Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 19 April 2021
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors
This article is part of the Research Topic The Neuroethology of Social Behavior View all 10 articles

Resurgence of an Inborn Attraction for Animate Objects via Thyroid Hormone T3

  • 1Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
  • 2Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan

For inexperienced brains, some stimuli are more attractive than others. Human neonates and newly hatched chicks preferentially orient towards face-like stimuli, biological motion, and objects changing speed. In chicks, this enhances exposure to social partners, and subsequent attachment trough filial imprinting. Early preferences are not steady. For instance, preference for stimuli changing speed fades away after 2 days in chicks. To understand the physiological mechanisms underlying these transient responses, we tested whether early preferences for objects changing speed can be promoted by thyroid hormone 3,5,3′-triiodothyronine (T3). This hormone determines the start of imprinting’s sensitive period. We found that the preference for objects changing speed can be re-established in female chicks treated with T3. Moreover, day-1 chicks treated with an inhibitor of endogenous T3 did not show any preference. These results suggest that the time windows of early predispositions and of sensitive period for imprinting are controlled by the same molecular mechanisms.

Introduction

Early experience plays a crucial role in shaping neural and behavioural development. However, early experience effects are stronger during certain periods of development (Hensch, 2005). An example is provided by filial imprinting (Spalding, 1873; Hess, 1959; Bateson, 1979; Horn, 2004; McCabe, 2013; Vallortigara and Versace, 2018). Shortly after birth or hatching, the young of some animals, usually of precocial species, learn to recognise their social partners (e.g., the mother and siblings) by simply being exposed to them. In the young domestic fowl (Gallus gallus), for instance, imprinting usually occurs within 24–48 h from hatching. Lorenz (1935) used the term “critical period” to refer to the fact that rather than being available throughout the lifespan, filial imprinting is shown only during a limited period of life. This time window can be extended for a few more days when a proper stimulus is not immediately available (Bolhuis, 1991). Being a flexible period with the characteristics of a self-terminating process (Bolhuis, 1991), imprinting is now better known as a “sensitive period.” Sensitive periods are windows of plasticity during brain development, in which experience has a powerful effect (Hensch, 2005; Dehorter and Del Pino, 2020).

The thyroid hormone 3,5,3′-triiodothyronine (T3) is implicated in the timing of the sensitive period for imprinting (Yamaguchi et al., 2012). In domestic chicks, imprinting causes a rapid inflow of T3 in the brain, particularly in the intermediate medial mesopallium (IMM), an associative telencephalic region involved in learning the features of the imprinting object (Horn, 2004; Yamaguchi et al., 2012). Via Wnt-signalling pathway, the enzyme Dio2 (type 2 iodothyronine deiodinase), localised in vascular endothelial cells of the brain, converts the inactive form thyroxine (T4) into the active form T3 (Yamaguchi et al., 2012, 2018). Endogenous T3 level in the brain peaks around the peri-hatch period, and decays within a few days if not boosted by imprinting. Injection of iopanoic acid (IOP), a potent inhibitor of Dio2, impairs visual imprinting during the sensitive period (Yamaguchi et al., 2012). After the sensitive period, administration of exogenous T3 allows non-imprinted chicks to imprint, re-opening the sensitive period for memory formation (Yamaguchi et al., 2012).

Re-opening of sensitive periods has been obtained also by other pharmacological agents (Batista et al., 2016, 2018; Yamaguchi et al., 2016; Aoki et al., 2018), and it has been discovered for phenomena other than filial imprinting, such as ocular dominance in non-human mammals (Hensch and Quinlan, 2018) and absolute pitch in humans (Gervain et al., 2013).

Although imprinting can occur with either naturalistic stimuli (resembling a conspecific) or artificial objects, a large amount of evidence shows the existence of spontaneous unlearned preferences for animate features (Rosa-Salva et al., 2021). These preferences act as a sort of canalisation mechanism to direct the newborns’ attention, favouring exposure to stimuli that are more likely to be social partners (Di Giorgio et al., 2017; Versace et al., 2018; Rosa-Salva et al., 2021). Preferences for animacy cues, that set apart animate from non-animate objects, have been described in newly hatched chicks, comprising, e.g., preferences for face-like stimuli (Rosa-Salva et al., 2010), biological motion stimuli (Vallortigara et al., 2005; Miura and Matsushima, 2016; Miura et al., 2020) and self-propelled objects that move with variable speed (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016; for review see: Di Giorgio et al., 2017; Lorenzi and Vallortigara, 2021; Vallortigara, 2021). The same animacy cues operate on human newborns and other species, in particular for the preference for speed changes we are dealing with here (see in the human newborns Di Giorgio et al., 2021 and see for reviews: Di Giorgio et al., 2017; Lorenzi and Vallortigara, 2021; Rosa-Salva et al., 2021; Vallortigara, 2021).

