- 1Department of Food Management, School of Food, Agricultural and Environmental Sciences, Miyagi University, Sendai, Japan
- 2Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
- 3Brain, Mind and Markets Laboratory, Department of Finance, Faculty of Business and Economics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
Subjective values for food rewards guide our dietary choices. There is growing evidence that value signals are constructed in the brain by integrating multiple types of information about flavor, taste, and nutritional attributes of the foods. However, much less is known about the influence of food-extrinsic factors such as labels, brands, prices, and packaging designs. In this mini-review article, we outline recent findings in decision neuroscience, consumer psychology, and food science about the effect of extrinsic factors on food value computations in the human brain. To date, studies have demonstrated that, while the integrated value signal is encoded in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, information on the extrinsic factors of the food is encoded in diverse brain regions previously implicated in a wide range of functions: cognitive control, memory, emotion and reward processing. We suggest that a comprehensive understanding of food valuation requires elucidation of the mechanisms behind integrating extrinsic factors in the brain to compute an overall subjective value signal.
Introduction
The valuation of food is central in our daily decision-making about what to eat. Dysfunctional food valuation is often associated with the development of obesity and eating-disorders (Yokum et al., 2011; Carnell et al., 2012; Foerde et al., 2015). Human neuroimaging studies have begun to uncover the neural basis of food valuation (Rangel, 2013; Giuliani et al., 2018) by combining functional magnetic resonance neuroimaging (fMRI) with careful assessment of subjective values for food items. In a typical experimental design, participants inside the MRI scanner are shown images of food and are asked to report their subjective values for each of those food items (see Figure 1A for details). Accumulating evidence suggests that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) encodes subjective value signals for various types of potential outcomes including food rewards (Figure 1B; Chib et al., 2009; Lebreton et al., 2009; Bartra et al., 2013; Chikazoe et al., 2014; Clithero and Rangel, 2014; Gross et al., 2014).
Figure 1. Value signals in the brain. (A) Example timeline of the experimental task (called Becker-DeGroot-Marschak auction: Becker et al., 1964). In this task, participants report their “willingness to pay” (i.e., subjective value) for food items. On each trial, they make a bid for one item in the context of auction. Importantly, the auction mechanism is carefully designed so that the optimal strategy for the participants is to always bid the number closest to their true subjective value for obtaining that item. The food image is adopted from Food-pics (Blechert et al., 2014). (B) Neural correlates of value signals. Subjective value signals are correlated with neural activity (blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal) in the vmPFC. vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
How is it the value signal for a food reward is constructed in the human brain? Previous studies suggest that individuals compute the value of a food item by integrating information about multiple attributes from biologically relevant intrinsic factors (e.g., macronutrients, tastes, and flavors) to higher-order extrinsic factors (e.g., labels, brands, prices, and packaging designs; e.g., Steptoe et al., 1995; Satterthwaite and Fellows, 2018).
Researchers have examined the effects of nutrient factors with experimental designs using images of food as stimuli (Figure 1A; Tang et al., 2014; Suzuki et al., 2017; DiFeliceantonio et al., 2018). For example, food valuation is driven by the caloric content tracked in the vmPFC (Tang et al., 2014). Moreover, one study found that the subjective value of a food reward can be predicted by a linear combination of the constituent nutritive attributes (Suzuki et al., 2017). Multivariate decoding analyses on the neuroimaging data supported the possibility that information on the nutritive attributes of food is represented in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) and then integrated into the vmPFC to compute an overall subject value (Suzuki et al., 2017). Notably, an additional analysis in this study carefully ruled out the possibility that the lOFC contains information about low-level visual features of the food images (e.g., luminance and contrast). A subsequent study demonstrated supra-additive effects of fats and carbohydrates to food valuations beyond the linear combination (DiFeliceantonio et al., 2018), providing a potential account for overconsumption of high-fat/-carbohydrate food products (e.g., French fries).
