Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 16 November 2022
Sec. Motivation and Reward
This article is part of the Research Topic Sex Differences in the Neurobiology of Drug Relapse Vulnerability View all 6 articles

Neural cue reactivity is not stronger in male than in female patients with alcohol use disorder

  • 1Department of Addictive Behavior and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
  • 2Department of Biostatistics, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
  • 3Department of Clinical Psychology, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
  • 4Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Heidelberg-Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
  • 5Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany
  • 6Mannheim Center for Translational Neurosciences, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
  • 7Feuerlein Center on Translational Addiction Medicine, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany

Background: Males consume more alcohol than females, and alcohol use disorder (AUD) is more prevalent in males than females. However, females progress faster to AUD. Sex differences in neural alcohol cue reactivity were previously observed in young social drinkers, indicating a role of hypersensitivity to alcohol-related cues in very early stages of addiction. To our knowledge, this is the first study on patients diagnosed with AUD to test sex differences in neural reactivity to alcohol cues in order to widen previous findings.

Methods: We analyzed data from previous studies, using a well-established functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigm to compare neural reactivity to alcohol cues between 42 female and 124 male patients with AUD (mean age 45 and 46 years) in predefined regions of interest that were implicated by previous studies (ventral and dorsal striatum as well as caudate, putamen, amygdala, hippocampus, insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and medial prefrontal cortex) using independent samples t-tests. Post-hoc, effect size calculations were performed.

Results: Throughout all nine regions of interest, we found no statistically significant sex differences in neural reactivity toward alcoholic pictures alone or in comparison to neutral pictures (p > 0.05, FDR-corrected). Post-hoc effect size estimates indicated a magnitude between 0.137 and 0.418 (Hedge’s g) on alcohol reactivity to alcohol cues compared to neutral cues and indicate very small to less than medium effect sizes in the direction of higher cue reactivity in female patients.

Conclusion: Previous studies showed sex differences in neural alcohol cue reactivity in younger social and problematic alcohol drinkers, i.e., stronger striatal cue-reactivity in males. After correction for multiple comparisons, we did not observe significant sex differences in a cohort of middle-aged females and males with AUD. Sex differences that are present during early phases of addiction development might disappear at later stages of AUD and might thus be considered as clinically less relevant in patients with more severe AUD.

Introduction

Alcohol is responsible for 3 million deaths and 132.6 million disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) per year worldwide, which accounts for 5.3% of global deaths and 5.1% of DALYs [estimate of the World Health Organization for 2016 (WHO, 2018)]. Sex differences regarding alcohol consumption and alcohol use disorder (AUD) are widely accepted and can be explained by biological and psychosocial factors (Ceylan-Isik et al., 2010; Becker et al., 2017). In Europe and the Americas, males drink more alcohol than females and are 2 to 4 times more prone to AUD (Rehm et al., 2015; WHO, 2018). By contrast, females show an accelerated progression from initiation of alcohol use to AUD (Zilberman et al., 2004) and a higher vulnerability to the sequelae of chronic alcohol intake such as liver damage (Agabio et al., 2017). Whereas males with AUD drink alcohol more often to facilitate social interaction, females use it more frequently to regulate symptoms of anxiety and depression or stress (Peltier et al., 2019; Müller et al., 2021). Further, female compared to male patients with AUD were observed to report generally stronger substance craving (Boykoff et al., 2010), they also showed a stronger relation between craving and negative affect throughout the detoxification process (Petit et al., 2017). However, recent observations indicate a closing gap with respect to sex differences in alcohol consumption and related consequences (White et al., 2015; Slade et al., 2016; White, 2020).

A stronger neural cue reactivity relates to negative outcomes of substance consumption, such as addiction severity, substance craving, or treatment outcomes, and the underlying brain regions include circuits associated with reward, motivation, and goal directed behavior, as well as habit learning (Kühn and Gallinat, 2011; Jasinska et al., 2014). Specifically, meta-analyses have shown that alcohol cues induce activation of subcortical and prefrontal brain regions, i.e., ventral striatum (VS), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) (Schacht et al., 2013). To our knowledge, only two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have been published so far to report on sex-differing effects of neural alcohol cue reactivity (Seo et al., 2011; Kaag et al., 2019). During brief guided imagery, male social drinkers (N = 20, mean age 32.5 years) showed stronger stress-related activation than female social drinkers (N = 23, mean age 30.9 years) in the medial prefrontal cortex, rostral anterior cingulate cortex, posterior insula, amygdala, and hippocampus and weaker alcohol cue-reactivity in the superior and middle frontal gyrus (Seo et al., 2011). During a visual alcohol cue task, alcohol compared to neutral stimuli elicited stronger reactivity of the VS and dorsal striatum (DS) with medium to large effect sizes (VS ηp2 = 0.10, DS ηp2 = 0.11) in males (N = 28, mean age 26.0 years) compared to females (N = 27, mean age 24.9 years) with problematic alcohol consumption (Kaag et al., 2019). Notably, substance use, anxiety, and/or depressed mood did not account for the sex effects on reactivity of the DS. Taken together, the existing literature on sex differences in neuronal reactivity to alcohol cues is limited. Only small cohorts with rather young and non-dependent subjects have been investigated. A better understanding of sex differences in alcohol cue reactivity will inform the development of personalized strategies to treat patients with AUD, i.e., sex-sensitive prevention, diagnosis, and treatment approaches (Lenz et al., 2012; Barker and Taylor, 2019).