These biological priors, whose main function seems to speed up learning by canalising imprinting, also operate only during transient windows of sensitivity in development. Visually naïve chicks show a spontaneous preference for the head (face-like) region of a stuffed hen during the first 2 days post-hatching, which then fades away on day 3 (Johnson et al., 1989). The spontaneous preference for objects moving with visible speed changes (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016) shows a window of sensitivity in three genetically selected and isolated breeds of chicks for only the first day of life, then disappearing on day 3 (Versace et al., 2019). Similarly, the biological motion preference occurs only within the first few days of life (Miura and Matsushima, 2012). Importantly, appearing slightly later on day 2 of life, biological motion preference exhibits also sex differences, with females being choosier than males when approaching a biological motion stimulus (Miura and Matsushima, 2012).

From the second day of life, precocious sexually dimorphic behaviours start to emerge in chicks (Andrew, 1966). Due to different levels of social motivation and aggression, males and females exhibit different attitudes towards familiar and unfamiliar individuals (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara et al., 1990; Vallortigara, 1992; Versace et al., 2017; Lemaire et al., 2020; Santolin et al., 2020). Progressively, females develop strong social cohesive behaviours with familiar subjects, while males engage more in aggressive and explorative ones (McBride and Foenander, 1962; McBride et al., 1969). At the neurobiological level, imprinting-related gene expression in the brain shows remarkable differences between sexes (Yamaguchi et al., 2012); among others, an upregulation of Dio2 gene enabling imprinting, which appears to be quantitatively different between males and females (Yamaguchi et al., 2012).

Here we report that the thyroid hormone T3 can modulate the timing of one window of early preferences for animacy cues, i.e., change of speed. Different from the sensitive period for imprinting, this preference does not depend on a particular experience with stimuli but rather canalises the animal’s attention towards particular stimuli, working as a spontaneous, unlearned biological prior.

We devised three different experimental conditions. The first was aimed to confirm that the same window of sensitivity for the spontaneous animacy preference conveyed by visible speed changes shown in genetically selected strains (Versace et al., 2019) also exists in the strain of broiler chicks we were using in the lab. We confirmed that the preference is there on post-hatching day 1, but fades away on day 2. To check whether T3 influences the duration of this window of sensitivity, we performed two experiments. First, in order to show that inhibition of endogenous T3 action can abolish the animacy preference, we injected the inhibitor IOP on post-hatching day 1, and compared IOP-injected chicks with vehicle-injected peers. Second, in order to show that the animacy preference can be re-established by exogenous T3 administration, we injected chicks with T3 on post-hatching day 3 and compared T3-injected chicks with vehicle-injected peers.

Results

The results for the not-injected condition are shown in Figure 1. The permutation test revealed a significant main effect of testing Day (F(1,67) = 5.40, p < 0.05), but did not reveal any effect of Sex (F(1,67) = 1.17, p = 0.28) nor interaction (F(1,67) = 0.05, p = 0.83; see Supplementary Table 1 for the number of subjects tested in each condition for each sex, and for results split by sex). Chicks tested on day 1 showed a significant preference for animacy (V(35) = 458, p < 0.05, d = 0.44), whereas chicks tested on day 3 did not show any preference (V(36) = 310, p = 0.72, d = 0.08).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Preference for animacy for the not-injected chicks tested 1 or 3 days after hatching. To best represent the data, we used raincloud plots. Asterisks indicate significant differences from chance (black dotted line). Black asterisk between the two groups indicates a significant difference between testing Days. * indicates p < 0.05. Filled red dots represent female and blue male subjects, empty dots represent outliers, which were removed from the analyses.

The results for chicks treated on day 1 with the IOP (Dio2-inhibitor) are shown in Figure 2. The permutation test revealed a significant main effect of Treatment (F(1,54) = 5.74, p < 0.05), but did not reveal any effect of Sex (F(1,54) = 1.10, p = 0.30) nor interaction (F(1,54) = 0.66, p = 0.42; see Supplementary Table 1). The vehicle-injected group showed a significant preference for animacy (V(28) = 347, p < 0.001, d = 1.01), whereas the IOP-injected group did not show any preference (V(30) = 268.5, p = 0.46, d = 0.14). As expected, at this age no sex differences were apparent.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Preference for animacy for the chicks tested 1 day after hatching injected with IOP or with vehicle. To best represent the data, we used raincloud plots. Asterisks indicate significant differences from chance (black dotted line). Black asterisk between the two groups indicates a significant difference between IOP- and vehicle-injected chicks. *** indicates p < 0.001, * indicates p < 0.05. Filled red dots represent female and blue male subjects, empty dots represent outliers, which were removed from the analyses.