Construction of the value signal through actual consumption/tasting (i.e., oral sensing) of the food has also been of considerable concern in human neuroscience. For instance, oral sensory representation of fat and sucrose have been found in the vmPFC, including the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (de Araujo and Rolls, 2004). Grabenhorst and colleagues further demonstrated that the vmPFC and lOFC track the pleasantness of oral fat texture via functional connectivity with the oral somatosensory cortex (Grabenhorst et al., 2010; Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2014).
Another line of study has attempted to characterize the neural encoding of quality, intensity, and preference for certain tastes and flavors (Small et al., 2007). One study demonstrated that pleasantness, or quality, of taste, is represented in the lOFC, while intensity is represented in the insular cortex and amygdala (Small et al., 2003). By combining careful experimental designs with multivariate analyses on neuroimaging data, recent studies have elaborated on how taste and flavor information is processed in the brain (e.g., Howard et al., 2015; Chikazoe et al., 2019; Avery et al., 2020). Chikazoe and colleagues showed taste qualities (sweet, salty, bitter, and sour) are represented in the insular cortex, which supports the notion that the insular is the primary gustatory cortex in humans (Small, 2010). Howard and colleagues revealed, by the careful manipulation of identities (qualities) of odor stimuli, that the lOFC encodes identity-specific values, while the vmPFC encodes subjective values independent of the identity. Although the extent to which taste and flavor contribute to the computation of subjective values beyond the nutritive and caloric contents remains elusive (de Araujo et al., 2020), these findings together suggest that the lOFC plays a pivotal role in representing multiple types of information about intrinsic factors of food (e.g., macronutrients, tastes, and flavors) which are then integrated in the vmPFC to compute an overall subjective value.
Despite the advancement in our understanding of the influences of intrinsic factors, much less is known about the effects of higher-order extrinsic factors on food valuation in the brain. Increasing evidence in consumer psychology and food science suggests that food valuation can be influenced by various factors outside of the food itself, such as labels, brands, prices, social information, and packaging designs (e.g., Okamoto and Dan, 2013; Higgs, 2015; Piqueras-Fiszman and Spence, 2015; Motoki et al., 2018, 2020). In this mini-review article, we discuss our current knowledge about how extrinsic factors affect food valuation in the brain while maintaining a focus on human neuroimaging (fMRI) studies. Although previous articles have discussed related issues (Plassmann et al., 2012; Okamoto and Dan, 2013; Stasi et al., 2018), the current review provides a new perspective by emphasizing how information about food-extrinsic factors gets integrated into the brain to compute an overall subjective value.
Extrinsic Factors of Food Valuation
Labels
In our everyday dietary choices, we often acquire a significant amount of information from the label attached to the food product (e.g., Piqueras-Fiszman and Spence, 2015; Motoki et al., 2020). A seminal study by de Araujo et al. (2005) examined how cognitive and semantic information modulates our food valuation. They exposed participants to an odor (isovaleric acid with cheddar cheese flavor) with different visual word labels, either “cheddar cheese” or “body odor.” The participants were found to rate the subjective value of the odor as more unpleasant when labeled “body odor” than when labeled “cheddar cheese.” Furthermore, the modulation of the unpleasantness rating was reflected in the neural activity in the vmPFC, suggesting that cognitive and semantic information can modulate food value signals in the vmPFC.
Enax et al. (2015a) compared two different ways of presenting the nutritional information in a food label. In the control condition, nutritional information was provided by a purely information-based textual label. Whereas, in the main condition, the same information was provided by a label with color codes: green and red signaling healthy and unhealthy foods, respectively. The behavioral data showed that the labeling method significantly affected participants’ willingness to pay (WTP) for food items. Nutritional labels with color codes increased WTP for healthy foods compared with the purely information-based labels. The neuroimaging data revealed that consistent with the previous findings (Plassmann et al., 2007), WTP for each food item was significantly correlated with neural activity in the vmPFC, regardless of the type of nutritional label. Furthermore, the red signals, indicating unhealthiness, were found to increase neural activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), which has been implicated in self-control (Hare et al., 2009), and modulate its functional connectivity with the vmPFC. These results are consistent with the notion that top-down self-control signals in the dlPFC modulate the food value signals represented in the vmPFC to enable an overall decision.
