Skip to main content

CORRECTION article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 30 April 2021
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors

Corrigendum: Wired for motherhood: induction of maternal care but not maternal aggression in virgin female CD1 mice

  • 1Laboratory of Functional Neuroanatomy (NeuroFun), Unitat Predepartamental de Medicina, Facultat de Ciències de la Salut, Universitat Jaume I, Castelló de la Plana, Spain
  • 2Departaments de Biologia Cel·lular i de Biologia Funcional, Facultat de Ciències Biològiques, Universitat de València, València, Spain

A Corrigendum on
Wired for motherhood: induction of maternal care but not maternal aggression in virgin female CD1 mice

by Martín-Sánchez, A., Valera-Marín, G., Hernández-Martínez, A., Lanuza, E., Martínez-García, F., and Agustín-Pavón, C. (2015). Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:197. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00197

In the original article, there was an error. We were informed by Dr. Bruce S. Svare that our interpretation of their results published in Svare and Gandelman (1976) was incorrect, and hence we hereby request the following correction.

Corrections have been made to Discussion, Maternal Aggression is Only Present in Lactating Females, Paragraphs 2 and 4. The corrected paragraphs are shown below.

This finding contrasts, however, with the results by McDermott and Gandelman (1979) that showed that some virgin female mice displayed aggression after 9 days of continuous exposure to 1 day-old pups that were renewed every day. Therefore, there is conflicting evidence relative to the factors promoting the onset of maternal aggression. Early studies suggested that sensory cues from the pups were the key factor triggering maternal aggression in mice. In particular suckling-induced nipple stimulation—but not lactation—would trigger and maintain maternal aggression in mouse dams (Svare et al., 1980). This was supported by two main lines of evidence. On the one hand, parturient females thelectomyzed prepartum or immediately postpartum became not aggressive (Svare and Gandelman, 1976a). On the other, Svare and Gandelman (1976b) ovariectomized virgin females and treated them with oestradiol benzoate and progesterone for 19 days to induce nipple growth, and fostered them with pups that were observed to attach themselves to the nipples. Treated virgin females exhibited aggression, but not milk production. Importantly, hormone-treated virgin females did not display maternal aggression before suckling stimulation from newborn pups, suggesting that hormonal treatment per se is not able to provoke aggressive behavior (Svare and Gandelman, 1976b).

(…)

However, a key role of endocrine agents in maternal aggression is further supported by the expression of maternal aggression before parturition, in late-pregnant mice (Mann et al., 1984, who called it pregnancy-induced aggression) and rats (Caughey et al., 2011), in which no nipple stimulation by pups has occurred yet. Maybe it is the hormonal stimuli leading to nipple growth, together with the presence of a nest to defend, rather than nipple stimulation per se, what causes maternal attacks. This would fit both our results and those from other studies. McDermott and Gandelman (1979) reported that those virgin females exposed to 1-day-old pups for 9 days that displayed maternal aggression showed enlarged and more numerous nipples than their counterparts that, in the same conditions, were not aggressive. This indicates that a continuous exposure to pups for 9 days had caused hormonal changes leading to both nipple growth and aggressiveness, by means of unknown mechanisms. Apparently these hormonal changes did not occur in our godmothers even if they had spent 5 days caring for pups, likely because of the presence in the same cage of the dam nursing the pups for most of the time.

The authors apologize for this error and state that this does not change the scientific conclusions of the article in any way. The original article has been updated.

References

Caughey, S. D., Klampfl, S. M., Bishop, V. R., Pfoertsch, J., Neumann, I. D., Bosch, O. J., et al. (2011). Changes in the intensity of maternal aggression and central oxytocin and vasopressin V1a receptors across the peripartum period in the rat. J. Neuroendocrinol. 23, 1113–1124. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02224.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, M. A., Konen, C., and Svare, B. (1984). The role of progesterone in pregnancy-induced aggression in mice. Horm. Behav. 18, 140–160. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(84)90039-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, N. J., and Gandelman, R. (1979). Exposure to young induces postpartum-like fighting in virgin female mice. Physiol. Behav. 23, 445–448. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(79)90041-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svare, B., and Gandelman, R. (1976a). A longitudinal analysis of maternal aggression in Rockland-Swiss albino mice. Dev. Psychobiol. 9, 437–446. doi: 10.1002/dev.420090506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svare, B., and Gandelman, R. (1976b). Suckling stimulation induces aggression in virgin female mice. Nature 260, 606–608. doi: 10.1038/260606a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svare, B., Mann, M., and Samuels, O. (1980). Mice: suckling stimulation but not lactation important for maternal aggression. Behav. Neural Biol. 29, 453–462. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(80)92654-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aggression, communal nesting, maternal care, maternal sensitization, outbred strain

Citation: Martín-Sánchez A, Valera-Marín G, Hernández-Martínez A, Lanuza E, Martínez-García F and Agustín-Pavón C (2021) Corrigendum: Wired for motherhood: induction of maternal care but not maternal aggression in virgin female CD1 mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:671259. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.671259

Received: 23 February 2021; Accepted: 25 March 2021;
Published: 30 April 2021.

Edited and reviewed by: Xiao-Dong Wang, Zhejiang University, China

Copyright © 2021 Martín-Sánchez, Valera-Marín, Hernández-Martínez, Lanuza, Martínez-García and Agustín-Pavón. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carmen Agustín-Pavón, deagusti@uji.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.