The results for the chicks injected with T3 on day 3 are shown in Figure 3. As expected, the permutation test revealed at this age a significant interaction between Treatment and Sex (F(1,57) = 25.02, p < 0.001) but did not reveal any main effect of Treatment (F(1,57) = 1.27, p = 0.26) or Sex (F(1,57) = 0.20, p = 0.65). Females and males showed a different pattern within each group, T3-injected (W(29) = 166.5, p < 0.01, d = 1.17) and vehicle-injected (W(32) = 39.5, p < 0.001, d = 1.40). T3- and vehicle-injected females showed a significant difference in their preferences (W(29) = 183.5, p < 0.001, d = 1.59). T3-injected females showed a significant preference for animacy (V(14) = 85, p < 0.05, d = 0.63), whereas vehicle-injected females showed a significant preference for the non-animacy stimulus (V(15) = 11, p < 0.01, d = 1.0). T3- and vehicle-injected males also showed a marginally significant difference in their preferences (W(32) = 61, p < 0.05, d = 0.98). However, in spite of a trend for an inverted pattern with respect to females, T3-injected males did not show any significant preference for the stimuli (V(15) = 27, p > 0.05, d = 0.53) nor did vehicle-injected males (V(17) = 116, p > 0.05, d = 0.47).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Preference for animacy for the chicks tested 3 days after hatching injected with T3 or vehicle. To best represent the data, we used raincloud plots. Black asterisks indicate a significant difference between sexes (females red and males blue) within each injection group. Red asterisks above a red horizontal line indicate a significant difference between females of each injection group. Blue asterisks above a blue horizontal line indicate a significant difference between males of each injection group. Red asterisks indicate a significant difference from chance in females. *** indicates p < 0.001, ** indicates p < 0.01, * indicates p < 0.05. Filled red dots represent female and blue male subjects, empty dots represent outliers, which were removed from the analyses.

Discussion

Untreated chicks showed a clear spontaneous preference for visible speed changes, a self-propelled cue to animacy (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016), on post-hatching day 1 that disappeared on day 3. Restricted sensitivity windows to animacy motion cues seem thus to exist in chicks similar to those for the head region of the mother hen and for biological motion (Johnson et al., 1989; Miura and Matsushima, 2012). Similar sensitivity windows have been described in humans as well. During the first month of life infants preferentially look at schematic face-like configurations over identical stimuli in a non-face configuration, whereas the same is not observed at 3 and 5 months of age (Johnson et al., 1991). Also, the intensity of selective EEG responses to face-like patterns tends to decrease over time after the first hours of life (Buiatti et al., 2019).

The thyroid hormone T3 appears to play a crucial role in the sensitivity window for spontaneous motion animacy preference. Animacy preference disappeared on day 1 in chicks injected with IOP, an inhibitor of Dio2 the enzyme converting the inactive form T4 into the active T3. While vehicle-injected day 1 chicks had a significant preference for animacy, consistently with previous findings (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016).

On day 3, administration of T3 seems to restore animacy preference, at least in females. Sex differences, which are well-known to be associated with critical period regulation by thyroid hormones (see Miura and Matsushima, 2012; Yamaguchi et al., 2012; Batista and Hensch, 2019), seem to be complicated at this age also by the effect of the injecting needle in itself. T3-injected females preferred the animacy stimulus, while vehicle-injected females preferred the non-animacy one. Given that we do not expect any physiological effect of vehicle (as indeed this is the case for day-1 treated chicks), it seems that in older chicks the mere, though mild, pain experience of injecting the needle interacts with the expression of animacy preferences at least in females (males showed a somewhat similar effect but with inverted direction).

Precocious sex differences in social motivation and aggression are commonly observed in chicks (Andrew, 1966). Functionally, it has been argued that they may arise from different levels of social motivation (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara et al., 1990) and attitudes towards novelty (Vallortigara, 1992) in the two sexes. Females tend to engage more than males in social reinstatement and usually prefer to approach familiar individuals (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara et al., 1990; Vallortigara, 1992), whereas males tend to approach unfamiliar ones (Vallortigara, 1992; Versace et al., 2017; Santolin et al., 2020). In natural populations fowls exhibit territorial behaviour wherein single dominant cocks maintain and patrol a large territory within which several highly social-aggregated females live (McBride and Foenander, 1962; McBride et al., 1969). This social organisation may favour the prevalence of gregarious and affiliative behaviours in females and aggressive and exploratory behaviours in males.