Top-down modulation of the brain valuation region is potentially more prominent in obese female participants, compared to the healthy controls (Ng et al., 2011). Ng et al. (2011) scanned obese and normal females with fMRI while the participants were anticipating receiving a milkshake. Critically, in the experiment, identical milkshakes were delivered with a label indicating it was either “regular” or “low-fat.” Relative to female participants with normal weight, obese participants showed greater activation in the vmPFC in response to receiving the regular labeled milkshake (vs. low-fat labeled one), which could contribute to excessive consumption of high-fat foods. A possible way to account for the result would be an excess top-down modulation of the food valuation in obese females, though the study did not provide neural evidence for the modulatory process (e.g., connectivity analyses).
Grabenhorst et al. (2008) examined the effects of labels including taste-related (“rich and delicious taste”) descriptions on food valuation by using umami-taste stimuli. They found that pleasantness ratings from tasting the stimuli were increased by the taste-related labels, while intensity ratings were not. Consistent with the behavioral results, the neural representation of the pleasantness in the vmPFC was enhanced by the taste-related labels, while the representation of the taste intensity in the insular cortex was intact. The results suggest that food value signals represented in the vmPFC are modulated by top-down information about taste. The same research group also investigated how the inclusion of health-related properties (e.g., “high in calories”), as well as taste-related properties (e.g., “sweet and juicy”) on labels, influenced food valuation and choice (Grabenhorst et al., 2013). The taste-related labels were found to enhance the neural representation of taste pleasantness in the amygdala, a core region in the emotional brain system (Pessoa, 2017). Similarly, the health-related labels enhanced activity in the amygdala, and the amygdala activity predicted the participant’s behavioral shift towards healthier choices, highlighting a potential role of emotion in food. An explanation of these findings (Grabenhorst et al., 2008, 2013) could be that top-down information about taste and healthiness in the amygdala modulates food value signals in the vmPFC. An interesting avenue for future research would be to elucidate the modulation process by employing connectivity analyses and brain stimulation techniques (see “Discussion” section for details).
In today’s society, many people are conscious about whether their food is produced ethically and sustainably. One study showed a positive effect of an “organic” label on food valuation (Linder et al., 2010). The authors found that participants’ WTP was significantly higher for food items possessing an organic label rather than those without. Furthermore, the presentation of organically labeled foods increased the neural activity in the ventral striatum, and the increased striatal activity accounted for the individual differences in concern for natural food gradients and daily organic food buying behavior. Another study examined the effect of “fair-trade” labels highlighting the ethical sustainability of the food product (Enax et al., 2015b). In the neuroimaging experiment, participants were asked to report their WTP for food items presented with or without a fair-trade label. The behavioral and neuroimaging data revealed that the presence of the label increased WTP, while also increasing neural activity in brain regions such as ventral striatum, anterior cingulate cortex, and superior frontal gyrus. The presentation of the label was also found to modulate functional connectivity between these regions and the vmPFC, which signaled WTP. These results suggest that the fair-trade label influenced the valuation of food in the vmPFC through the functional connectivity with the regions that track the label information.
Price
Our daily purchasing behavior is guided not only by a preference for an item but also by the price (e.g., Jaeger, 2006). Underpriced goods are generally preferred, while overpriced goods are avoided. Hare et al. (2008) demonstrated in their neuroimaging experiment that both the WTP and the price of food determined participants’ purchasing decisions. Furthermore, the decision value of food (defined as the WTP minus the price) was found to be encoded in the lOFC, while WTP was encoded in the vmPFC. Another study (Knutson et al., 2007) suggests that, in purchasing various goods including food, the brain computes the decision value in the vmPFC, while the ventral striatum tracks WTP and the insular cortex tracks price.