This study aimed to identify sex differences in brain reactivity to visual alcohol cues within specific regions of interest (ROI) in a larger sample of male and female patients in treatment for AUD. Based on the existing literature (Kaag et al., 2019), we hypothesized stronger alcohol cue reactivity in the dorsal striatum (DS) and ventral striatum (VS) in males compared to females. Based on previously reported findings and as corticostriatal-limbic regions are relevant in AUD (Zeng et al., 2021), we further examined sex differences in alcohol cue reactivity in the ACC, the medial prefrontal cortex (medPFC), the insula, the amygdala, and the hippocampus (including a core circuit for emotion regulation, Ochsner et al., 2004).

Materials and methods

Procedure and participants

Between 2008 and 2015, treatment-seeking patients with AUD from the Department of Addictive Behavior and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health (CIMH), Mannheim, Germany, participated in one of three studies, respectively, previously conducted in our lab (e.g., Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2011, 2012; Kiefer et al., 2015; Bach et al., 2020, 2022). Baseline functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) examination prior to any study intervention was used for the here presented analyses [N = 178 (23.6% female)]. The diagnosis of an alcohol dependency, corresponding to a moderate to severe AUD, was made by trained medical staff following the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10). A diagnosis of alcohol dependency following the ICD-10 or Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) can be considered as moderate to severe AUD according to the DSM, 5th edition (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Hasin et al., 2013; Hoffmann and Kopak, 2015). All study participants were given full particulars about the study and open questions were answered. Subsequently, all participants provided written informed consent prior to study participation. The local Ethics Committee II of the Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, approved the corresponding studies (approval number study 1 + 2: 2007-095F-MA; study 3: 2011-303-MA).

Following study inclusion, a structured clinical interview was conducted to examine possible comorbid mental disorders (Wittchen et al., 1997). Individuals with substance use disorder besides alcohol and nicotine or comorbid mental disorders within the last 12 months, neurological or severe somatic conditions, a history of brain injury, a use of psychotropic substances or medication (except during detoxification), and contraindications for an MRI examination were excluded from study participation. Prior to scanning, participants were free from any detoxification medication for at least 3 days, abstained from alcohol for 3−21 days, and did not show withdrawal symptoms [measured with the Clinical Institute Withdrawal Assessment scale (CIWA-Ar), Sullivan et al., 1989].

Before proceeding with the fMRI data acquisition, sociodemographic information was collected and participants completed self-report questionnaires. Alcohol craving and AUD severity were examined using the Obsessive-Compulsive Drinking Scale-German (OCDS-G; Anton, 2000; Mann and Ackermann, 2000) and the Alcohol Dependence Scale (ADS; Skinner and Horn, 1984; Doyle and Donovan, 2009), respectively. The last drinking day prior to the fMRI examination was assessed, and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1961), the trait questions from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Laux et al., 1981), and the Fagerstroem Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton et al., 1991) were applied.

Magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition

Functional magnetic resonance imaging paradigm

A well-established fMRI paradigm was used to examine neural alcohol cue reactivity (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010, 2012). In brief, alcohol-related (beer, wine, and schnapps) and neutral stimuli were presented in pseudo-randomized blocks. Slight changes in the application of the paradigm arose due to the combined data from three different studies. In study 1 and study 2, participants were asked to evaluate their alcohol craving following each alcohol-related or neutral block of stimuli (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010; studies 1 and 2, N = 113). In study 3, pictures were only passively viewed, and craving was not assessed between alcohol-related or neutral blocks of stimuli (Bach et al., 2020; N = 65).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging acquisition

Scanning was performed using a 3T whole-body tomograph (MAGNETOM Trio, TiM-technology; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). During all studies, images were presented to patients using MRI Audio/Video Systems goggles (Resonance Technology Inc., Los Angeles, CA, United States). The paradigm was presented using Presentation® software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, United States). For each subject, T2*-weighted echo-planar images (EPI) were acquired in a transversal orientation 30°clockwise to the bicommissural line (AC-PC line) using the same parameters for all three studies [repetition time (TR) = 2.41 s, echo time (TE) = 25 ms, flip angle = 80°, 42 slices, slice thickness: 2 mm, 1 mm gap, voxel size 3 × 3 × 3 mm3, field of view (FOV) 192 × 192 mm2, 64 × 64 in-plane resolution]. Additionally, a 5:21 min anatomical scan was performed to acquire a T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE (Magnetization Prepared-Rapid Gradient Echo) dataset [192 sagittal slices, TR = 2.30 s, TE = 3.03 ms, inversion time (TI) = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, slice thickness: 1 mm, 0.5 mm gap, voxel dimensions 1 × 1 × 1.5 mm3, FOV 256 × 256 mm2, 256 × 256 in-plane resolution].