Sex differences have also been described for biological motion preferences, whose window of sensitivity occurs exactly around day 3 (Miura and Matsushima, 2012). Therefore, it could be that the injection of T3 affected both sexes similarly, but that they exhibited different behavioural responses coherent with their natural preferences that become apparent from day 3. Females would thus preferentially approach the animacy stimulus, engaging in social reinstatement with the most familiar, as predisposed, object. Whereas males, less socially motivated, would engage in explorative responses towards the unfamiliar, non-predisposed, object.

It could also be that the efficiency of T3 transport from muscle through blood vessels and finally to the brain is different between males and females. This may point towards a possible limitation of the method adopted here and suggests different T3 administration strategies.

From a neurobiological perspective, sex differences related to imprinting have been found in the brain. Several genes upregulate during imprinting, but only some do it in a sex dimorphic way. The Dio2 gene expresses more in males than in females during imprinting, while Tubby-like protein 1 gene, producing an upstream cell signalling protein, expresses more in females than males (Yamaguchi et al., 2012). Such differences might underly the behavioural differences between the two sexes observed in the present study in older chicks.

Note that vehicle-injected females on day 3 preferred the non-animacy stimulus, whereas males had a trend to prefer the animacy one. From day 3, chicks start to be more fearful due to the rising of hormonal levels (Schaller and Emlen, 1962; Rogers, 1995). Avoidance behaviours increase progressively from day 3 on, but with different timings in the two sexes. Males show a weaker avoidance response than females until post-hatching day 4 (Schaller and Emlen, 1962). Sex differences observed in the vehicle group on day 3 might arise from the different reactions to the needle’s stressful event. The fear caused by the injection in females might have evoked an avoidance response towards the most animate object, the animacy stimulus, making them walk to the opposite end of the apparatus. In males, less avoidant and more prone to aggression at this age, the same event could have evoked a tendency to approach the most animate stimulus.

It is also worth noting that early during development the levels of different thyroid hormones (T3 and T4, thyroxine) in the brain show sex-dependent differences. The onset of the surge of T4 in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) precedes that of females, while the onset of T3 in females precedes that of males (Yamaguchi et al., 2017). Strengthening the hypothesis of an interaction between T3 and other sexually dimorphic hormones, response to animacy in visually naïve chicks involves brain regions rich in sex steroid hormone receptors (Mayer et al., 2016, 2017a, 2017b; Lorenzi et al., 2017), part of the so-called Social Behavior Network, which appears to be highly conserved in vertebrates (Newman, 1999; O’Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Goodson and Kingsbury, 2013; Lorenzi et al., 2017).

Thyroid hormones are key regulators of vertebrates’ brain development (Van Herck et al., 2013). Among others, T3 directly influences the expression of genes by binding to its nuclear receptors, which act as transcription factors (Harvey and Williams, 2002). Therefore, abnormalities in the level of T3 during development may result in permanent impairments. In the present study, inhibiting T3 function gave rise to a lack of the spontaneous approach behaviour towards animacy stimuli. Abnormalities in spontaneous preferences for animacy at birth have been linked to autistic spectrum disorder (Sgadò et al., 2018; Lorenzi et al., 2019). Interestingly, human neonates at high familial risk for autism exhibit anomalous preferential looking patterns to animacy cues provided by schematic face-like and biological motion stimuli (Di Giorgio et al., 2016).

Conclusion

In conclusion, we showed that providing exogenous T3 restores the predisposition to orient towards motion-animacy cue on day 3 post-hatch, whereas inhibiting the endogenous T3 conversion prevents the predisposition to appear on day 1. The similarity of thyroid hormone effects on imprinting and on early predispositions is unlikely to be coincidental. Furthermore, re-opening the neural plasticity to the effects of environmental stimuli without restoring at the same time the biological priors for proper environmental stimulation could appear worthless. The two processes should be functionally and temporally linked, and probably for this reason they share the same molecular ground.

Materials and Methods

Animals

All experiments were carried out in compliance with the applicable European Union and Italian laws, and guidelines for animals’ care and use. All the experimental procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Trento OPBA and by the Italian Health Ministry (permit number 1139/2015).

Fertilised eggs of the Aviagen Ross 308 strain were provided by a commercial hatchery (Azienda Agricola Crescenti, Brescia, Italy). Upon arrival, eggs were incubated under controlled temperature (37.7°C until post-hatching day 1, 33°C until post-hatching day 3) and humidity (60%) within incubators (FIEM MG140/200 Rural LCD EVO). Incubators were kept in darkness, preventing the chicks from any visual experience during incubation and hatching prior to testing. We employed a total of 190 domestic chicks (G. gallus; 95 females; the exact number of chicks in each condition and sex is shown in Supplementary Table 1). Sex was determined by feather dimorphism.