In some cases, a high-priced item may be overvalued based on the belief that expensiveness implies enhanced quality. That is, the price information can reinforce the preference (i.e., WTP) for the item. For example, an expensive wine sometimes sells better than a comparable low-priced alternative. Plassmann et al. (2008) addressed this issue with a focus on the experienced pleasantness from consuming a glass a few drops of wine. In their neuroimaging experiment, participants were asked to sample different wines and report their experienced pleasantness. The critical manipulation was that, unknown to the participants, the identical wines were administered with different instructions about their retail price (i.e., a high price in half of the cases and a low price in the other cases). They found that price information about the wine was capable of manipulating participants’ experienced pleasantness. That is, the expectation of higher-priced wine increased subjective reports of flavor pleasantness. Moreover, the change in the pleasantness reports was reflected in the neural activity of the vmPFC. A follow-up study formally tested the tripartite relationship among price, pleasantness, and neural activity (Schmidt et al., 2017). The results of a multilevel mediation analysis revealed that the effect of price information on experienced pleasantness of wine tasting was mediated by neural activity in the brain valuation system including the vmPFC, ventral striatum, and anterior prefrontal cortex.
Brand
Brand images can also influence our food choices. One study tested the effect of brand images by examining behavioral and neural responses to soft drink taste tests (McClure et al., 2004). In the experiment, the authors recruited participants who expressed either a preference for Coke or Pepsi. Within the experiment, participants were asked to choose between the two types of soda based on blind-tasting. The data analysis showed that participants’ choice pattern was not significantly correlated with their self-reported preference, implying that our daily food choice is predominated by brand images rather than experienced tastes. Furthermore, the preference that was estimated from the choice data, but not the self-report, was found to be represented in the vmPFC. Moreover, they showed that disclosure of the brand information, Coke but not Pepsi, increased participants’ preference for Coke and neural activity in dlPFC and hippocampus. A follow-up study (Koenigs and Tranel, 2008) examined the effect of the Coke brand cue in patients with lesions in the vmPFC. The authors demonstrated that the Coke label did not alter the patients’ preference, suggesting a causal role of vmPFC in processing brand information for food valuation.
Another study (Kühn and Gallinat, 2013) prepared an artificial beverage that consisted of equal parts of Coca Cola, Pepsi Cola, and River Cola. The artificial beverage was delivered to participants with different brand name cues: Coca Cola, Pepsi Cola, River Cola, and T Cola. The participants exhibited a preference for drinks associated with the famous brands (i.e., Coca and Pepsi Cola) over the others (i.e., River and T Cola) despite the chemical composition being identical. Furthermore, neural activity in the vmPFC was more responsive to the pleasantness rating from consuming the beverage when the cue signaled famous brands compared to the others. These two studies (McClure et al., 2004; Kühn and Gallinat, 2013) together suggest that brand images, possibly encoded in the dlPFC and the hippocampus, modulate value signals in the vmPFC.
Social Information
Social information, such as the opinions of others, influences our judgment, preferences, and decision-making including food choice (e.g., Klucharev et al., 2009; Izuma, 2013; Higgs, 2015; Suzuki et al., 2016). Nook and Zaki (2015) investigated the social influence on food valuation. They conducted a neuroimaging experiment in which participants: (i) first rated how much they wanted to eat a series of foods; (ii) observed peer ratings for the foods; and (iii) again rated each of the food. As expected, the behavioral data showed a social conformity effect: that is, participants’ ratings about the foods were conformed to the peers’ ratings. At the neural level, an agreement between the participants’ and the peers’ ratings, as compared to disagreement, provoked neural activity in the ventral striatum, and the strength of the striatal activity predicted the individual differences in the degree of social conformity. Furthermore, the anterior prefrontal cortex was found to track information about the healthiness of the foods in the initial rating, but tracked popularity (i.e., information about peer ratings) in the second rating.