Data analysis

Pre-processing and individual statistical analyses of brain imaging data were performed using SPM12 (Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, London, United Kingdom). Subsequent descriptive and statistical analyses were performed in SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging data preprocessing

For each functional data set, the first five scans were excluded from the analyses to avoid artifacts caused by magnetic saturation effects. The remaining scans were spatially realigned to correct for head motion, temporally realigned to minimize temporal differences in slice acquisition, and normalized to the SPM12 tissue probability map template provided by MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, QC, Canada). Subsequent smoothing of the images was performed using an isotropic Gaussian kernel (6 mm full width at half maximum, FWHM).

First-level functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses

Statistical analyses of the pre-processed fMRI data on the first-level (single subject) were performed by modeling the contrasts for alcohol-related stimuli (“alcohol” to implicit baseline), neutral stimuli (“neutral” to implicit baseline), and “alcohol > neutral,” i.e., neural reactivity during the presentation of alcohol-related stimuli vs. neutral stimuli. A general linear model (GLM) on a voxel-by-voxel basis was applied and convolution with the canonical hemodynamic response function was used. A high-pass filter was used (cut-off at 128 s). All six motion parameters from the preprocessing were included as regressors of no interest. A quality check was performed and subjects with excessive head movement (>3 mm/3°) or other artifacts were excluded from the subsequent analysis.

Anatomical masks and regions of interest data extraction

Preselected ROI are based on previous research on neural correlates of cue reactivity, cognitive control, and reward processing in AUD (Ochsner et al., 2004; Seo et al., 2011; Schacht et al., 2013; Kaag et al., 2019). The anatomical masks for the left and right hemispheres separately were created using the human WFU_pickatlas v31. The implemented aal (anatomical automatic labeling) atlas was used for mean data extraction of the ACC, medPFC (superior medial PFC), amygdala, and hippocampus. The implemented IBASPM71 atlas was used for mean data extraction of the caudate, insula, and putamen. Following Kaag et al. (2019), the VS was defined as the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and the DS was defined as a combined mask of the putamen and caudate (i1) minus the ventral striatum (i2) using the ImCalc function in SPM12 (i1–i2). In total, nine bilateral ROI were created. Data extraction of parameter estimates was performed for the individual contrasts “alcohol,” “neutral,” and “alcohol > neutral” within each of the aforementioned ROI. An automatized script based on SPM12 was used for extracting and averaging the data (mean) per ROI.

Analysis of sociodemographic, psychometric, and regions of interest data

Group characteristics with respect to study and sex were examined and are reported in the Supplementary material S1. Regarding ROI data, extreme outliers (values more than three box lengths from either hinge) were defined following the inspection of boxplots and the computation of inter quartile ranges in SPSS and excluded from subsequent analyses if they appeared for the contrasts “alcohol” or “neutral” (N = 12). N = 166 data sets (42 females) were available for subsequent analyses.

Variables that could potentially influence neural cue reactivity were examined (days of abstinence, smoking status, age; see Supplementary material S2). Subsequently, independent samples t-tests for continuous data or chi-square tests for dichotomous data were calculated to test for sex differences. Correction for multiple comparisons was applied using the false discovery rate (FDR) with the Benjamini and Hochberg method (Storey, 2002). Exploratory analyses to examine the influence of craving and severity of AUD on sex effects were conducted. Univariate GLM were used to assess the influence of sex as well as craving for alcohol and severity of AUD on neural cue reactivity within the nine predefined ROI for the contrasts “alcohol,” “neutral,” and “alcohol > neutral.” Uncorrected results are reported in the following. As for both, craving and severity of AUD, no interaction effects with sex emerged, these terms were removed from the analyses and only main effects are reported (Supplementary Tables S2, S3). All analyses included age as a potentially confounding variable.

Lastly, independent samples t-tests were used to test for sex differences with respect to the category of beverage (beer, wine, and schnapps) and are reported in the Supplementary material S3.

Effect size calculations

As for effect size (ES) calculations based on the previous analyses, Hedges’ g was estimated in SPSS. Post-hoc power analyses and sample size calculations using the here observed ES were conducted using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007).

In order to generate further exploratory value from our analyses, additional voxel-wise calculations of ES and corresponding confidence intervals were performed post-hoc. The procedure follows Gerchen et al. (2021). In short, whole brain two-sample t-tests were calculated in SPM12 for the contrast “alcohol > neutral” for “males > females.” Using the resulting t-maps and applying the “g_es_ci” Matlab script (Gerchen et al., 2021), the standardized ES Hedges’ g and the corresponding 95-% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated for all voxels in the nine ROI separately. Subsequently all ROI masks were resized to the same MNI dimensions as the ES g-map. Finally and separately for each of the aforementioned ROI, an in-house Matlab script was used to firstly create a data matrix including the ES as well as the lower and upper bound of the corresponding CI per voxel; secondly sort the data from low to high ES values; thirdly plot the distribution of ES including the lower and upper CI; and fourthly extract the median ES for each ROI (see Supplementary Figure S1).

Results

Sample characteristics

After exclusion of individuals with extreme outliers (N = 12), the study sample consisted of N = 166 individuals with AUD (25% females, 68% smokers). No significant difference in the sex ratio (male vs. female) emerged when comparing the three studies [χ2(2) = 2.621, p = 0.270]. No significant sex differences were observed for the severity of dependence, craving, days of abstinence, and further variables (p > 0.05), see Table 1. Results from analysis examining these variables with respect to all three studies can be found in the Supplementary material S1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample description.