Apparatus and Stimuli

We used the same apparatus and stimuli as in previous studies investigating motion-animacy preference in newly hatched chicks (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016, 2018; Lorenzi et al., 2017, 2019). A detailed description of the apparatus, stimuli and procedure used can be found in Rosa-Salva et al. (2016) (Exp. 2). Briefly, the apparatus consisted of a white corridor (85 × 30 × 30 cm, see Figure 4) with two opposite high-frequency monitor screens (ASUS MG248Q, 24″, 120 Hz). Three areas subdivided the corridor: a central one (starting area: 45 cm long) and two lateral ones (choice areas: 20 cm long each). A small step (1.5 cm high) on each side delimited the boundaries between starting and choice areas. A video-camera, centrally located above the apparatus, recorded animals’ behaviour. The two stimuli were displayed on the screens and represented a red circle (diameter 3 cm) moving horizontally. One stimulus was moving at a constant speed (≈4.64 cm/s on our monitors) while the other one was visibly changing speed (the slower speed being ≈3.37 cm/s and the faster one being ≈19.64 cm/s), a reliable cue to animacy (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016). We counterbalanced the position of the stimuli on the screens between subjects.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Schematic representation of the experimental apparatus. A white corridor with two video-screens at the two opposite ends playing the stimuli. On the left monitor is depicted the stimulus changing speed (dotted line represents faster speed), on the right monitor is represented the constant moving stimulus. The chick is represented in the starting area at the centre. Two little steps divide it from the two choice areas. For representative purposes one of the two longitudinal walls is represented translucent.

Intramuscular Injections

Iopanoic acid (IOP 10 mM, TCI I0300, Tokyo Chemical Industry Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) was dissolved in 0.05M NaOH solution at 1 mM and rebuffered to pH = 8.5 by 6M HCl. 3,3′,5-Triiodo-L-thyronine (T3, 100 μM, Sigma-Aldrich, T-2877) was dissolved in 0.002M NaOH and 0.9% NaCl. Vehicle solutions were also prepared to control for any effect of the injections. Respectively, the vehicle for IOP was a 0.05M NaOH solution buffered to pH = 8.5 by 6M HCl, while the vehicle for T3 was a 0.9% NaCl and 0.002M NaOH solution (vehicle-injected groups). One hour before testing, each subject was carefully taken from the incubator in complete darkness. A black hood on the head prevented any source of visual stimulation during the intramuscular injection to thigh. Control chicks underwent the same procedure without receiving any injection (not-injected groups). Immediately after, each chick was placed back to the dark incubator. To distinguish single individuals in the darkness while keeping the same auditory environment experienced before injection, we placed the chicks in individual compartments within the same incubator.

Testing

One hour after injection (according to the different groups assigned), each chick was individually tested for the spontaneous preference for animacy for 10 min. After placing each subject in the starting area, the time spent in each sector of the corridor was recorded. In order to measure the animal preference for motion-animacy, we considered the ratio (%) of time spent near the animacy stimulus over the total choice time using the formula:

Preferenceforanimacy=timespentclosetoanimacytimespentclosetobothstimuli×100.

The preference score could range from 0% (full preference for the non-animacy stimulus) to 100% (full preference for animacy), while 50% represented the absence of preference. Permanence in the starting area for the total duration of the test was considered as no choice and led to the exclusion from further analyses (this occurred in about 47% of the cases). Sexing of the subjects occurred at the end of the procedure of testing.

Statistical Analysis

The statistical analyses were performed using RStudio v1.1 and the following packages: goftest (Faraway et al., 2019), nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2020), tidyr (Wickham et al., 2020), plyr (Wickham, 2011), dplyr (Wickham et al., 2020), reshape (Wickham, 2007), lsr (Navarro, 2013), ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016), and lmPerm (Wheeler and Torchiano, 2016).

The number of subjects required in each group was a priori determined with a power analysis (Champely et al., 2020) with an effect size of 0.96, and an alpha of 0.05. Results showed that 18 individuals were required per group and per sex to achieve a power of 0.80.

To detect the presence of outliers, we used a Multivariate Model Approach using Cook’s distance. Subjects having a Cook’s distance four times greater than the group mean were considered as outliers and discarded from further analyses (Kannan and Manoj, 2015). We identified six outliers from different groups. To assess the normality of data distribution, we looked at the distribution of residuals (Q–Q plot).