Packaging Design
Packaging design can also affect our flavor expectation and preference for food (e.g., Basso et al., 2014; Spence et al., 2019; Tijssen et al., 2019). One study (Van der Laan et al., 2012) asked participants to choose between two options of the same snack contained within different packaging designs. The neuroimaging data showed that several brain regions, including the striatum, encoded outcomes of the package-based choices. Reimann et al. (2010) investigated how aesthetic packages and well-known brands influence our purchasing behavior. Comparing the two packages (i.e., aesthetic and standardized) and the two brands (i.e., well-known and unknown) conditions, they found that food items in aesthetics packages were more likely to be chosen despite higher prices when compared to well-known brands in standardized packages. The preference for aesthetic packages was reflected in the neural activity of the vmPFC, striatum, and middle to posterior cingulate cortex.
Discussion
Consumer psychology and food science have a long history of demonstrating that our preference for a food reward is modulated by higher-order extrinsic factors (e.g., labels, brand images, prices, social information, and packaging designs; e.g., Okamoto and Dan, 2013; Higgs, 2015; Piqueras-Fiszman and Spence, 2015). In this mini-review article, we have discussed recent advancements in decision neuroscience in our understanding of how extrinsic factors affect food value computation in the brain.
Accumulating evidence from human neuroimaging studies has consistently demonstrated that, while vmPFC encodes overall value signals by integrating information about the extrinsic factors of foods (e.g., Enax et al., 2015a; Schmidt et al., 2017), the various extrinsic factors are encoded in diverse brain regions such as the dlPFC (McClure et al., 2004; Enax et al., 2015a), the amygdala (Grabenhorst et al., 2013), the ventral striatum (Linder et al., 2010; Van der Laan et al., 2012; Nook and Zaki, 2015; Schmidt et al., 2017), and the hippocampus (McClure et al., 2004; see Figure 2). These regions cover a wide range of brain functions: cognitive control (dlPFC), emotion (amygdala), reward processing (ventral striatum), and memory (hippocampus). Interestingly, the involvement of these diverse brain regions contrasts the food value computation based on intrinsic factors, in which information about macronutrients, tastes, and flavors appear to be integrated with the lOFC and sent to the vmPFC where the overall subjective value is computed (e.g., Suzuki et al., 2017).
Figure 2. Brain regions encoding extrinsic factors of food. (A) Hippocampus. (B) Amygdala. (C) The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (top) and ventral striatum (bottom).
The findings discussed in this review are broadly consistent with the notion that extrinsic factors of food reward modulate the value signal in the vmPFC through functional connectivity with multiple brain regions that track information about each extrinsic factor. However, more evidence is needed to deepen the understanding of how the multiple types of information become integrated within the brain to compute an overall food value (see Suzuki et al., 2015; Suzuki and O’Doherty, 2020 for similar issues in social decision-making). For example, to elucidate the integration process, it would be helpful to examine the nature of functional and anatomical connectivity among the brain regions engaged in the food valuation (e.g., vmPFC and dlPFC). Although several studies to date have aimed to address the issue by employing psychophysiological interaction analysis (Friston et al., 1997), the regression-based connectivity analysis cannot test for the directionality of the connections. Future studies could provide a more comprehensive view by combining various approaches that allow examination of the causal and anatomical interactions among brain regions, such as dynamic causal modeling of the neuroimaging data (e.g., Stephan et al., 2010; Hare et al., 2011), transcranial magnetic stimulation of the neural activity (e.g., Hill et al., 2017; Polanía et al., 2018), and diffusion tensor imaging (e.g., Assaf and Pasternak, 2008).