Neural cue reactivity

Following independent samples t-tests and FDR correction for multiple testing, no significant sex differences regarding the contrasts “alcohol,” “neutral,” or “alcohol > neutral” were observed for the DS and VS (main hypothesis) or additional ROI (exploratory hypothesis) (p > 0.05) (see Tables 2A–C). Post-hoc analyses revealed that the sample sizes of the present data sets were not sufficient for detecting the corresponding effects with a power of ≥0.80 and an alpha error probability of ≤0.05.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Independent samples t-tests for all nine regions of interest for the contrasts “alcohol”, “neutral”, and “alcohol > neutral”.

The additional ES calculation (based on the effect size calculation for each voxel as described before) resulted in median ES for the contrast males > females between −0.093 (Caudate) and −0.247 (Putamen). As depicted in Supplementary Figure S1 the lack of significant group differences emerged once again.

Exploratory analyses of further sex effects

Post-hoc exploratory analyses were conducted to examine the possibly interacting influence of craving or severity of dependence on sex-effects in order to broaden our understanding of the present data.

When only adjusting for age, a significant sex effect was observed for the contrast “alcohol > neutral” on putamen activity (F(1, 166) = 5.666, p = 0.018, partial η2 = 0.034; males-females: MeanDiff = −0.125 (0.052), 95%-CI [−0.228, −0.021]). No further significant sex effects were observed.

When adjusting for craving (OCDS-G) and age, no significant interaction of sex and severity of craving was observed, and the interaction term was removed for subsequent analyses. An effect of sex was observed on neural activity of the putamen and medPFC for “alcohol > neutral” [putamen: F(1, 160) = 6.296, p = 0.013, partial η2 = 0.038; males-females: MeanDiff = −0.133 (0.053), 95%-CI [−0.238, −0.028]; medPFC: F(1, 160) = 4.035, p = 0.046, partial η2 = 0.025; males-females: MeanDiff = −0.241 (0.120), 95%-CI [−0.477, −0.004]; Supplementary Table S2C] and on neural activity of the putamen, DS, and amygdala for “neutral” (see Supplementary Table S2B). No significant sex effects emerged for other contrasts or ROI. OCDS-G score excerpted a significant influence on neural activity for the contrast “alcohol” (putamen, VS, amygdala, and hippocampus, Supplementary Table S2A). Please see Supplementary Table S2 for more details.

When adjusting for AUD severity (ADS) and age, no significant interaction of sex and severity of AUD was observed, and the interaction term was removed for subsequent analyses. A significant effect of sex effect was observed on neural reactivity of the putamen and medPFC for “alcohol > neutral” [putamen: F(1, 159) = 6.306, p = 0.013, partial η2 = 0.038; males-females: MeanDiff = −0.134 (0.052), 95%-CI [−0.240, −0.029]; medPFC: F(1, 159) = 4.145, p = 0.043, partial η2 = 0.025; males-females: MeanDiff = −0.244 (0.120), 95%-CI [−0.482, −0.007]; see Supplementary Table S3C] and on neural activity of the putamen, DS, and amygdala for “neutral” (see Supplementary Table S3B). No significant sex effects emerged for other contrasts or ROI. ADS score did not excerpt a significant influence on neural activity. Please see Supplementary Table S3 for more details.

Further, sex differences in neural reactivity with regard to specific alcoholic beverages (i.e., beer, wine, and schnapps) were examined as they might present a sex-specific bias. No statistically significant group differences were observed (pFDRcorr > 0.05). Analyses procedure and results are reported in the Supplementary material S3.

Whole-brain analyses on sex-differences in neural alcohol cue-reactivity for the contrast “alcohol > neutral” are reported in the Supplementary material S4. Males compared to females showed significantly less activation within the bilateral superior and middle occipital gyri, left lingual and fusiform gyri as well as the precentral and inferior frontal gyri, and right cuneus (family wise error-corrected threshold of p < 0.05).

Discussion

The aim of the study was to identify sex differences in neural reactivity toward alcohol stimuli of brain regions relevant for AUD in a sample of treatment-seeking in-patients with AUD. Here, we did not observe the hypothesized significantly higher alcohol cue reactivity in striatal regions in males vs. females. Remarkably, in the VS we did not even identify a single voxel with an effect in the hypothesized direction. We further did not observe significant sex differences in other brain regions commonly involved. On a descriptive level, in our data all ROI showed a tendency toward larger cue reactivity in females with ES on averaged data ranging from −0.137 to −0.418 (Table 2C) and median voxel-wise ES ranging from −0.093 to −0.247 (Supplementary Figure S1) for the task contrast “alcohol > neutral.”

Examining humans and rodents, prenatal and adult sex hormone activities have been related to alcohol consumption, brain structure, and neural function (Hermans et al., 2010; Lenz et al., 2012, 2017, 2018; Huber et al., 2018; Siegmann et al., 2019). Previous studies showed sex-specific neural reactivity toward alcohol cues with stronger effects in the superior and middle frontal gyrus in women with social alcohol drinking (Seo et al., 2011) and stronger striatal reactivity in men with problematic alcohol consumption (Kaag et al., 2019). Further, female-specific estradiol effects to induce dorsolateral striatal drug response have been observed in cocaine-dependent rats (Cummings et al., 2014). In nicotine-dependent women, neural reactivity of the putamen to smoking compared to non-smoking cues has further been shown to be dependent on the menstrual cycle status (Franklin et al., 2019). Interestingly, our uncorrected results (Table 2C) suggest that women with AUD might show stronger putamen reactivity toward substance cues - even though the direction of the effect opposes our hypothesis.