As parametric assumptions were not met, we used non-parametric tests. In not-injected condition, to determine whether the testing Day (two levels: day 1 and day 3) and Sex (two levels: female and male) affected animacy preference, we performed a permutation test using F-test probabilities. A similar test was conducted for the IOP- and T3-injected conditions, to determine whether the treatments (two levels: IOP/T3 and respective vehicles) and Sex (Two levels: female and male) affected animacy preference.

To determine whether the preference was statistically different between groups within each condition (not-, IOP-, T3-injected), we conducted two-sample Wilcoxon tests. To examine whether each group had a significant preference for either stimulus, we conducted one-sample Wilcoxon tests against chance level (50%). We calculated Cohen’s d (d) for each Wilcoxon test performed.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Trento OPBA and by the Italian Health Ministry.

Author Contributions

EL, BL, TM, GV, and EV: conceptualisation. EL, BL, TM, and GV: methodology. BL: software. EL and BL: formal analysis, investigation, writing–original draft, and visualization. GV and TM: resources. GV, TM, and EV: writing-review and editing. GV: funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013)/ Advanced Grant ERC PREMESOR G.A. (n 295517) to GV.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Authors are particularly thankful to Orsola Rosa-Salva for fruitful discussion and advices.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.675994/full#supplementary-material

References

Andrew, R. J. (1966). Precocious adult behaviour in the young chick. Anim. Behav. 14, 485–500. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(66)80050-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoki, N., Yamaguchi, S., Fujita, T., Mori, C., Fujita, E., Matsushima, T., et al. (2018). GABA-A and GABA-B receptors in filial imprinting linked with opening and closing of the sensitive period in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Front. Physiol. 9:1837. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, P. (1979). How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Anim. Behav. 27:470–486. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90184-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batista, G., and Hensch, T. K. (2019). Critical period regulation by thyroid hormones: potential mechanisms and sex-specific aspects. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 12:77. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2019.00077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batista, G., Johnson, J. L., Dominguez, E., Costa-Mattioli, M., and Pena, J. L. (2016). Translational control of auditory imprinting and structural plasticity by eIF2α. eLife 5:e17197. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batista, G., Johnson, J. L., Dominguez, E., Costa-Mattioli, M., and Pena, J. L. (2018). Regulation of filial imprinting and structural plasticity by mTORC1 in newborn chickens. Sci. Rep. 8:8044. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26479-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Mechanisms of avian imprinting: a review. Biol. Rev. 66, 303–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1991.tb01145.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buiatti, M., Giorgio, E. D., Piazza, M., Polloni, C., Menna, G., Taddei, F., et al. (2019). Cortical route for facelike pattern processing in human newborns. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 4625–4630. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1812419116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cailotto, M., Vallortigara, G., and Zanforlin, M. (1989). Sex differences in the response to social stimuli in young chicks. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1, 323–327. doi: 10.1080/08927014.1989.9525502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Champely, S., Ekstrom, C., Dalgaard, P., Gill, J., Weibezahl, S., Anandkumar, A., et al. (2020). Package ‘pwr’.

Google Scholar

Dehorter, N., and Del Pino, I. (2020). Shifting developmental trajectories during critical periods of brain formation. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 14:283. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2020.00283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Frasnelli, E., Rosa-Salva, O., Maria Luisa, S., Puopolo, M., Tosoni, D., et al. (2016). Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low – and high-risk for Autism. Sci. Rep. 6:26395. doi: 10.1038/srep26395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Loveland, J. L., Mayer, U., Rosa-Salva, O., Versace, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2017). Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: early attachment in chicks and babies. Behav. Brain Res. 325(Pt B), 90–104. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Lunghi, M., Vallortigara, G., and Simion, F. (2021). Newborns’ sensitivity to speed changes as a building block for animacy perception. Sci. Rep. 11:542. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79451-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraway, J., Marsaglia, G., Marsaglia, J., and Baddeley, A. (2019). Goftest: Classical Goodness-of-Fit Tests for Univariate Distributions (Version 1.2-2) [Computer software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=goftest (accessed December 2, 2019).

