (2019b). Handedness and sex effects on lateral biases in human cradling: three meta-analyses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 104, 30–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.035
Papadatou-Pastou, M., Ntolka, E., Schmitz, J., Martin, M., Munaf,ò, M. R., Ocklenburg, S., et al. (2020). Human handedness: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146, 481–524. doi: 10.1037/bul0000229
Pfeifer, L. S., Heyers, K., Ocklenburg, S., and Wolf, O. T. (2021). Stress research during the COVID-19 pandemic and beyond. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 131, 581–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.045
Pickar-Oliver, A., and Gersbach, C. A. (2019). The next generation of CRISPR-Cas technologies and applications. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 20, 490–507. doi: 10.1038/s41580-019-0131-5
Reissland, N., Hopkins, B., Helms, P., and Williams, B. (2009). Maternal stress and depression and the lateralisation of infant cradling. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 263–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01791.x
Rodway, P., and Schepman, A. (2022). Who goes where in couples and pairs? Effects of sex and handedness on side preferences in human dyads. Laterality. 1–28. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2022.2090573. [Epub ahead of print].
Rogers, L. J., Rigosi, E., Frasnelli, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2013). A right antenna for social behaviour in honeybees. Sci. Rep. 3, 2045. doi: 10.1038/srep02045
Schmitz, J., Metz, G. A. S., Güntürkün, O., and Ocklenburg, S. (2017). Beyond the genome-Towards an epigenetic understanding of handedness ontogenesis. Prog. Neurobiol. 159, 69–89. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.10.005
Ströckens, F., Güntürkün, O., and Ocklenburg, S. (2013). Limb preferences in non-human vertebrates. Laterality 18, 536–575. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2012.723008
Suter, S. E., Huggenberger, H. J., and Schächinger, H. (2007). Cold pressor stress reduces left cradling preference in nulliparous human females. Stress 10, 45–51. doi: 10.1080/10253890601141259
Vallortigara, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of left and right: costs and benefits of lateralization. Dev. Psychobiol. 48, 418–427. doi: 10.1002/dev.20166
Vallortigara, G., and Rogers, L. J. (2020). A function for the bicameral mind. Cortex 124, 274–285. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.018
Vauclair, J. (2022). Maternal cradling bias: a marker of the nature of the mother-infant relationship. Infant Behav. Dev. 66, 101680. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101680
Vogel, S., Fernández, G., Joëls, M., and Schwabe, L. (2016). Cognitive adaptation under stress: a case for the mineralocorticoid receptor. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.003
Wiberg, A., Ng, M., Al Omran, Y., Alfaro-Almagro, F., McCarthy, P., Marchini, J., et al. (2019). Handedness, language areas and neuropsychiatric diseases: insights from brain imaging and genetics. Brain 142, 2938–2947. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz257
Zänkert, S., Bellingrath, S., Wüst, S., and Kudielka, B. M. (2019). HPA axis responses to psychological challenge linking stress and disease: what do we know on sources of intra- and interindividual variability? Psychoneuroendocrinology 105, 86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.10.027
Keywords: laterality, lateralization, hemispheric asymmetries, social neuroscience, social touch, embracing, hugging
Citation: Pfeifer LS, Heyers K, Berretz G, Metzen D, Packheiser J and Ocklenburg S (2022) Broadening the scope: Increasing phenotype diversity in laterality research. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:1048388. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1048388
Received: 19 September 2022; Accepted: 11 October 2022;
Published: 28 October 2022.
Edited by:
Luca Tommasi, University of Studies G. d'Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, ItalyReviewed by:
Eliza L. Nelson, Florida International University, United StatesCopyright © 2022 Pfeifer, Heyers, Berretz, Metzen, Packheiser and Ocklenburg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Lena Sophie Pfeifer, lena.pfeifer@rub.de
†These authors share first authorship
‡ORCID: Lena Sophie Pfeifer orcid.org/0000-0001-8803-6515
Katrin Heyers orcid.org/0000-0002-7428-5200
Gesa Berretz orcid.org/0000-0002-3513-1946
Dorothea Metzen orcid.org/0000-0002-4250-0076
Julian Packheiser orcid.org/0000-0001-9805-6755
Sebastian Ocklenburg orcid.org/0000-0001-5882-3200