Several factors might explain the absence of the expected sex differences in our cohort.

First, considering both previous studies and our analyses, we studies individuals with AUD compared to social or heavy drinkers. Our data might indicate that sex differences in alcohol cue reactivity disappear from early to later phases of AUD development (see for e.g., Kaag et al., 2019). The incentive salience of alcohol stimuli represents a major contributor to alcohol use during early AUD, and more habit-associated mechanisms become relevant during later stages, i.e., severe AUD. Indeed, striatal neural reactivity toward alcohol cues has been suggested to be contingent on several factors. A shift from VS to DS cue reactivity from heavy social drinking to alcohol dependence (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010) and increased reactivity of the putamen during the first 2 weeks of abstinence (Bach et al., 2020) have been observed.

Second, previous studies including smaller sample sizes might have over-estimated ES. Our study was sufficiently powered to detect sex differences with medium ES. However, the Hedges’ g ES of the sex differences observed here varied between | 0.005| for the “alcohol” contrast in the amygdala and | 0.418| for the “alcohol > neutral” contrast in the putamen, which can be considered as very small to small. Further and with respect to a power of 0.80, it is possible that medium size effects might have been missed, even though they truly exist. To detect the maximal effect that we identified with this power, an even larger sample size of N = 182 (91 males and 91 females) would have been necessary.

When using a slightly different approach (Gerchen et al., 2021), voxel-wise ES estimation as well as corresponding CI were calculated and depicted. Besides the visual representation of the Hedges’ g ES per voxel within one ROI, displaying the CI allows for assessing the range of values that the ES of a specific voxel lies within – in our analyses with a probability of 95%. This procedure further allows for navigating the uncertainty of how well this sample of N = 166 individuals with AUD estimates the ES within a specific ROI for comparing alcohol vs. neutral cues when assessing sex differences, i.e., narrow CIs represent precise estimations. We could also, for example, inspect the figure with a reference of g = 0.2 (small effect) to evaluate the question of how many voxel within a specific ROI might have a probable ES superior to this or use an ES interval to examine the number of voxel within this range. The position of the ES curve with respect to null of the Y-axis (ES) also indicates a bias within the overall data distribution and can be quantified using the median effect size as a characteristic value of this ROI. Importantly, our analyses and visual presentation of the data does not only result in a deeper understanding of the lack of significant sex effects, as compared to solely stating a p-value. It further serves the community regarding the planning of the sample size and direction of effect (rather females > males) for future, confirmatory studies. However, our results suggest that possible sex effects are rather of small size. Thus, they could be of limited clinical relevance and might not significantly contribute to improving therapy outcomes. Even though sex differences were observed previously, e.g., on an endocrine level, they might not be strongly relevant in terms of a pathological neurobiological phenotype.

Third, differences in age might account for the divergence of the previous and our current findings. Our study population was older (45.4 and 46.1 years for females and males) than the cohorts of Kaag et al. (2019) (24.9 and 26.0 years) and Seo et al. (2011) (30.9 and 32.5 years). However and after adjusting for age, there were also no sex effects on neural cue reactivity of the VS or DS.

Sex differences in the development, maintenance, and phenomenology of AUD have to be acknowledged in research and treatment, and differences in alcohol drinking between men and women decline (Mccaul et al., 2019; White, 2020). Even though females are more at risk for severe somatic sequelae of alcohol, they are, compared to males, underrepresented in treatment settings for AUD (Mccaul et al., 2019). However, we also did not observe significant interaction of main effects of sex with either severity of craving or AUD. The uncorrected results indicate that craving influences neural cue reactivity toward alcoholic stimuli in both male and female patients with AUD as we observed that alcohol craving excerpted an influence on neural reactivity toward alcoholic stimuli in the putamen, VS, amygdala, and hippocampus (see Supplementary Table S2). The relation between self-reported craving and neural cue reactivity is well documented for alcohol and other substances (Jasinska et al., 2014).

Several limitations have to be discussed. The sample was unevenly balanced with respect to males and females. As it consisted of treatment-seeking patient, one cannot exclude that individual patients already have acquired and possibly used craving regulation strategies during the experiment. Patients were not instructed to respond in any specific way to the presented stimuli but were asked to rate their craving between blocks of alcohol and neutral stimuli in studies 1 and 2, but not study 3. Not giving specific instruction with regard to how to look at the stimuli [e.g., (not) regulating one’s craving] might also contribute to the here presented findings. Also, time since the last cigarette was not assessed but represents a major factor influencing alcohol cue-reactivity. It should further be noted that certain aspects, typically observed in female study participants, were not addressed in the current analyses due to the original study design, especially hormonal medication and menstrual cycle status. Both are of importance since oral contraceptives exert hormonal effects on the central nervous system (Hampson, 2020), mesolimbic incentive processing changes during the menstrual cycle (Dreher et al., 2007), and alterations in estradiol and progesterone activities associate with AUD and related phenotypes (Mühle et al., 2019; Weinland et al., 2021). In a monetary incentive delay task, women show stronger ventral striatal activation during the premenstrual than during the follicular phase (Ossewaarde et al., 2011). Also, a premenstrual volume increase of the amygdala has been reported and correlated with higher stress-induced negative affect (Ossewaarde et al., 2013). Cue reactivity in striatal regions, such as the putamen, were found to be modulated by menstrual cycle status in cigarette smokers (Franklin et al., 2019). Moreover, we did not correct for diurnal variations in craving (West and Schneider, 1987).