Google Scholar

Gervain, J., Vines, B. W., Chen, L. M., Seo, R. J., Hensch, T. K., Werker, J. F., et al. (2013). Valproate reopens critical-period learning of absolute pitch. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:102. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodson, J. L., and Kingsbury, M. A. (2013). What’s in a name? Considerations of homologies and nomenclature for vertebrate social behavior networks. Horm. Behav. 64, 103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, C. B., and Williams, G. R. (2002). Mechanism of thyroid hormone action. Thyroid 12, 441–446. doi: 10.1089/105072502760143791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hensch, T. K. (2005). Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hensch, T. K., and Quinlan, E. M. (2018). Critical periods in amblyopia. Vis. Neurosci. 35:E014. doi: 10.1017/S0952523817000219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, E. H. (1959). Imprinting. Science 130, 133–141. Google Scholar

Horn, G. (2004). Pathways of the past: the imprint of memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 108–120. doi: 10.1038/nrn1324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., Davies, D. C., and Horn, G. (1989). A sensitive period for the development of a predisposition in dark-reared chicks. Anim. Behav. 37, 1044–1046. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90148-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kannan, K. S., and Manoj, K. (2015). Outlier detection in multivariate data. Appl. Math. Sci. 9, 2317–2324. doi: 10.12988/ams.2015.53213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaire, B. S., Rugani, R., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2020). Response of male and female domestic chicks to change in the number (quantity) of imprinting objects. Learn. Behav. 49, 54–66. doi: 10.3758/s13420-020-00446-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, K. (1935). Der kumpan in der umwelt des vogels. J. Ornithol. 83, 137–213. doi: 10.1007/BF01905572

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzi, E., Mayer, U., Rosa-Salva, O., and Vallortigara, G. (2017). Dynamic features of animate motion activate septal and preoptic areas in visually naïve chicks (Gallus gallus). Neuroscience 354, 54–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzi, E., Pross, A., Rosa-Salva, O., Versace, E., Sgadò, P., and Vallortigara, G. (2019). Embryonic exposure to valproic acid affects social predispositions for dynamic cues of animate motion in newly-hatched chicks. Front. Physiol. 10:501. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzi, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2021). “Evolutionary and neural bases of the sense of animacy,” in Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition, eds A. B. Kaufman, J. Call, and J. C. Kaufman (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Mayer, U., Rosa-Salva, O., Lorenzi, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2016). Social predisposition dependent neuronal activity in the intermediate medial mesopallium of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Behav. Brain Res. 310, 93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, U., Rosa-Salva, O., Morbioli, F., and Vallortigara, G. (2017a). The motion of a living conspecific activates septal and preoptic areas in naive domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 423–432. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, U., Rosa-Salva, O., and Vallortigara, G. (2017b). First exposure to an alive conspecific activates septal and amygdaloid nuclei in visually-naïve domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Behav. Brain Res. 317, 71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride, G., and Foenander, F. (1962). Territorial behaviour in flocks of domestic fowls. Nature 194, 102–102. doi: 10.1038/194102a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride, G., Parer, I. P., and Foenander, F. (1969). The social organization and behaviour of the feral domestic fowl. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 2, 125–181. doi: 10.1016/S0066-1856(69)80003-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, B. J. (2013). Imprinting. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 375–390. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miura, M., and Matsushima, T. (2012). Preference for biological motion in domestic chicks: sex-dependent effect of early visual experience. Anim. Cogn. 15, 871–879. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0514-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miura, M., and Matsushima, T. (2016). Biological motion facilitates filial imprinting. Anim. Behav. 116, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miura, M., Nishi, D., and Matsushima, T. (2020). Combined predisposed preferences for colour and biological motion make robust development of social attachment through imprinting. Anim. Cogn. 23, 169–188. doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01327-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, D. (2013). Learning Statistics with R: A Tutorial for Psychology Students and Other Beginners: Version 0.5. Adelaide, SA: University of Adelaide.

Google Scholar

Newman, S. W. (1999). The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior a node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877, 242–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, L. A., and Hofmann, H. A. (2011). The Vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3599–3639. doi: 10.1002/cne.22735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., Roy, S. D., Sarkar, D., and Heisterkamp, S. (2020). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models (Version 3.1-148) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (accessed February 4, 2021).

Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J. (1995). The Development of Brain and Behaviour in the Chicken. Wallingford: CAB International.