Author Contributions
KM and SS wrote the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Number 19K23384 (KM) and the Mishima Kaiun Memorial Foundation (SS).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Assaf, Y., and Pasternak, O. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: a review. J. Mol. Neurosci. 34, 51–61. doi: 10.1007/s12031-007-0029-0
Avery, J. A., Liu, A. G., Ingeholm, J. E., Riddell, C. D., Gotts, S. J., and Martin, A. (2020). Taste quality representation in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 40, 1042–1052. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1751-19.2019
Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T., and Kable, J. W. (2013). The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of bold fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage 76, 412–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063
Basso, F., Robert-Demontrond, P., Hayek, M., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Roth, M., et al. (2014). Why people drink shampoo? Food imitating products are fooling brains and endangering consumers for marketing purposes. PLoS One 9:e100368. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100368
Becker, G. M., DeGroot, M. H., and Marschak, J. (1964). Measuring utility by a single-response sequential method. Behav. Sci. 9, 226–232. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830090304
Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., and Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front. Psychol. 5:617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00617
Carnell, S., Gibson, C., Benson, L., Ochner, C. N., and Geliebter, A. (2012). Neuroimaging and obesity: current knowledge and future directions. Obes. Rev. 13, 43–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789x.2011.00927.x
Chib, V. S., Rangel, A., Shimojo, S., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2009). Evidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 29, 12315–12320. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2575-09.2009
Chikazoe, J., Lee, D. H., Kriegeskorte, N., and Anderson, A. K. (2014). Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1114–1122. doi: 10.1038/nn.3749
Chikazoe, J., Lee, D. H., Kriegeskorte, N., and Anderson, A. K. (2019). Distinct representations of basic taste qualities in human gustatory cortex. Nat. Commun. 10:1048. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-08857-z
Clithero, J. A., and Rangel, A. (2014). Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1289–1302. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst106
de Araujo, I. E., and Rolls, E. T. (2004). Representation in the human brain of food texture and oral fat. J. Neurosci. 24, 3086–3093. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0130-04.2004
de Araujo, I. E., Rolls, E. T., Velazco, M. I., Margot, C., and Cayeux, I. (2005). Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron 46, 671–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.021
de Araujo, I. E., Schatzker, M., and Small, D. M. (2020). Rethinking food reward. Annu. Rev. Psychol 71, 139–164. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011643
DiFeliceantonio, A. G., Coppin, G., Rigoux, L., Thanarajah, S. E., Dagher, A., Tittgemeyer, M., et al. (2018). Supra-additive effects of combining fat and carbohydrate on food reward. Cell Metab. 28, 33.e3–44.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.05.018
Enax, L., Hu, Y., Trautner, P., and Weber, B. (2015a). Nutrition labels influence value computation of food products in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Obesity 23, 786–792. doi: 10.1002/oby.21027
Enax, L., Krapp, V., Piehl, A., and Weber, B. (2015b). Effects of social sustainability signaling on neural valuation signals and taste-experience of food products. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:247. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00247
Foerde, K., Steinglass, J. E., Shohamy, D., and Walsh, B. T. (2015). Neural mechanisms supporting maladaptive food choices in anorexia nervosa. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1571–1573. doi: 10.1038/nn.4136
Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., and Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage 6, 218–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291
Giuliani, N. R., Merchant, J. S., Cosme, D., and Berkman, E. T. (2018). Neural predictors of eating behavior and dietary change. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1428, 208–220. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13637
Grabenhorst, F., and Rolls, E. T. (2014). The representation of oral fat texture in the human somatosensory cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2521–2530. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22346
Grabenhorst, F., Rolls, E. T., and Bilderbeck, A. (2008). How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: top-down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1549–1559. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm185
Grabenhorst, F., Rolls, E. T., Parris, B. A., and d’Souza, A. A. (2010). How the brain represents the reward value of fat in the mouth. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1082–1091. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp169
Grabenhorst, F., Schulte, F. P., Maderwald, S., and Brand, M. (2013). Food labels promote healthy choices by a decision bias in the amygdala. NeuroImage 74, 152–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.012