Consequently, future studies are necessary to investigate neural cue reactivity in a longitudinal design to address the question, whether prominent sex differences in neural cue reactivity diminishes over time and whether the small effect of increased cue reactivity in females might be more pronounced when specific phases of the menstrual cycle are analyzed separately. In terms of losing and regaining control over alcohol consumption, the causal association between female-specific aspects such as hormonal fluctuations and the progression from social drinking to severe AUD, as well as examining the complex interplay between sex and further (clinical) factors are subject to future research. Addressing the hypotheses of stronger (striatal) alcohol cue-reactivity in females when examining underlying causalities for the faster progression from drinking to being alcohol-dependent might open new perspectives that might then be translated into sex- and gender-oriented addiction therapy.

Conclusion

In this study, we did not observe significant sex differences in neural alcohol cue reactivity in patients with AUD. We further found only very small to less than medium effect sizes in the direction of higher cue reactivity in female patients. Previous studies in younger social and problematic alcohol drinkers indicate sex differences in neural cue reactivity within reward-related brain regions (males > females). Women might differ from men in terms of neural reactivity to alcohol cues during early phases of the addiction development. However, our data indicates that a clinically significant effect of sex differences might diminish in later phases of AUD.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Local Ethics Committee II of the Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany (approval number study 1 + 2: 2007-095F-MA; study 3: 2011-303-MA). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SG, PB, and BL designed the current study. SG analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. SH, HT, MG, SV-K, and PB supported the data analysis. PB and BL supported to write the manuscript. SG, SH, PB, and BL interpreted the data. SV-K, PK, and FK conceived and designed the original experiments. PK and FK procured the funding of the original studies. All authors commented on the manuscript and provided intellectual input.

Funding

This project was supported in part by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation): Project ID 402170461 (Heinz et al., 2020), Project ID 324164820 (GRK2350/1), and Center Grant SFB636/D6. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

For the publication fee, we acknowledge financial support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft within the funding program “Open Access Publikationskosten” as well as by Heidelberg University.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1039917/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.nitrc.org/projects/wfu_pickatlas/

References

Agabio, R., Pisanu, C., Gessa, G. L., and Franconi, F. (2017). Sex differences in alcohol use disorder. Curr. Med. Chem. 24, 2661–2670. doi: 10.2174/0929867323666161202092908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anton, R. F. (2000). Obsessive–compulsive aspects of craving: development of the obsessive compulsive drinking scale. Addiction 95, 211–217. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach, P., Reinhard, I., Koopmann, A., Bumb, J. M., Sommer, W. H., Vollstädt-Klein, S., et al. (2022). Test-retest reliability of neural alcohol cue-reactivity: is there light at the end of the magnetic resonance imaging tube? Addict. Biol. 27:e13069. doi: 10.1111/adb.13069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach, P., Weil, G., Pompili, E., Hoffmann, S., Hermann, D., Vollstädt-Klein, S., et al. (2020). Incubation of neural alcohol cue reactivity after withdrawal and its blockade by naltrexone. Addict. Biol. 25:e12717. doi: 10.1111/adb.12717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, J. M., and Taylor, J. R. (2019). Sex differences in incentive motivation and the relationship to the development and maintenance of alcohol use disorders. Physiol. Behav. 203, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, J. B., Mcclellan, M. L., and Reed, B. G. (2017). Sex differences, gender and addiction. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 136–147. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boykoff, N., Schneekloth, T. D., Hall-Flavin, D., Loukianova, L., Karpyak, V. M., Stevens, S. R., et al. (2010). Gender differences in the relationship between depressive symptoms and cravings in alcoholism. Am. J. Addict. 19, 352–356. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2010.00057.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceylan-Isik, A. F., Mcbride, S. M., and Ren, J. (2010). Sex difference in alcoholism: who is at a greater risk for development of alcoholic complication? Life Sci. 87, 133–138. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummings, J. A., Jagannathan, L., Jackson, L. R., and Becker, J. B. (2014). Sex differences in the effects of estradiol in the nucleus accumbens and striatum on the response to cocaine: neurochemistry and behavior. Drug Alcohol Depend. 135, 22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyle, S. R., and Donovan, D. M. (2009). A validation study of the alcohol dependence scale. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 70, 689–699. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreher, J. C., Schmidt, P. J., Kohn, P., Furman, D., Rubinow, D., and Berman, K. F. (2007). Menstrual cycle phase modulates reward-related neural function in women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 2465–2470.

Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, T. R., Jagannathan, K., Ketcherside, A., Spilka, N., and Wetherill, R. R. (2019). Menstrual cycle phase modulates responses to smoking cues in the putamen: preliminary evidence for a novel target. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 198, 100–104. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.01.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerchen, M. F., Kirsch, P., and Feld, G. B. (2021). Brain-wide inferiority and equivalence tests in fMRI group analyses: selected applications. Hum. Brain Mapp. 42, 5803–5813. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampson, E. (2020). A brief guide to the menstrual cycle and oral contraceptive use for researchers in behavioral endocrinology. Horm. Behav. 119:104655. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasin, D. S., O’brien, C. P., Auriacombe, M., Borges, G., Bucholz, K., Budney, A., et al. (2013). DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorders: recommendations and rationale. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 834–851. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12060782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., and Fagerstrom, K. O. (1991). The fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire. Br. J. Addict. 86, 1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinz, A., Kiefer, F., Smolka, M. N., Endrass, T., Beste, C., Beck, A., et al. (2020). Addiction research consortium: losing and regaining control over drug intake (ReCoDe)-from trajectories to mechanisms and interventions. Addict. Biol. 25:e12866. doi: 10.1111/adb.12866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermans, E. J., Bos, P. A., Ossewaarde, L., Ramsey, N. F., Fernández, G., and Van Honk, J. (2010). Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal Bold response during reward anticipation in healthy women. Neuroimage 52, 277–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, N. G., and Kopak, A. M. (2015). How well do the DSM-5 alcohol use disorder designations map to the Icd-10 disorders? Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 39, 697–701. doi: 10.1111/acer.12685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, S. E., Zoicas, I., Reichel, M., Mühle, C., Büttner, C., Ekici, A. B., et al. (2018). Prenatal androgen receptor activation determines adult alcohol and water drinking in a sex-specific way. Addict. Biol. 23, 904–920. doi: 10.1111/adb.12540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasinska, A. J., Stein, E. A., Kaiser, J., Naumer, M. J., and Yalachkov, Y. (2014). Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: a survey of human neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 38, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaag, A. M., Wiers, R. W., De Vries, T. J., Pattij, T., and Goudriaan, A. E. (2019). Striatal alcohol cue-reactivity is stronger in male than female problem drinkers. Eur. J. Neurosci. 50, 2264–2273. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, F., Kirsch, M., Bach, P., Hoffmann, S., Reinhard, I., Jorde, A., et al. (2015). Effects of D-cycloserine on extinction of mesolimbic cue reactivity in alcoholism: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology 232, 2353–2362. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3882-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühn, S., and Gallinat, J. (2011). Common biology of craving across legal and illegal drugs - a quantitative meta-analysis of cue-reactivity brain response. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 1318–1326. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07590.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., and Spielberger, C. (1981). STAI. State-trait-angstinventar. Göttingen: Beltz Test GmbH.

Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Mühle, C., and Kornhuber, J. (2018). Low digit ratio (2D:4D) and late pubertal onset indicate prenatal hyperandrogenziation in alcohol binge drinking. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 86, 370–378. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Mühle, C., Braun, B., Weinland, C., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Behrens, J., et al. (2017). Prenatal and adult androgen activities in alcohol dependence. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 136, 96–107. doi: 10.1111/acps.12725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenz, B., Müller, C. P., Stoessel, C., Sperling, W., Biermann, T., Hillemacher, T., et al. (2012). Sex hormone activity in alcohol addiction: integrating organizational and activational effects. Prog. Neurobiol. 96, 136–163. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, K., and Ackermann, K. (2000). Die OCDS-G: psychometrische kennwerte der deutschen version der obsessive compulsive drinking scale. Sucht 46, 90–100.

Google Scholar

Mccaul, M. E., Roach, D., Hasin, D. S., Weisner, C., Chang, G., and Sinha, R. (2019). Alcohol and women: a brief overview. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 43, 774–779. doi: 10.1111/acer.13985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mühle, C., Barry, B., Weinland, C., Kornhuber, J., and Lenz, B. (2019). Estrogen receptor 1 gene variants and estradiol activities in alcohol dependence. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 92, 301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, C. P., Mühle, C., Kornhuber, J., and Lenz, B. (2021). Sex-dependent alcohol instrumentalization goals in non-addicted alcohol consumers versus patients with alcohol use disorder: longitudinal change and outcome prediction. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 45, 577–586. doi: 10.1111/acer.14550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23, 483–499.

Google Scholar

Ossewaarde, L., Van Wingen, G. A., Kooijman, S. C., Backstrom, T., Fernandez, G., and Hermans, E. J. (2011). Changes in functioning of mesolimbic incentive processing circuits during the premenstrual phase. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 612–620. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ossewaarde, L., Van Wingen, G. A., Rijpkema, M., Backstrom, T., Hermans, E. J., and Fernandez, G. (2013). Menstrual cycle-related changes in amygdala morphology are associated with changes in stress sensitivity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1187–1193. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peltier, M. R., Verplaetse, T. L., Mineur, Y. S., Petrakis, I. L., Cosgrove, K. P., Picciotto, M. R., et al. (2019). Sex differences in stress-related alcohol use. Neurobiol. Stress 10, 100149–100149.