Google Scholar

Rosa-Salva, O., Fiser, J., Versace, E., Dolci, C., Chehaimi, S., Santolin, C., et al. (2018). Spontaneous learning of visual structures in domestic chicks. Animals 8:135. doi: 10.3390/ani8080135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa-Salva, O., Grassi, M., Lorenzi, E., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2016). Spontaneous preference for visual cues of animacy in naïve domestic chicks: the case of speed changes. Cognition 157, 49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa-Salva, O., Mayer, U., Versace, E., Hébert, M., Lemaire, B. S., and Vallortigara, G. (2021). Sensitive periods for social development: interactions between predisposed and learned mechanisms. Cognition 104552. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa-Salva, O., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2010). Faces are special for newly hatched chicks: evidence for inborn domain-specific mechanisms underlying spontaneous preferences for face-like stimuli. Dev. Sci. 13, 565–577. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00914.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santolin, C., Rosa-Salva, O., Lemaire, B. S., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2020). Statistical learning in domestic chicks is modulated by strain and sex. Sci. Rep. 10:15140. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72090-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, G. B., and Emlen, J. T. (1962). The ontogeny of avoidance behaviour in some precocial birds. Anim. Behav. 10, 370–381. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90060-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sgadò, P., Rosa-Salva, O., Versace, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2018). Embryonic exposure to valproic acid impairs social predispositions of newly-hatched chicks. Sci. Rep. 8:5919. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24202-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spalding, D. (1873). Instinct, with original observations on young animals. Macmillan’s Mag. 27, 282–293.

Google Scholar

Vallortigara, G. (1992). Affiliation and aggression as related to gender in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). J. Comp. Psychol. 106, 53–57. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.106.1.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallortigara, G. (2021). Born Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Vallortigara, G., Cailotto, M., and Zanforlin, M. (1990). Sex differences in social reinstatement motivation of the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) revealed by runway tests with social and nonsocial reinforcement. J. Comp. Psychol. 104, 361–367. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.104.4.361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallortigara, G., Regolin, L., and Marconato, F. (2005). Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns. PLoS Biol. 3:e208. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallortigara, G., and Versace, E. (2018). “Filial imprinting,” in Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, eds J. Vonk and T. Shackelford (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Van Herck, S. L. J., Geysens, S., Delbaere, J., and Darras, V. M. (2013). Regulators of thyroid hormone availability and action in embryonic chicken brain development. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 190, 96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2013.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, E., Martinho-Truswell, A., Kacelnik, A., and Vallortigara, G. (2018). Priors in animal and artificial intelligence: where does learning begin? Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 963–965. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, E., Ragusa, M., and Vallortigara, G. (2019). A transient time window for early predispositions in newborn chicks. Sci. Rep. 9:18767. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-55255-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, E., Spierings, M. J., Caffini, M., Ten Cate, C., and Vallortigara, G. (2017). Spontaneous generalization of abstract multimodal patterns in young domestic chicks. Anim. Cogn. 20, 521–529. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1079-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler, B., and Torchiano, M. (2016). lmPerm: Permutation Tests for Linear Models (Version 2.1.0) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmPerm (accessed August 02, 2016).

Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2007). Reshaping data with the reshape package. J. Stat. Softw. 21, 1–20. doi: 10.18637/jss.v021.i12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2011). The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 40, 1–29. doi: 10.18637/jss.v040.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Cham: Springer.

Google Scholar

Wickham, H., Henry, L., and RStudio. (2020). tidyr: Tidy Messy Data (Version 1.1.1) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=tidyr (accessed March 03, 2021).

Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, S., Aoki, N., Kitajima, T., Iikubo, E., Katagiri, S., Matsushima, T., et al. (2012). Thyroid hormone determines the start of the sensitive period of imprinting and primes later learning. Nat. Commun. 3:1081. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, S., Aoki, N., Matsushima, T., and Homma, K. J. (2018). Wnt-2b in the intermediate hyperpallium apicale of the telencephalon is critical for the thyroid hormone-mediated opening of the sensitive period for filial imprinting in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Horm. Behav. 102, 120–128. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, S., Aoki, N., Takehara, A., Mori, M., Kanai, A., Matsushima, T., et al. (2016). Involvement of nucleotide diphosphate kinase 2 in the reopening of the sensitive period of filial imprinting of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Neurosci. Lett. 612, 32–37.

Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, S., Hayase, S., Aoki, N., Takehara, A., Ishigohoka, J., Matsushima, T., et al. (2017). Sex differences in brain thyroid hormone levels during early post-hatching development in Zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). PLoS One 12:e0169643. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: animacy, thyroid hormone, avian, sensitive period, plasticity, T3

Citation: Lorenzi E, Lemaire BS, Versace E, Matsushima T and Vallortigara G (2021) Resurgence of an Inborn Attraction for Animate Objects via Thyroid Hormone T3. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:675994. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.675994

Received: 04 March 2021; Accepted: 24 March 2021;
Published: 19 April 2021.

Edited by:

Gervasio Batista, Harvard University, United States

Reviewed by:

Brian McCabe, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom
Koichi J. Homma, Teikyo University, Japan

Copyright © 2021 Lorenzi, Lemaire, Versace, Matsushima and Vallortigara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elena Lorenzi, elena.lorenzi@unitn.it; Giorgio Vallortigara, giorgio.vallortigara@unitn.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.