Gross, J., Woelbert, E., Zimmermann, J., Okamoto-Barth, S., Riedl, A., and Goebel, R. (2014). Value signals in the prefrontal cortex predict individual preferences across reward categories. J. Neurosci. 34, 7580–7586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5082-13.2014
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., and Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646–648. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450
Hare, T. A., O’Doherty, J., Camerer, C. F., Schultz, W., and Rangel, A. (2008). Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 28, 5623–5630. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1309-08.2008
Hare, T. A., Schultz, W., Camerer, C. F., O’Doherty, J. P., and Rangel, A. (2011). Transformation of stimulus value signals into motor commands during simple choice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 18120–18125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1109322108
Higgs, S. (2015). Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite 86, 38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.021
Hill, C. A., Suzuki, S., Polania, R., Moisa, M., O’Doherty, J. P., and Ruff, C. C. (2017). A causal account of the brain network computations underlying strategic social behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1142–1149. doi: 10.1038/nn.4602
Howard, J. D., Gottfried, J. A., Tobler, P. N., and Kahnt, T. (2015). Identity-specific coding of future rewards in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, 5195–5200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1503550112
Izuma, K. (2013). The neural basis of social influence and attitude change. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 456–462. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.03.009
Jaeger, S. R. (2006). Non-sensory factors in sensory science research. Food Qual. Prefer. 17, 132–144. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.03.004
Klucharev, V., Hytönen, K., Rijpkema, M., Smidts, A., and Fernández, G. (2009). Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron 61, 140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.027
Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D., and Loewenstein, G. (2007). Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron 53, 147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.010
Koenigs, M., and Tranel, D. (2008). Prefrontal cortex damage abolishes brand-cued changes in cola preference. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 1–6. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm032
Kühn, S., and Gallinat, J. (2013). Does taste matter? How anticipation of cola brands influences gustatory processing in the brain. PLoS One 8:e61569. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061569
Lebreton, M., Jorge, S., Michel, V., Thirion, B., and Pessiglione, M. (2009). An automatic valuation system in the human brain: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuron 64, 431–439. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.040
Linder, N. S., Uhl, G., Fliessbach, K., Trautner, P., Elger, C. E., and Weber, B. (2010). Organic labeling influences food valuation and choice. NeuroImage 53, 215–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.077
McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., and Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron 44, 379–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019
Motoki, K., Saito, T., Nouchi, R., Kawashima, R., and Sugiura, M. (2018). The paradox of warmth: ambient warm temperature decreases preference for savory foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 69, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.04.006
Motoki, K., Saito, T., Park, J., Velasco, C., Spence, C., and Sugiura, M. (2020). Tasting names: systematic investigations of taste-speech sounds associations. Food Qual. Prefer. 80:103801. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103801
Ng, J., Stice, E., Yokum, S., and Bohon, C. (2011). An fMRI study of obesity, food reward, and perceived caloric density. Does a low-fat label make food less appealing? Appetite 57, 65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.03.017
Nook, E. C., and Zaki, J. (2015). Social norms shift behavioral and neural responses to foods. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1412–1426. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00795
Okamoto, M., and Dan, I. (2013). Extrinsic information influences taste and flavor perception: a review from psychological and neuroimaging perspectives. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 24, 247–255. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2012.11.001
Pessoa, L. (2017). A network model of the emotional brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 357–371. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.002
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., and Spence, C. (2015). Sensory expectations based on product-extrinsic food cues: an interdisciplinary review of the empirical evidence and theoretical accounts. Food Qual. Prefer. 40, 165–179. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.09.013
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., and Rangel, A. (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J. Neurosci. 27, 9984–9988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., Shiv, B., and Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 1050–1054. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706929105