Google Scholar

Petit, G., Luminet, O., Cordovil De Sousa Uva, M., Monhonval, P., Leclercq, S., Spilliaert, Q., et al. (2017). Gender differences in affects and craving in alcohol-dependence: a study during alcohol detoxification. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 421–431. doi: 10.1111/acer.13292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehm, J., Anderson, P., Barry, J., Dimitrov, P., Elekes, Z., Feijão, F., et al. (2015). Prevalence of and potential influencing factors for alcohol dependence in Europe. Eur. Addict. Res. 21, 6–18.

Google Scholar

Schacht, J. P., Anton, R. F., and Myrick, H. (2013). Functional neuroimaging studies of alcohol cue reactivity: a quantitative meta-analysis and systematic review. Addict. Biol. 18, 121–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00464.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, D., Jia, Z., Lacadie, C. M., Tsou, K. A., Bergquist, K., and Sinha, R. (2011). Sex differences in neural responses to stress and alcohol context cues. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1998–2013. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegmann, E. M., Bouna-Pyrrou, P., Lenz, B., and Kornhuber, J. (2019). Digit ratio (2D:4D) in relation to substance and computer use: a meta-analysis. J. Neural Transm. 126, 623–636. doi: 10.1007/s00702-019-02002-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, H. A., and Horn, J. L. (1984). Alcohol dependence scale (ADS): user’s guide. Ontario, CA: Addiction Research Foundation.

Google Scholar

Slade, T., Chapman, C., Swift, W., Keyes, K., Tonks, Z., and Teesson, M. (2016). Birth cohort trends in the global epidemiology of alcohol use and alcohol-related harms in men and women: systematic review and metaregression. BMJ Open 6:e011827. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storey, J. D. (2002). A direct approach to false discovery rates. J. R. Stat. Soc. 64, 479–498.

Google Scholar

Sullivan, J. T., Sykora, K., Schneiderman, J., Naranjo, C. A., and Sellers, E. M. (1989). Assessment of alcohol withdrawal: the revised clinical institute withdrawal assessment for alcohol scale (CIWA-Ar). Br. J. Addict. 84, 1353–1357. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb00737.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollstädt-Klein, S., Loeber, S., Kirsch, M., Bach, P., Richter, A., Buhler, M., et al. (2011). Effects of cue-exposure treatment on neural cue reactivity in alcohol dependence: a randomized trial. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 1060–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollstädt-Klein, S., Loeber, S., Richter, A., Kirsch, M., Bach, P., Von Der Goltz, C., et al. (2012). Validating incentive salience with functional magnetic resonance imaging: association between mesolimbic cue reactivity and attentional bias in alcohol-dependent patients. Addict. Biol. 17, 807–816. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00352.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Buhler, M., Klein, O., Ende, G., et al. (2010). Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction 105, 1741–1749. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinland, C., Mühle, C., Kornhuber, J., and Lenz, B. (2021). Progesterone serum levels correlate negatively with craving in female postmenopausal in-patients with alcohol use disorder: a sex- and menopausal status-separated study. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 110:110278. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R., and Schneider, N. (1987). Craving for cigarettes. Br. J. Addict. 82, 407–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1987.tb01496.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, A. M. (2020). Gender differences in the epidemiology of alcohol use and related harms in the United States. Alcohol. Res. 40:01.

Google Scholar

White, A., Castle, I. J., Chen, C. M., Shirley, M., Roach, D., and Hingson, R. (2015). Converging patterns of alcohol use and related outcomes among females and males in the United States, 2002 to 2012. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 39, 1712–1726. doi: 10.1111/acer.12815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WHO (2018). Global status report on alcohol and health 2018. http://www.who.int/substance_abuse/publications/global_alcohol_report/en/ [Accessed June 02, 2020]

Google Scholar

Wittchen, H.-U., Wunderlich, U., Gruschwitz, S., and Zaudig, M. (1997). SKID I. Strukturiertes klinisches interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen. Interviewheft und beurteilungsheft. Eine deutschsprachige, erweiterte Bearb. d. amerikanischen originalversion des SKID I. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Zeng, J., Yu, S., Cao, H., Su, Y., Dong, Z., and Yang, X. (2021). Neurobiological correlates of cue-reactivity in alcohol-use disorders: a voxel-wise meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 294–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilberman, M., Tavares, H., and El-Guebaly, N. (2004). Gender similarities and differences. J. Addict. Dis. 22, 61–74. doi: 10.1300/J069v22n04_06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sex difference, alcohol cue reactivity, alcohol use disorder, addiction, craving, fMRI

Citation: Gerhardt S, Hoffmann S, Tan H, Gerchen MF, Kirsch P, Vollstädt-Klein S, Kiefer F, Bach P and Lenz B (2022) Neural cue reactivity is not stronger in male than in female patients with alcohol use disorder. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:1039917. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1039917

Received: 08 September 2022; Accepted: 20 October 2022;
Published: 16 November 2022.

Edited by:

Daniel Manvich, Rowan University School of Osteopathic Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Amanda Elton, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, United States
Reagan Wetherill, University of Pennsylvania, United States
Erica Nicole Grodin, University of California, Los Angeles, United States

Copyright © 2022 Gerhardt, Hoffmann, Tan, Gerchen, Kirsch, Vollstädt-Klein, Kiefer, Bach and Lenz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sarah Gerhardt, sarah.gerhardt@zi-mannheim.de

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.