Plassmann, H., Ramsøy, T. Z., and Milosavljevic, M. (2012). Branding the brain: a critical review and outlook. J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 18–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2011.11.010
Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., and Ruff, C. C. (2018). Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 174–187. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4
Reimann, M., Zaichkowsky, J., Neuhaus, C., Bender, T., and Weber, B. (2010). Aesthetic package design: A behavioral, neural, and psychological investigation. J. Cons. Psychol. 20, 431–441. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.06.009
Rangel, A. (2013). Regulation of dietary choice by the decision-making circuitry. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1717–1724. doi: 10.1038/nn.3561
Satterthwaite, M., and Fellows, L. K. (2018). Characterization of a food image stimulus set for the study of multi-attribute decision-making. MNI Open Res. 2:4. doi: 10.12688/mniopenres.12791.1
Schmidt, L., Skvortsova, V., Kullen, C., Weber, B., and Plassmann, H. (2017). How context alters value: the brain’s valuation and affective regulation system link price cues to experienced taste pleasantness. Sci. Rep. 7:8098. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08080-0
Small, D. M. (2010). Taste representation in the human insula. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 551–561. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0266-9
Small, D. M., Bender, G., Veldhuizen, M. G., Rudenga, K., Nachtigal, D., and Felsted, J. (2007). The role of the human orbitofrontal cortex in taste and flavor processing. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1121, 136–151. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.002
Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, Y. E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., and Parrish, T. (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron 39, 701–711. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00467-7
Spence, C., Velasco, C., and Petit, O. (2019). “The consumer neuroscience of packaging,” in Multisensory Packaging: Designing New Product Experiences, eds C. Velasco and C. Spence (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 319–347.
Stasi, A., Songa, G., Mauri, M., Ciceri, A., Diotallevi, F., Nardone, G., et al. (2018). Neuromarketing empirical approaches and food choice: a systematic review. Food Res. Int. 108, 650–664. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2017.11.049
Stephan, K. E., Penny, W. D., Moran, R. J., den Ouden, H. E. M., Daunizeau, J., and Friston, K. J. (2010). Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage 49, 3099–3109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.015
Steptoe, A., Pollard, T. M., and Wardle, J. (1995). Development of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite 25, 267–284. doi: 10.1006/appe.1995.0061
Suzuki, S., Adachi, R., Dunne, S., Bossaerts, P., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2015). Neural mechanisms underlying human consensus decision-making. Neuron 86, 591–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.019
Suzuki, S., Cross, L., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2017). Elucidating the underlying components of food valuation in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1780–1786. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0008-x
Suzuki, S., Jensen, E. L., Bossaerts, P., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2016). Behavioral contagion during learning about another agent’s risk-preferences acts on the neural representation of decision-risk. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 113, 3755–3760. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1600092113
Suzuki, S., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2020). Breaking human social decision making into multiple components and then putting them together again. Cortex 127, 221–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.02.014
Tang, D. W., Fellows, L. K., and Dagher, A. (2014). Behavioral and neural valuation of foods is driven by implicit knowledge of caloric content. Psychol. Sci. 25, 2168–2176. doi: 10.1177/0956797614552081
Tijssen, I. O. J. M., Smeets, P. A. M., Goedegebure, R. P. G., Zandstra, E. H., de Graaf, C., and Jager, G. (2019). Colouring perception: package colour cues affect neural responses to sweet dairy drinks in reward and inhibition related regions. Appetite 142:104378. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104378
Van der Laan, L. N., De Ridder, D. T. D., Viergever, M. A., and Smeets, P. A. M. (2012). Appearance matters: neural correlates of food choice and packaging aesthetics. PLoS One 7:e41738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041738
Keywords: food, reward, value, preference, decision-making, consumer psychology, fMRI
Citation: Motoki K and Suzuki S (2020) Extrinsic Factors Underlying Food Valuation in the Human Brain. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:131. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00131
Received: 09 May 2020; Accepted: 02 July 2020;
Published: 24 July 2020.
Edited by:
Junichi Chikazoe, National Institute for Physiological Sciences (NIPS), JapanReviewed by:
Fabian Grabenhorst, University of Cambridge, United KingdomJames Howard, Northwestern University, United States
Copyright © 2020 Motoki and Suzuki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Kosuke Motoki, motokik@myu.ac.jp; Shinsuke Suzuki, shinsuke.szk@gmail.com